Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 22 October 2014
Sec. Pathological Conditions

Saccadic body turns in walking Drosophila

  • Department of Cellular Neurobiology, Georg-August University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany

Drosophila melanogaster structures its optic flow during flight by interspersing translational movements with abrupt body rotations. Whether these “body saccades” are accompanied by steering movements of the head is a matter of debate. By tracking single flies moving freely in an arena, we now discovered that walking Drosophila also perform saccades. Movement analysis revealed that the flies separate rotational from translational movements by quickly turning their bodies by 15 degrees within a tenth of a second. Although walking flies moved their heads by up to 20 degrees about their bodies, their heads moved with the bodies during saccadic turns. This saccadic strategy contrasts with the head saccades reported for e.g., blowflies and honeybees, presumably reflecting optical constraints: modeling revealed that head saccades as described for these latter insects would hardly affect the retinal input in Drosophila because of the lower acuity of its compound eye. The absence of head saccades in Drosophila was associated with the absence of haltere oscillations, which seem to guide head movements in other flies. In addition to adding new twists to Drosophila walking behavior, our analysis shows that Drosophila does not turn its head relative to its body when turning during walking.

Introduction

As most insects lack stereoscopic vision (Land, 1999) to gauge the distance of surrounding objects, visual cues created by self-motion must be exploited. Locusts and mantids, for example, perform peering movements with their heads to deduce the distance of objects from the resulting motion parallax (Kral and Poteser, 1997). Motion parallax is also exploited by wasps that perform curved learning flights to remember the sites of their nests (Zeil, 1993, 1996). In more general terms, any movement of the animal's head will create an image shift on the retina—a phenomenon known as optic flow (Gibson et al., 1955). During translations, close objects will travel faster across the retina than distant ones, providing distance information, whereas no such information can be deduced during pure rotations when all objects travel across the retina with equal speeds (Koenderink and Doorn, 1987).

To facilitate distance estimation, insects thus should (i) separate translational movements from rotations and (ii) turn quickly to reduce the rotation time. Both strategies have been reported for flying Drosophila (Heisenberg and Wolf, 1979; Tammero and Dickinson, 2002) as well as other insects (Land, 1973; Collett and Land, 1975; Buelthoff et al., 1980; Zeil, 1986; Geurten et al., 2010), which all seem to structure their locomotion into prolonged phases of predominantly translational movement that are interspersed by fast saccadic turns. The active movement of the head during these saccades was analyzed with varying results (Land, 1973; Geiger and Poggio, 1977), but could be clarified by high-speed observations in freely flying insects (Schilstra and van Hateren, 1998): The head rotates relatively to the body, reducing the saccade duration even further. The role of head body coordination in walking insects is gaining new momentum (Ribak et al., 2009; Kress and Egelhaaf, 2012, 2014a) and questions the information content of the optic flow obtained during walking (Kress and Egelhaaf, 2014b). Nonetheless optic flow has been shown to allow walking Drosophila to estimate distances of up to 80 times the length of its body (Schuster et al., 2002). We now tested whether walking Drosophila temporally separate rotations from translations and found that this separation is present and that the walking flies perform body saccades.

Materials and Methods

Walking Trajectories

56 male and 57 female adult Canton S wild-type flies were released one by one into circular arenas (43 mm diameter, 3.5 mm height). The arenas were produced using an Ultimaker 3D printer (Ultimaking LTD, Geldermalsen, Netherlands). The lower 1.5 mm of each arena was filled with 1% agarose containing 1% glucose, leaving the upper 2 mm for the flies to walk around. Each arena was illuminated by three Honeycomb LED lamps (IS, Imaging Solutions GmbH, Eningen, Germany), and the flies were filmed from above with a MotionTraveller 500 camera (IS). Movies were recorded using TroublePix software (NorPix Inc., Montreal, Canada) and trajectories were subsequently traced with ivTools (Jens P. Lindemann and Elke Braun, https://toolkit.cit-ec.uni-bielefeld.de/components/tools/ivtools). Only sequences during which the flies did not follow the wall but walked freely through the arena were included in the analysis. The total recording time was about 16 min, yielding half a million frames.

Prototypical Movement Patterns

Prototypical movement patterns (PMs) were deduced as described by Braun et al. (2010): Distances between subsequent fly positions were determined as the squared Euclidian distance, and the respective thrust, slip, and yaw velocities were deduced and z-scored individually. To identify the most common velocity combinations, we used two different clustering algorithms, agglomerative hierarchical clustering, and k-means clustering (MacQueen, 1967; Milligan and Cooper, 1987; Murtagh and Contreras, 2012): agglomerative hierarchical clustering (Ward's criterion) was used to narrow down the number of possible PMs. Because hierarchical clustering is only possible for small data sets, the data was chopped into 200 chunks in a round-robin fashion. Less than 20 possible PMs were identified, which were then tested for the whole data set by k-means clustering. We tested all PMs between number 2 and 20 and every fifth class between 25 and 50. PMs that describe the data set best were then narrowed down using the quality and the stability of the clustering as operational criteria: quality was calculated as the distance between the PMs divided by their individual density. Stability was assessed by omitting 10, 25, and 50% of the data in a round-robin fashion (step size equaling 2% of the data) to test whether the clustering can be reproduced. For more details on clustering movement trajectories see Braun et al. (2010) and Hofmann et al. (2014).

