- 1Anatomy and Programme in Neuroscience, Department of Medicine, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
- 2Division of Functional Neuroanatomy, Institute of Anatomy, Department of Medicine, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- 3Institute of Human Movement Sciences and Sport, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- 4Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
The calcium-binding protein parvalbumin (PV) is a recognized marker of short-axon GABA-ergic neurons in the cortex and the hippocampus. However in addition, PV is expressed by excitatory, glutamatergic neurons in various areas of the brain and spinal cord. Depending on the location of these neurons, loading of their synaptic vesicles with glutamate is mediated by either of three vesicular glutamate transporters (VGlut): VGlut1, VGlut2, or VGlut3. Driven by our interest in one of these glutamatergic/PV-expressing cell clusters—the lateral hypothalamic parvafox nucleus—we investigated the functions of this population of neurons by the selective deletion of VGlut2 expression in PV-expressing cells according to the Cre/Lox-approach. PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice are phenotypically characterized by deficits in locomotion and vocalization, by a decreased thermal nociception, and by an increased social dominance. We conducted a search of the Allen Brain Atlas for regions that might co-express the genes encoding PV and VGlut2, and that might thus contribute to the manifestation of the observed phenotypes. Our survey revealed several structures that could contribute to the deficits in locomotion and vocalization, such as the red, the subthalamic and the deep cerebellar nuclei. It also disclosed that a shift in the balance of afferental glutamatergic neurotransmission to the periaqueductal gray matter might be accountable for the decrease in sensitivity to pain and for the increase in social dominance. As a whole, this study broadens the state of knowledge about PV-expressing excitatory neurons.
Introduction
Although the calcium-binding protein parvalbumin (PV) is predominantly expressed in inhibitory, GABA-ergic neurons (Celio and Heizmann, 1981; Celio, 1986, 1990; Kawaguchi et al., 1987; Röhrenbeck et al., 1987; Stichel et al., 1988; DeFelipe et al., 1989; Demeulemeester et al., 1989, 1991; Hendry et al., 1989; Blümcke et al., 1990; Lewis and Lund, 1990; Van Brederode et al., 1990; Williams et al., 1992), its presence has also been demonstrated in specific populations of excitatory, glutamatergic neurons. Among these populations are somatosensory afferents for epicritical sensibility (Celio, 1990), corticostriatal nerve cells (Jinno and Kosaka, 2004), thalamic relay-neurons (Jones and Hendry, 1989; Jones, 2001; Tanahira et al., 2009), motor and visual areas of the neocortex (Tanahira et al., 2009), and the core region of the parvafox nucleus in the lateral hypothalamus (Girard et al., 2011; Mészár et al., 2012).
Three different types of vesicular transporters exist to load synaptic vesicles with glutamate: VGlut1, VGlut2, and VGlut3. Whereas VGlut1 is the prevalent vesicular glutamate transporter in telencephalic regions, such as the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus, VGlut2 is abundant in subcortical structures, such as the thalamus, the hypothalamus, and the brainstem (Kaneko and Fujiyama, 2002; Liguz-Lecznar and Skangiel-Kramska, 2007; El Mestikawy et al., 2011).
In addition to one of these three VGlut transporters, some populations of nerve cells co-express markers of GABA-neurotransmission [e.g., glutamate-decarboxylase (GAD) and vesicular GABA-transporter (VGaT); (Docherty et al., 1987; Boulland et al., 2004; Fremeau et al., 2004; Conti et al., 2005; Seal and Edwards, 2006; El Mestikawy et al., 2011; Vaaga et al., 2014)].
Most of the PV-expressing neurons in the somatosensory cortex that project to the striatum are glutamatergic and have been demonstrated to express VGlut1 (Jinno and Kosaka, 2004). On the other hand, the PV-expressing neurons in the core region of the parvafox nucleus have been demonstrated to express VGlut2 (Girard et al., 2011; Mészár et al., 2012). In addition, PV-expressing neurons in various thalamic relay neurons (Jones and Hendry, 1989; Jones, 2001; Lewis et al., 2001; Rotaru et al., 2005; Tanahira et al., 2009; Timbie and Barbas, 2015) and in the auditory calyx of Held (Felmy and Schneggenburger, 2004) presumably express VGlut2 as well. VGlut3 is expressed in interneurons of the cortex and hippocampus (Fremeau et al., 2004), typically known to express GABA and parvalbumin. Studies directly demonstrating a coexistence of PV and VGlut2 in neurons are largely lacking. We decided to use a different approach toward bridging this gap in knowledge.
In the present study, we investigated the phenotypic consequences of eliminating the expression of VGlut2 specifically in PV-expressing neurons using the Cre/Lox-approach. A neuron's ability for glutamatergic neurotransmission depends on the presence of the respective vesicular glutamate transporter in its synaptic membrane. Accordingly, in our Cre/Lox-mouse line, any glutamatergic neuron that expresses PV and that depends solely on VGlut2 to transport glutamate into synaptic vesicles will be deficient with regard to glutamatergic neurotransmission.
A whole battery of behavioral tests were applied to the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice. Since our study was originally driven by our interest in exploring the functions of the hypothalamic parvafox nucleus, these behavioral tests were mainly selected based on the connections of the parvafox nucleus with the periaqueductal gray matter (PAG). These connections preferentially indicate a possible involvement of the parvafox nucleus in analgesia, vocalization, and defense circuits (Celio et al., 2013; Bilella et al., 2016; Szabolcsi et al., 2017). In addition, a study that evaluated the effects of interfering with the fast synaptic inhibition onto PV-expressing neurons (Leppä et al., 2011) has also contributed to our selection of behavioral tests.
The aim of the present study was to correlate the behavioral phenotype of the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice with the regions of the brain that may contain neurons co-expressing PV and VGlut2.
Materials and Methods
Mice
B6;129P2-Pvalbtm1(cre)Arbr/J mice (hereafter called “PV-Cre”), expressing the Cre-recombinase gene under the control of the Pvalb promoter were kindly provided by Silvia Arber (FMI, Basel, Switzerland) and also purchased from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME, USA). Slc17a6tm1Lowl/J mice (hereafter called “VGlut2-Lox”), carrying a floxed Slc17a6 gene were kindly provided by Bradford Lowell (Harvard University, Boston, USA) (Tong et al., 2007). We crossed these two mouse strains to generate PvalbCre/+; Slc17a6Lox/Lox mice. Crossing these latter, we generated PvalbCre/Cre; Slc17a6Lox/Lox mice (hereafter referred to as “KO”) lacking VGlut2 protein expression in PV-expressing cells, PvalbCre/+; Slc17a6Lox/Lox mice (hereafter referred to as “HET”), in which we expect a lower Cre expression and thus lower recombinase activity, and Pvalb+/+; Slc17a6Lox/Lox mice (hereafter referred to as controls [“CTR”]). Mice were genotyped using classical PCR techniques according to protocols available from the supplier. Mice had free access to water and rodent chow and rooms were maintained under a 12 h light/dark cycle where not otherwise mentioned. This study was carried out in accordance with the institutional guidelines of the University of Fribourg. The protocol was approved by the Veterinary Commission for Animal Research of the Canton of Fribourg (permission nr. 2010-27-Fr) or of the Canton of Zurich (ZH 204/2008 and 29/2012).
Western Blot of Brain Extracts
We isolated brains of mice of all three genotypes (CTR/HET/KO) and homogenized them after removal of the telencephalon and cerebellum. We extracted soluble proteins for Western blotting as described elsewhere (Maetzler et al., 2009). We used SDS-PAGE (10%) to separate proteins (40 μg). After electrophoresis, we transferred the proteins onto nitrocellulose membranes (MS solution, Chemie Brunschwig, Basel, Switzerland) and blocked the membranes for 30 min at room temperature in 5% non-fat milk in TBS before incubating them overnight at 4°C with the primary antibody rabbit anti VGlut2 (Synaptic Systems, Göttingen, Germany), diluted 1:5,000 or 1:10,000 in 2% non-fat milk in TBS. After three rinses in TBS, we incubated the membranes for 2 h with HRP conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (Sigma-Aldrich, Buchs, Switzerland) diluted 1:7,000 in TBS. We rinsed the membranes three times, developed them using ECL technology (Merck Millipore, Schaffhausen, Switzerland) and quantified the bands with Alpha VIEW SA software (California, USA). For normalization, we used the integral of the protein signals per sample of the Ponceau Red-stained membranes.
Detection of VGlut2 by Immunofluorescence
The immunofluorescence technique was performed as previously described (Gerig and Celio, 2007; Mészár et al., 2012). In short, CTR, HET, and KO mice (n = 3/genotype) were perfused with 0.9% NaCl, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde, diluted in 0.1M phosphate buffered saline, pH 7.3. The brains were dissected and immersed in 30% sucrose for one night, and then frozen on dry ice. Eighty micrometers of thin cryomobile (Reichert Jung) sections were prepared and incubated in parallel in 24-well plates with the primary antiserum against VGlut2 (Synaptic Systems, Göttingen, Germany), diluted 1:10,000 to 1:20,000. The efficacy and specificity of this antiserum against VGlut2 was established by immunoblotting experiments (see section Western Blot of Brain Extracts). Incubation with the biotinylated secondary antibody was followed by exposure to streptavidine-Cy3 (Alexa 550). The sections were mounted on glass slides for histological inspection in either a Leica 6,000 epifluorescence microscope (equipped with a Hamamatsu C4742-95 camera) or a digital slide-scanner (Nanozoomer, Hamamatsu).
