Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 25 June 2014
Sec. Learning and Memory
This article is part of the Research Topic Progress in Episodic Memory Research View all 46 articles

Impairments in episodic-autobiographical memory and emotional and social information processing in CADASIL during mid-adulthood

\r\n      Angelica Staniloiu,*Angelica Staniloiu1,2*Friedrich G. WoermannFriedrich G. Woermann3Hans J. Markowitsch,\r\n   Hans J. Markowitsch1,4
  • 1Physiological Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 2Hanse Institute of Advanced Science, Delmenhorst, Germany
  • 3MRI Unit, Bethel Epilepsy Center, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 4Center of Excellence “Cognitive Interaction Technology” (CITEC), University of Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany

Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) – is the most common genetic source of vascular dementia in adults, being caused by a mutation in NOTCH3 gene. Spontaneous de novo mutations may occur, but their frequency is largely unknown. Ischemic strokes and cognitive impairments are the most frequent manifestations, but seizures affect up to 10% of the patients. Herein, we describe a 47-year-old male scholar with a genetically confirmed diagnosis of CADASIL (Arg133Cys mutation in the NOTCH3 gene) and a seemingly negative family history of CADASIL illness, who was investigated with a comprehensive neuropsychological testing battery and neuroimaging methods. The patient demonstrated on one hand severe and accelerated deteriorations in multiple cognitive domains such as concentration, long-term memory (including the episodic-autobiographical memory domain), problem solving, cognitive flexibility and planning, affect recognition, discrimination and matching, and social cognition (theory of mind). Some of these impairments were even captured by abbreviated instruments for investigating suspicion of dementia. On the other hand the patient still possessed high crystallized (verbal) intelligence and a capacity to put forth a façade of well-preserved intellectual functioning. Although no definite conclusions can be drawn from a single case study, our findings point to the presence of additional cognitive changes in CADASIL in middle adulthood, in particular to impairments in the episodic-autobiographical memory domain and social information processing (e.g., social cognition). Whether these identified impairments are related to the patient’s specific phenotype or to an ascertainment bias (e.g., a paucity of studies investigating these cognitive functions) requires elucidation by larger scale research.

Introduction

In 1991, a French group (Tournier-Lasserve et al., 1991) described the occurrence of an “[a]utosomal dominant syndrome with strokelike episodes and leukoencephalopathy” (Title of the publication). They studied 45 family members and found in 9 of them transient ischemic attacks and strokes, together with widespread involvement of the white matter. Two years later they described autopsy findings of one of the nine patients – a woman who died at age 59 (Baudrimont et al., 1993). When this working group in 1993 coined the acronym cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) for this disease condition, they stated that related cases had been published since 1977 (Tournier-Lasserve et al., 1993). In a more recent review, Chabriat et al. (2009) remarked that the first case of CADASIL might have been described in 1955 in two sisters by van Bogaert (1955) and categorized at the time as Binswanger’s disease.

Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy is regarded as a hereditary, autosomal dominant “disease with high penetrance in which occlusion of small arteries in the brain of adults results in small deep brain infarcts and progressive accumulation of demyelination areas in the brain” (André, 2010, p. 287). Intrafamilial phenotypic variability has been reported. Phenotypic variations in monozygotic twins were identified and attributed to environmental factors and epigenetic effects (Dichgans et al., 1998; Mykkanen et al., 2004, 2009). De novo mutations and homozygous cases were also infrequently described (Chabriat et al., 2009). A mutation in the NOTCH3 gene in chromosome 19 apparently first leads to a microangiopathy of the small arteries supplying the brain, and – as a consequence – ultimately to dementia (Joutel et al., 1996). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) sometimes shows neural abnormalities from about age 30 onward, strokes may manifest themselves from about age 35 onward, mood disorders after age 40, and dementia may develop between ages 50 and 60 (Sabbadini et al., 1995; Chabriat et al., 2009; André, 2010). Seizures may happen in 5–10% of the patients (Chabriat et al., 2009). Neuroimaging data typically include multifocal white matter lesions, which affect (anterior) temporal lobe and external capsula (Chabriat et al., 1995; Joutel et al., 1996; Brass et al., 2009; Jacqmin et al., 2010; Epelbaum et al., 2011). Changes in basal ganglia, thalamus, brainstem and corpus callosum, and frontal white matter can also be detected (Brass et al., 2009; Chabriat et al., 2009). There is no effective disease modifying treatment available (Dichgans et al., 2008).

Cognitive impairments are considered to be the second most common manifestation of CADASIL. Several studies have linked gene defects to changes in cognition; however, the relation between genotype and brain development and genotype and cognition is very complex. This might also hold true for aberrations related to chromosome 19 (Grimwood et al., 2004; Van der Aa et al., 2010). In patients with CADASIL, the development and progression of changes in cognition have prevailingly been connected to the occurrence of recurrent strokes, but other mechanisms may play a role (Amberla et al., 2004; Peters et al., 2005).

Executive dysfunctions, which were reported to already be present in patients with CADASIL of age 35–50 years, and alterations in processing speed belong to the initial cognitive changes (Peters et al., 2005; Chabriat et al., 2009). Other cognitive impairments, which have been described, are deficits in attention and concentration, visuo-spatial skills, language, verbal and visual memory, and reasoning (Chabriat et al., 2009). Performance on recognition tasks is usually relatively or partially preserved. According to our knowledge, there are no accounts of formal assessment of the retrograde episodic-autobiographical memory in patients with CADASIL with the autobiographical memory interview (Kopelman et al., 1990) or its variants. Most studies which were carried out in patients with CADASIL used testing paradigms, which corresponded to old views of episodic memory (e.g., testing of memory for lists of pairs of words) (Tulving, 1972). Some authors stated that autobiographical memory is “typically impaired from the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and vascular dementia” (Naylor and Clare, 2008, p. 591). Others argued that “in comparison with AD patients, those with a diagnosis of vascular dementia display a relative preservation of episodic memory” (Peters et al., 2005, p. 2078). One study reported impairments in facial affect recognition in patients with CADASIL (Valenti et al., 2013). Their sample of patients had a higher rate of co-morbid depressive symptoms, which may have confounded the findings. Furthermore, no other complex tests of face-emotion processing were carried out and no tests on social cognition were performed. Overall there is a dearth of data in the CADASIL population on the ability for social information processing (Adolphs, 2010), namely social perception (e.g., face perception), social cognition (theory of mind, empathy, social judgment), and social regulation (emotion regulation, monitoring/error correction, self-reflection, deception).

Herein, we aim to impart novel insights into the nature and extent of changes in the episodic-autobiographical memory and social information processing in CADASIL, by reviewing the neuropsychological profile of a patient with a diagnosis of CADASIL, possibly due to a de novo mutation in NOTCH3 gene, and a strong premorbid educational and occupational achievement, who became symptomatic in his early to mid forties.

Case Report

SP is a 47-year old man who self referred to our facility for a neuropsychological assessment, several months after he had received a diagnosis of CADASIL. Apart from a long standing problem with color perception and a questionable history of meningoencephalitis in childhood, SP had reportedly been in good health until his early 40s, when he out of the blue experienced an episode of diplopia (double-seeing). SP sought medical advice. He received a diagnosis of cranial mononeuropathy with left trochlear nerve (IV) paresis of unclear etiology, and a brief treatment with glucocorticoids. The diplopia remitted after a couple of weeks. Serum and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) laboratory results were negative. Borrelia serology was also interpreted as unremarkable at the time. X-rays of the thorax showed no abnormalities. A head computed tomography (CT) demonstrated no evidence of intracerebral bleeding, but pointed to modifications in prefrontal white matter. MRI provided no evidence of infarcts or tumors. It evidenced changes in the white matter, namely in the periventricular areas and corpus callosum. The brain changes were judged as being old at the time and attributed to a questionable history of meningoencephalitis in childhood. Two years later, SP experienced a recurrence of the diplopia, which was linked to unilateral abducens nerve (VI) palsy. While the etiology of the sixth nerve palsy could not be ascertained, the seemingly progressive white matter abnormalities detected by structural MRI raised at the time the suspicion of multiple sclerosis and prompted a comprehensive work up. The diplopia recovered after approximately 2 weeks after another brief trial of glucocorticoid anti-inflammatories. SP’s symptoms however broadened. He developed word finding difficulties, dizziness, and problems with orientation. Later both complex focal seizures and grand mal seizures occurred. Thorough medical and laboratory investigations were carried out and several therapeutic trials were initiated. SP stopped driving an automobile and stated that he has not driven any since his diagnosis of seizures. Until CADASIL was proven by genetic studies, SP’s working diagnoses encompassed multiple sclerosis, neuroborreliosis (Lyme’s disease), viral encephalitis, a mitochondrial disease with leukoencephalopathy, a hereditary metabolic leukoencephalopathy, and a cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Serological screening testing with enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) was positive for IgG antibodies against Borrelia burgdorferi (markers of previous infection), but negative for IgM antibodies (markers of actual infection). Confirmatory Western immunoblot tests followed. They were negative for IgM antibodies against B. burgdorferi, but IgG blots yielded positive results. The CSF evaluations provided evidence of positive IgG antibodies against B. burgdorferi, but were again negative for IgM antibodies. CSF to serum anti-B. burgdorferi antibody index was however mildly elevated. CSF analysis was positive for oligoclonal IgG bands Type III, but revealed no proteinemia or pleocytosis or abnormalities in glucose and lactate levels. SP had no meningitis signs or symptoms. Serum cell blood counts and differentials, liver and renal function tests were within normal limits. Despite insufficient and debated evidence for active neuroborreliosis, SP received a 21-day parenteral course of a cephalosporin antibiotic (Ceftriaxone 2 g a day), with unclear benefits. For the control of seizure activity, treatment with sodium divalproate was begun, with good seizure control and no significant subjective untoward effects. Contradictory findings of oligoclonal bands on the protein electrophoresis of CSF, a negative family history and progressive brain abnormalities affecting the white matter maintained for a substantial period the suspicion of multiple sclerosis (Zeman et al., 1993; Dichgans et al., 1999; Compston and Coles, 2008; Bentley et al., 2011). In the face of repeated and additional laboratory investigations the diagnosis of multiple sclerosis was subsequently discarded. Several factors strongly argued against viewing the infection with B. burgdorferi as a primary mechanism for the cognitive and physical changes of SP (Yamamoto et al., 2011; Eikeland et al., 2012), in addition to the genetic testing (see below). As mentioned above, SP received an adequate, evidence-based treatment with an antibiotic that is very active against Lyme borreliosis and crosses the blood–brain barrier (Stanek et al., 2012), despite questionable support for an active diagnosis of neuroborreliosis. There are indeed authors who argue that positive B. burgdorferi immunological markers in CSF may not mean that the individual has active neuroborreliosis (Roos and Berger, 2007). They reportedly could represent proof of a previous infection (Stanek et al., 2012) or leakage of antibodies from serum across the blood–brain barrier, in the context of a CADASIL-related increase in the permeability of the blood–brain barrier (Roos and Berger, 2007; Collongues et al., 2012). Furthermore, asymptomatic infection with Borrelia may occur relatively frequently in Europe (Steere et al., 2003). Additionally, the existence and magnitude of chronic courses of neuropsychological deficits after symptomatic B. burgdorferi infection are debated (Shadick et al., 1999; Eikeland et al., 2012; Stanek et al., 2012). Some authors reported that neuropsychological deficits pertaining to memory and executive functions may follow a chronic course (Eikeland et al., 2012) in a subset of patients, lasting for more than 6 months (post-Lyme borreliosis syndrome) (Stanek et al., 2012). Shadick et al. (1999) found that subjective memory impairments were reported by patients with a history of Lyme disease 6 years after the infection. However, performance on objective memory tests was deemed in this study as being comparable with that of matched healthy participants without a history of Lyme disease. Finally, although brain changes may occasionally persist after successful antibiotic treatment in neuroborreliosis (Agarwal and Sze, 2009; Hildenbrand et al., 2009), SP’s pattern of brain changes had several unique elements that supported the final diagnosis of CADASIL (Chabriat et al., 2009) (see below). It was therefore concluded that the characteristics of the patient’s features mandated consideration of other diagnostic entities.

