Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 01 December 2015
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors

Social Facilitation of Cognition in Rhesus Monkeys: Audience Vs. Coaction

  • 1ImpAct Team, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, U1028, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Lyon, France
  • 2Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • 3Institut de Médecine Environnementale, Paris, France

Social psychology has long established that the mere presence of a conspecific, be it an active co-performer (coaction effect), or a passive spectator (audience effect) changes behavior in humans. Yet, the process mediating this fundamental social influence has so far eluded us. Brain research and its nonhuman primate animal model, the rhesus macaque, could shed new light on this long debated issue. For this approach to be fruitful, however, we need to improve our patchy knowledge about social presence influence in rhesus macaques. Here, seven adults (two dyads and one triad) performed a simple cognitive task consisting in touching images to obtain food treats, alone vs. in presence of a co-performer or a spectator. As in humans, audience sufficed to enhance performance to the same magnitude as coaction. Effect sizes were however four times larger than those typically reported in humans in similar tasks. Both findings are an encouragement to pursue brain and behavior research in the rhesus macaque to help solve the riddle of social facilitation mechanisms.

Introduction

An individual's behavior changes merely because an observer is hovering nearby. Since Allport's 1924 Social psychology (Allport, 1924), this phenomenon is referred to as “social facilitation,” a historical misnomer as others' mere presence facilitates the emission of well-learned responses but impairs the acquisition of new responses (Zajonc, 1965). Social facilitation affects all human behaviors, from food consumption (Herman, 2015) to cognition (Bond and Titus, 1983). It was initially described in presence of co-performers (coaction effect) but subsequent research demonstrated that the presence of passive spectators (audience effect) suffices to produce the same change (Stroebe, 2012). Social psychology proposed several theoretical models to explain social facilitation (Strauss, 2002) but behavior alone cannot tease them apart. Recently, we showed that cognitive neuroscience and its nonhuman primate animal model, the rhesus macaque, could help solve this long debated issue by revealing which brain functions are altered by social presence (Monfardini et al., 2015). This novel approach is currently handicapped, however, by the fact that we do not know to which extent social facilitation in rhesus macaques parallels what social psychology has taught us about human social facilitation.

Social facilitation studies in rhesus macaques are few and far between; only three have been conducted over the last 80 years (Harlow and Yudin, 1933; Stamm, 1961; Ferrari et al., 2005). Two (Harlow and Yudin, 1933; Ferrari et al., 2005) show that macaques eat more in presence of co-eaters than alone (as do humans, Herman, 2015, or capuchin monkeys, Visalberghi and Addessi, 2000; Dindo and de Waal, 2007). The remaining one (Stamm, 1961) provides the proof of concept that the presence of a co-performer also helps macaques when food serves as a reward for a cognitive task rather than being simply made available for consumption (a change recently reported in capuchins as well; Dindo et al., 2009). The aim of the present study was to complete the patchy knowledge of social facilitation in macaques in two ways. First, we wanted to confirm Stamm's early (Stamm, 1961) demonstration of a social facilitation of cognition. Second, we wanted to establish that the present other needs not be a co-performer for the change to occur, and that a simple spectator suffices. To this aim, seven adult rhesus macaques (two dyads and one triad), trained to perform a simple cognitive task consisting in touching images to get food rewards, were alternatively tested alone vs. with a familiar companion serving as either a passive spectator or an active co-performer.

Methods

Subjects

Seven French-born rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), aged 4–11 years, participated in the study: two male-female dyads paired in adulthood, unsuccessfully in one case (dyad 1, the two animals were housed side by side but not together due to the male's unpredictable bouts of aggressiveness), successfully in the other case (dyad 2, the two animals had been living together for 3 years at the start of the experiment), and a triad of same-age females living together since birth. The monkeys had free access to water and received normal daily food rations of fresh fruits and monkey chow (40–130 kcal/kg/day) after testing completion. All animals were familiarized with handling and testing procedures for 4 weeks before the present data were collected. Data from the female triad were partially presented in Monfardini et al. (2015) (see next section for detail). Work complied with European Union Directive 2010/63/EU and was approved by French Animal Ethics Committee C2EA42-CELYNE.

Procedure

Seven pictures of neutral objects (e.g., an armchair, a farm tractor, a knit cap) were pseudo-randomly presented on the right or on the left side of the screen always in the same order. Touching the image led to a positive visual feedback plus a reward (~5 tiny beads of dry pasta); the trial time course (Figure 1A) allowed a maximum response rate of 8 responses/min. Each testing session lasted 15 min. For social testing, two animals were placed side by side; each monkey could see and hear the other one but not reach the other's screen or treats. The animals first alternated 1 day alone (Figure 1B), 1 day in the passive presence of a housemate (or neighbor for dyad 1; audience, Figure 1C), and then 1 day alone, 1 day in the presence of a housemate/neighbor doing the same task (coaction, Figure 1D). For the triad, half of the social sessions were carried out in the presence of one partner, the other half in the presence of the other partner; this group's audience scores were included in Monfardini et al. (2015) where they were correlated with changes in brain metabolism and changes in plasma cortisol level.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Trial time-course (A). An image appeared on the screen. If the animal touched it within 30 s, a 5 s positive feedback appeared (green screen) and a reward (dry pasta beads) was delivered. Otherwise, a 5 s negative feedback appeared (a red screen for an incorrect touch, a gray screen for a no response) and no reward was delivered. Testing conditions (B-D). The animals were tested alone (B), in the presence of an idle companion (C) or in the presence of an active companion doing the same task (D).