Potential Co-Expression of the Pvalb, Slc17A6, GAD, and Slc32A1 Genes
In-situ hybridization images from corresponding levels available in the Allen Brain Atlas (ABA, http://mouse.brain-map.org) (Lein et al., 2007) for the expression of Pvalb-mRNA (ABA-experiment RP_071204_01_E06) and Slc17a6 mRNA (the gene encoding VGlut2 protein; ABA-experiment RP_050921_01_E03) were compared with the aim to detect neurons potentially co-expressing both mRNAs. Each candidate area was listed, the number of a section containing the respective area in the two experiments was indicated, and the overall intensity of labeling in this area was rated [+ until +++] for both genes. The evaluation took into consideration also the shape, size, and distribution of the neurons. The same approach was chosen to detect coexistence of Pvalb/Slc17a6 with the expression of GAD and Slc32a1 (coding VGaT), two markers for GABAergic neurotransmission.
Behavioral Experiments
Behavioral experiments were conducted either at the University of Fribourg or after transportation of the animals and acclimatization for at least 1 week, at the Institute of Anatomy of the University of Zurich. The experiments described in this study involved a total of 124 adult mice of both sexes: 55 CTR (21M, 34F), 33 HET (19M, 14F), and 36 KO (18M, 18F). However, every single experiment involved only a subgroup of mice, usually of both sexes. The general health of the mice was closely monitored and mice showing signs of weakness or disease were excluded from the experiments and sacrificed. Mice were weighed regularly throughout the experiments and the mean of all measurements (1–27 measurements per mouse) was calculated for every mouse.
Nest Assessment
A group of individually housed mice (12 CTR [6M, 6F], 13 HET [8M, 5F], and 6 KO [3M, 3F]) was provided in the evening with one piece of Kleenex® but no other environmental enrichment items. The next morning, photographs were taken of every cage from three different directions of view to allow for a blinded observer to rate the quality of individual nests built overnight on a scale from 1 to 5 as described elsewhere (Deacon, 2006).
Open Field
A cohort of group-housed mice (10 CTR [5M, 5F], 9 HET [6M, 3F], and 10 KO [5M, 5F]) was subjected to an open field test in an empty polypropylene cage (550 mm long × 370 mm wide × 200 mm high). Mice were placed into the cage center and their behavior was recorded during 5 min from above by a video camera. The occurrence of rearing and grooming was recorded manually during the test, the number of fecal boli deposited by each mouse was counted at the end of the 5 min and locomotor activity was estimated from the movies as the number of cage line crossings (2 lines in both dimensions, separating the cage into a total of 9 rectangles). Line crossings were analyzed off-line using Observer XT 11 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands).
Hot Plate
To assess pain sensitivity, two different groups of mice were used. Hot plate 1 was conducted at the University of Zurich on the same group of 31 mice already used for nest assessment. Hot plate 2 was conducted at the University of Fribourg on a group of mice almost identical to the one tested in open field (7 CTR [4M, 3F], 9 HET [6M, 3F], and 8 KO [4M, 4F]). Mice were placed in a glass chamber mounted on a hot plate maintained at 52 ± 0.1°C (IITC Life Science, Woodland Hills, CA, USA). Latency to shaking and licking of hindpaws as well as to jumping was recorded. Hindpaw licking and jumping served as experimental endpoints, but if neither of the two occurred within 40 s (cut-off time), the mouse was removed from the plate to prevent tissue damage.
Vocalization Experiments
Dyadic Interactions Between Pairs of Genotype-Matched Female Mice
Out of the 20 female mice already used for ActiviScope and individually housed over several weeks, four per genotype with similar weights were chosen for this experiment. Pairs of genotype-matched mice (weight range: 15–25 g, age range 8–9 months) were allowed to interact together during 5 min, while behavior was recorded by a video camera installed from above. Every mouse served as the resident in one interaction and as the intruder in a second one, thus there was a total of four interactions per genotype. Ultrasonic vocalizations were acquired by an ultrasonic condenser microphone (CM16/CMPA, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany), located at constant distance from the cage. The acoustic signals were amplified and then digitized at 300 kHz with 16-bit resolution (UltraSoundGate 116H, Avisoft Bioacoustics) and displayed on a computer in real-time with Recorder_USGH software (Avisoft Bioacoustics). Off-line analysis of behavior was performed with Observer XT 11 by recording social investigation of the intruder by the resident to obtain total durations of anogenital, body and nose-to-nose sniffing for every interaction. The ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) emitted by the genotype-matched pairs were analyzed off-line using SASLab Pro 5.1 (Avisoft Bioacoustics) by manually selecting individual elements. Spectrograms were produced with the following parameters: FFT length = 512, frame size = 75%, window: hamming, overlap = 50%, and a cut-off frequency of 33 kHz was set to eliminate insignificant low-frequency noise. Call activity (the number of USV per minute of interaction) was determined as well as mean USV duration, mean peak frequency (the frequency at maximal amplitude within each USV) and mean peak amplitude (maximal amplitude per USV). In addition, USV were classified according to rules slightly modified from previous work of two other groups (Scattoni et al., 2008; Grimsley et al., 2011).
Dyadic Interactions Between Pairs of Genotype-Matched Male Mice
A total of 15 male mice (5 CTR, 5 HET, 5 KO; weight range: 21–30 g and age range 8–9 months for 4 CTR, 4 HET, 3 KO; 18–22 g and 3–3.5 months for 1 CTR, 1 HET, 2 KO) were tested in genotype-matched dyadic interactions. As these males were group-housed, all but one mouse were removed from the cage of the resident mouse, and a genotype-matched intruder mouse from a different cage and litter was inserted. Every individual mouse was used in 3–4 interactions (1 HET in only two) and as both resident and intruder. As described above for females, behavior and USV were recorded during 5 min. In case of a fight lasting at least 10 s, one male was removed for another 10 s and then reintroduced into the cage. Ultrasonic vocalizations produced by each pair were analyzed as described above, and Observer XT 11 served to register fight occurrence and obtain a total duration of fights for every interaction.
Dyadic Interactions Between Pairs of Genotype-Matched Mice of Opposite Sexes
Another group of 23 mice (7CTR [4M, 3F], 9 HET [5M, 4F], 7 KO [5M, 2F]; weight range: 16–26 g and age range 3–4 months) was used for dyadic interactions between a male (resident) and a female (intruder) of the same genotype. Males as well as females were group-housed, thus all cage mates were again removed from the cage of the resident and a genotype-matched intruder female from a different litter was inserted for 5 min. Every male was used once as the resident, whereas individual females were used as intruder in one to three interactions. This experiment thus involved a total of 14 interactions (4 CTR, 5 HET, 5 KO) and it was repeated 5 times with intervals of at least 2 days in between in an attempt to equalize estrous cycle phases of all females throughout the experiment. Mouse pairs were identical across the 5 experimental days, but the order of genotypes within every block of three interactions was varied systematically (KO-HET-CTR on days 1 and 2, CTR-HET-KO on days 3 and 4, HET-KO-CTR on day 5) to rule out any effects of testing order. Again, behavior and USV were recorded during 5 min and USV analyzed as described above. No action was taken when males mounted females or obviously tried to do so, but such behavior was recorded using Observer XT 11 and a total duration of mounting attempts was obtained for every interaction. In addition to call activity, cumulative USV duration was calculated as the summed duration of all individual vocalizations produced by each pair within the 5 min.
Tube Test: Male Dominance Hierarchy
A group of male mice, already used for hot plate 2 and vocalization experiments (4-5 CTR, 4 HET, and 3-5 KO), was tested for social hierarchy in the tube test as described by others (Wang et al., 2011). As mice were rather too small for the tube, a ruler was used to separate the internal diameter into two compartments, which prevented the two cagemates from crossing each other. The same pairs were tested on a total of 7 experiment days. Experiment days 1–3 were before hot plate 2, days 4–6 were after hot plate 2, and the last test was conducted on experiment day 7 after the dyadic interactions between male mice to check whether fights may have caused any changes in social dominance hierarchy. Within each cage of 3–5 males, mice were ranked by their winning times that could vary between 0 and 3 (2–3 cages) or 0 and 5 (1 cage). To make ranks comparable, the 5 mice of the larger cage could obtain ranks 1.5 and 2.5 in addition to 1, 2, and 3. Tube test data were analyzed in two different ways: a (genotype × time) ANOVA was run to investigate genotype and time effects on the obtained ranks and a chi-square test was used to detect a possible dependence of the percentage of interactions won on genotype.