Blood work results were remarkable for mildly increased homocysteine levels, slightly increased sodium levels, slightly increased hematocrit, low folic acid level, borderline low vitamin B12 levels, slightly increased glucosylated hemoglobin, and evidence of mild dyslipidemia. Cholesterol fractions HDL and LDL reached normal limits with lipid lowering medication (a statin). Serology for vasculitis and autoimmune diseases (systemic lupus erythematosus) and lues and human immuno deficiency virus (HIV) was negative. There was no indication of infection with cytomegalic virus, herpes simplex virus, Toxoplasma gondii, or active infection with varicella zoster. Thyroid function tests and ceruloplasmin plasma levels were within normal limits. Investigation of AD serum and CSF biomarkers yielded unremarkable results (Jahn, 2013). Due to suboptimal vitamin B values, replacement with both vitamin B12 and folic acid was instituted. An angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor (ACEI) was apparently started for borderline blood pressure values, with good results. Electrocardiogram (ECG) studies revealed a normal sinus rhythm. Several electroencephalograms (EEG) were carried out. The most recent EEG with hyperventilation test was unremarkable. Electrophysiological investigations of tibial, peroneal, sural, and median nerves did not yield any significant results. Electromyography was unremarkable, with the exception of some unspecific changes of the right vastus lateralis muscle. The neuro-ophthalmology exam was suggestive of a sub-clinical optic nerve (II) neuropathy with bilateral pathological visual evoked potentials as well as bilateral temporal pallor of the papilla. Incidentally, optic nerve changes of ischemic origin in CADASIL were described in some case reports (Rufa et al., 2005) and delays in the visual cortical responses were also presented in case series (Parisi et al., 2000, 2003). Auditory evoked potentials were on the left side mildly slowed down, consistent with a change in the pontine area. Interestingly, abnormal brainstem auditory evoked potentials were also depicted in a member of one Italian family with CADASIL by Burganza et al. (1996). In the case of SP, the somato-sensory evoked potential of the median nerve was unremarkable.

Genetic testing was performed and confirmed an Arg133Cys mutation in the NOTCH3 gene, which has been linked to CADASIL in previous reports (Mykkanen et al., 2004, 2009). Given that both biological parents of SP were healthy and well above 71 years (the oldest age when CADASIL was ever reported to first manifest itself clinically) and that there was no clear evidence of a family history of CADASIL, a spontaneous de novo mutation was suspected (Joutel et al., 2000; Coto et al., 2006; Bentley et al., 2011; MacArthur et al., 2014). The results of genetic testing, neuroimaging, and the constellation of signs and symptoms supported the final diagnosis of CADASIL.

Materials and Methods

Neuroimaging Data Examination

Several imaging investigations were performed, such as CT scan of the brain (as described above), repeated head MRIs, ultrasound, and angiography. Extracranial and transcranial duplex ultrasound evidenced a normal vertebrobasilar artery and lack of significant changes in common carotid artery and external and internal carotid arteries. Magnetic resonance angiography of the head and neck yielded no significant abnormalities.

T1 and T2 sequences of his central nervous system were obtained with a conventional 1.5-T MRI device. Earlier scans pointed to abnormalities in periventricular white matter, corpus callosum, and centrum semiovale (Chabriat et al., 2009). Later, multiple lacunar lesions were described bilaterally in the basal ganglia and in the left frontal region. Abnormalities were also reported in the brainstem and signs of microbleeds were detected in the thalamus. The most recent neuroimaging exam (which was performed 1 month prior to the present neuropsychological examination) underlined the presence of chronic lacunar defects and gliotic changes affecting bilaterally fronto-parietal regions and basal ganglia. Subacute lacunar infarcts were visualized in the left parietal white matter in addition. Lacunar defects at this time were also identified in the right temporal lobe and adjacent to the right amygdala. Gliotic changes affecting both anterior temporal lobes were also visualized. Figure 1 illustrates the most relevant changes.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Both chronic and acute changes in a patient with proven CADASIL. Chronic lacunar defects and gliotic changes affecting bilateral fronto-parietal regions including basal ganglia on both sides [(A); axial FLAIR image]. Subacute lacunar infarct in the left parietal white matter [(B); axial diffusion weighted image]. Gliotic changes affecting both anterior temporal lobes are thought to be typical for CADASIL [(C,D); axial FLAIR and coronal T2 weighted images; please note: lacunar defects in the right temporal lobe are adjacent to the right-sided amygdala].

Anamnesis: Personal and Family History

SP denied a history of alcohol or drug use. He reported several environmental allergies. The last neurological exam, performed several months before the assessment was significant for a left light facial paresis.

At the time of the neuropsychological assessment, SP was taking aspirin, a hypolipidemic drug (a statin), an ACEI drug for hypertension, a proton pump inhibitor medication for gastro-esophageal reflux, sodium divalproate for seizure protection, folic acid, and vitamin B12. He reported tolerating well the medication and that both his seizures and blood pressure were well controlled on the current regimen. Last valproic acid trough blood levels were within therapeutic range.

SP’s family history was significant for a sibling suffering from a hearing deficit with adult onset, of unclear origin (questionable acoustic neurinoma). Although cranial neuropathies including involvement of nerve VIII have occasionally been reported in patients with CADASIL, according to SP’s knowledge his sibling’s condition was stable and unremarkable for other symptoms. Parents were in their 70s and healthy according to SP’s knowledge. SP described his birth and postpartum development as uneventful. According to his knowledge, he achieved developmental milestones at a normal age. SP reported being involved in a long-term romantic relationship. He was holding for several years an important and rewarding academic position in business and economy, but was faced with the prospects of a premature forced retirement since he had received the diagnosis of CADASIL. SP appeared still confident in his intellectual abilities and judged himself as being fit for his profession. Although SP had undergone other neuropsychological assessments in the past, he wanted to have a second opinion and therefore contacted our department for a new evaluation.

Informant History

Informant history was obtained from SP’s partner and from reviewing copies of consultants and health care provider reports, which SP made available to us. SP’s partner did not elaborate on SP’s difficulties, but opined that SP was still able to deal with the demands of his work, as they substantially relied on routines. SP alluded to some interpersonal difficulties with the partner that led to a temporary separation after his diagnosis with CADASIL, but did not give further details. In previous reports, it was remarked that SP had encountered difficulties at work since his diagnosis of CADASIL, where people had viewed him as scattered and questioned his professional competence. SP disagreed with these voiced concerns and stated that people questioned his work ability for strategic reasons. He perceived himself as being more patient than prior to his diagnosis. He denied problems with memory, although he admitted to engaging in computerized cognitive (including memory) training regularly. Health providers documented a reduction in affect range, mimic and gesturing, and voice monotony, but no conspicuous problems with personal boundaries. They noted that SP described having close social contacts with some co-workers, but no other regular social contacts.

Neuropsychological Examination

For the purpose of the assessment, SP was interviewed and evaluated with several tests. SP gave informed written consent for the participation in the assessment and study and publication of the report. The study adhered to the declaration of Helsinki.