Saccades

Saccades were defined using a yaw velocity threshold of 200 deg*s−1 as the threshold criterion. The peak velocity of each saccade was determined using the “findpeaks” routine of Matlab (R2012b, The Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Using these peaks as trigger points, saccades were averaged over a 100 ms time window.

Head and Thorax Movements

To simultaneously assess head and body trajectories, flies were filmed while walking through a labyrinth. This labyrinth consisted of two small rectangular boxes (24 x 24 x 3 mm) connected by a 1 cm long 2 x 2 mm tunnel, with a right angle at half its length. Flies were filmed as described above, whereby we used a custom-made planar macroscopic objective to optically resolve thorax and head. Custom-made 3D templates of the fly thorax and head were fitted frame-by-frame to the respective body parts using ivTools (http://www.ivtools.org/ivtools/index.html) to deduce their orientation.

Retinal Images and Image Shifts

Ommatidial maps were adapted from Petrowitz et al. (2000) (blowfly Calliphora vicina), Stürzl et al. (2010) and Seidl (1986) (honey bee Apis mellifera), and Buchner (1971) and Dickson et al. (2006) (Drosophila). A complete data set for the Drosophila eye can be found at http://code.astraw.com/Drosophila_eye_map/ (courtesy of Dr. A. Straw). The available map for Calliphora is incomplete in that it covers only the frontal part of the eye. Maps were always made for one eye and then mirrored to simulate the opposite eye. Photoreceptor acceptance angles of Drosophila were taken from Gonzalez-Bellido et al. (2011), and the corresponding values for Apis and Calliphora from Laughlin and Horridge (1971) and Smakman et al. (1984), respectively. Acceptance angle data was fitted with a Gaussian. In case of A. mellifera, two Gaussians were used for vertical and horizontal acceptance angles (Laughlin and Horridge, 1971). Retinal inputs for panoramic images were calculated by projecting the Gaussians onto the image along the ommatidial axes. To ensure that the entire input of each ommatidium is covered, we extended the Gaussians to five times the standard deviation σ. We than calculated a weighted mean of the panoramic section defined by the Gaussian with the Gaussian filter strength as weights. To rebuild the optical images, we stitched together the Voronoi cells (Lejeune Dirichlet, 1850; Voronoi, 1908) around the ommatidial axes of the eye. To deduce the retinal image shifts that ensue from yaw rotations, we used fifteen full panoramic images of nature scenes (licensed under creative commons by Janne Voutilainen and Aldo Hoeben). Images are shown in Supplementary Figure 1. Ten images show forest scenes and five show close ups of flowers or trees. We also tested ten random images with 3600 x 1800 pixels each and a 1/f2 spatial distribution (Field, 1987; van der Schaaf and van Hateren, 1996; Saremi and Sejnowski, 2013) (see Supplementary Figure 2). We then rotated the images by 360° in steps of 0.1° and calculated the resulting retinal image difference (Zeil, 2012) for each ommatidium. The resulting retinal image differences were averaged across ommatidia. The median image difference over different images was calculated for each rotation and each of the three species. For comparison, we employed the same methods to analyze the raw images, averaging over all pixels instead of ommatidia. The median image difference of the raw image between 170 to 180° and −170 to −180° was used to normalize all image difference functions for a given image.

Haltere Movements

To test for haltere oscillations, we replaced one side of the labyrinth with Perspex glass, allowing us to observe the flies from the side. 10 animals, including 5 males and 5 females were filmed while they walked through the labyrinth. Subsequently, the same animals were tethered on their thorax and filmed during fictive walking and flight. To elicit walking, we allowed the flies to grab a small Styrofoam ball, whose removal initiated flight. By touching the legs of the flying flies with the ball, landing behavior was initiated.

Results

Walking Flies Separate Translations and Rotations

To test whether walking flies might separate translational and rotational movements, we recorded the trajectories of 103 Canton-S wild-type flies walking freely in an arena at 500 frames per second and screened their body trajectories for reoccurring prototypical movement patters (PMs; Braun et al., 2010). To identify PMs, the respective yaw, slip, and thrust velocities were deduced from the trajectory data, and the most common velocity combinations were extracted using clustering algorithms (see Materials and Methods). Five PMs were identified (Figure 1A), representing translations (PMs 1,2 in Figure 1A), rotations (PMs 3,4), and resting (PM5). Resting (PM5) amounted to 63% of the sampling time, translations (PMs 1,2) for 29%, and only 9% for rotations (PMs 3,4). Consistent with observations on walking Calliphora, (Kress and Egelhaaf, 2012, 2014a) walking Drosophila showed some residual rotations during translatory phases (Figure 1B), yet the respective rotational velocities were much lower than during phases of turning. Hence, during Drosophila walking, (i) translational movements are temporally separated from fast rotations and (ii) translations temporally dominate over rotations.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Body trajectories of walking Drosophila. (A) Prototypical movement patterns (PMs) of adult Drosophila walking freely in a circular arena (56 male 57 female flies, recorded @ 500 fps). Colored arrows highlight the velocity combination that characterizes each PM, and gray arrows indicate the maximum speeds. The color code is given by the legend to the left. For each PM, its respective abundance in the data set is presented in percent, along with its average duration. (B) Distributions of the three velocities (thrust, slip, yaw) during translational movements and saccadic turns. Yaw velocities cluster around zero during translations but not during rotations. (C) Example of a single trajectory. Circles mark the center of mass of the animal and lines depict the long axis of its body. To facilitate following the trajectory, gray scales indicate time. Dashed boxes highlight three saccades. (D) Yaw angles and velocities of the trajectory in (B) plotted against time. Orange rectangles mark the three saccades that are highlighted in panel B. (E) Mean velocity of 1885 individual saccades, identified by using a yaw velocity of 200 deg*s−1 as the threshold criterion and arranged so that their peak velocity is at 0 ms. Leftward and rightward turns were separated prior to the analysis, yielding velocity profiles for each of them that are virtually mirror-symmetric. (F) Corresponding yaw angles documenting that, within the 100 ms time window, the flies turned by on average 15°. (G) Distribution of saccade durations in milliseconds.