Statistics
For the analysis of behavioral experiments that involved male as well as female mice, we ran two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with genotype (CTR/HET/KO or CTR/MUT) and sex (M/F) as between-subjects factors. For the analysis of behavioral experiments that involved only males, only females, or only pairs of opposite sexes, we ran one-way ANOVAs with genotype (CTR/HET/KO) as between-subjects factor. For the analysis of behavioral experiments that involved several experimental phases (three-chamber test, IntelliCage, Morris water maze), we used experimental phase as additional between-subjects factor. A Tukey test was chosen for post-hoc analysis of all ANOVAs to compare genotypes. Statistical tests other than ANOVAs are explicitly stated in the Results section (three-chamber test: T-tests; tube test: chi square test; vocalization experiments: correlation analyses). IBM SPSS Statistics 21.0 was used for all statistical analyses and to create graphs. The whisker ends in boxplots represent the most extreme values, still within 1.5 inter-quartile ranges of the quartiles displayed in the boxes. Circles in boxplots represent outliers (distance from the boxes: 1.5–3 interquartile ranges), whereas asterisks represent extreme values (distance from the boxes: >3 interquartile ranges). Normality was assumed for all comparisons of group means. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
Results
Western Blot (VGlut2 Protein Quantification)
Averaging the quantities of the VGlut2 protein calculated for the two different conditions (concentrations of the primary antibody), the mean quantity of the VGlut2 protein in the KO mouse reached 82.7% and in the HET mouse 86.2% of the CTR mouse values (set to 100%; Figure 1). It seems reasonable that 83% of VGlut2 protein remain expressed in the brain of a KO mouse, as most neurons that express VGlut2 are not expected to express PV, and VGlut2 expression has thus remained intact in these neurons.
Figure 1. Immunoblot of brain extracts: In the brain extracts of all three genotypes, the VGlut2 antiserum recognizes a band at MW of ~64 kDa, corresponding to the VGlut2 protein (arrow). The intensity of the band is stronger in the CTR than in both HET and KO mice. Quantification of the band densities with Alpha VIEW software yielded mean quantities of the VGlut2 protein in the KO of 82.7% and in the HET of 86.2% of the CTR values (set to 100%).
Detection of VGlut2 by Immunofluorescence
The immunofluorescent staining for VGlut2 was very strong in all genotypes. In overviews, it was difficult to distinguish differences between the staining of control sections and those of HET or KO mice. The immunolabelling for VGlut2 was limited to synaptic endings, whereas perikarya remained unstained. Therefore, it was not possible to confirm the absence of VGlut2 in cell bodies of the 14 brain regions in which mRNA for Pvalb and Slc17a6 may be co-expressed (see section Potential Co-expression of the Pvalb and the Slc17a6 Genes and Table S1). To circumvent this disadvantage, we searched for differences in the density of terminal fields in certain regions of the brain. We centered our attention to a zone of the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray in which axons of the PV/VGlut2 neurons of the hypothalamic parvafox nucleus terminate (Celio et al., 2013). In this so called Su3 (supraoculomotor) region, the density of terminals in CTR, HET and KO brain slices was evaluated qualitatively on Nanozoomer scans (Figure 2). There was a hint of a difference in the density of VGlut2 positive endings, but a detailed morphometric analysis would be necessary to confirm this impression.
Figure 2. Immunfluorescence staining with the VGlut-2 antiserum. The two upper nanozoomer images (A,B) show a low magnification of the periaqueductal gray, with the Su3-region demarcated on the right in CTR (A; F31) and KO (B; F33) mice. The Su3 receives inputs from the PV/VGlut2 neurons of the parvafox nucleus of the lateral hypothalamus and we supposed that a difference in density of terminals could be recognized at the light microscopic level. This is indeed the case, the immunolabelling for VGlut2-terminals being more intensive in the CTR (C and terminal density in the inset) than in the KO-Su3 (D and terminal density in the inset). Aq, aqueduct; mlf, medial longitudinal fasciculus; PAG, periaqueductal gray; Su3, supraoculomotor nucleus; Su3C, supraoculomotor cap. The asterisks in the insets indicate the approximate region in (C,D), from which the high magnification image was obtained.
Potential Co-Expression of the Pvalb and the Slc17A6 Genes
Screening of the Allen Brain Atlas revealed 65 brain regions in which the mRNAs of both Pvalb and Slc17a6 were expressed (Table S1). In most of these regions, the pattern of distribution of the label for the two types of mRNA differed substantially. However, in 14 of the regions (highlighted in gray in Table S1), the labeling for the two genes occurred at least partially in corresponding loci or occupied the entire cross-section through the respective region. Hence, these 14 regions may—to varying extents—contain neurons that co-express the two genes. The implicated regions include (1) motor areas, such as the deep cerebellar, the red and the subthalamic nuclei, and (2) sensory ones, such as the cuneate and gracile nuclei, as well as the cochlear and vestibular nuclei, and the principal sensory nucleus of the trigeminal nerve. In addition, they include (3) the caudal portion of the pontine reticular nucleus, (4) the ventral anterior-lateral complex of the thalamus, and (5) the parvafox nucleus in the hypothalamus. Each of the phenotypic traits observed in PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mice (and presented hereafter) could thus arise as a consequence of eliminating the expression of VGlut2 in the neurons of any of these 14 regions.
Potential Co-Expression of the Pvalb and the Slc17A6 Genes and Markers for GABA Transmission (GAD and Slc32A1 Genes)
To evaluate the occurrence of markers for GABA-neurotransmission in the 14 nuclei with a probable co-expression of Pvalb and Slc17a6 (coding for VGlut2), the Allen Database was screened for the expression of GAD and Slc32a1 (coding for VGaT) (Table S1). In only one brain region were these four genes expressed abundantly throughout the entire region, namely in the medial vestibular nucleus. In all other nuclei, the number of GABA and/or VGaT expressing neurons was lower or distributed differently from those expressing VGlut2 and PV.
Phenotypic Traits Resulting from the Elimination of Slc17A6 Expression in PV-Expressing Neurons
Mice in which the PV-expressing cells lacked VGlut2 (KO mice) had a lower body weight than their wild-type littermates (CTR mice, Figure 3A), and were hypoactive according to several criteria. In the open field test, hypoactivity was manifested as a reduction in locomotive and exploratory activity (Figures 4A,B). The deficits in locomotion were both qualitative and quantitative. The KO mice experienced difficulties in maintaining a horizontal body axis while walking and swimming (Figure S2C). In addition, they built nests of lower quality than the CTR mice (Figure 3B). The threshold for thermal pain appeared higher in KO and HET mice than in CTR mice (Figures 5B,D). KO mice vocalized less than CTR mice (Figures 6A, 8A), and female pairs of KO mice exclusively produced vocalizations of the less complex patterns (Figure 6F). As assessed by the tube test, KO and HET males were more dominant than CTR males (Figure 9). However, according to the three-chamber test, social behavior did not differ between genotypes (Figures S5A,B,C,E,F,G). Similarly, anxiety- and stress-related parameters (Figure 4C, Figures S4A–E), memory performance (Figures S2A, S3G,H), and circadian rhythms (Figure S1) did not differ consistently between the genotypes. Table 1 summarizes the statistically significant differences between the genotypes, whereas Table S2 summarizes sex differences and statistical interaction (genotype × sex).
Figure 3. Body weight of mice at different ages. (A) Overall body weight was lower in both KO and HET than in CTR mice. CTR mice were heavier than HET mice, and the latter were heavier than KO mice. Furthermore, male mice were heavier than female mice. Data originate from all mice used for behavioral experiments (n = 55 CTR, 31 HET, 36 KO). Nest assessment: KO mice build nests of lower quality than CTR and HET mice (B). Circles filled in light and dark gray represent data of single female and male mice, respectively. There was no sex difference in the quality of nests built overnight by the individually housed mice, however CTR mice built nests of higher quality than KO mice. Quality ratings were assessed by a blinded observer. (n = 12 CTR, 13 HET, 6 KO). (#p < 0.05, ###p < 0.001).
Figure 4. Open field: reduced locomotor- and exploratory activities and grooming. Locomotor activity, estimated from the total number of quadrant line crossings, was higher in males than in females and lower in both KO and HET than in CTR mice (A). Vertical exploration expressed as the total number of rearing was as well lower in both KO and HET than in CTR mice (B). The number of fecal boli deposited was lower in KO than in CTR (C). Boxes filled in light and dark gray represent data of female and male mice, respectively. (n = 10 CTR, 9 HET, 10 KO). (#p < 0.05, ### p < 0.001).
Figure 5. Reactivity to thermal pain: lower pain sensitivity in KO and HET than in CTR mice. Latency to shaking (A,C) and latency to endpoint (B,D) in hot plate 1 (A,B) and hot plate 2 (C,D). In both cohorts of mice, latency to endpoint was longer in both KO and HET than in CTR mice (B,D), whereas latency to hindpaw shaking did not differ between genotypes (A,C). Boxes filled in light and dark gray represent data of female and male mice, respectively. (n = 9 CTR, 12 HET, 6 KO for hot plate 1, n = 7 CTR, 9 HET, 8 KO for hot plate 2). (#p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01).
Figure 6. Dyadic interactions between female pairs of mice: lower frequency of vocalization and restricted repertoire in KO-mice. Mean call activity was lower in KO and HET than CTR pairs (A). Mean USV duration was lowest in KO and lower in HET than CTR pairs (B). Mean peak frequency was higher in KO than CTR pairs (C). There was a trend for a higher total duration of resident → intruder sniffing (all body areas) in KO and HET than in CTR pairs (D). In the sample as a whole, we found a positive correlation between mean call activity and the total duration of resident → intruder sniffing (all body areas) (E). Black triangles, circles filled in gray and empty squares represent data of KO, HET and CTR pairs, respectively. KO pairs produced only USV belonging to the less complex types, whereas both HET and CTR pairs produced USV of the entire repertoire (F). (n = 4 CTR, 4 HET, 4 KO). (#p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01, ###p < 0.001).