SP had a 1-day appointment in our unit, during which a substantial number of neuropsychological tests were administered. SP did not recall or report the nature of neuropsychological tests he had undergone in the past; however, the copies of health care providers reports indicated that he had at least one evaluation with consortium to establish a registry for AD (CERAD) test battery, more than 3 months prior to our meeting (Morris et al., 1988). This battery contains: mini-mental state examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1977), the Trail Making Test part A and B, a test for phonetic fluency, an abbreviated version of the Boston naming test (15 items), a verbal fluency (animal category) test, a verbal memory test (word list), and a visual memory test and visuo-spatial skill tests.

The tests that were applied in our department were the following:

• Tests for handedness and brain lateralization: The lateral preference inventory (LPI) for measurement of handedness, footedness, earedness, and eyedness (Ehrenstein and Arnold-Schulz-Gahmen, 1997).

• Tests for the estimation of intelligence and overall cognitive status: Abbreviated Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test-Revised (Block test and Picture Completion test) (Dahl, 1972). MWWT-B or Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest-B (Lehrl, 2005), a German version of the National Adult Reading Test NART (Nelson, 1982); DemTect, a dementia screening instrument consisting of six verbal and numerical subtests (Calabrese and Kessler, 2000); Mini-Mental-State-Examination (Folstein et al., 1977; Kessler et al., 1990).

• Tests for the evaluation of attention, concentration, and processing speed: Trail Making Test A and B (TMT-A + TMT-B; Lezak, 1995; Reitan, 1958); d2-Test (Brickenkamp and Zillmer, 1998); Attention Index of the German version of the Wechsler memory scale-revised (WMS-R; Härting et al., 2000).

• Tests for short-term memory and working memory: Wechsler memory scale-revised; digit span and block span forward and backward.

• Test for the evaluation of constructional functions and planning: Copy administration of the Rey–Osterrieth Figure Test (Osterrieth, 1944; Lezak, 1995).

• Tests for the evaluation of the verbal and non-verbal explicit anterograde long-term memory: Wechsler memory scale-revised (Härting et al., 2000); verbal learning memory test (VLMT) (Helmstaedter et al., 2001); Rey–Osterrieth-Figure (copy trial followed by delayed recall after 30 min) (Lezak, 1995); Doors Test of the Doors and People Test (Baddeley et al., 1994); Emotional Pictures Test (Cramon et al., 1993).

• Tests for the evaluation of retrograde memory: Extended autobiographical memory interview (Seidl et al., 2006), a German language adaptation of the autobiographical memory interview of Kopelman et al. (1990); Famous Faces Test (Fujiwara et al., 2008).

• Test for the evaluation of procedural memory: Mirror reading test (Cramon et al., 1993; Borsutzky et al., 2008, 2010). For the mirror reading test the variant that is presented by Borsutzky et al. (2008, 2010) was applied.

• Tests for the assessment of executive functions, including problem solving, cognitive flexibility: TMT-B (Lezak, 1995); Tower of Hanoi (Borys et al., 1982; Spitz et al., 1982; Lezak, 1995); California Card Sorting Test (Delis et al., 1992); problem solving test of Cronin-Golomb et al. (1987a,b); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Nelson, 1976).

• Tests of verbal fluency: Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COW; Golden et al., 2002); Supermarket task (word production; subtest of the DemTect); Boston naming test (Kaplan et al., 1983; Lezak, 1995; Spreen and Strauss, 1998; Golden et al., 2002).

• Tests for evaluation of malingering tendencies: Test of memory malingering (TOMM; Tombaugh, 1996; Teichner and Wagner, 2004; Greiffenstein et al., 2008); Rey 15-Item-Test (Lezak, 1995).

• Tests for evaluation of emotional processing: Florida Affect Battery (translated as Tübingen Affect Battery; Bowers et al., 1991; Breitenstein et al., 1996); Emotional Pictures Test (Cramon et al., 1993).

• Tests for mood, personality, and psychopathological and psychological load screening: Beck depression interview (BDI-II; Hautzinger et al., 2006); the symptom checklist revised or SCL-90R (Hessel et al., 2001); Freiburg-personality-inventory-revised (FPI-R; Fahrenberg et al., 2001). The SCL-90R assesses psychiatric symptom load and psychological distress. It contains nine subscales, such as somatization, obsessive–compulsiveness, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, anger–hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid thinking, and psychoticism. The general psychological distress level is appraised based on a global severity index, which is derived from all subscales. The FPI-R offers an assessment of personality across 12 dimensions: life satisfaction, social orientation, motivation to achieve, inhibition, excitability, aggressiveness, stress, physical complaints, health worries, openness, extraversion, and neuroticism. Furthermore, we gave SP a self-rating questionnaire in which he had to estimate how much his personality has changed from the time prior to the CADASIL diagnosis to the present (Crook and Larrabee, 1990; Fast and Fujiwara, 1999).

• Tests for social information processing (face-emotion perception, social cognition, risk-taking behavior): German adaptation of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Reinhold and Markowitsch, 2007); Multiple-Choice-ToM-test (MCTT; Adenauer et al., 2005); Florida Affect Battery; Bowers et al., 1991); Game of Dice Test (Brand et al., 2005; Brand and Markowitsch, 2010). The Multiple-Choice-ToM-Test (Adenauer et al., 2005; Staniloiu et al., 2013) version that we used demanded the patient to read 16 short stories. (The test is available in two variants, one with 30 short written stories and another one with 16 stories.) After each story the patient had to infer the mental states of a protagonist of the story by making use of a forced multiple choice layout with four answering alternatives (but only one right answer). The multiple choice configuration allows distinguishing three different types of faults: (a) mental states inferences that are “excessive”; (b) mental states inferences that are “too positive”; (c) selection of the distractor answer that indicates a neutral answer (i.e., a non-mental state inference such as physical causation).

The game of dice task (Brand et al., 2005; Brand and Markowitsch, 2010) is a fictitious gambling setting, presented via computer, with explicit rules for virtual gains, losses, and winning stratagems. It evaluates decision-making under risk.

Results

General Behavioral Observations

SP was cooperative, casually, but appropriately dressed, with good eye contact and fully oriented. He demonstrated willingness to take part in all tests and showed consistent effort to perform. He reported having some problems with concentration when too much interference was present. He subjectively appraised his memory as being good. He denied problems with mood, apart from some anxiety feelings related to the future of his job. He voiced hope that the results of the tests would indicate that he still was fit to work in his profession. He answered readily all questions and worked without measurable or notable tendencies to worsen, distort or falsify his performance. He promptly followed instructions. SP was clinically unimpaired with respect to audition, but had, according to his report, a long standing history of a color perception deficit. The latter however did not appear to interfere in a conspicuous way with his ability to recognize colors on the applied tests or subtests that used colored items, such a subtest of WMS-R (Härting et al., 2000), the Doors Test of the Doors and People Test (Baddeley et al., 1994), or the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

Below we present and discuss the results of the tests that SP undertook over a period of about 7 h (with breaks). A summary of the findings of SP’s evaluation is provided in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of neuropsychological testing results.

Laterality

SP was clearly lateralized to the right.

Language and Word Knowledge

SP’s knowledge of terms and words, as assessed with the Boston naming test, was evidently above average (cf. Table 1). Given that a version of the Boston naming test had been administered several months prior to the assessment in our department, one might be inclined to attribute the results to a practicing effect. It is however noteworthy mentioning that the previously used variant only included 15 items. Furthermore, SP’s performance at that time had been deemed within normal limits.

Attention, Concentration, and Processing Speed

SP’s performance on tests of attention and concentration was non-uniform (cf. Table 1). In the Attention Index of the WMS-R he only gained 71 points. Similarly, in the d2-test his performance placed him in the lowest percentile. On the other hand, he achieved average scores in the Trail Making Test A and B. The results on Trail Making Test A and B were unexpected, given that a number of studies identified pronounced impairments in performance on these tests in patients with CADASIL without dementia and even without a history of stroke (Peters et al., 2005; Dichgans et al., 2008). Several factors might account for SP’s execution of the last two tests. Possibly, the task required here was more similar to what SP did in his professional life compared to the tasks in the other two tests; therefore he had probably more routine and subsequently made less errors. Another clarification might be the practicing effect (Bartels et al., 2010). Although SP did not recall or communicate that he had these tests before, reviewing the copies of reports revealed that he had been tested with the Trail Making Test A and B several months prior and had at that time achieved below average scores. Furthermore, there are indications that SP engaged in self-directed training of cognitive abilities at home (Smith et al., 2007).

Intelligence and Global Cognitive Performance

Several evaluation procedures were used for assessing SP’s intellectual abilities. The tests applied revealed that SP had a quite high verbal intelligence; however, his performance on both non-verbal parts of IQ testing and in tests that are used for preliminary investigation of the suspicion of dementia (DemTect, MMSE) was situated at low or borderline levels. As SP’s academic formation and profession required verbal skills and a broad vocabulary to a considerable extent, the high scores on verbal intelligence tests are not surprising (Denzler et al., 1986; Christensen et al., 1997; Parkin and Java, 1999). They point to a stability of measures of crystallized intelligence (Christensen et al., 1997) on a background of high educational achievement in this patient, in comparison to measures of fluid intelligence (Christensen et al., 1997; Manard et al., 2014). The scores obtained with instruments for short assessment of suspicion of dementia were however surprisingly low, given SP’s educational and occupational attainments. SP scored 27 on the MMSE. His performance on the DemTect [a global cognitive assessment instrument that includes measures for mental flexibility and verbal (category) fluency] was suggestive of “mild cognitive impairment” (cf. Table 1) (Peters et al., 2005). The discrepancy between the results on the two tests could be attributed to a larger degree of underestimation of executive dysfunction in MMSE (Benisty et al., 2008).