Data Analysis

Scores (response per min) in the alone condition were the same whether collected in alternation with the audience or with the coaction condition [paired t-test t(6) = 1.2, p = 0.26]. They were therefore pooled into a single alone measure and compared to the two social conditions using a parametric ANOVA with the Huynh-Feldt correction for repeated measures followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests for repeated measure. Effect size was estimated using Cohen's d (mean difference between social and alone scores divided by the pooled within-condition standard deviation), as well as Cohen's Drm (d*SQRT(2*(1-r))), which corrects for the correlation r between measures in within-subject designs (Lakens, 2013).

Then, we calculated for each social session, the percent change in response rate relative to the animal's average performance during alone sessions (individual Δ scores). One sample t-tests were used to determine whether these individual Δ scores significantly differed from 0, and paired t-tests to compare audience and coaction effects for each individual. Finally, because the results revealed variable Δs across groups, Pearson's correlations were used to test the idea of a link between mean Δs over the two social conditions and the degree of intimacy between observer and observee as objectivized by the index: I = years of shared housing/years of age (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the animals' characteristics, response rate, and Δ scores.

Results

The animals completed on average 19 sessions (range 16–20) in the alone condition, 15 sessions (10–21) in the audience condition, and 11 sessions (8–17) in the coaction condition. The monkeys touched the images on the screen to obtain a food treat twice more often under audience and coaction than under alone testing [F(2, 12) = 16.0; p = 0.001; Figure 2A]. Each social condition differed from the alone condition (audience: p = 0.009, coaction: p = 0.002), while not differing from each other (p = 0.80). Effect sizes were systematically superior to the 0.8 value reflecting a large effect (Lakens, 2013). Cohen's d amounted to 1.4 for audience and 1.8 for coaction, and Cohen's Drm to 1.3 for audience and 1.6 for coaction.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Response rate (mean ± sem) for the group (A) and for each individual (B). In (B), scores are illustrated for the alone condition (Al, dark gray bars), the two social conditions taken together (Soc, light gray bars), and for audience (o) and coaction (•) separately. ***p ≤ 0.001, *p ≤ 0.05.

At the individual level, each of the seven monkeys displayed a change (mean Δ) significantly differing from zero in both the audience and coaction conditions (all p's ≤ 0.04). In most cases (5/7), audience and coaction effects were indistinguishable. A significant difference emerged in two monkeys though (Figure 2B): the (assertive) male of dyad 2 performed better in the coaction condition [t(17) = 2.1, p = 0.05], whereas the (subdued) bottom-ranking female of the triad performed better in the audience condition [t(34) = 2.3, p = 0.03]. Mean Δs over the two social conditions were positively correlated with intimacy scores (r = 0.75, p = 0.05). The largest facilitation occurred in the triad of lifetime companions, the smallest in the dyad of neighbors, while the dyad of adulthood companions fell in between (Table 1).

Discussion

The present study confirms an early demonstration of social facilitation of cognition in rhesus macaques (Stamm, 1961), and demonstrates that, as in humans, coaction is not necessary for this phenomenon to occur. The mere presence of a passive spectator produces the very same change. Effect sizes were four times larger than those reported for equivalent tasks in humans (Bond and Titus, 1983). Some variations across individuals and groups hinted at potential moderators of social facilitation for future studies to investigate.

The present coaction effect adds to the rare earlier evidence that macaques (and capuchins) are socially facilitated also when food serves as a reward for a task, rather than being simply made available for consumption (Stamm, 1961; Dindo et al., 2009). The present audience effect provides a convincing example of the mere presence effect in nonhuman animals. In the 1960s, social psychologist Zajonc gave equal weight to human data and nonhuman data from a variety of animals including cockroaches, chickens, or rats, considering that “a social psychology confined to man is as parochial as a chemistry confined to gold” (Rajecki, 2010). Later, however, the capacity of animal studies to arrange (i) a presence condition without social learning of some sort and (ii) a solitary condition that is not a source of stress was called into question (Guerin and Innes, 2009). Here, the “present other” was of no help to solve the task at hand, so social learning (whether cueing, contagion, or imitation) cannot explain the social facilitation effect. A stress-related explanation can also reasonably be ruled out as we compared the cortisol response elicited in the present female triad by the alone vs. the audience condition, and found no difference (Monfardini et al., 2015).

The present social facilitation is in line with the enhanced response rate described in simple cognitive tasks in humans (Bond and Titus, 1983). Likewise, the fact that both audience and coaction facilitate performance is consistent with human studies reporting equipotentiality of the two effects in the cognitive domain (Fonseca and Garcia-Marques, 2013). These similarities suggest that social facilitation is a form of social influence that arose through evolution and operates regardless of the level of language, culture, or intelligence. This strengthens the idea that rhesus macaques constitute a valuable animal model of human social facilitation and that brain and behavior studies in this classical neuroscience model (Capitanio and Emborg, 2008) could provide the mean for a fresh look at one of the oldest topics in social psychology (Stroebe, 2012).

