Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 05 August 2010
Sec. Learning and Memory
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroethology View all 23 articles

Probing the natural scene by echolocation in bats

  • 1 Department of Psychology and Institute for Systems Research, Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
  • 2 Institute of Biology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark

Bats echolocating in the natural environment face the formidable task of sorting signals from multiple auditory objects, echoes from obstacles, prey, and the calls of conspecifics. Successful orientation in a complex environment depends on auditory information processing, along with adaptive vocal-motor behaviors and flight path control, which draw upon 3-D spatial perception, attention, and memory. This article reviews field and laboratory studies that document adaptive sonar behaviors of echolocating bats, and point to the fundamental signal parameters they use to track and sort auditory objects in a dynamic environment. We suggest that adaptive sonar behavior provides a window to bats’ perception of complex auditory scenes.

Introduction

Scene Analysis: Overview and Background

The sensory world of an animal is noisy, complex, and dynamic. From a barrage of stimuli, an animal must detect, sort, group, and track biologically relevant signals. Parsing, integrating, and organizing complex sensory stimuli to support species-specific survival behaviors are tasks of perception, which invoke higher level processes of scene analysis, both within and across modalities.

The perceptual organization of sound, commonly referred to as auditory scene analysis, has received a great deal of research attention in the human literature over the past 20 years (Bregman, 1990), and represents an important advance in our understanding of auditory perception. In human psychoacoustic research, this problem has been successfully studied in laboratory experiments with simplified acoustic stimuli, but there remains the challenge, in humans and other animals, to understand perception of complex sounds in the natural environment.

Auditory scene analysis is a fundamental problem faced by all organisms that use acoustic signals for social communication, territorial defense, navigation or predator evasion. To understand the processes that support the analysis of natural scenes is further complicated by the fact that an animal’s perception of stimuli may depend upon its behavioral goal, biological state, and environmental context. Here, we consider the problem of natural scene analysis by turning to the echolocating bat, an animal that can negotiate a complex auditory world in complete darkness (Griffin, 1958). For the echolocating bat, the analysis of auditory scenes builds upon its active production of sounds that reflect from objects in the environment (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). The bat adaptively adjusts the features of its sonar vocalizations in response to information obtained from echo returns; and therefore, the bat’s sonar vocalizations provide a window into its perceptual world. Specifically, the directional aim, timing, frequency content, intensity, and duration of sonar signals used by a bat to “illuminate” the environment have a direct impact on the information available to its acoustic imaging system. In turn, the bat’s perception of the echo scene guides its adjustments of the features of subsequent sonar vocalizations. Therefore, the bat’s adaptive sonar behavior can shed light on the fundamental processes that underlie auditory scene analysis by echolocation. Indeed, the bat’s active sonar behavior allows us to listen in on the signals that are used to perform a variety of auditory tasks. Here we review laboratory and field studies that point to the features echolocating bats use to represent information about a complex acoustic environment and the general importance of an animal’s actions to its perception of natural scenes.

The Echolocating Bat as a Model for Scene Analysis

Diversity of Echolocating Bats

Bats are the most ecologically diverse group of mammals, with more than 1100 extant species of which around 950 echolocate (Simmons et al., 2008). Each species of bat has a distinct repertoire of signals that it uses for echolocation, and the features of these sounds determine the acoustic information available to its sonar imaging system. Bat sonar signals fall broadly into two categories, constant frequency (CF) used by some specialized species and frequency modulated (FM) signals employed by the majority of bats studied so far. Bats using CF signals typically forage in dense foliage, and some of these species lower the frequency of their sonar vocalizations as they fly to compensate for Doppler shifts in returning echoes (Schnitzler, 1968), stabilizing echo returns around a reference frequency and isolating Doppler shifts in echoes that come from fluttering insect prey (Schnitzler et al., 1983). FM-bats hunting in the open generally produce signals of narrow bandwidth, whereas FM-bats foraging closer to vegetation, typically produce shorter, broadband signals (Schnitzler and Kalko, 1998), which are well suited for 3-D target localization (Simmons, 1973; Moss and Schnitzler, 1995) and for separating figure and ground (Moss and Surlykke, 2001; Moss et al., 2006). Work is in progress to develop telemetry microphones small enough for bats to carry in natural flight (see e.g. Hiryu et al., 2005), but to date there are not any recordings of the sonar calls and echoes from the field, which would directly demonstrate the complexity of a bat’s auditory scene. However, behavioral observations and echolocation call design in different contexts and habitats give indirect evidence of the challenges bats encounter and the acoustic tools they use to deal with a broad range of perceptual tasks.