Visual-Constructive Skills

Performance in copying the Rey–Osterrieth Figure was within normal limits (cf. Table 1). We did not find out evidence that he had taken part in this test before.

Short-Term Memory and Working Memory

In the Wechsler Memory Scale subtests for short-term and working memory he was somewhat impaired. In digit span forward he was normal, being able to repeat five digits. In all other measures (digit span backward, block span forward, and backward) he only achieved four digits.

Long-Term memory

A large number of tests were applied to assess SP’s long-term memory abilities. They can be grouped into tests of anterograde, retrograde, episodic-autobiographical, semantic, procedural, and priming memory (cf. Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2012, and Tulving, 2005).

Anterograde memory

On all tests of anterograde memory – with the exception of two simple recognition tasks (the less demanding section of the Doors test, Emotional Pictures Test) – SP performed below the level of normal individuals. The finding of relatively preserved recognition in comparison to recall is congruent with other reports (Peters et al., 2005) (Figure 2). Verbal material was best retrieved (VLMT, verbal memory index of the WMS-R), while retrieval of visual material was the most compromised (ROF, visual memory index of the WMS-R, complex part of the doors test). Incidentally, these results are also consistent with other reports in the CADASIL literature (Amberla et al., 2004; Buffon et al., 2006). Interestingly, difficulties with retrieving visual material have also been reported in other conditions with lesions of fronto-striatal connections, such as Parkinson’s disease (Smith et al., 2010; Souchay et al., 2013). Savage et al. (1999) proposed that fronto-striatal dysfunctions might lead to impairments in retrieving non-verbal material (Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure) due to an organizational deficit in the copy condition, related to problems with shifting mental or spatial sets. One might argue that a more favorable performance on verbal memory tasks might reflect SP’s background. Alternatively, it may bear connections with sub-clinical visual system impairments and partly with color perception abnormalities (Glen et al., 2012). SP’s performance was particularly poor when a delay of half an hour had to be bridged between learning and free recall (ROF, long-term memory index of the WMS-R) (cf. Table 1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Copying (A) and reproduction by heart after half an hour (B) of the Rey–Osterrieth figure.

Retrograde memory

Retrograde semantic memory recognition. SP recognized famous persons very well (famous faces test) (cf. Table 1).

Retrograde autobiographical memory recall. SP spontaneously generated an impoverished account of specific details of autobiographical episodes from this past (autobiographical memory interview), resembling in this respect patients with a prodrome of Alzheimer’s dementia (Seidl et al., 2006). This finding might have been compounded in his case by the severe executive functions deficit (Brand and Markowitsch, 2008). Additionally, SP showed impaired retrieval of visual material. One might therefore argue that a compromised capacity to retrieve visual material might have also contributed to the production of schematic, overgeneralized autobiographical memories, with meager detail specificity (Bauer, 1982; Ogden, 1993; Greenberg et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2010; Souchay and Smith, 2013).

Procedural memory

SP’s procedural skills, as assessed by his capacity to carry out mirror image reading appeared to be intact (cf. Table 1), despite presence of damage in the basal ganglia. Intact performance on the mirror image reading might reflect not only the preserved ability to learn and store new procedural skills, but may be supported by various cognitive components, including high levels of verbal intelligence (Markowitsch and Härting, 1996). Incidentally, other authors found uncompromised performance on mirror reading in patients with various fronto-striatal lesions (Schmidtke et al., 2002).

Malingering

In the two instruments applied to investigate tendencies for malingering (Rey 15-Item-Test; TOMM), SP performed without any error and subsequently showed no tendency to malinger (cf. Table 1). We did not expect SP to feign impairments, but administered these tests to be on the safe side with respect to interpreting his performance in tests of memory.

Executive functions, cognitive flexibility, risk taking, and problem solving abilities

SP’s problem solving and executive abilities were largely impaired, with the only exceptions being the TMT-B and a test of concept formation (Cronin-Golomb et al., 1987a,b). We already commented above about the possible practicing effects on TMT-B test results, which may have led to improvements in performance over time. In the problem solving test of Cronin-Golomb et al. (1987a,b), SP was shown 17 sheets of paper; each leaf presented a drawing on the left side of the paper and three drawings on the right side (e.g., a crescent moon on the left side and a penguin, a woodpecker, and an owl on the right side). He had to tell which of the three drawings on the right side matches best the drawing on the left side. In this test, the correct answer can be given by simply excluding the more unlikely answers. In this way, the test resembles the more simple long-term memory tests, in which only recognition is required (as opposed to free recall).

In tests, where SP had to actively generate material or ideas – such as in the California card sorting test (Delis et al., 1992), the tower of Hanoi, the word fluency [COW], the supermarket task, and the Wisconsin card sorting test – his performance was impaired. In the game of dice task, SP behaved in strong contrast with his scholar background in business and economics. He showed no evidence of a sound or reasonable strategy about how to gain money in the fictitious game and consequently behaved randomly with respect to his choices. The findings of impairments in executive functions (planning, problem solving, set-shifting, cognitive flexibility, categorization, error detection, and monitoring) are in agreement with reports in the literature (Peters et al., 2005; Buffon et al., 2006). Contrary to other descriptions, we did not find evidence of relative preservation of performance on letter fluency tasks in comparison to category fluency tasks (Amberla et al., 2004; Dichgans, 2009).

Tests for social information processing (perception and interpretation of emotional states, theory of mind)

SP was impaired (partly even severely impaired) in tests of affect discrimination, naming, selection, and matching as well as facial identity discrimination [Florida (Tübingen) Affect Battery (Subtests 1-)]. Tests for social cognition (“Augen-ToM-Test” or Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test or RMET, multiple choice theory of mind test or MCTT) (cf. Table 1) yielded indices of below average performance as well. Both on Florida (Tübingen) Affect Battery subtests and “Augen-ToM-Test” SP encountered significant problems with the emotion of fear. Impairments in recognizing facial emotions (especially fear) have recently been reported in a group of patients with CADASIL by Valenti et al. (2013). In contrast to our patient, who showed no indication of depressive symptomatology, a substantial number of patients in the study of Valenti et al. (2013) endorsed depressive symptoms.

Tests for mood, personality, and psychopathological and psychological load screening

The screening instrument Beck depression inventory did not yield scores suggestive of an affective (depressive) disorder. The symptom check list (SCL-90-R) analysis did not generate abnormal scores. In the FPI-R, SP chose descriptors’ values suggesting reduced life satisfaction, self-focused attitude, self control, very little aggressiveness, and moderately reduced concern about health. He perceived himself as being mildly introverted and emotionally stable. He experienced himself as being highly passive and with decreased motivation for achievement. The latter finding is of interest, given a high percentage of patients with CADASIL experiencing apathy (Chabriat et al., 2009). We however did not identify any conspicuous evidence of apathetic behavior. On the openness subscale of the FPI-R, SP scored within normal limits. In the self-evaluation questionnaire, SP only gave partial responses with respect to changes since his CADASIL diagnosis. He indicated that he was very afraid of having to take a premature, forced retirement, and of losing his partner. He reported increased appetite and relinquishing some of his previous hobbies.

Figure 3 gives an overview of his test results, demonstrating that performance on a large array of the applied tests was on the deficient side. This is particularly evident in the illustration showing the distribution of inferior, normal, and superior test performance (Figure 3B), and especially for test results on anterograde memory, where his performance for all tests was situated on the inferior level (Figure 3C).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Graphical illustrations of test results. (A) Performance distribution in all 40 tests applied; scores roughly refer to standard deviations: 1/2 = less than a standard deviation below (−) or above (+) 0; ±1 or ±2 refer to about 1 or 2 standard deviations below or above baseline. (B) Performance distribution of tests with an inferior to normal (−), a normal (0), and a superior to normal performance (+). (C) Distribution of performance in tests measuring anterograde memory.

Discussion

While the diagnosis of CADASIL is established (Hervé and Chabriat, 2010; Russell, 2010; Assareh et al., 2011; Yamamoto et al., 2011; Federico et al., 2012), case descriptions with a detailed neuropsychological examination identifying areas of deficiency and preservation can enrich the understanding of this condition and provide novel insights and ideas for larger scale research.

SP’s case features a constellation of unique characteristics in our opinion. The clinical manifestations have included less common symptoms and findings, such as seizures, hypertension, diplopia, sub-clinical involvement of optic nerve as well as of the nerve VIII (André, 2010; Yamamoto et al., 2011). The misleading clinical presentation, pattern of laboratory results, and the seemingly negative family history of CADASIL are also atypical traits, which have posed a substantial challenge to securing a definitive diagnosis. The pattern of laboratory results pointed to features of dysregulated immunity. These features may have been underpinned by a history of infection with B. burgdorferi (Zeman et al., 1993). Alternatively, they may reflect an unusual form of CADASIL, with inflammatory-like processes (Dichgans et al., 1999; Bentley et al., 2011; Collongues et al., 2012). SP’s distinctiveness might also pertain to his intellectual background and his profession. Another peculiar aspect is the progression of his cognitive functioning, encompassing marked deteriorations in a large number of cognitive domains being documented shortly after the diagnosis of CADASIL, limited insight into cognitive problems and reasonably maintained ability to put forth a façade of preserved intellectual functioning. SP did not endorse symptoms of depression and did not meet criteria for other psychiatric disorders. The bulk of neuropsychological examination results stayed in strong contradiction with his own appraisals of his cognitive difficulties. Furthermore, SP’s structural imaging indicated substantial brain damage, involving subcortical structures, white matter as well as cortical areas.