As evoked in the Introduction, neuroimaging could help solve the riddle of social facilitation mediator (Monfardini et al., 2015). Monkeys present an advantage over humans in this new endeavor: the large effect sizes they display (> 1.2 standard deviation unit), well above the modest effect sizes typically observed in humans (~0.3 standard deviation unit; Bond and Titus, 1983). The amplitude of the neural changes revealed by neuroimaging being correlated with the amplitude of the accompanying behavioral changes (Monfardini et al., 2015), the neural signature of social facilitation could be easier to uncover in monkeys than in humans. Regarding behavior, rhesus macaques might help explore moderators of social facilitation that have heretofore been neglected. The variations observed here, across groups (greater facilitation in lifetime female companions), and across individuals (greater audience effect in a subdued female, vs. greater coaction effect in an assertive male), respectively suggest a moderating influence of the observer-observee degree of familiarity (Herman, 2015), and a moderating influence of gender and/or personality traits (Uziel, 2007) for future studies to investigate in larger samples of animals.

Funding

INSERM, CNRS, UCBL, Institut de Médecine Environnementale, NEURODIS Foundation, ANR-O8-BLAN-0068-1, James S. McDonnell Foundation, LABEX CORTEX-ANR-11-LABX-0042.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ivan Balansard for veterinary care; Clément Hynaux for help with data collection; Gislène Gardechaux for animal care; Frédéric Volland, Roméo Salemme, and Eric Koun for technical and engineering assistance.

References

Allport, F. H. (1924). Social Psychology. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Bond, C. F., and Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: a meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychol. Bull. 94, 265–292. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capitanio, J. P., and Emborg, M. E. (2008). Contributions of non-human primates to neuroscience research. Lancet 371, 1126–1135. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60489-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dindo, M., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Partner effects on food consumption in brown capuchin monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 9, 1–9. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dindo, M., Whiten, A., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2009). Social facilitation of exploratory foraging behavior in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Am. J. Primatol. 71, 419–426. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, P. F., Maiolini, C., Addessi, E., Fogassi, L., and Visalberghi, E. (2005). The observation and hearing of eating actions activates motor programs related to eating in macaque monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 161, 95–101. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonseca, R., and Garcia-Marques, T. (2013). Back to basics: socially facilitated situated cognition. Soc. Cogn. 31, 147–161. doi: 10.1521/soco.2013.31.2.147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerin, B., and Innes, J. (2009). Social Facilitation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Harlow, H. F., and Yudin, H. C. (1933). Social behavior of primates. I. Social facilitation of feeding in the monkey and its relation to attitudes of ascendance and submission. J. Comp. Psychol. 16, 171–185. doi: 10.1037/h0071690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, C. P. (2015). The social facilitation of eating. A review. Appetite 86, 61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol. 4:863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monfardini, E., Redouté, J., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Hynaux, C., Fradin, J., Huguet, P., et al. (2015). Others' sheer presence boosts brain activity in the attention (but not the motivation) network. Cereb. Cortex bhv067. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv067. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajecki, D. W. (2010). Zajonc, cockroaches, and chickens, c. 1965-1975: a characterization and contextualization. Emot. Rev. 2, 320–328. doi: 10.1177/1754073910375477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamm, J. S. (1961). Social facilitation in monkeys. Psychol. Rep. 45, 479–484. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1961.8.3.479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, B. (2002). Social facilitation in motor tasks: a review of research and theory. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 3, 237–256. doi: 10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00019-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroebe, W. (2012). The truth about triplett (1898), but nobody seems to care. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 54–57. doi: 10.1177/1745691611427306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: a review and meta-analysis. J. Res. Pers. 41, 579–601. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visalberghi, E., and Addessi, E. (2000). Seeing group members eating a familiar food enhances the acceptance of novel foods in capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 60, 69–76. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science 149, 269–274. doi: 10.1126/science.149.3681.269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social facilitation, social presence, audience, coaction, monkeys

Citation: Reynaud AJ, Guedj C, Hadj-Bouziane F, Meunier M and Monfardini E (2015) Social Facilitation of Cognition in Rhesus Monkeys: Audience Vs. Coaction. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:328. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00328

Received: 20 August 2015; Accepted: 13 November 2015;
Published: 01 December 2015.

Edited by:

Regina Marie Sullivan, New York University School of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Ludise Malkova, Georgetown University Medical Center, USA
Anne-Pierre Goursaud, Emory University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Reynaud, Guedj, Hadj-Bouziane, Meunier and Monfardini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Martine Meunier, bWFydGluZS5tZXVuaWVyQGluc2VybS5mcg==;
Elisabetta Monfardini, ZWxpc2FiZXR0YS5tb25mYXJkaW5pQGluc2VybS5mcg==

These authors are co-last authors.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.