Hearing and Spatial Localization by Bats

The echolocating bat’s auditory system receives and processes sonar calls and echoes from its environment for the task of spatial orientation, but it is essentially a “standard mammalian auditory system” (Suga, 1988, 1990; Covey and Casseday, 1999). The hearing range of bats extends from around 1 kHz to well above 100 kHz or even much higher, depending on the species (Moss and Schnitzler, 1995). Frequency sensitivity and resolution are extremely high in some CF-bats (Long and Schnitzler, 1975) and these auditory functions are supported by mechanical specializations of the basilar membrane and large populations of neurons tuned to sounds around the echo reference frequency of their biosonar (Bruns, 1980; Neuweiler et al., 1980). By contrast, auditory behavior, basilar membrane mechanics, and frequency tuning of neurons in the central auditory systems of FM bats show patterns similar to other mammals (Kössl and Vater, 1995; Moss and Schnitzler, 1995).

Many of the same cues used by other animals to localize sound and to process complex patterns of acoustic information are exploited by bats for spatial orientation and perception by sonar. Binaural cues for sound localization are used to estimate the azimuthal position of an acoustic target (Popper and Fay, 1995; Blauert, 1996). Monaural cues are also important for assigning a location in azimuth, but are considered essential for determining the elevation of a sound in space (Batteau, 1967; Wightman and Kistler, 1989, 1997). The bat’s pinna-tragus system produces changes in the spectrum of incoming echoes, creating patterns of interference that are used by the bat to estimate target elevation (Wotton et al., 1996). Inter-aural spectral cues produced by the directionality of the two-ear system may provide additional information for determining target angle in the vertical plane (Grinnell and Grinnell, 1965; Aytekin et al., 2004). The bat estimates the third spatial dimension, target range, from the time delay between the outgoing vocalization and returning echo (Simmons, 1973), and FM-bats show extraordinary spatial selectivity along the range axis (Simmons, 1979; Menne et al., 1989; Moss and Schnitzler, 1989, 1995; Simmons et al., 1990; Surlykke, 1992).

Sonar Behavior for Insect Capture

As an insectivorous bat flies toward its prey, the features of its sonar vocalizations change. Sonar emission patterns have been used to divide the bat’s foraging sequence into different phases: search, approach, and terminal buzz, typical for all aerial insectivores as well as for trawling species hunting for prey on or just above water surfaces (Griffin et al., 1960; Simmons et al., 1979). In general, search phase signals of aerial insectivorous bats are comparatively long in duration and narrow in bandwidth (Schnitzler and Kalko, 1998). Search signals are well suited for target detection, with energy concentrated in a restricted frequency band over an extended time window. In the big brown bat, for example, the repetition rate of search signals is low (5–10 Hz), call duration long (up to 15–20 ms), and frequency modulation shallow (sweeping from about 26 to 24 kHz). Once the bat detects and selects a prey item, it produces approach phase signals at a higher repetition rate (20–80 Hz) with steep FM (fundamental sweeping from about 65–25 kHz) and reduced duration (2–5 ms). In the final phase of capture, terminal buzz signals shorten further (0.5–1 ms) and are produced at a very high repetition rate (up to around 170 calls per second) (Surlykke and Moss, 2000). The broadband approach and terminal phase signals are adapted for target localization in azimuth, elevation, and range (Moss and Schnitzler, 1995).