Walking Drosophila Perform Saccades

A temporal segregation of translational and rotational movements, as revealed by PM analysis, was also seen within single walking trajectories (Figure 1C). Changes in the heading of the flies were associated with rapidly changing yaw angles and sharp yaw velocity peaks (Figure 1D). Selecting heading changes with absolute yaw velocities exceeding 200° per second and using the respective velocity maxima as trigger points, we averaged the yaw velocities for 1140 heading changes of the 103 experimental flies (Figure 1E). Thereby we discarded 3348 slow rotations, whose velocities were below threshold. Average yaw velocities displayed the bell-shaped form that characterizes saccadic eye movements in mammals (Land, 1992; Stanford et al., 2010) and saccadic body turns in insects (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004; Ribak et al., 2009; Kress and Egelhaaf, 2014a). During a saccadic turn, the flies changed their heading on average by about 15° (Figure 1F) within 40 to 120 ms (median 90 ms Figure 1G). These angular heading changes are smaller than those reported for tethered flying Drosophila (ca. 90° in 100 ms, Tammero and Dickinson, 2002) or even freely flying Drosophila which are nearly twice as fast as tethered animals (ca. 90° in 45 ms, Fry et al., 2003), but close to those of walking blowflies (ca. 15° within 50 ms, Blaj and van Hateren, 2004), which also perform saccades while they walk (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004; Kress and Egelhaaf, 2014a,b).

Saccadic Body Turns are not Associated with Additional Head Movements

Insects cannot directly move their eyes; in order to change their gaze direction, they have to turn the head about the body or, alternatively, the body together with the head. Because of the small size of Drosophila, resolving the relative orientations of the head and the rest of the body requires imaging with a high spatial resolution, which is only possible within a confined space. We spatially confined the flies by letting them pass through an L-shaped labyrinth, which forced them to change their course by an angle of 90° (Figure 2B). Seven flies were filmed while each passed the labyrinth three times from either side. Respective head and thorax orientations were subsequently deduced by fitting the head and thorax with 3D templates (Figure 2A lower frame). Manual analysis of a total of 16,000 frames revealed that when turning in the labyrinth, the flies also performed saccades (Figures 2B–D). Three to six subsequent saccades were observed while the flies passed the 90° corner (Figure 2B). Body and head rotations performed in the labyrinth are similar to those seen in the circular arena (compare Figure 1D and Figure 2C). Moreover, averaged yaw velocities and yaw angles were virtually identical for thorax and head (Figures 2C), as were the respective distributions of the saccade amplitudes and angular velocities (Figures 2D,E). The temporal disparity between the peak velocities of head and body (Figure 2F) was about 2 ms for approximately 75% of the saccades. Hence, during the saccadic turns, the head does not move faster than the thorax, identifying the saccadic turns of walking Drosophila as pure body saccades in which the head moves together with the body. This saccadic behavior differs from the saccades of e.g., flying honeybees and walking blowflies, which rotate the head faster than the body (see Figure 3A), reducing the effective duration of the saccades (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004; Boeddeker, 2010): the different rotation speeds of head and body were seen when we extracted their respective yaw angles for blowflies from (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004), where the relative angles between head and body—the φ-angles—reach up to 4° (Figure 3B). In Drosophila, by contrast, the yaw-angles of the head superimposed with those of the body, yielding φ-angles of maximally 1° (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Head movements during saccadic turns. (A) Snapshots of a fly turning around the right angle in the labyrinth (top) and fitted head and thorax templates (bottom) used to deduce their respective orientations. (B) Example trace of the yaw velocities (top) and yaw angles (bottom) of the head (solid blue lines) and body (dashed blue lines) of a fly walking through the labyrinth. Head and body were traced separately by two different investigators (head: B.G., thorax: P.J.). (C) Average yaw velocities (top) and yaw angles (bottom) obtained for 114 saccades observed in the labyrinth. Colored areas depict the 95% confidence intervals. (D) Histogram of yaw angle amplitudes during saccades (dark blue: body, light blue: head). (E) Distribution of the peak yaw velocities during saccades (color code as in D). (F) Distribution of the time lapse between the velocity peak of head and body during the saccades.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparison of the head and body movements of walking Drosophila, walking blowflies (“Calliphora”), and flying honey bees (“Apis”). (A) Respective average yaw velocities during saccades. Note that for bees only the probability density is shown. (B) Average yaw angles (top) of the head and body and the difference between them (bottom) during saccadic body rotations. (C) Distributions of the angles between head and body (the φ-angles) observed during the entire trajectories, including saccades, and the trajectories between them. (D) Correlation coefficients between the yaw angles of the head and the body during the saccades. Calliphora is not included in this panel since no such data is available for this species. Data for Apis was taken from Boeddeker (2010) and data for Calliphora from Blaj and van Hateren (2004).