Motricity
The two most obvious phenotypic characteristics of the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice compared to their wild-type littermates were their low body weight (Figure 3A) [F(2, 1, 193) = 140.79, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p < 0.001, HET vs. KO p < 0.001, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001] and their hypoactivity, which, in the open field (Figure 4A) and ActiviScope Tests (Figure S1) was manifested as a reduction in locomotor activity [open field, number of line crossings: F(2, 23) = 10.26, p = 0.001; CTR vs. HET p = 0.02, HET vs. KO p = 0.46, CTR vs. KO p = 0.001; ActiviScope, homecage activity counts: F(2, 45) = 17.31, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p < 0.001, HET vs. KO p = 0.73, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001]. In the open field test, KO and HET mice also reared less than CTR mice (Figure 4B) [rearing number: F(2, 23) = 13.64, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p = 0.03, HET vs. KO p = 0.56, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001]; likewise, KO mice groomed less than CTR mice (not shown). In the IntelliCage test, KO and HET mice approached the corners of the cage less often than CTR mice (Figure S3A) [e.g., FAP: F(1, 72) = 68.0, p < 0.001], but remained within its confines longer (Figure S3C) [F(1, 52) = 16.1, p < 0.001]. Moreover, the KO mice drank an insufficient quantity of water, and thus had to be removed from the IntelliCage. On the other hand, neither in the elevated zero maze (Figure S4F) [F(1, 23) = 1.36, p = 0.26], nor in the three-chamber test (Figures S5D,H) [F(2, 66) = 1.43, p = 0.25; CTR vs. HET p = 0.30, HET vs. KO p = 0.56, CTR vs. KO p = 0.94], did locomotor activity, defined as the total distance traveled, differ between genotypes. In these two tests, other performance-determining factors presumably masked the genotypic differences in locomotor activity, which were obvious in the more basic tests.
Deficits in locomotion were most obvious in the KO-mice, but, as aforementioned, were also apparent to some extent in the HET-genotype. KO-mice had a wobbly gait, which worsened with increasing age. They tended to overbalance when walking on solid ground and were usually unable to maintain a horizontal body axis whilst swimming. This deficit probably accounted for the marked tendency of KO- and HET-mice to swim in circles (Figure S2C), rather than in straight lines, in the Morris water maze [cumulative circling: F(2, 20) = 15.0, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p = 0.001, HET vs. KO p = 0.053, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001]. The KO mice that completed this test also swam significantly less rapidly than their wild-type littermates (Figure S2B) [F(2, 20) = 8.66, p = 0.002; CTR vs. HET p = 0.068, HET vs. KO p = 0.031, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001].
Pain
One of the clearest genotypic differences was revealed by the hot plate test (Figure 5). Both the KO- and the HET-mice licked their hindpaws or jumped off the plate significantly later than CTR-mice (Figures 5B,D) [hot plate 1: F(2, 21) = 7.42, p = 0.004; CTR vs. HET p = 0.003, HET vs. KO p = 1.00, CTR vs. KO p = 0.015; hot plate 2: F(2, 18) = 5.98, p = 0.01; CTR vs. HET p = 0.036, HET vs. KO p = 0.92, CTR vs. KO p = 0.02]. Interestingly, there were no genotypic differences in the latency to hindpaw shaking (Figures 5A,C) [hot plate 1: F(2, 21) = 1.96, p = 0.17; hot plate 2: F(2, 18) = 2.83, p = 0.09]. This finding indicates that the three groups of mice all experienced the sensation of heat after a comparable delay, but that the threshold for the perception of thermal pain was higher in the KO- and the HET-mice than in CTR-mice.
Vocalization
During the dyadic interactions between two females, the frequency of the ultrasonic vocalizations that were produced (mean call activity) was lower in the KO- and in the HET- than in the CTR-mice (Figure 6A) [F(2, 9) = 51.07, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p < 0.001, HET vs. KO p = 0.17, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001]. During the interactions between two males, this parameter was tendentially lower in the KO- and the HET- than in the CTR-mice (Figure 7A) [F(2, 20) = 3.37, p = 0.055; CTR vs. HET p = 0.099, HET vs. KO p = 1.0, CTR vs. KO p = 0.082]. During the interactions between two females (Figure 6B) [F(2, 9) = 80.40, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p < 0.001, HET vs. KO p = 0.019, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001] and between a female and a male (Figure 8D) [F(2, 55) = 12.30, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p = 0.021, HET vs. KO p = 0.046, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001], but not during those between two males (not shown), the duration of the ultrasonic vocalizations was shorter in the KO- than in the HET-mice, and shorter in the latter than in CTR-mice. Likewise during the interactions between two females (Figure 6C) [F(2, 9) = 4.40, p = 0.046; CTR vs. HET p = 0.29, HET vs. KO p = 0.40, CTR vs. KO p = 0.038] and between a female and a male (Figure 8E) [F(2, 55) = 3.63, p = 0.034; CTR vs. HET p = 0.11, HET vs. KO p = 0.83, CTR vs. KO p = 0.033], but not between two males (not shown), the peak frequency of the ultrasonic vocalizations was higher in the KO-than in the CTR-mice. Genotypic differences in the peak amplitude of the ultrasonic vocalizations were observed only during the interactions between a female and a male, with that in KO- and HET-mice being lower than that in the CTR-mice (Figure 8F) [F(2, 55) = 13.33, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p = 0.008, HET vs. KO p = 0.094, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001). Furthermore during the interactions between two females, mean call activity correlated with the total duration of resident → intruder sniffing (all body areas) (Figure 6E).
Figure 7. Dyadic interactions between male pairs of mice: lower fight duration in KO mice. Mean call activity tended to be lower in HET and KO than in CTR pairs (A). Total duration of fights was lower in KO than in CTR pairs (HET were indifferent from both other groups due to high variability) (B). (n = 5 CTR, 5 HET, 5 KO). (#p < 0.05).
Figure 8. Dyadic interactions between pairs of opposite sexes: difference in duration, frequency and amplitude between MUT and CTR mice. Mean call activity was lower in KO than in HET and CTR pairs (A). Total duration of mounting attempts was lower in KO than in CTR pairs (B). In the sample as a whole, we found a positive correlation between the total duration of USV and the total duration of mounting attempts (C). Mean USV duration was shorter in KO and HET than in CTR pairs (D). Mean peak frequency was higher in KO than in CTR pairs (E). Finally, mean peak amplitude was lower in KO and HET than in CTR pairs (F). None of the parameters was significantly altered by repetition. (n = 7 CTR, 9 HET, 7 KO). (#p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01, ###p < 0.001).
Aggression and Dominance
During the dyadic interactions between the genotype-matched males, the readiness to fight was less marked in KO- than in CTR-mice (Figure 7B) [F(2, 20) = 3.68, p = 0.044; CTR vs. HET p = 0.24, HET vs. KO p = 0.64, CTR vs. KO p = 0.037]. Whether the KO-males were indeed less aggressive than the CTR-males could not be ascertained, since the general hypoactivity of the former animals could have accounted for the findings. Inter-genotypic trials (tube test) were more often won by the HET- and the KO-mice than by CTR-mice (Figure 9) [F(2, 68) = 26.1, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p < 0.001, HET vs. KO p = 0.48, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001]. These findings indicate that the mice in which the VGlut2-dependent glutamatergic neurotransmission of the PV-expressing neurons was suppressed manifested behavioral dominance over their wild-type littermates.
Figure 9. Social dominance: HET and KO mice are more dominant than CTR mice. In the tube test, 1 was the most common rank among both HET and KO mice, whereas 3 was the most common rank among CTR mice (A). Ranks remained very stable over time. (B) shows the development of the mean rank over time for three groups of mice based on their rank reached in the first test, whereas (C) shows the development of the mean rank per genotype over time. Of all their interactions, HET, KO and CTR mice won 82.9, 54.7, and 16.7%, respectively (D). (n = 5 CTR, 4 HET, 5 KO). (###p < 0.001).
Social Interactions and Reproduction
In the dyadic interactions, significant genotypic differences occurred in the total duration of body sniffing only between two females (not shown), being shorter in the KO- and the HET- than in the CTR-mice [F(2, 9) = 6.66, p = 0.017; CTR vs. HET p = 0.042, HET vs. KO p = 0.89, CTR vs. KO p = 0.020]. With respect to the other types of sniffing, as well as to the parameter of sniffing in general (Figure 6D) [F(2, 9) = 2.12, p = 0.18; CTR vs. HET p = 0.66, HET vs. KO p = 0.51, CTR vs. KO p = 0.16], trends in the same direction were observed. In accord with our data on vocalizations, this finding suggests that the KO- and the HET-mice might be less socially motivated than the CTR-mice.
In the dyadic interactions between a male and a female, the total duration of the male's attempts to mount the females was shorter in the KO- than in the CTR-mice (Figure 8B) [F(2, 55) = 5.03, p = 0.010; CTR vs. HET p = 0.106, HET vs. KO p = 0.49, CTR vs. KO p = 0.007]. However, whether the findings really reflected differences in sexual motivation or merely differences in general locomotor activity could not be ascertained.
Our attempts to interbreed the KO-mice generally failed. This finding suggests that they were less fertile than the HET-mice, which produced most of the pups used in the present study.