The sound production pattern of a foraging bat is illustrated in Figure 1. The calls characteristic of search, approach, and terminal buzz phases of insect pursuit are more than a stereotyped sequence of sonar signals. They form part of a complex set of adaptive behaviors to changing acoustic information (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). The bat’s active control over the timing, duration, intensity, and bandwidth of its outgoing sonar transmissions allows this animal to regulate the flow of acoustic information it uses to perceive the auditory objects that comprise its dynamic environment.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. When an insectivorous bat pursues an insect it changes the calls in a typical sequence of search, approach and terminal buzz phase calls, where duration gradually decreases, while band width and repetition rate increase. A shows a series of cartoon images, depicting a bat pursuing an insect. The sonar beam pattern is aimed in different directions during the search phase. Towards the end of the approach phase and through the terminal buzz, the sonar beam is directed at the prey. B shows spectrograms of typical FM echolocation signals produced by the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, at each of the phases of insect pursuit. The lower panels illustrate that a bat hunting insects out in the open sky, where all other objects are far away, will often encounter fairly “clean” echo returns from isolated objects, whereas a bat hunting close to vegetation will encounter a far more complex auditory scene composed of echoes from many objects arriving at different delays and intervals.

From Simple to Complex Auditory Scenes

Adaptations of call design to a bat’s habitat and hunting strategy point to distinct acoustic features in the animal’s auditory world and the problems its perceptual system must solve. Bats hunting insects out in the open sky, where all other objects are far away, often encounter fairly “clean” echo returns from isolated objects. The acoustic “snapshots” from the open sky are far less complex than those a bat encounters when hunting close to vegetation. Prey capture in mid-air may involves a limited set of cues, primarily echo delay and interaural differences of echo returns (Popper and Fay, 1995; Thomas et al., 2004). In addition to these target location cues, there is probably a more comprehensive set of situation-dependent cues comprising the bat’s auditory space percepts (Müller and Schnitzler, 1999), including distance to ground or bank (Verboom et al., 1999).

At the moment of calling, the bat hears its outgoing sonar vocalization, probably at reduced intensity due to the attenuation caused by the middle ear muscles (Jen and Suga, 1976; Kick and Simmons, 1984). In between calls, there may be environmental sounds (e.g. wind noise or calls of neighboring bats) impinging on the bat’s ears, but primarily there are comparatively long gaps, except when a nearby potential insect prey appears, reflecting an echo back to the bat (Figures 1B,C). The behaviors of aerial hunting bats support the notion of such a simple acoustic situation. Aerial hunting bats will try to capture pebbles or other objects thrown into the air (Griffin, 1958), indicating that they operate by a very simple rule, assigning any object echo to a potential prey item. However, they will not continue reacting to repeated pebble throws, demonstrating that if necessary, bats can make use of their extremely fine echo discrimination ability to differentiate between objects (Griffin et al., 1965; Simmons et al., 1974; Habersetzer and Vogler, 1983). Bats flying closer to vegetation adapt their signals to be shorter and more broadband (Neuweiler, 1989; Schnitzler and Kalko, 1998; Schnitzler et al., 2003). The most cluttered habitat is encountered by bats taking insects right next to or gleaning from vegetation (Figures 1B,D). The bats inhabiting this zone have adapted their calls to be extremely short and broadband (Fenton, 1990; Kingston et al., 1999). The similarity of changes in call structure, i.e. short duration and increase of bandwidth and sweep rate close to clutter, across many species from many families, point to their functional significance in a complicated scene, where short broadband calls probably serve to enhance discrimination of objects against background. Many bats are flexible in the call design (Kalko and Schnitzler 1993; Schnitzler and Kalko, 1998), and for example the genus Tadarida is considered “extremely adaptable” (Simmons et al., 1979), but access to a variety of habitats is not just a question of being able to produce the optimal sonar calls. Foraging in a complex habitat close to vegetation also requires broad wings and slow maneuverable flight, and some bats like Tadarida may be restricted to flying out in open or semi-open space due to their size and long, narrow wings (Fenton, 1990). This emphasizes that active behavior in a natural environment requires a combination of perception and motor skills.