Judging from the φ-angle distribution obtained for the whole labyrinth passage (Figure 3C), Drosophila is able to move its head about its body by up to 20°: the φ-angle distribution closely resembled those reported for flying honeybees (Boeddeker, 2010). According to Blaj (2004), the respective distribution for walking Calliphora is shifted toward smaller angles (maximum φ-angles around 10°), though recent studies (Kress and Egelhaaf, 2014a) suggest that larger φ-angles occur in this species as well. For Drosophila, we cross-correlated the yaw velocities of the head and body during the whole trajectory and found that it peaks at zero phase-lag, whereas the head leads the body by ca. 8 ms in flying honeybees (Boeddeker, 2010) (Figure 3D). Hence, unlike bees, walking Drosophila does not move its head relative to the body during saccadic body turns.

Head Movements Could Speed Up Saccades but Would Hardly Change the Retinal Image

Blowflies and honeybees surpass Drosophila in terms of visual acuity: the acceptance angle of Drosophila photoreceptors is about 10-fold larger (Laughlin and Horridge, 1971; Smakman et al., 1984; Gonzalez-Bellido et al., 2011), and the bee eye also comprises about 8 times more ommatidia than that of Drosophila (Buchner, 1971; Seidl, 1986; Dickson et al., 2008; Stürzl et al., 2010). Using available information about species-specific ommatidium numbers, positions, and orientations (Buchner, 1971; Seidl, 1986; Petrowitz et al., 2000; Dickson et al., 2008; Stürzl et al., 2010), we calculated how different panoramas are mapped onto the retinae of these insects. We modeled the field of view of each ommatidium by fitting Gaussians to published photoreceptor acceptance angles. As expected, Mercator plots of the predicted retinal inputs were more blurred for Drosophila than for blowflies and bees (Figure 4A). Using sinusoidal gratings with different angular frequencies as panoramas, we next determined how the input of each ommatidium changes when the panorama shifts vertically by half a wavelength (Figure 4B). This revealed that blowflies and bees should be able to optically resolve objects with a horizontal angular extension of 1° and Drosophila of 7°, consistent with experimental observations (Nordström and O'Carroll, 2006; Fox and Frye, 2014; Fox et al., 2014).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Retinal inputs and image shifts. (A) Voronoi cells computed for the eyes of Drosophila, Apis, and Calliphora (top row), and respective retinal images (subsequent rows) deduced for three different images (left). The image in the lower row was taken by Janne Voutilainen (for a complete list of the images used, see Supplemental Materials). Note that for Calliphora, only the central field of view is covered (for data sources, see Materials and Methods). (B) Mean ommatidial luminescence difference, caused by shifting sinusoidal stripe patterns with different sine periods by half a sine period. The differences were normalized to the maximal pixel wise difference of the original images and plotted against the sine period. (C) Mean ommatidial luminescence difference caused by shifting images depicting a naturalistic scenes in steps of 0.1° around the animal from −180 to +180° (for images, see Supplementary Figure 1). The differences were normalized to the corresponding pixel wise difference of the image and plotted against the rotation angles. Lines: means; transparent areas: 95% confidence intervals. A zoomed in version of the data in the dashed box is plotted in Figure 4D. For respective data obtained with artificial images with an 1/f spatial distribution, see Supplementary Figure 2). (D) Close-up from panel C. Note that Drosophila generates a smaller ommatidial luminescence difference by turning by more than 15° (as seen during the body saccades) than Calliphora generates by turning about 4° (as seen during a Calliphora head saccade).

Using panoramic photographs of natural scenes (images courtesy of Janne Voutilainen and Aldo Hoeben) we determined the rotational image difference (Zeil, 2012) to assess how the contrast will change for each ommatidium when the head rotates (Figures 4C,D). For a head rotation of 4°, as observed in blowflies, we obtained relative contrast changes of the retinal image of ca. 48% for blowflies and 33.3% for honeybees. For Drosophila, we obtained a contrast change of only 14% for the same rotation (Figure 4D). To achieve a contrast change of 35%, Drosophila would have to turn its head by 15 instead of 4°. We also tested 10 panoramas with a 1/f2 frequency distribution (Field, 1987; van der Schaaf and van Hateren, 1996; Saremi and Sejnowski, 2013), which yielded quantitatively equivalent results (see Supplementary Figure 2). Hence, by turning its whole body, Drosophila generates the same contrast change that blowflies reach by solely rotating their head. In principle, active head rotations during saccadic body turns of Drosophila could reduce the rotation time, as is the case in blowfly and also honeybees (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004; Boeddeker, 2010). Due to the low spatial acuity of the Drosophila eye, however, such additional head rotations would result in minor contrast changes that the eye might not be able to detect.