Interestingly, SP’s “crystallized” (verbally based) intelligence was still outstanding in one test and above average in another. This finding is in opposition to the results of Matsuda and Saito (1998) who in elderly patients with mild Alzheimer’s dementia found deficits to be stronger in crystallized intelligence in comparison to age-matched healthy controls, while “fluid” intelligence measures did not deviate significantly from those of healthy participants. However, SP showed severely compromised performance on a number of intellectual measurements. In this way he resembled the three patients studied by Dominguez-Sanchez et al. (2011), who all lost a significant intelligence quotient and general intellectual capacity during the progression of CADASIL (7 years follow-up).

In the memory domain, SP showed a number of impairments. His subaverage performance in short-term memory and working memory tasks likely bears relations to changes in parietal and prefrontal areas (Markowitsch et al., 1999) and in frontal–subcortical circuits (Amberla et al., 2004). Massive impairments were noted on tests of anterograde (conscious) long-term memory. Similarly to his crystallized intelligence, his old knowledge was largely maintained. SP was still very able to recognize and name portraits of prominent persons (famous faces test). However, when he had to actively generate episodes of his own life, SP provided an impoverished, rudimentary, semanticized account.

SP’s deficits with consciously acquiring mnemonic information for long-term storage could have stemmed from his damage to temporal as well as parietal areas and fiber connections (Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2012; Staniloiu and Markowitsch, 2012). There are numerous meticulous documentations of anterograde memory impairments after bilateral damage of medio-temporal regions. Furthermore, studies show that hippocampal damage might be a route of impairment in anterograde conscious memory in CADASIL (as well as in patients with seizures) (Jokeit et al., 1997; O’Sullivan et al., 2009; Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2012). Damage to the thalamus and diencephalic tracts may constitute another source of anterograde memory impairments in patients with CADASIL (Markowitsch, 1991; O’Sullivan et al., 2004; Chabriat et al., 2009; Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2012). Interestingly, evidence of microbleeds in the thalamus was reported in the case of SP. Additionally, damage within prefrontal regions may also partly contribute to impairments in the acquisition of semantic and episodic mnemonic information for long-term storage (Rieger and Markowitsch, 1996; Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2012).

As SP’s ability for long-term concentration was impaired as well (particularly evident from the multiple omissions in the d2-Test), this might have added to both his anterograde and retrograde memory impairments. The attentional deficits may have stemmed from damage in parietal and frontal areas and their connections, and possibly from diencephalic and brainstem lesions (Mesulam, 1990). Parietal regions have been implicated not only in attentional processes, but also in short-term memory, episodic memory, self referential processing, and time perception (Babinsky et al., 1996; Beblo et al., 2001; Cabeza et al., 2011). They are opined to be engaged in modulation of top-down (frontal) and bottom-up (medial temporal lobe areas) episodic memory-related processes (Beblo et al., 2001; Cabeza et al., 2011; Souchay et al., 2013). The impairments in retrograde episodic-autobiographical memory observed in SP might deliver support for the idea that intact right fronto-temporal connections are important for “ecphorizing” old, emotionally tagged events (Fink et al., 1996; Kroll et al., 1997). As mentioned previously, SP had lacunar defects in the right temporal lobe and adjacent to the right amygdala as well as gliotic changes affecting both anterior temporal lobes.

Consistent with established descriptions in the literature, SP showed a pronounced executive dysfunction syndrome. Planning functions and the ability to overview necessary steps in solving more complex tasks were particularly deficient. This cannot only be inferred from SP’s severe impairments in the Wisconsin card sorting test and California card sorting test, but also from the way he performed in the d2-Test where he should have crossed out all letters “d” which were accompanied by two small lines above or below them. He had a very high number of omissions, being apparently simply focused to reach the end of each of the 14 long letter lines – an aim usually impossible to accomplish.

A striking finding was the large extent of deficits in face-emotion processing tests and theory of mind tasks. In the Florida Affect Battery (Tübingen Affect Battery), SP’s performance was compromised on both simple and complex tasks. These results might reflect damage of cortical and subcortical areas (and their fiber connections), which are recruited in face and emotion processing (Herholz et al., 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2004; Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2011). As mentioned above, in the case of SP we visualized lacunar defects in the right temporal lobe and in regions neighboring the right amygdala. Amygdalar dysfunction and/or damage of anterior hippocampus could lead to impairments of fear processing (Fanselow and Dong, 2010; Feinstein et al., 2011). The mentalizing deficits of SP might have originated from disruptions of temporal, parietal, and frontal regions and their connections (Abu-Akel, 2003) as well as damage of basal ganglia (Bodden et al., 2010; Kemp et al., 2013).

SP’s apparently reduced insight and awareness into his cognitive deficits (especially in the memory domain) could have both biological and psychological underpinnings (Naylor and Clare, 2008; André, 2010). From a neuropsychological point of view, the diminished insight may mirror a collapsing executive system, faulty self knowledge updating processes, and impaired self awareness (Naylor and Clare, 2008; Klein and Gangi, 2010). With respect to neural correlates, these insight deficits may be underlain by the observed changes in fronto-parietal regions (Wagner et al., 2005; Stone et al., 2006; Metzinger, 2008; Nyberg et al., 2010; Souchay et al., 2013). From a psychological perspective, the deficient insight may reflect a desire to mask or deny his difficulties, in order to sustain a positive sense of identity (Naylor and Clare, 2008).

Conclusion

The present case report highlights a number of new as well as more established facets of CADASIL.

The most striking and novel aspect of our case is the extent of impairments in the episodic-autobiographical domain and social information processing (face-emotion processing, social cognition). These changes had not been documented to these degrees in patients with CADASIL. Whether these identified impairments are related to the patient’s specific phenotype (e.g., history of seizures, markers of dysregulated immunity) or to an ascertainment bias (e.g., a paucity of studies investigating these cognitive functions) will need to be clarified by larger scale research. The systematic investigation of social information processing in CADASIL patients is important as deficits in this domain might constitute hidden and often unrecognized and neglected sources of occupational and interpersonal difficulties. Although the informant history did not provide a clear corroboration of SP’s deficits in social information processing, it is possible that at least some of his occupational and interpersonal difficulties might have been underpinned by his problems with social information processing.

Our case furthermore underlines the large phenotypic variability of CADASIL, which may be shaped by gene-environment interplays (Yamamoto et al., 2011). Although rarely reported, presentations with markers of dysregulated immunity might occur, as our case points to. The dysregulated immunity might reflect the presence of a concurrent infectious or immune condition, or a gene-environmental interplay (Bentley et al., 2011). These aspects have possible implications for the differential diagnosis and for the exploration of immune-modulating treatment approaches.

Finally, our case description suggests that the cognitive decline caused by CADASIL may follow a differentiated trajectory in patients with high intellectual and educational attainment in comparison to those with lower levels of educational and occupational achievement. In this respect CADASIL may have a similar impact on cognition as AD – though usually starting much earlier in life. For AD it has been repeatedly found that individuals starting from a high intellectual background will deteriorate faster and more profoundly than those starting from a moderate intellectual background (Scarmeas et al., 2006; Wilson et al., 2010). A high degree of cognitive reserve in patients with CADASIL might initially act as a buffer against the consequences of brain changes, braking the cognitive decline. When a critical burden of brain lesions is reached, this adaptation mechanism might break down, unraveling the consequences of brain damage and apparently getting translated into a more rapid cognitive decline in these patients in comparison to patients with a lower cognitive reserve (Perneczky et al., 2009). Though no firm conclusions can be drawn from studying a single case, our case report joins proposals for extending and making use of the concept of cognitive reserve for the understanding of vascular cognitive deficits (Stern, 2002; Zieren et al., 2013).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the patient for his willingness to take part in the study and for his cooperation. Informed consent was obtained from the patient for participation in the study and publication of the results. Research has been carried out in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Research was supported by CITEC and the Hanse Institute of Advanced Science. We also gratefully acknowledge support for the Article Processing Charge by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the Open Access Publication Funds of Bielefeld University Library.

References

Abu-Akel, A. (2003). A neurobiological mapping of theory of mind. Brain Res. Rev. 43, 29–40. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00190-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adenauer, H., Kessler, J., Brand, M., Fleck, S., Lechmann, C., Prim, W., et al. (2005). “Vergleich verschiedener deutschsprachiger Theory-of-Mind Tests bei Menschen mit Asperger Syndrom [comparison of different German-language theory-of-mind tests in human beings with Asperger’s syndrome],” in Presentation at the 78 Congress of the German Society for Neurology (Wiesbaden).

Adolphs, R. (2010). Conceptual challenges and directions for social neuroscience. Neuron 65, 752–767. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Agarwal, R., and Sze, G. (2009). Neuro-Lyme disease: MR imaging findings. Radiology 253, 167–173. doi:10.1148/radiol.2531081103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amberla, K., Waljas, M., Tuominen, S., Almkvist, O., Poyhonen, M., Tuisku, S., et al. (2004). Insidious cognitive decline in CADASIL. Stroke 35, 1598–1602. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000129787.92085.0a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

André, C. (2010). Pathogenesis, clinical and radiological findings and treatment. Arg. Neuropsiquiatr. 68, 287–299. doi:10.1590/S0004-282X2010000200026

CrossRef Full Text

Assareh, A., Mather, K. A., Schofield, P. R., Kwok, J. B. J., and Sachdev, P. S. (2011). The genetics of white matter lesions. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 17, 525–540. doi:10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00181.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Babinsky, R., Spiske, K., Engel, H., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1996). Attentional dysfunction associated with a discrete left parietal lesion. Neurol. Psychiatr. Brain Res. 4, 139–142.