(Spatial) Memory
The IntelliCage test afforded no evidence of the existence of memory deficits in the KO- and the HET-mice. On the contrary, they tended to perform even better than the CTR-genotype, making fewer errors in some of the tasks (Figures S3G,H) [PPRA: F(1, 56) = 11.6, p = 0.001; PPRP: F(1, 55) = 12.9, p < 0.001].
In the Morris water maze, none of the genotypes learned to find their way to the hidden platform. However, according to most of the measures of training performance (e.g., escape latency) (Figure S2A) [acquisition phase: F(2, 20) = 6.17, p = 0.008; CTR vs. HET p = 0.02, HET vs. KO p = 0.47, CTR vs. KO p = 0.005; reversal phase: F(2, 20) = 12.9, p < 0.001; CTR vs. HET p < 0.001, HET vs. KO p = 0.65, CTR vs. KO p < 0.001], the KO- and the HET-mice performed less well than the CTR-mice. Once again, an unambiguous interpretation of these findings is not possible, since the locomotor deficits of the KO- and the HET-mice might have contributed to their poor performance in this test. Particularly the tendencies of the KO-mice to swim in circles (Figure S2C) and to float on the surface of water (Figure S2D) [F(2, 20) = 2.35, p = 0.12; CTR vs. HET p = 0.59, HET vs. KO p = 0.11, CTR vs. KO p = 0.028] increased the likelihood of their requiring more time than the CTR-mice to locate the platform. We also observed that the KO-and the HET-mice hit the platform without subsequently attempting to climb onto it. Factors such as the degree of motivation or of exhaustion may have been of relevance in this context.
On the basis of the data gleaned from the IntelliCage test and the Morris water maze, we presume that the KO- and the HET-mice manifested no signs of deficits in memory.
Anxiety and Stress
The parameters deemed to reflect anxiety or stress in the elevated zero maze revealed no consistent differences between KO-mice and their wild-type littermates (Figure S4) [entries into open sectors: F(1, 23) = 9.68, p = 0.005 [CTR>MUT]; time in open sectors: F(1, 23) = 0.01, p = 0.91; unprotected head dips: F(1, 23) = 0.95, p = 0.34; stretched attend postures: F(1, 23) = 6.47, p = 0.02 [CTR>MUT]; fecal boli deposited: F(1, 23) = 10.60, p = 0.003 [CTR>MUT]]. In accordance with this, thigmotaxis in the Morris water maze did not differ between genotypes (not shown).
Discussion
We generated a mouse line in which the expression of the Slc17a6 gene, which encodes the vesicular glutamate transporter protein VGlut2, was selectively eliminated from PV-expressing neurons. These PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice allowed us to study the phenotypic consequences of eliminating VGlut2-dependent glutamatergic neurotransmission in this neuronal population. The efficacy of such a Cre-Lox approach, used in combination with Slc17a6-Lox mutant mice, has been confirmed previously (Tong et al., 2007; Wallén-Mackenzie et al., 2009; Birgner et al., 2010). Knocking out VGlut2 in PV- neurons with the Cre/Lox technique leads to a loss of ~17% of this neurotransmitter transporter in the brain, and this decrease in concentration could be documented pictorially in individual brain nuclei rich in PV/VGlut2 terminals (e.g., Su3 of the PAG). Since most PV-expressing neurons in the central nervous system use GABA as a neurotransmitter, their function remains intact. In contrast, the entirety of neurons that rely solely on VGlut2 to load the synaptic vesicles with glutamate, and that in addition express PV, are deficient in glutamatergic neurotransmission. However, not all these PV/VGlut2 neurons are functionally silenced; those that co-express GABA-markers (GAD and VGaT) may still release GABA from nerve terminals (Root et al., 2014). Out of the 14 brain regions that may co-express the mRNAs of both Pvalb and Slc17a6 (see Table S1), the medial vestibular nucleus is the only region, in which the same neuron express markers of GABA-ergic neurotransmission (GAD or VGaT) (screening of the Allen Brain Atlas, Table S1). It is thus conceivable, that in PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice, medial vestibular neurons that express PV are deficient in glutamatergic-, but are still capable of GABA-ergic neurotransmission. This fact may influence the control of posture, thus contributing to the difficulties in locomotion observed in the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mice (see section Locomotion).
In these “dual neurotransmitter” neurons, co-transmission of two “antagonistic” compounds involves spatial segregation of vesicles (Hattori et al., 1991; El Mestikawy et al., 2011; Vaaga et al., 2014), encoding of parallel signals that operate along different timescales (El Mestikawy et al., 2011) and differential release (Burnstock, 2007), depending on Ca2+ sensitivities (Vaaga et al., 2014). Co-released transmitters may also influence distinct target neurons (Nusbaum et al., 2001) and the packaging- (El Mestikawy et al., 2011) and release of one neurotransmitter may also be modulated by the other (Docherty et al., 1987). The deletion of VGluts in monoamine and cholinergic neurons leads to altered anxiety-related behaviors, sensitivity to psychostimulants and locomotion (Birgner et al., 2010; El Mestikawy et al., 2011).
The aims of the present study were to characterize the phenotype of the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice using a battery of behavioral tests, and to investigate the brain regions which may underlie these phenotypic traits, of which the most conspicuous included deficits in locomotion and vocalization, decreased sensitivity to thermal pain and increased dominance.
Locomotion
The observed deficits in locomotion may stem from disturbances in motor centers that rely on VGlut2 for synaptic transmission (e.g., the red, the subthalamic or the deep cerebellar nuclei) (Table S1), or from a predominance of GABA-ergic transmission, e.g., in the neurons of the medial vestibular nucleus, which normally mediates body posture. Alternatively, some locomotor deficits could arise indirectly, via a disturbance in motivational drive. The wobbly gait might be attributed to a failure in kinesthetic sensibility, which would be a consequence of the silencing of neurons in the gracile and the cuneate nuclei (Table S1) that depend on VGlut2 to process proprioceptive information arriving from the periphery. The VGlut2-positive ventral anterolateral complex of the thalamus (Table S1), which is involved in the coordination and planning of movement might also be involved. In the Morris water maze, KO mice tended to float on the surface of the water. Although this trait probably reflects a motor deficit, it could also be attributable to a lack of energy (exhaustion) or motivation to find the platform and escape from the water.
Ultrasonic Vocalization
On the basis of data gleaned from experiments with squirrel monkeys, two separate vocal pathways have been proposed. The limbic pathway, which relays in the hypothalamus (Velley, 1986; Wild et al., 2003) and PAG (Carrive and Morgan, 2012), is involved in non-verbal emotional vocal expression (Jürgens and Zwirner, 1996). It is also presumed to control the amplitude, pitch and intonation of vocalizations (Davis et al., 1996). The role of the hypothalamus in the limbic vocal pathway might be a modulation of the preparedness to vocalize (Dujardin and Jurgens, 2006). The PAG, on the other hand, has been postulated to participate in the induction of relaxed emotional states (Jürgens, 1991). Not only primates, but also mice produce emotional vocal expressions. Ultrasonic vocalizations of mice—both interaction-induced and female-induced—are today believed to not only bear a communicative signal facilitating the establishment of social contacts (Panksepp et al., 2007; Portfors, 2007; Hammerschmidt et al., 2009; Scattoni et al., 2011), but also to reflect a positive affective state (Panksepp and Lahvis, 2007; Panksepp et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2008; Malkesman et al., 2010).
In our study, we found PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice to vocalize less than control mice (mean call activity). Moreover, whereas female pairs of CTR and HET mice emitted USV of all 11 types that we distinguished, female pairs of KO mice produced only five types characterized by less complex patterns (short, flat, upward, downward, and chevron calls). Both the reduced call activity (Lahvis et al., 2011) and the reduced spectrum of vocalization patterns (Chabout et al., 2012) observed in PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice appear to indicate reduced positive affect and/or a reduced level of social motivation.
The neurons of the hypothalamic parvafox nucleus that are functionally connected to the limbic pathway of vocalizations and project to the PAG (Celio et al., 2013; Bilella et al., 2016) have presumably been impacted by the abolishment of the glutamatergic signaling, which could explain the observed deficits in vocalization. This hypothesis is supported by findings from our own previous studies, which revealed tickled rats to produce significantly fewer 50-kHz calls after excitotoxic lesioning of the parvafox nucleus than before (Roccaro-Waldmeyer et al., 2016). Alternatively, the observed deficits in vocalization might result from a deficit in the fine motoric control of the laryngeal musculature, caused by a disruption of VGlut2 dependent glutamatergic transmission in corresponding motor centers (Table S1).
Pain
An adequate interpretation of findings appertaining to the perception of pain in the hot plate test could conceivably be hampered by deficits in locomotion. However, unlike in the Morris water maze, the KO- and the HET-mice responded to the stimulus in a manner indistinguishable from that in CTR-mice. Furthermore, the administration of drugs that impair motor performance neither prolongs the endpoint latencies in the hot plate test nor interferes with the effect of morphine on this parameter (Plummer et al., 1991). We therefore conclude that an increase in the threshold for the perception of pain, and not a deficit in locomotion, was responsible for the delayed responses of the KO- and the HET-mice to the heat stimulus.