Absence of Head Movements Associates with the Absence of Haltere Oscillations

Head rotations in flying and walking blowflies are associated with active haltere vibrations (Nalbach and Hengstenberg, 1994; Haag et al., 2010), which continuously oscillate while the flies walk (Sandeman and Markl, 1980). We tested for such haltere oscillations in walking Drosophila and found that there are none. Haltere oscillations were observed only upon take-off and during flight, yet they immediately ceased upon landing (see Figure 5). Unlike walking blowflies, Drosophila neither performs head saccades nor does it oscillate its halteres while it walks.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Absence of haltere oscillations. (A) Percentage of flies oscillating their halteres under different conditions (free walking, N = 9; tethered walking, N = 6; tethered flying, N = 6; after landing, N = 3; after take-off, N = 3). (B) Time delay between the last tarsus leaving the ball (“take off on ball”) or the first tarsus touching the ball (“landing on ball”) and the respective on- and offsets of haltere oscillations (N = 3 each, means ± 1 SD).

Discussion

The walking behavior of Drosophila imagines has been studied on various levels of complexity, ranging from molecular (Brierley et al., 2012; Baek et al., 2013; Bidaye et al., 2014; Desai et al., 2014) and neuronal mechanisms (Chiappe et al., 2010; Seelig et al., 2010; Ofstad et al., 2011; Ping et al., 2011) to leg movements (Strauß and Heisenberg, 1990; Kain et al., 2013; Wosnitza et al., 2013), walking trajectories (Mendes et al., 2013), and group behavior (Liu et al., 2011; Billeter et al., 2012; Hahn et al., 2013). Walking trajectories and movements have been studied in the context of object fixation, and walking was also shown to facilitate motion detection (Rosner et al., 2009; Chiappe et al., 2010). The need to detect motion is reportedly reflected by the locomotion behavior of flying Drosophila (Heisenberg and Wolf, 1979; Tammero and Dickinson, 2002; Bender and Dickinson, 2006; Censi et al., 2013; Muijres et al., 2014) and other dipteran species. For Drosophila, retinal object motion was also shown to serve as a cue for distance estimation (Schuster et al., 2002). Our analysis shows that walking Drosophila separate translational movements from rotational ones by performing saccadic turns. These turns are slower than those performed by flying Drosophila, with their duration resembling those reported for the saccadic rotations of walking blowflies and bees. We further show that additional, correlated head movements do not accompany the body saccades of walking Drosophila, documenting that, while walking, Drosophila lacks the head saccades that reduce the effective turning duration in blowflies and bees. This absence of head saccades seems to reflect optical constraints imposed by the eye, whose visual acuity is lower in Drosophila than in these other insect species. The absence of head saccades also associates with the absence of haltere oscillations. In blowflies, active haltere oscillations are implicated in the generation of head saccades, indicating that walking Drosophila lack both these head saccades and their haltere control.

In addition to facilitating distance estimation, the saccadic movement strategy used by Drosophila might reduce motion blur. Motion blur occurs when the speed by which the image changes exceeds the integration time of the photoreceptors and reduces the perceived image contrast. In insects, motion blur sets in when the stimulus travels faster than one ommatidial acceptance angle (Δρ in deg.) during the integration time of the photoreceptors (Δt in s) (Land, 2003). Photoreceptor integration times Δt are about 20 ms for Drosophila (Juusola, 2000; Hardie, 2001; Niven et al., 2004) and 5–10 ms for Calliphora (Tatler et al., 2000). The respective ommatidial acceptance angles Δρ for the two species are 10 (Drosophila: Gonzalez-Bellido et al., 2011) and 1° (Calliphora: Smakman et al., 1984). These figures translate into motion blur thresholds of 500 deg*s−1 for Drosophila and 100–200 deg*s−1 for Calliphora. To avoid motion blur, head stabilization thus seems less critical for Drosophila than for Calliphora.

According to our analysis, walking Drosophila, like flying ones, separate rotations from translations to facilitate motion vision. During walking, however, this separation seems incomplete. During translations, substantial rotations persist, including head rotations with angles of up to 20° relative to the body. Head rotations during translations have also been observed in flying bees (Boeddeker, 2010) and walking blowflies (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004; Kress and Egelhaaf, 2014a), and Drosophila during flight (Fox and Frye, 2014). In walking blowflies, these head rotations reportedly arise from the walking apparatus and depend on the substrate the animal is walking on (Kress and Egelhaaf, 2012, 2014a), and in tethered Drosophila they were implicated in wide-field image fixation (Fox and Frye, 2014). Judging from our data, about half of the head turning angles of walking Drosophila lie below 5°, causing retinal image shifts of less than fifteen percent. Given these rather marginal image shifts, one might speculate that flying Drosophila also do not perform head-saccades, although their halteres oscillate.

Author Contributions

Bart Geurten and Martin Göpfert designed the study. Kristina Corthals and Bart Geurten conducted the experiments. Philipp Jähde constructed the 3D templates used in manual tracing. Philipp Jähde, Kristina Corthals and Bart Geurten analyzed the trajectories. Bart Geurten analyzed, modeled and plotted the data. Martin Göpfert and Bart Geurten wrote the manuscript with intellectual contributions from all other authors.