Baddeley, A., Emslie, H., and Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). Doors and People Test. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., and Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bartels, C., Wegrzyn, M., Wiedl, A., Ackermann, V., and Ehrenreich, H. (2010). Practice effects in healthy adults: a longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing. BMC Neurosci. 11:118. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-11-118

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baudrimont, M., Dubas, F., Joutel, A., Tournier-Lasserve, E., and Bousser, M. G. (1993). Autosomal dominat leukoencephalopathy and subcortical ischemic stroke. A clinical study. Stroke 24, 122–125. doi:10.1161/01.STR.24.1.122

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bauer, R. M. (1982). Visual hypoemotionality as a symptom of visual-limbic disconnection in man. Arch. Neurol. 39, 702–708. doi:10.1001/archneur.1982.00510230028009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beblo, T., Piefke, M., Driessen, M., Markowitsch, H. J., and Woermann, F. G. (2001). Contribution of retrosplenial and parietal brain regions to episodic memory: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 13(Part 2), S640. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(01)91983-4

CrossRef Full Text

Benisty, S., Hernandez, K., Viswanathan, A., Reyes, S., Kurtz, A., O’Sullivan, M., et al. (2008). Diagnostic criteria of vascular dementia in CADASIL. Stroke 39, 838–844. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.490672

CrossRef Full Text

Bentley, P., Wang, T., Malik, O., Nicholas, R., Ban, M., Sawcer, S., et al. (2011). CADASIL with cord involvement associated with a novel and atypical NOTCH3 mutation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 82, 855–860. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2010.223297

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bodden, M. E., Mollenhauer, B., Trenkwalder, C., Cabanel, N., Eggert, K. M., Unger, M. M., et al. (2010). Affective and cognitive theory of mind in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Dis. 16, 466–470. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.04.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borsutzky, S., Fujiwara, E., Brand, M., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2008). Confabulations in alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychologia 46, 3133–3143. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borsutzky, S., Fujiwara, E., Brand, M., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2010). Susceptibility to false memories in patients with ACoA aneurysm. Neuropsychologia 48, 2811–2823. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borys, S. V., Spitz, H. H., and Doans, B. A. (1982). Tower of Hanoi performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children as a function of solution length and goal state. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 33, 87–110. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(82)90008-X

CrossRef Full Text

Bowers, D., Blonder, L. X., and Heilman, K. M. (1991). The Florida Affect Battery. Miami, FL: Florida University Press.

Brand, M., Fujiwara, E., Borsutzky, S., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2005). Decision-making deficits of Korsakoff patients in a new gambling task with explicit rules: associations with executive functions. Neuropsychology 19, 267–277. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.267

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brand, M., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2008). “The role of the prefrontal cortex in episodic memory,” in Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience: Episodic Memory Research, Vol. 18, eds E. Dere, A. Easton, and J. P. Huston (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 317–342.

Brand, M., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2010). Mechanisms contributing to decision-making difficulties in late adulthood: theoretical approaches, speculations and empirical evidence. Gerontology 56, 435–440. doi:10.1159/000300569

CrossRef Full Text

Brass, S. D., Smith, E. E., Arboleda-Velasquez, J. F., Copen, W. A., and Frosch, M. P. (2009). Case records of the Massachusetts General Hospital. Case 12-2009. A 46-year-old man with migraine, aphasia, and hemiparesis and similarly affected family members. N. Engl. J. Med. 360, 1656–1665. doi:10.1056/NEJMcpc0810839

CrossRef Full Text

Breitenstein, C., Daum, I., Ackermann, H., Lütgehetmann, R., and Müller, E. (1996). Erfassung der Emotionswahrnehmung bei zentralnervösen Läsionen und Erkrankungen: psychometrische Gütekriterien der “Tübinger Affekt Batterie”. Neurol. Rehabil. 2, 93–101.

Brickenkamp, R., and Zillmer, E. (1998). The d2 Test of Attention. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

Buffon, F., Porcher, R., Hernandez, K., Kurtz, A., Pointeau, S., Vahedi, K., et al. (2006). Cognitive profile in CADASIL. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 77, 175–180. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.068726

CrossRef Full Text

Burganza, M., Bertolasi, L., de Grandis, D., Bongiovanni, L. G., Polo, A., Cavallaro, T., et al. (1996). CADASIL: neurophysiological study of a large Italian family. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 98, 61. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(96)83500-0

CrossRef Full Text

Cabeza, R., Mazuz, Y. S., Stokes, J., Kragel, J. E., Woldorff, M. G., Ciaramelli, E., et al. (2011). Overlapping parietal activity in memory and perception: evidence for the attention to memory model. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3209–3217. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00065

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calabrese, P., and Kessler, J. (2000). DemTect. Frankfurt: EISAI and Karlsruhe, Pfizer.

Chabriat, H., Joutel, A., Dichgans, M., Tournier-Lasserve, E., and Bousser, M. G. (2009). Cadasil. Lancet Neurol. 8, 643–653. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70127-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chabriat, H., Vahedi, K., Iba-Zizen, M. T., Joutel, A., Nibbio, A., Nagy, T. G., et al. (1995). Clinical spectrum of CADASIL: a study of 7 families. Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy. Lancet 346, 934–939. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(95)91557-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jorm, A. F., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Rodgers, B., et al. (1997). Education and decline in cognitive performance: compensatory but not protective. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 12, 323–330. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1166(199703)12:3<323::AID-GPS492>3.0.CO;2-N

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Collongues, N., Derache, N., Blanc, F., Labauge, P., de Seze, J., and Defer, G. (2012). Inflammatory-like presentation of CADASIL: a diagnostic challenge. BMC Neurol. 12, 78. doi:10.1186/1471-2377-12-78

CrossRef Full Text

Compston, A., and Coles, A. (2008). Multiple sclerosis. Lancet 372, 1502–1517. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61620-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coto, E., Menendez, M., Navarro, R., Garcia-Castro, M., and Alvarez, V. (2006). A new de novo NOTCH3 mutation causing CADASIL. Eur. J. Neurol. 13, 628–631. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2006.01337.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cramon, D. Y., von Markowitsch, H. J., and Schuri, U. (1993). The possible contribution of the septal region to memory. Neuropsychologia 31, 1159–1180. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90065-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cronin-Golomb, A., Rho, W. A., Corkin, S., and Growdon, J. H. (1987a). Abstract reasoning in age-related neurological disease. J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 24, 79–83.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cronin-Golomb, A., Rho, W. A., Corkin, S., and Growdon, J. H. (1987b). Relational abilities in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1, 298.

Crook, T. H., and Larrabee, G. J. (1990). A self-rating scale for evaluating memory in everyday life. Psychol. Aging 5, 48–57. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.5.1.48

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dahl, G. (1972). WIP-Reduzierter Wechsler-Intelligenztest [Abbreviated Wechsler Intelligence Test]. Meisenheim am Glan: Hain.

Delis, D. C., Squire, L. R., Bihrle, A., and Massman, P. (1992). Componential analysis of problem-solving ability: performance of patients with frontal lobe damage and amnesic patients on a new card sorting test. Neuropsychologia 30, 683–697. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(92)90039-O

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Denzler, P., Kessler, J., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1986). Possibilities and weaknesses of psychopathometric diagnosis of dementia. Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 54, 382–392. doi:10.1055/s-2007-1001943

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dichgans, M. (2009). Cognition in CADASIL. Stroke 40(Suppl.), S45–S47. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.534412

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dichgans, M., Markus, H. S., Salloway, S., Verkkoniemi, A., Moline, M., Wang, Q., et al. (2008). Donepezil in patients with subcortical vascular cognitive impairment: a randomised double-blind trial in CADASIL. Lancet Neurol. 7, 310–318. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70046-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dichgans, M., Mayer, M., Uttner, I., Bruning, R., Muller-Hocker, J., Rungger, G., et al. (1998). The phenotypic spectrum of CADASIL: clinical findings in 102 cases. Ann. Neurol. 44, 731–739. doi:10.1002/ana.410440506

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dichgans, M., Wick, M., and Gasser, T. (1999). Cerebrospinal fluid findings in CADASIL. Neurology 53, 233. doi:10.1212/WNL.53.1.233

CrossRef Full Text

Dominguez-Sanchez, F. J., Lasa-Aristu, A., and Goni-Imizcoz, M. (2011). Intelligence impairment, personality features and psychopathology disturbances in a family affected with CADASIL. Span. J. Psychol. 14, 936–943. doi:10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.39

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ehrenstein, W. H., and Arnold-Schulz-Gahmen, B. E. (1997). Auge, Ohr, Hand und Fuß: Bestimmung des Individuellen Lateralitätsprofils [Eye, Ear, Hand and Foot: Determination of the Individual Laterality Profile]. Dortmund: IfADo.

Eikeland, R., Ljostad, U., Mygland, A., Herlofson, K., and Lohaugen, G. C. (2012). European neuroborreliosis: neuropsychological findings 30 months post-treatment. Eur. J. Neurol. 19, 480–487. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2011.03563.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Epelbaum, S., Benisty, S., Reyes, S., O’Sullivan, M., Jouvent, E., During, M., et al. (2011). Verbal memory impairment in subcortical ischemic vascular disease: a descriptive analysis in CADASIL. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 2172–2182. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.12.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fahrenberg, J., Hampel, R., and Selg, H. (2001). Das Freiburger Persönlichkeitsinventar (FPI-R) [Freiburg Personality Inventory (FPI.R)], 7 Edn. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Fanselow, M. S., and Dong, H. W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65, 7–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fast, K., and Fujiwara, E. (1999). Selbsteinschätzungsfragebogen [Self-Rating Questionnaire]. Bielefeld: University of Bielefeld.