The hot plate test is presumed to measure the central (supraspinal) rather than the spinal components of the pathways that contribute to nociception (Caggiula et al., 1995; Bourne et al., 2014). Only three of the structures implicated in nociception pathways were revealed in the survey of the Allen Brain Atlas to potentially co-express PV and VGlut2, namely, the PAG, the anterior pretectal and the ventral posterior nucleus of the thalamus, the last of which relays information borne by spinothalamic pain afferents to the somatosensory cortex. Although the ventral posterior nucleus is listed in Table S1 as a site in which both PV- and VGlut2-expressing neurons occur, the putatively double-labeled population is comparatively small. An involvement of the anterior pretectal thalamic nucleus is also unlikely, since the mRNA expression patterns of Pvalb and Slc17a6 are not suggestive of substantial co-localization. Moreover, activation of the anterior pretectal thalamic nucleus results in anti-nociception (Rees and Roberts, 1993). Hence, any interference with its function would lower not raise the threshold for the perception of pain. This rational process of elimination leaves the PAG, the lateral and ventrolateral quadrants of which play an important role in processing and mediating the responses to somatic and visceral noxious stimuli (Mouton and Holstege, 1998). These lateral and ventrolateral quadrants are the terminus of inputs that stem from the lateral hypothalamic parvafox nucleus (see Figure 2), that co-express PV and VGlut2 (Celio et al., 2013; Bilella et al., 2016), and that are thus functionally silenced in PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice. It remains to be established whether the imbalance caused by the absence of the hypothalamic input contributes to the elevated threshold for the perception of thermal pain that was observed in the KO- and the HET-mice.
Aggression and Dominance
The tube test was first introduced as a model to measure dominance in 1961 (Lindzey et al., 1961), and it is still considered superior to other approaches that have since been elaborated to identify social hierarchies (Wang et al., 2014). The hierarchical ranks established by our mice remained very stable over the seven test days. In a previous study, identical ranking positions were achieved on two subsequent test days in 59% of cases (Wang et al., 2011). In our group of animals, the prevalence was 87% (65 out of 75 cases). The medial prefrontal cortex, which is a source of afferents to the lateral hypothalamus, has been implicated in behavioral traits related to the hierarchy of dominance (Zink et al., 2008; Chiao, 2010; Wang et al., 2011). Other regions that are implicated include all other areas of the prefrontal cortex, portions of the occipitotemporal and the parietal cortices, and subcortical structures, such as the amygdalar and the dorsal raphe nuclei, the hypothalamus and the PAG (Wang et al., 2014). On the basis of our survey of the Allen Brain Atlas, the anterior amygdalar area and small assemblages of neurons in many cortical zones putatively co-express PV and VGlut2, and may thus contribute to the phenotypical trait of dominance in the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice. The hypothalamus and the PAG are implicated in defense and aggression (Hess, 1949) and as well listed in Table S1 as possible sites in which subpopulations of neurons co-express PV and VGlut2. The lateral PAG is the terminus of projections from the hypothalamic parvafox nucleus (Bilella et al., 2016), and it has long been thought to be the seat of defensive behavior in threatening situations (Bandler and Keay, 1996; Bandler et al., 2000). It is conceivable that the superior performance of the KO- and the HET-mice in the dominance-gauging tube test may be associated with functional competence of this lateral hypothalamic nucleus.
Social Interaction
Data gleaned from the dyadic interactions suggested that KO-mice might be less socially orientated than CTR-mice. However, in the three-chamber test, no genotypic differences in either sociability or social novelty were observed. Although the three genotypes performed as expected during the sociability phase (more time spent in the side chamber with the social stimulus), none of them did so during the phase of social novelty (more time spent in the side chamber with the newly introduced mouse). Deficits in locomotion could conceivably have influenced the findings in the KO-mice. But in the face of this possibility is the negating circumstance that the total distance traveled did not differ between the genotypes. Hence, the three-chamber test afforded no evidence of genotypic differences in social intercourse. Other sociability tests that depend less heavily on locomotor performance would help to eliminate the ambiguities.
KO-mice built nests of inferior quality, as compared to CTR-mice. Laboratory mice generally value greatly nest-building material, and they are usually highly motivated to undertake this task (Roper, 1973; Van De Weerd et al., 1998; Jirkof, 2014). A reduced performance in nest-building could be indicative of a depressed state of well-being (Arras et al., 2007; Deacon, 2012; Jirkof et al., 2013; Jirkof, 2014), which, together with a lack of motivation, might account for the poorer quality of the nests built by the KO-mice.
Novelty and Limitations
To our knowledge, this study represents the first attempt toward a brain-wide description of the population of neurons co-expressing PV and VGlut2. Prior studies in this context were purely anatomic and have merely focused on one specific brain region (most often thalamic nuclei). This study provides evidence for further brain regions, among them motor and sensory areas, to also contain neurons co-expressing PV and VGlut2, and gives an idea of the functional significance of the entirety of these neurons.
The limitations of this study consist in the unfeasibility of a brain-wide direct demonstration of PV and VGlut2 co-expression on a cellular level, as well as in the unpredictability of the changes arising secondary to the mutation per se. As a consequence of the functional silencing of the co-expressing neurons, downstream neurons are deprived of glutamatergic input activity and thus expected to be affected as well. The nature of the changes caused to downstream neurons depends on many factors. Conceivable are changes in spine number and/or spine shape, in the distribution of postsynaptic glutamate receptors, and even in downstream signaling machinery (Yuste and Bonhoeffer, 2004; Turrigiano, 2008; Sigler et al., 2017). Furthermore, in those neurons with dual neurotransmission (glutamate and GABA) (Seal and Edwards, 2006; Zander et al., 2010; El Mestikawy et al., 2011; Vaaga et al., 2014), the deficiency in one neurotransmitter (glutamate) could lead to dominance of the remaining one (GABA).
Conclusion
The locomotor deficits of the PV-Cre;VGlut2-Lox mutant mice most likely stem from one or several of the motoric brain structures that co-express PV and VGlut2. The vocalization deficits might likewise reflect a disturbance of the fine motor control of the laryngeal musculature; an interference with the limbic vocal pathway at the level of the hypothalamic-PAG axis could also play a role. The deficits in thermal nociception and possibly also the increase in social dominance are probably explained, at least partially, by an impaired control of the PAG by afferents that are deprived of VGlut2 in this line of mutant mice.
Author Contributions
DR-W designed and conducted the experiments, analyzed and interpreted results, and wrote the paper. DM conducted experiments, analyzed and interpreted results. EV designed and conducted experiments. LP designed and conducted experiments, analyzed and interpreted results. DW designed the experiments, analyzed and interpreted results and wrote the paper. FG and MC designed the experiments and wrote the paper.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Ingrid Drescher, Claudia Meyer, Simone Eichenberger and Laurence Clément for their excellent technical support, and Silvia Arber (FMI, Basel) and Bradford Lowell (BIDMC, Harvard University) for the generous gift of mice.
This work was supported by the Canton of Fribourg and the Swiss National Foundation (31003A-144036 and 31103A-160325).
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00146/full#supplementary-material
Abbreviations
ABA, Allen Brain Atlas; CTR, control(s) (PV+/+;VGlut2Lox/Lox); F, female; HET, heterozygous (PVCre/+;VGlut2Lox/Lox); KO, knock-out (PVCre/Cre;VGlut2Lox/Lox); M, male; MUT, mutants (HET+KO); PAG, periaqueductal gray; PV, parvalbumin protein; Pvalb, gene encoding the protein parvalbumin; Slc17a6, gene encoding the protein Solute Carrier Family 17, Member 6 (= VGlut2); USV, ultrasonic vocalizations;VGlut2, vesicular glutamate transporter 2, encoded by the gene Slc17a6.
References
Arras, M., Rettich, A., Cinelli, P., Kasermann, H. P., and Burki, K. (2007). Assessment of post-laparotomy pain in laboratory mice by telemetric recording of heart rate and heart rate variability. BMC Vet. Res. 3:16. doi: 10.1186/1746-6148-3-16
Bandler, R., and Keay, K. A. (1996). Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray and the integration of emotional expression. Prog. Brain Res. 107, 285–300. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61871-3
Bandler, R., Keay, K. A., Floyd, N., and Price, J. (2000). Central circuits mediating patterned autonomic activity during active vs. passive emotional coping. Brain Res. Bull. 53, 95–104. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00313-0
Bilella, A., Alvarez-Bolado, G., and Celio, M. R. (2016). The Foxb1-expressing neurons of the ventrolateral hypothalamic parvafox nucleus project to defensive circuits. J. Comp. Neurol. 524, 2955–2981. doi: 10.1002/cne.24057
Birgner, C., Nordenankar, K., Lundblad, M., Mendez, J. A., Smith, C., Le Greves, M., et al. (2010). VGLUT2 in dopamine neurons is required for psychostimulant-induced behavioral activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 389–394. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910986107
Blümcke, I., Hof, P. R., Morrison, J. H., and Celio, M. R. (1990). Distribution of parvalbumin immunoreactivity in the visual cortex of Old World monkeys and humans. J. Comp. Neurol. 301, 417–432. doi: 10.1002/cne.903010307
Boulland, J. L., Qureshi, T., Seal, R. P., Rafiki, A., Gundersen, V., Bergersen, L. H., et al. (2004). Expression of the vesicular glutamate transporters during development indicates the widespread corelease of multiple neurotransmitters. J. Comp. Neurol. 480, 264–280. doi: 10.1002/cne.20354
Bourne, S., Machado, A. G., and Nagel, S. J. (2014). Basic anatomy and physiology of pain pathways. Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am. 25, 629–638. doi: 10.1016/j.nec.2014.06.001
Burnstock, G. (2007). Physiology and pathophysiology of purinergic neurotransmission. Physiol. Rev. 87, 659–797. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00043.2006
Caggiula, A. R., Epstein, L. H., Perkins, K. A., and Saylor, S. (1995). Different methods of assessing nicotine-induced antinociception may engage different neural mechanisms. Psychopharmacology 122, 301–306. doi: 10.1007/BF02246552
Carrive, P., and Morgan, M. M. (2012). “Periaqueductal Gray,” in The Human Nervous System, 3rd Edn., eds J. K. Mai and G. Paxinos (Amsterdam: Academic Press; Elsevier), 367–400.