Funding Statements

This work was supported by the Collaborative Research Center SFB 889 of the German Science Foundation (project C2, to Martin C. Göpfert).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Janne Voutilainen and Aldo Hoeben for sharing their panoramic photographs, Dr. Heribert Gras for comments on the experimental design and fruitful discussions, Robert Kossen for his help during experiments, Dr. Christian Spalthoff for contributing some of his exquisite insect illustrations and two reviewers for a fair, extensive and helpful discussion. This study was supported by the DFG Collaborative Research Center SFB889 (C2, to Martin C. Göpfert), the BMBF Göttingen Bernstein Center (A1, to Martin C. Göpfert), and the Open Access Publication Funds of the University of Göttingen.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00365/abstract

References

Baek, M., Enriquez, J., and Mann, R. S. (2013). Dual role for Hox genes and Hox co-factors in conferring leg motoneuron survival and identity in Drosophila. Development 140, 2027–2038. doi: 10.1242/dev.090902

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, J. A., and Dickinson, M. H. (2006). A comparison of visual and haltere-mediated feedback in the control of body saccades in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 4597–4606. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02583

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidaye, S. S., Machacek, C., Wu, Y., and Dickson, B. J. (2014). Neuronal control of Drosophila walking direction. Science 344, 97–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1249964

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billeter, J.-C., Jagadeesh, S., Stepek, N., Azanchi, R., and Levine, J. D. (2012). Drosophila melanogaster females change mating behaviour and offspring production based on social context. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 2417–2425. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.2676

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaj, G. (2004). Walking and Vision in Blowflies. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Groningen.

Blaj, G., and van Hateren, J. H. (2004). Saccadic head and thorax movements in freely walking blowflies. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 190, 861–868. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0541-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boeddeker, N. (2010). The fine structure of honeybee head and body yaw movements in a homing task. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277, 1899–1906. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.2326

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, E., Geurten, B., and Egelhaaf, M. (2010). Identifying prototypical components in behaviour using clustering algorithms. PLoS ONE 5:e9361. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009361

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brierley, D. J., Rathore, K., VijayRaghavan, K., and Williams, D. W. (2012). Developmental origins and architecture of Drosophila leg motoneurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 1629–1649. doi: 10.1002/cne.23003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchner, E. (1971). Dunkelanregung des Stationären Flugs der Fruchtfliege Drosophila. Ph.d. disscustion, University of Tübingen.

Buelthoff, H., Poggio, T., and Wehrhahn, C. (1980). 3-D analysis of flight trajectories of flies (Drosophila melanogaster). Z. Naturforsch. 35c, 811–845.

Google Scholar

Censi, A., Straw, A. D., Sayaman, R. W., Murray, R. M., and Dickinson, M. H. (2013). Discriminating external and internal causes for heading changes in freely flying Drosophila. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9:e1002891. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002891

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiappe, M. E., Seelig, J. D., Reiser, M. B., and Jayaraman, V. (2010). Walking modulates speed sensitivity in Drosophila motion vision. Curr. Biol. 20, 1470–1475. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.072

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collett, T. S., and Land, M. F. (1975). Visual spatial memory in a hoverfly. J. Comp. Physiol. 100, 59–84. doi: 10.1007/BF00623930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desai, B. S., Chadha, A., and Cook, B. (2014). The stum gene is essential for mechanical sensing in proprioceptive neurons. Science 343, 1256–1259. doi: 10.1126/science.1247761

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickson, W. B., Straw, A. D., and Dickinson, M. H. (2008). Integrative model of Drosophila flight. AIAA J. 46, 2150–2164.

Google Scholar

Dickson, W. B., Straw, A. D., Poelma, C., and Dickinson, M. H. (2006). “An integrative model of insect flight control,” in 44th AIAA Aerospace Sciences Meeting and Exhibit (Reno, NV: American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics).

Field, D. J. (1987). Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 4:2379. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.4.002379

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J. L., Aptekar, J. W., Zolotova, N. M., Shoemaker, P. A., and Frye, M. A. (2014). Figure-ground discrimination behavior in Drosophila. I. Spatial organization of wing-steering responses. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 558–569. doi: 10.1242/jeb.097220

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J. L., and Frye, M. A. (2014). Figure-ground discrimination behavior in Drosophila. II. Visual influences on head movement. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 570–579. doi: 10.1242/jeb.080192

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fry, S. N., Sayaman, R., and Dickinson, M. H. (2003). The aerodynamics of free-flight maneuvers in Drosophila. Science 300, 495–498. doi: 10.1126/science.1081944

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiger, G., and Poggio, T. (1977). On head and body movements of flying flies. Biol. Cybern. 25, 177–180. doi: 10.1007/BF00365214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geurten, B. R. H., Kern, R., Braun, E., and Egelhaaf, M. (2010). A syntax of hoverfly flight prototypes. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 2461–2475. doi: 10.1242/jeb.036079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J., Olum, P., and Rosenblatt, F. (1955). Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. Am. J. Psychol. 68, 372–385. doi: 10.2307/1418521