Federico, A., Donato, I. D., Bianchi, S., Di Palma, C., Taglia, I., and Dotti, M. T. (2012). Hereditary cerebral small vessel disease: a review. J. Neurol. Sci. 322, 25–30. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2012.07.041

CrossRef Full Text

Feinstein, J. S., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A., and Tranel, D. (2011). The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Curr. Biol. 21, 34–38. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M., Bruckbauer, T., Kessler, J., and Heiss, W.-D. (1996). Cerebral representation of one’s own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J. Neurosci. 16, 4275–4282.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1977). Minimal mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text

Fujiwara, E., Brand, M., Kracht, L., Kessler, J., Diebel, A., Netz, J., et al. (2008). Functional retrograde amnesia: a multiple case study. Cortex 44, 29–45. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2005.09.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Glen, F. C., Crabb, D. P., Smith, N. D., Burton, R., and Garway-Heath, D. F. (2012). Do patients with glaucoma have difficulty recognizing faces? Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, 3629–3637. doi:10.1167/iovs.11-8538

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Golden, C. J., Espe-Pfeifer, P., and Wachsler-Felder, J. (2002). Neuropsychological Interpretation of Objective Psychological Tests. New York, NY: Kluver.

Greenberg, D. L., Eacott, M. J., Brechin, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2005). Visual memory loss and autobiographical amnesia: a case study. Neuropsychologia 43, 1493–1502. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Greiffenstein, M. F., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., and Baker, W. J. (2008). Test of memory malingering and word memory test: a new comparison of failure concordance rates. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 801–807. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2008.07.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grimwood, J., Gordon, L. A., Olsen, A., Terry, A., Schmutz, J., Lamerdin, J., et al. (2004). The DNA sequence and biology of human chromosome 19. Nature 428, 529–535. doi:10.1038/nature02399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., and Kessler, J. (2000). Die Wechsler-Memory-Scale Revised [Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised]. Bern: Huber.

Hautzinger, M., Keller, F., and Kühner, C. (2006). Beck-Depressionsinventar II (BDI-II) [Beck Depression Inventory]. Frankfurt/Main: Harcourt Test Services.

Helmstaedter, C., Lendt, M., and Lux, S. (2001). Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest (VLMT) [Verbal Learning and Memory Test]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Herholz, K., Kessler, J., Ehlen, P., Lenz, O., Kalbe, E., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2001). The role of prefrontal cortex, precuneus, and cerebellum during face-name association learning. Neuropsychologia 39, 643–650. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00144-5

CrossRef Full Text

Hervé, D., and Chabriat, H. (2010). Cadasil. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 23, 269–276. doi:10.1177/0891988710383570

CrossRef Full Text

Hessel, A., Schumacher, J., Geyer, M., and Brähler, E. (2001). Symptom-checkliste SCL-90-R: testtheoretische überprüfung und normierung an einer bevölkerungsrepräsentativen stichprobe [symptom-check list SCL-90-R: test theoretical screening and standardization using a population representative sample]. Diagnostica 47, 27–39. doi:10.1026//0012-1924.47.1.27

CrossRef Full Text

Hildenbrand, P., Craven, D. E., Jones, R., and Nemeskal, P. (2009). Lyme neuroborreliosis: manifestations of a rapidly emerging zoonosis. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 30, 1079–1087. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A1579

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacqmin, M., Herve, D., Viswanathan, A., Guichard, J. P., During, M., Dichgans, M., et al. (2010). Confluent thalamic hyperintensities in CADASIL. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 30, 308–313. doi:10.1159/000319607

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jahn, H. (2013). Memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15, 445–454.

Jokeit, H., Seitz, R., Markowitsch, H. J., Neumann, N., Ebner, A., and Witte, O. W. (1997). Prefrontal asymmetric interictal glucose hypometabolism and cognitive impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 120, 2283–2294. doi:10.1093/brain/120.12.2283

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joutel, A., Corpechot, C., Ducros, A., Vahedi, K., Chabriat, H., Mouton, P., et al. (1996). NOTCH3 mutations in CADASIL, a hereditary adult-onset condition causing stroke and dementia. Nature 383, 707–710. doi:10.1038/383707a0

CrossRef Full Text

Joutel, A., Dodick, D. D., Parisi, J. E., Cecillon, M., Tournier-Lasserve, E., and Bousser, M. G. (2000). De novo mutation in the NOTCH3 gene causing CADASIL. Ann. Neurol. 47, 388–391. doi:10.1002/1531-8249(200003)47:3<388::AID-ANA19>3.3.CO;2-H

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.

Kemp, J., Berthel, M. C., Dufour, A., Despres, O., Henry, A., Namer, I. J., et al. (2013). Caudate nucleus and social cognition: neuropsychological and SPECT evidence from a patient with focal caudate lesion. Cortex 49, 559–571. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.01.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kessler, J., Markowitsch, H. J., and Denzler, P. (1990). Der Mini-Mental-Status Test [The Mini-Mental-Status-Examination Test]. Weinheim: Beltz-Test-Verlag.

Klein, S. B., and Gangi, C. E. (2010). The multiplicity of self: neuropsychological evidence and its implications for the self as a construct in psychological research. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 1–15. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05441.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., and Baddeley, A. (1990). The Autobiographical Memory Interview. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.

Kroll, N., Markowitsch, H. J., Knight, R., and von Cramon, D. Y. (1997). Retrieval of old memories – the temporo-frontal hypothesis. Brain 120, 1377–1399. doi:10.1093/brain/120.8.1377

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lehrl, S. (2005). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest (MWT-B) [Multiple Choice Lexis Intelligence Test]. Balingen: Spitta Verlag.

Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

MacArthur, D. G., Manolio, T. A., Dimmock, D. P., Rehm, H. L., Shendure, J., Abecasis, G. R., et al. (2014). Guidelines for investigating causality of sequence variants in human disease. Nature 508, 469–476. doi:10.1038/nature13127

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Manard, M., Carabin, D., Jaspar, M., and Collette, F. (2014). Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed. BMC Neurosci. 15:7. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-15-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markowitsch, H. J. (1991). “Memory disorders after diencephalic damage: heterogeneity of findings,” in Memory Mechanisms: A Tribute to G. V. Goddard, eds W. C. Abraham, M. C. Corballis, and K. G. White (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 175–194.

Markowitsch, H. J., and Härting, C. (1996). Interdependence of priming performance and brain-damage. Int. J. Neurosci. 85, 291–300. doi:10.3109/00207459608986690

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markowitsch, H. J., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., von Stockhausen, H.-M., Ghaemi, M., and Heiss, W.-D. (1999). Short-term memory deficit after focal parietal damage. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 21, 784–796. doi:10.1076/jcen.21.6.784.853

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markowitsch, H. J., and Staniloiu, A. (2011). Amygdala in action: relaying biological and social significance to autobiographic memory. Neuropsychologia 49, 718–733. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markowitsch, H. J., and Staniloiu, S. (2012). Amnesic disorders. Lancet 380, 1429–1440. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61304-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuda, O., and Saito, M. (1998). Crystallized and fluid intelligence in elderly patients with mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int. Psychoger. 10, 147–154. doi:10.1017/S1041610298005250

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mesulam, M.-M. (1990). Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann. Neurol. 28, 597–613. doi:10.1002/ana.410280502

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Metzinger, T. (2008). Empirical perspectives from the self-model theory of subjectivity: a brief summary with examples. Prog. Brain Res. 168, 215–245. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68018-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morris, J. C., Mohs, R. C., Rogers, H., Fillenbaum, G., and Heyman, A. (1988). Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 24, 641–652.

Mykkanen, K., Junna, M., Amberla, K., Bronge, L., Kaariainen, H., Poyhonen, M., et al. (2009). Different clinical phenotypes in monozygotic CADASIL twins with a novel NOTCH3 mutation. Stroke 40, 2215–2218. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.528661

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mykkanen, K., Savontaus, M. L., Juvonen, V., Sistonen, P., Tuisku, S., Tuominen, S., et al. (2004). Detection of the founder effect in Finnish CADASIL families. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 12, 813–819. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201221

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Naylor, E., and Clare, L. (2008). Awareness of memory functioning, autobiographical memory and identity in early-stage dementia. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 18, 590–606. doi:10.1080/09602010701608681

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 12, 313–324. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(76)80035-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.