Celio, M. R. (1986). Parvalbumin in most gamma-aminobutyric acid-containing neurons of the rat cerebral cortex. Science 231, 995–997. doi: 10.1126/science.3945815
Celio, M. R. (1990). Calbindin D-28k and parvalbumin in the rat nervous system. Neuroscience 35, 375–475. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(90)90091-H
Celio, M. R., and Heizmann, C. W. (1981). Calcium-binding protein parvalbumin as a neuronal marker. Nature 293, 300–302. doi: 10.1038/293300a0
Celio, M. R., Babalian, A., Ha, Q. H., Eichenberger, S., Clement, L., Marti, C., et al. (2013). Efferent connections of the parvalbumin-positive (PV1) nucleus in the lateral hypothalamus of rodents. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 3133–3153. doi: 10.1002/cne.23344
Chabout, J., Serreau, P., Ey, E., Bellier, L., Aubin, T., Bourgeron, T., et al. (2012). Adult male mice emit context-specific ultrasonic vocalizations that are modulated by prior isolation or group rearing environment. PLoS ONE 7:e29401. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029401
Chiao, J. Y. (2010). Neural basis of social status hierarchy across species. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 803–809. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.006
Conti, F., Candiracci, C., and Fattorini, G. (2005). Heterogeneity of axon terminals expressing VGLUT1 in the cerebral neocortex. Arch. Ital. Biol. 143, 127–132. doi: 10.4449/aib.v143i2.351
Davis, P. J., Zhang, S. P., Winkworth, A., and Bandler, R. (1996). Neural control of vocalization: respiratory and emotional influences. J. Voice 10, 23–38. doi: 10.1016/S0892-1997(96)80016-6
Deacon, R. (2012). Assessing burrowing, nest construction, and hoarding in mice. J. Visual. Exp. e2607. doi: 10.3791/2607
Deacon, R. M. (2006). Assessing nest building in mice. Nat. Protoc. 1, 1117–1119. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.170
DeFelipe, J., Hendry, S. H., and Jones, E. G. (1989). Visualization of chandelier cell axons by parvalbumin immunoreactivity in monkey cerebral cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 2093–2097. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.6.2093
Demeulemeester, H., Arckens, L., Vandesande, F., Orban, G. A., Heizmann, C. W., and Pochet, R. (1991). Calcium binding proteins and neuropeptides as molecular markers of GABAergic interneurons in the cat visual cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 84, 538–544. doi: 10.1007/BF00230966
Demeulemeester, H., Vandesande, F., Orban, G. A., Heizmann, C. W., and Pochet, R. (1989). Calbindin D-28K and parvalbumin immunoreactivity is confined to two separate neuronal subpopulations in the cat visual cortex, whereas partial coexistence is shown in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Neurosci. Lett. 99, 6–11. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90255-3
Docherty, M., Bradford, H. F., and Wu, J. Y. (1987). Co-release of glutamate and aspartate from cholinergic and GABAergic synaptosomes. Nature 330, 64–66. doi: 10.1038/330064a0
Dujardin, E., and Jurgens, U. (2006). Call type-specific differences in vocalization-related afferents to the periaqueductal gray of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Behav. Brain Res. 168, 23–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.10.006
El Mestikawy, S., Wallen-Mackenzie, A., Fortin, G. M., Descarries, L., and Trudeau, L. E. (2011). From glutamate co-release to vesicular synergy: vesicular glutamate transporters. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 204–216. doi: 10.1038/nrn2969
Felmy, F., and Schneggenburger, R. (2004). Developmental expression of the Ca2+-binding proteins calretinin and parvalbumin at the calyx of Held of rats and mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 1473–1482. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03604.x
Fremeau, R. T. Jr., Voglmaier, S., Seal, R. P., and Edwards, R. H. (2004). VGLUTs define subsets of excitatory neurons and suggest novel roles for glutamate. Trends Neurosci. 27, 98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.11.005
Gerig, A. T., and Celio, M. R. (2007). The human lateral tuberal nucleus: immunohistochemical characterization and analogy to the rodent PV1-nucleus. Brain Res. 1139, 110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.093
Girard, F., Meszar, Z., Marti, C., Davis, F. P., and Celio, M. (2011). Gene expression analysis in the parvalbumin-immunoreactive PV1 nucleus of the mouse lateral hypothalamus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1934–1943. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07918.x
Grimsley, J. M., Monaghan, J. J., and Wenstrup, J. J. (2011). Development of social vocalizations in mice. PLoS ONE 6:e17460. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017460
Hammerschmidt, K., Radyushkin, K., Ehrenreich, H., and Fischer, J. (2009). Female mice respond to male ultrasonic ‘songs' with approach behaviour. Biol. Lett. 5, 589–592. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0317
Hattori, T., Takada, M., Moriizumi, T., and Van Der Kooy, D. (1991). Single dopaminergic nigrostriatal neurons form two chemically distinct synaptic types: possible transmitter segregation within neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 309, 391–401. doi: 10.1002/cne.903090308
Hendry, S. H., Jones, E. G., Emson, P. C., Lawson, D. E., Heizmann, C. W., and Streit, P. (1989). Two classes of cortical GABA neurons defined by differential calcium binding protein immunoreactivities. Exp. Brain Res. 76, 467–472. doi: 10.1007/BF00247904
Jinno, S., and Kosaka, T. (2004). Parvalbumin is expressed in glutamatergic and GABAergic corticostriatal pathway in mice. J. Comp. Neurol. 477, 188–201. doi: 10.1002/cne.20246
Jirkof, P. (2014). Burrowing and nest building behavior as indicators of well-being in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 234, 139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.02.001
Jirkof, P., Fleischmann, T., Cesarovic, N., Rettich, A., Vogel, J., and Arras, M. (2013). Assessment of postsurgical distress and pain in laboratory mice by nest complexity scoring. Lab. Anim. 47, 153–161. doi: 10.1177/0023677213475603
Jones, E. G. (2001). The thalamic matrix and thalamocortical synchrony. Trends Neurosci. 24, 595–601. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01922-6
Jones, E. G., and Hendry, S. H. (1989). Differential calcium binding protein immunoreactivity distinguishes classes of relay neurons in monkey thalamic nuclei. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1, 222–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1989.tb00791.x
Jürgens, U. (1991). “Neurochemical study of PAG control of vocal behavior,” in The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray Matter, eds A. Depaulis and R. Bandler (Munich: Springer), 11–21.
Jürgens, U., and Zwirner, P. (1996). The role of the periaqueductal grey in limbic and neocortical vocal fold control. Neuroreport 7, 2921–2923. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199611250-00023
Kaneko, T., and Fujiyama, F. (2002). Complementary distribution of vesicular glutamate transporters in the central nervous system. Neurosci. Res. 42, 243–250. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00009-3
Kawaguchi, Y., Katsumaru, H., Kosaka, T., Heizmann, C. W., and Hama, K. (1987). Fast spiking cells in rat hippocampus (CA1 region) contain the calcium-binding protein parvalbumin. Brain Res. 416, 369–374. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90921-8
Lahvis, G. P., Alleva, E., and Scattoni, M. L. (2011). Translating mouse vocalizations: prosody and frequency modulation. Genes Brain Behav. 10, 4–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00603.x
Lein, E. S., Hawrylycz, M. J., Ao, N., Ayres, M., Bensinger, A., Bernard, A., et al. (2007). Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature 445, 168–176. doi: 10.1038/nature05453
Leppä, E., Linden, A. M., Vekovischeva, O. Y., Swinny, J. D., Rantanen, V., Toppila, E., et al. (2011). Removal of GABA(A) receptor gamma2 subunits from parvalbumin neurons causes wide-ranging behavioral alterations. PLoS ONE 6:e24159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024159
Lewis, D. A., and Lund, J. S. (1990). Heterogeneity of chandelier neurons in monkey neocortex: corticotropin-releasing factor- and parvalbumin-immunoreactive populations. J. Comp. Neurol. 293, 599–615. doi: 10.1002/cne.902930406
Lewis, D. A., Cruz, D. A., Melchitzky, D. S., and Pierri, J. N. (2001). Lamina-specific deficits in parvalbumin-immunoreactive varicosities in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia: evidence for fewer projections from the thalamus. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1411–1422. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1411
Liguz-Lecznar, M., and Skangiel-Kramska, J. (2007). Vesicular glutamate transporters (VGLUTs): the three musketeers of glutamatergic system. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 67, 207–218.