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Bellido, P. T., Wardill, T. J., and Juusola, M. (2011). Compound eyes and retinal information processing in miniature dipteran species match their specific ecological demands. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 4224–4229. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014438108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haag, J., Wertz, A., and Borst, A. (2010). Central gating of fly optomotor response. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 20104–20109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009381107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn, N., Geurten, B., Gurvich, A., Piepenbrock, D., Kästner, A., Zanini, D., et al. (2013). Monogenic heritable autism gene neuroligin impacts Drosophila social behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 252, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.06.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardie, R. C. (2001). Phototransduction in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 3403–3409.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Heisenberg, M., and Wolf, R. (1979). On the Fine Structure of Yaw Torque in Visual Flight Orientation of Drosophila melanogaster M. J. Comp. Physiol. A 130, 113–130. doi: 10.1007/BF00611046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, V., Geurten, B. R. H., Sanguinetti-Scheck, J. I., Gómez-Sena, L., and Engelmann, J. (2014). Motor patterns during active electrosensory acquisition. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:186. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00186

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juusola, M. (2000). Light adaptation in Drosophila photoreceptors: I. response dynamics and signaling efficiency at 25degrees C. J. Gen. Physiol. 117, 3–25. doi: 10.1085/jgp.117.1.3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kain, J., Stokes, C., Gaudry, Q., Song, X., Foley, J., Wilson, R., et al. (2013). Leg-tracking and automated behavioural classification in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 4, 1910. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2908

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenderink, J. J., and Doorn, A. J. (1987). Facts on optic flow. Biol. Cybern. 56, 247–254. doi: 10.1007/BF00365219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kral, K., and Poteser, M. (1997). Motion parallax as a source of distance information in locusts and mantids. J. Ins. Behav. 1, 145–163. doi: 10.1007/BF02765480

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kress, D., and Egelhaaf, M. (2012). Head and body stabilization in blowflies walking on differently structured substrates. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 1523–1532. doi: 10.1242/jeb.066910

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kress, D., and Egelhaaf, M. (2014a). Gaze characteristics of freely walking blowflies in a goal-directed task. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 3209–3220. doi: 10.1242/jeb.097436

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kress, D., and Egelhaaf, M. (2014b). Impact of stride-coupled gaze shifts of walking blowflies on the neuronal representation of visual targets. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:00307. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00307

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, M. F. (1973). Head movement of flies during visually guided flight. Nature 243, 299–300. doi: 10.1038/243299a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, M. F. (1992). Visual tracking and pursuit: humans and arthropods compared. J. Insect Physiol. 38, 939–951. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(92)90002-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, M. F. (1999). Motion and vision: why animals move their eyes. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 185, 341–352. doi: 10.1007/s003590050393

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, M. F. (2003). Visual Acuity in Insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 42, 147–177. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.42.1.147

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laughlin, S. B., and Horridge, G. A. (1971). Angular sensitivity of the retinula cells of dark-adapted worker bee. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiol. 74, 329–335. doi: 10.1007/BF00297733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lejeune Dirichlet, G. (1850). Über die Reduktion der positiven quadratischen Formen mit drei unbestimmten ganzen Zahlen. J. für die Reine und Angew. Math. 40, 209–227. doi: 10.1515/crll.1850.40.209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W., Liang, X., Gong, J., Yang, Z., Zhang, Y.-H., Zhang, J.-X., et al. (2011). Social regulation of aggression by pheromonal activation of Or65a olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 896–902. doi: 10.1038/nn.2836

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacQueen, J. B. (1967). Some methods for classification and analysis of multivariate observations. Proc. 5th Berkeley Symp. Math. Stat. Probab. 1, 281–297.

Google Scholar

Mendes, C. S., Bartos, I., Akay, T., Márka, S., and Mann, R. S. (2013). Quantification of gait parameters in freely walking wild type and sensory deprived Drosophila melanogaster. Elife 2:e00231. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milligan, G. W., and Cooper, M. C. (1987). Methodology review: clustering methods. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 11, 329–354. doi: 10.1177/014662168701100401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muijres, F. T., Elzinga, M. J., Melis, J. M., and Dickinson, M. H. (2014). Flies evade looming targets by executing rapid visually directed banked turns. Science 344, 172–177. doi: 10.1126/science.1248955

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murtagh, F., and Contreras, P. (2012). Algorithms for hierarchical clustering: an overview. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Data Min. Knowl. Discov. 2, 86–97. doi: 10.1002/widm.53

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nalbach, G., and Hengstenberg, R. (1994). The halteres of the blowfly Calliphora. J. Comp. Physiol. A 175, 695–708. doi: 10.1007/BF00191842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niven, J. E., Vähäsöyrinki, M., Juusola, M., and French, A. S. (2004). Interactions between light-induced currents, voltage-gated currents, and input signal properties in Drosophila photoreceptors. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 2696–2706. doi: 10.1152/jn.01163.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordström, K., and O'Carroll, D. C. (2006). Small object detection neurons in female hoverflies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 273, 1211–1216. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3424

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ofstad, T. A., Zuker, C. S., and Reiser, M. B. (2011). Visual place learning in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 474, 204–207. doi: 10.1038/nature10131