Nyberg, L., Kim, A. S., Habib, R., Levine, B., and Tulving, E. (2010). Consciousness of subjective time in the brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 22356–22359. doi:10.1073/pnas.1016823108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ogden, J. A. (1993). Visual object agnosia, prosopagnosia, achromatopsia, loss of visual imagery, and autobiographical amnesia following recovery from cortical blindness: case M.H. Neuropsychologia 31, 571–589. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90053-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure complex: contribution à létude de la perception et de la mémoire [test of copying a complex figure: contribution to the study of perception and memory]. Arch. Psychol. 30, 286–356.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

O’Sullivan, M., Ngo, E., Viswanathan, A., Jouvent, E., Gschwendtner, A., Saemann, P. G., et al. (2009). Hippocampal volume is an independent predictor of cognitive performance in CADASIL. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 890–897. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.09.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O’Sullivan, M., Singhal, S., Charlton, R., and Markus, H. S. (2004). Diffusion tensor imaging of thalamus correlates with cognition in CADASIL without dementia. Neurology 62, 702–707. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000113760.72706.D2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parisi, V., Pierelli, F., Fattapposta, F., Bianco, F., Parisi, L., Restuccia, R., et al. (2003). Early visual function impairment in CADASIL. Neurology 60, 2008–2010. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000070411.13217.7E

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parisi, V., Pierelli, F., Malandrini, A., Carrera, P., Olzi, D., Gregori, D., et al. (2000). Visual electrophysiological responses in subjects with cerebral autosomal arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL). Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1582–1588. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00366-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parkin, A. J., and Java, R. I. (1999). Deterioration of frontal lobe function in normal aging: influences of fluid intelligence versus perceptual speed. Neuropsychology 13, 539–545. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.539

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perneczky, R., Pohl, C., Bornschein, S., Forstl, H., Kurz, A., and Diehl-Schmid, J. (2009). Accelerated clinical decline in well-educated patients with frontotemporal lobar degenerations. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 259, 362–367. doi:10.1007/s00406-009-0004-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peters, N., Opherk, C., Danek, A., Ballard, C., Herzog, J., and Dichgans, M. (2005). The pattern of cognitive performance in CADASIL: a monogenic condition leading to subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 2078–2085. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2078

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reinhold, N., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2007). Emotion and consciousness in adolescent psychogenic amnesia. J. Neuropsychol. 1, 53–64. doi:10.1348/174866407X180819

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reitan, R. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indication of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi:10.2466/PMS.8.7.271-276

CrossRef Full Text

Rieger, B., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1996). Implicit and explicit mnestic performance of patients with prefrontal, medial temporal, and basal ganglia damage. Neurol. Psychiatr. Brain Res. 4, 53–74.

Roos, K. L., and Berger, J. R. (2007). Is the presence of antibodies in CSF sufficient to make a definitive diagnosis of Lyme disease? Neurology 69, 949–950. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000271901.94853.24

CrossRef Full Text

Rufa, A., Malandrini, A., Dotti, M. T., Berti, G., Salvadori, C., and Federico, A. (2005). Typical pathological changes of CADASIL in the optic nerve. Neurol. Sci. 26, 271–274. doi:10.1007/s10072-005-0470-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Russell, M. B. (2010). Genetics of dementia. Acta Neurol. Scand. 190, 58–61. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0404.2010.01377.x

CrossRef Full Text

Sabbadini, G., Francia, A., Calandriello, L., Di Biasi, C., Trasimeni, G., Gualdi, G. F., et al. (1995). Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leucoencephalopathy (CADASIL). Clinical, Neuroimaging, pathological and genetic study of a large Italian family. Brain 118, 207–215. doi:10.1093/brain/118.1.207

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Savage, C. R., Baer, L., Keuthen, N. J., Brown, H. D., Rauch, S. L., and Jenike, M. A. (1999). Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 905–916. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00278-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scarmeas, N., Albert, S. M., Manly, J. J., and Stern, Y. (2006). Education and rates of cognitive decline in incident Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 77, 308–316. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.072306

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmidtke, K., Manner, H., Kaufmann, R., and Schmolck, H. (2002). Cognitive procedural learning in patients with fronto-striatal lesions. Learn. Mem. 9, 419–429. doi:10.1101/lm.47202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seidl, U., Markowitsch, H. J., and Schröder, J. (2006). “Die verlorene Erinnerung. Störungen des autobiographischen Gedächtnisses bei leichter kognitiver Beeinträchtigung und Alzheimer-Demenz,” in Warum Menschen sich erinnern können. Fortschritte in der Interdisziplinären Gedächtnisforschung [Why Human Beings can Remember. Progress in Interdisciplinary Memory Research], eds H. Welzer and H. J. Markowitsch (Stuttgart: Klett), 286–302.

Shadick, N. A., Phillips, C. B., Sangha, O., Logigian, E. L., Kaplan, R. F., Wright, E. A., et al. (1999). Musculoskeletal and neurologic outcomes in patients with previously treated Lyme disease. Ann. Intern. Med. 131, 919–926. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-131-12-199912210-00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, G. E., Pankratz, V. S., Negash, S., Machulda, M. M., Petersen, R. C., Boeve, B. F., et al. (2007). A plateau in pre-Alzheimer memory decline: evidence for compensatory mechanisms? Neurology 69, 133–139. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000265594.23511.16

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, S. J., Souchay, C., and Conway, M. A. (2010). Overgeneral autobiographical memory in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 46, 787–793. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Souchay, C., Guillery-Girard, B., Pauly-Takacs, K., Wojcik, D. Z., and Eustache, F. (2013). Subjective experience of episodic memory and metacognition: a neurodevelopmental approach. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:212. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00212

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Souchay, C., and Smith, S. J. (2013). Autobiographical memory in Parkinson’s disease: a retrieval deficit. J. Neuropsychol. 7, 164–178. doi:10.1111/jnp.12014

CrossRef Full Text

Spitz, H. H., Webster, N. A., and Borys, S. V. (1982). Further studies of the Tower of Hanoi problem-solving performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children. Dev. Psychol. 18, 922–930. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.922

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 2nd Edn. New York: Oxford University Press.

Stanek, G., Wormser, G. P., Gray, J., and Strle, F. (2012). Lyme borreliosis. Lancet 379, 461–473. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60103-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Staniloiu, A., Borsutzky, S., Woermann, F., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2013). Social cognition in a case of amnesia with neurodevelopmental mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 4:342. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00342

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Staniloiu, A., and Markowitsch, H. J. (2012). “The splitting of the brain: a reorientation towards fiber tracts damage in amnesia,” in Brain Damage: Causes, Management, and Prognosis, eds A. J. Schäfer and J. Müller (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 41–70.

Steere, A. C., Sikand, V. K., Schoen, R. T., and Nowakowski, J. (2003). Asymptomatic infection with Borrelia burgdorferi. Clin. Infect. Dis. 37, 528–532. doi:10.1086/376914

CrossRef Full Text

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813248

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stone, J., Smyth, R., Carson, A., Warlow, C., and Sharpe, M. (2006). La belle indifférence in conversion symptoms and hysteria: systematic review. Br. J. Psychiatry 188, 204–209. doi:10.1192/bjp.188.3.204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teichner, G., and Wagner, M. T. (2004). The test of memory malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 455–464. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00078-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). New York: Multi Health Systems.

Tournier-Lasserve, E., Iba-Zizen, M. T., Romero, N., and Bousser, M. G. (1991). Autosomal dominant syndrome with strokelike episodes and leukoencephalopathy. Stroke 22, 1297–1302. doi:10.1161/01.STR.22.10.1297

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tournier-Lasserve, E., Joutel, A., Melki, J., Weissenbach, J., Lathrop, G. M., Chabriat, H., et al. (1993). Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy maps to chromosome 19q12. Nat. Genet. 3, 256–259. doi:10.1038/ng0393-256

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tulving, E. (1972). “Episodic and semantic memory,” in Organization of Memory, eds E. Tulving and W. Donaldson (New York: Academic Press), 381–403.

Tulving, E. (2005). “Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human?,” in The Missing Link in Cognition: Evolution of Self-Knowing Consciousness, eds H. Terrace and J. Metcalfe (New York: Oxford University Press), 3–56.

Valenti, R., Salvadori, E., Pescini, F., Poggesi, A., Castellini, G., Antonini, S., et al. (2013). Facial affect recognition in CADASIL patients. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 28, 65–71. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs097

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Bogaert, L. (1955). Encephalopathie sous-corticale progressive (Binswanger) a evolution rapide chez deux soeurs. Acta Med. Hell. 24, 961–972.

Van der Aa, N., Vandeweyer, G., and Kooy, R. F. (2010). A boy with mental retardation, obesity and hypertrichosis caused by a microdeletion of 19p13.12. Eur. J. Med. Genet. 53, 291–293. doi:10.1016/j.ejmg.2010.05.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 1271–1278. doi:10.1038/nn1341

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wagner, A. D., Shannon, B. J., Kahn, I., and Buckner, R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 445–453. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, R. S., Barnes, L. L., Aggarwal, N. T., Boyle, P. A., Hebert, L. E., Mendes de Leon, C. F., et al. (2010). Cognitive activity and the cognitive morbidity of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 75, 990–995. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f25b5e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yamamoto, Y., Craggs, I., Baumann, M., Kalimo, H., and Kalaria, R. N. (2011). Molecular genetics and pathology of hereditary small vessel diseases of the brain. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 17, 94–113. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2990.2010.01147.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeman, A., McLean, B., Keir, G., Luxton, R., Sharief, M., and Thompson, E. (1993). The significance of serum oligoclonal bands in neurological diseases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 56, 32–35. doi:10.1136/jnnp.56.1.32

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zieren, N., Duering, M., Peters, N., Reyes, S., Jouvent, E., Herve, D., et al. (2013). Education modifies the relation of vascular pathology to cognitive function: cognitive reserve in cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 400–407. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.04.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: chromosome 19, gene mutation, episodic memory, problem solving, cognitive flexibility, social information processing

Citation: Staniloiu A, Woermann FG and Markowitsch HJ (2014) Impairments in episodic-autobiographical memory and emotional and social information processing in CADASIL during mid-adulthood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:227. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00227

Received: 27 February 2014; Accepted: 05 June 2014;
Published online: 25 June 2014.

Edited by:

Ekrem Dere, University Pierre and Marie Curie Paris 6, France

Reviewed by:

Boris Suchan, Ruhr University Bochum, Germany
Ben Schmand, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright: © 2014 Staniloiu, Woermann and Markowitsch. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Angelica Staniloiu, Physiological Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Universitaetsstrasse 25, Bielefeld D-33615, Germany e-mail: astaniloiu@uni-bielefeld.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.