Lindzey, G., Winston, H., and Manosevitz, M. (1961). Social dominance in inbred mouse strains. Nature 191, 474–476. doi: 10.1038/191474a0
Maetzler, W., Stunitz, H., Bendfeldt, K., Vollenweider, F., Schwaller, B., and Nitsch, C. (2009). Microcalcification after excitotoxicity is enhanced in transgenic mice expressing parvalbumin in all neurones, may commence in neuronal mitochondria and undergoes structural modifications over time. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 35, 165–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2990.2008.00970.x
Malkesman, O., Scattoni, M. L., Paredes, D., Tragon, T., Pearson, B., Shaltiel, G., et al. (2010). The female urine sniffing test: a novel approach for assessing reward-seeking behavior in rodents. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 864–871. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.018
Mészár, Z., Girard, F., Saper, C. B., and Celio, M. R. (2012). The lateral hypothalamic parvalbumin-immunoreactive (PV1) nucleus in rodents. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 798–815. doi: 10.1002/cne.22789
Mouton, L. J., and Holstege, G. (1998). Three times as many lamina I neurons project to the periaqueductal gray than to the thalamus: a retrograde tracing study in the cat. Neurosci. Lett. 255, 107–110. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00723-X
Nusbaum, M. P., Blitz, D. M., Swensen, A. M., Wood, D., and Marder, E. (2001). The roles of co-transmission in neural network modulation. Trends Neurosci. 24, 146–154. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01723-9
Panksepp, J. B., and Lahvis, G. P. (2007). Social reward among juvenile mice. Genes Brain Behav. 6, 661–671. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00295.x
Panksepp, J. B., Jochman, K. A., Kim, J. U., Koy, J. J., Wilson, E. D., Chen, Q., et al. (2007). Affiliative behavior, ultrasonic communication and social reward are influenced by genetic variation in adolescent mice. PLoS ONE 2:e351. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000351
Plummer, J. L., Cmielewski, P. L., Gourlay, G. K., Owen, H., and Cousins, M. J. (1991). Assessment of antinociceptive drug effects in the presence of impaired motor performance. J. Pharmacol. Methods 26, 79–87. doi: 10.1016/0160-5402(91)90057-C
Portfors, C. V. (2007). Types and functions of ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats and mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 46, 28–34.
Rees, H., and Roberts, M. H. (1993). The anterior pretectal nucleus: a proposed role in sensory processing. Pain 53, 121–135. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90072-W
Roccaro-Waldmeyer, D. M., Babalian, A., Muller, A., and Celio, M. R. (2016). Reduction in 50-kHz call-numbers and suppression of tickling-associated positive affective behaviour after lesioning of the lateral hypothalamic parvafox nucleus in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 298, 167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.11.004
Röhrenbeck, J., Wassle, H., and Heizmann, C. W. (1987). Immunocytochemical labelling of horizontal cells in mammalian retina using antibodies against calcium-binding proteins. Neurosci. Lett. 77, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(87)90508-8
Root, D. H., Mejias-Aponte, C. A., Zhang, S., Wang, H. L., Hoffman, A. F., Lupica, C. R., et al. (2014). Single rodent mesohabenular axons release glutamate and GABA. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1543–1551. doi: 10.1038/nn.3823
Roper, T. J. (1973). Nesting material as a reinforcer for female mice. Anim. Behav. 21, 733–740. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(73)80099-5
Rotaru, D. C., Barrionuevo, G., and Sesack, S. R. (2005). Mediodorsal thalamic afferents to layer III of the rat prefrontal cortex: synaptic relationships to subclasses of interneurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 490, 220–238. doi: 10.1002/cne.20661
Scattoni, M. L., Gandhy, S. U., Ricceri, L., and Crawley, J. N. (2008). Unusual repertoire of vocalizations in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. PLoS ONE 3:e3067. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003067
Scattoni, M. L., Ricceri, L., and Crawley, J. N. (2011). Unusual repertoire of vocalizations in adult BTBR T+tf/J mice during three types of social encounters. Genes Brain Behav. 10, 44–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00623.x
Seal, R. P., and Edwards, R. H. (2006). Functional implications of neurotransmitter co-release: glutamate and GABA share the load. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 6, 114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2005.12.001
Sigler, A., Oh, W. C., Imig, C., Altas, B., Kawabe, H., Cooper, B. H., et al. (2017). Formation and maintenance of functional spines in the absence of presynaptic glutamate release. Neuron 94, 304–311:e304. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.029
Stichel, C. C., Singer, W., and Heizmann, C. W. (1988). Light and electron microscopic immunocytochemical localization of parvalbumin in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat: evidence for coexistence with GABA. J. Comp. Neurol. 268, 29–37. doi: 10.1002/cne.902680104
Szabolcsi, V., Albisetti, G. W., and Celio, M. R. (2017). Parvalbumin-neurons of the ventrolateral hypothalamic parvafox nucleus receive a glycinergic input: a gene-microarray study. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 10:8. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2017.00008
Tanahira, C., Higo, S., Watanabe, K., Tomioka, R., Ebihara, S., Kaneko, T., et al. (2009). Parvalbumin neurons in the forebrain as revealed by parvalbumin-Cre transgenic mice. Neurosci. Res. 63, 213–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.12.007
Timbie, C., and Barbas, H. (2015). Pathways for emotions: specializations in the amygdalar, mediodorsal thalamic, and posterior orbitofrontal network. J. Neurosci. 35, 11976–11987. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2157-15.2015
Tong, Q., Ye, C., Mccrimmon, R. J., Dhillon, H., Choi, B., Kramer, M. D., et al. (2007). Synaptic glutamate release by ventromedial hypothalamic neurons is part of the neurocircuitry that prevents hypoglycemia. Cell Metab. 5, 383–393. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2007.04.001
Turrigiano, G. G. (2008). The self-tuning neuron: synaptic scaling of excitatory synapses. Cell 135, 422–435. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.008
Vaaga, C. E., Borisovska, M., and Westbrook, G. L. (2014). Dual-transmitter neurons: functional implications of co-release and co-transmission. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 29, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.04.010
Van Brederode, J. F., Mulligan, K. A., and Hendrickson, A. E. (1990). Calcium-binding proteins as markers for subpopulations of GABAergic neurons in monkey striate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 298, 1–22. doi: 10.1002/cne.902980102
Van De Weerd, H., Van Loo, P. L. P., Van Zutphen, L., Koolhaas, J., and Baumans, V. (1998). Strength of preference for nesting material es environmental enrichment for laboratory mice. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 55, 369–382. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00043-9
Velley, L. (1986). The role of intrinsic neurons in lateral hypothalamic self-stimulation. Behav. Brain Res. 22, 141–152. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(86)90035-5
Wallén-Mackenzie, A., Nordenankar, K., Fejgin, K., Lagerstrom, M. C., Emilsson, L., Fredriksson, R., et al. (2009). Restricted cortical and amygdaloid removal of vesicular glutamate transporter 2 in preadolescent mice impacts dopaminergic activity and neuronal circuitry of higher brain function. J. Neurosci. 29, 2238–2251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5851-08.2009
Wang, F., Kessels, H. W., and Hu, H. (2014). The mouse that roared: neural mechanisms of social hierarchy. Trends Neurosci. 37, 674–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.005
Wang, F., Zhu, J., Zhu, H., Zhang, Q., Lin, Z., and Hu, H. (2011). Bidirectional control of social hierarchy by synaptic efficacy in medial prefrontal cortex. Science 334, 693–697. doi: 10.1126/science.1209951
Wang, H., Liang, S., Burgdorf, J., Wess, J., and Yeomans, J. (2008). Ultrasonic vocalizations induced by sex and amphetamine in M2, M4, M5 muscarinic and D2 dopamine receptor knockout mice. PLoS ONE 3:e1893. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001893
Wild, B., Rodden, F. A., Grodd, W., and Ruch, W. (2003). Neural correlates of laughter and humour. Brain 126, 2121–2138. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg226
Williams, S. M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., and Leranth, C. (1992). The synaptology of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 320, 353–369. doi: 10.1002/cne.903200307
Yuste, R., and Bonhoeffer, T. (2004). Genesis of dendritic spines: insights from ultrastructural and imaging studies. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 24–34. doi: 10.1038/nrn1300
Zander, J. F., Munster-Wandowski, A., Brunk, I., Pahner, I., Gomez-Lira, G., Heinemann, U., et al. (2010). Synaptic and vesicular coexistence of VGLUT and VGAT in selected excitatory and inhibitory synapses. J. Neurosci. 30, 7634–7645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0141-10.2010
Keywords: parvalbumin, VGlut2, Cre/Lox-system, pain, ultrasonic vocalization, dominance, defense, parvafox nucleus
Citation: Roccaro-Waldmeyer DM, Girard F, Milani D, Vannoni E, Prétôt L, Wolfer DP and Celio MR (2018) Eliminating the VGlut2-Dependent Glutamatergic Transmission of Parvalbumin-Expressing Neurons Leads to Deficits in Locomotion and Vocalization, Decreased Pain Sensitivity, and Increased Dominance. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:146. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00146
Received: 21 March 2018; Accepted: 26 June 2018;
Published: 18 July 2018.
Edited by:
Antonella Gasbarri, University of L'Aquila, ItalyReviewed by:
Alberto Granato, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, ItalyFiorenzo Conti, Università Politecnica delle Marche, Italy
Copyright © 2018 Roccaro-Waldmeyer, Girard, Milani, Vannoni, Prétôt, Wolfer and Celio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Marco R. Celio, bWFyY28uY2VsaW9AdW5pZnIuY2g=