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrowitz, R., Dahmen, H., Egelhaaf, M., and Krapp, H. G. (2000). Arrangement of optical axes and spatial resolution in the compound eye of the female blowfly Calliphora. J. Comp. Physiol. A 186, 737–746. doi: 10.1007/s003590000127

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ping, Y., Waro, G., Licursi, A., Smith, S., Vo-Ba, D.-A., and Tsunoda, S. (2011). Shal/K(v)4 channels are required for maintaining excitability during repetitive firing and normal locomotion in Drosophila. PLoS ONE 6:e16043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribak, G., Egge, A. R., and Swallow, J. G. (2009). Saccadic head rotations during walking in the stalk-eyed fly (Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni). Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 1643–1649. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1721

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosner, R., Egelhaaf, M., Grewe, J., and Warzecha, A. K. (2009). Variability of blowfly head optomotor responses. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 1170–1184. doi: 10.1242/jeb.027060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandeman, D. C., and Markl, H. (1980). Head movements in flies (Calliphora) produced by deflexion of the halteres. J. Exp. Biol. 85, 43–60.

Google Scholar

Saremi, S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2013). Hierarchical model of natural images and the origin of scale invariance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 3071–3076. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1222618110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilstra, C., and van Hateren, J. H. (1998). Stabilizing gaze in flying blowflies. Nature 395, 654. doi: 10.1038/27114

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuster, S., Strauss, R., and Götz, K. (2002). Virtual-reality techniques resolve the visual cues used by fruit flies to evaluate object distances. Curr. Biol. 18, 1591–1594. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01141-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seelig, J. D., Chiappe, M. E., Lott, G. K., Dutta, A., Osborne, J. E., Reiser, M. B., et al. (2010). Two-photon calcium imaging from head-fixed Drosophila during optomotor walking behavior. Nat. Methods 7, 535–540. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1468

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidl, R. (1986). Die Sehfelder und Ommatidien-Divergenzwinkel von Arbeiterin, Königin und Drohn der Honigbiene (Apis mellifica). Darmstadt: Technische Hochschule.

Smakman, J. G. J., Hateren, J. H., and Stavenga, D. G. (1984). Angular sensitivity of blowfly photoreceptors: intracellular measurements and wave-optical predictions. J. Comp. Physiol. A 155, 239–247. doi: 10.1007/BF00612641

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanford, T. R., Shankar, S., Massoglia, D. P., Costello, M. G., and Salinas, E. (2010). Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 379–385. doi: 10.1038/nn.2485

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauß, R., and Heisenberg, M. (1990). Coordination of legs during straight walking and turning in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A 167, 403–412.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Stürzl, W., Boeddeker, N., Dittmar, L., and Egelhaaf, M. (2010). Mimicking honeybee eyes with a 280 degrees field of view catadioptric imaging system. Bioinspir. Biomim. 5:036002. doi: 10.1088/1748-3182/5/3/036002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tammero, L. F., and Dickinson, M. H. (2002). The influence of visual landscape on the free flight behavior of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 327–343.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatler, B., O'Carroll, D. C., and Laughlin, S. B. (2000). Temperature and the temporal resolving power of fly photoreceptors. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 186, 399–407. doi: 10.1007/s003590050439

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Schaaf, A., and van Hateren, J. H. (1996). Modelling the power spectra of natural images: statistics and information. Vision Res. 36, 2759–2770. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00002-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voronoi, G. (1908). Nouvelles applications des paramètres continus à la théorie des formes quadratiques. Premier mémoire. Sur quelques propriétés des formes quadratiques positives parfaites. J. für die reine und Angew. Math. (Crelle's J.) 1908, 97–102.

Google Scholar

Wosnitza, A., Bockemühl, T., Dübbert, M., Scholz, H., and Büschges, A. (2013). Inter-leg coordination in the control of walking speed in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 480–491. doi: 10.1242/jeb.078139

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeil, J. (1986). The territorial flight of male houseflies (Fannia canicularis L.). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19, 213–219. doi: 10.1007/BF00300862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeil, J. (1993). Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera). J. Comp. Physiol. A. 172, 207–222. doi: 10.1007/BF00189397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeil, J. (1996). The control of optic flow during learning flights. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 180, 25–37. doi: 10.1007/s003590050024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeil, J. (2012). Visual homing: an insect perspective. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Drosophila, saccades, optic flow, visual acuity, halteres, walking, head body coordination

Citation: Geurten BRH, Jähde P, Corthals K and Göpfert MC (2014) Saccadic body turns in walking Drosophila. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:365. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00365

Received: 23 July 2014; Accepted: 02 October 2014;
Published online: 22 October 2014.

Edited by:

Martin Giurfa, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique and Université Paul Sabatier-Toulouse III, France

Reviewed by:

Thomas Collett, University of Sussex, UK
Jessica Lynn Fox, Case Western Reserve University, USA

Copyright © 2014 Geurten, Jähde, Corthals and Göpfert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bart R. H. Geurten, Department of Cellular Neurobiology, Georg-August University of Göttingen, Julia-Lermontowa-Weg 3, D-37077 Göttingen, Germany e-mail: bgeurte@gwdg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.