The Auditory Soundscape of an Echolocating Bat

Echolocating bats use sonar to represent the location and features of objects in the natural environment. It has been suggested that bats can discriminate between different tree types based on statistical differences between returning echoes. The conclusion was based on theoretical analysis of information content in returning echoes (Müller and Kuc, 2000; Yovel et al., 2008). Thus, in theory, bats should be able to use sonar to discriminate between different plants, given enough time. However, a bat flying at high speed, with noise from wind, conspecifics, other bats and ultrasonic insects, having to dodge branches and other obstacles while detecting minute insect prey or fruit in between leaves and twigs, may not have the time or attentional resources to do so. Furthermore, natural interactions between bats and plants implies that it is, in fact, quite difficult for bats to discriminate between plants under natural conditions. The bat-pollinated neotropical vine Mucuna holtonii directs its echolocating pollinators, the phyllostomid bat Glossophaga commissarisi, to its virgin flowers by a small concave “mirror” that works like an optical cat’s eye, but in the acoustic domain, reflecting most of the energy of the bats’ echolocation calls back to the bat (Helversen and Helversen, 1999).

Perceptual Organization of Echo Streams in a Dynamic Environment

We hypothesize that the bat’s perceptual system organizes acoustic information from a complex and dynamic environment into echo streams, allowing it to track spatially distributed auditory objects (sonar targets) as it flies. We define an echo stream as a sequence of sonar returns that can be assigned to a distinct source or auditory object. The acoustic parameters that can contribute to perceptual streams are echo direction, duration, intensity, frequency, and timing.

Here we present in Figure 2 a schematic illustration of sonar streams constructed from coherent changes in echo delay, the bat’s cue for target distance. Figure 2 (upper panel) shows a bat in an environment that contains a single prey item and trees at different distances. In this simplified scenario, the insect and trees are located in front of the bat, and the bat receives echoes from these objects at different and changing delays. Each panel represents a new slice in time, separated by a fixed interval. In each panel, the bat’s position relative to the trees and insect changes, as the bat pursues its prey. Below, each horizontal line in the plot corresponds to a sonar vocalization, starting from 1500 ms before capture until time zero (from top to bottom on the left y-axis), when the bat intercepts the prey. The separation between the lines corresponds to the repetition rate of sonar vocalizations produced by the bat at different distances to the prey and obstacles. The resulting streams of echoes at changing delays are shown as open boxes with widths corresponding to sonar call duration and color coding as the insect (black) and trees (red, blue, and green) in the panel above. The signal durations and intervals are based on a pursuit sequence recorded from Eptesicus fuscus in the wild. The right y-axis shows the echolocation phases of insect pursuit from search to terminal buzz phase. As the bat flies closer to the trees and insect, the echo delays shorten. The echo amplitudes are estimated to illustrate relative differences due to changes in distances to the objects and the fact that bats reduce the output intensity as they close in on a target (Hartley and Suthers, 1989; Surlykke and Kalko, 2008). When the bat has passed an object the echo delay increases as the bat’s distance from these objects increases, and the echo amplitude decreases rapidly to reflect the directionality of the sonar call with low intensity radiated in the backward direction. Each of the reflecting objects appears as a distinct ridge with a particular slope, corresponding to the rate of delay change of echoes over time. In this display, one can visually identify and track the returning echoes from the trees and insect over time.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematically illustrates a bat in an environment that contains a single prey item and trees at different distances. Below, each horizontal line in the plot corresponds to a sonar vocalization, starting from 1500 ms before capture until time zero (top to bottom on the left y-axis), when the bat intercepts the prey. The separation between the lines corresponds to sonar call intervals decreasing with decreasing distance to the prey. The resulting streams of echoes at changing delays are shown as open boxes with widths corresponding to call duration and color coding of the insect (black) and trees (red, blue, and green) in the panel above. The signal durations and intervals are based on a pursuit sequence recorded from Eptesicus fuscus in the wild. The acoustic phases of insect pursuit from search to terminal buzz phase are indicated on the right y-axis. As the bat flies closer to the trees and insect, the echo delays shorten. Each of the reflecting objects appears as a distinct ridge with a particular slope, corresponding to the rate of delay change of echoes over time. In this display, one can visually identify and track the returning echoes from the trees and insect over time.

Figure 2 highlights two important aspects of bat echolocation which we hypothesize play an important role in the analysis of natural scenes. (1) Bats actively control the features of their sonar calls that are used to represent the environment. Here we show adjustments in call timing, duration, and amplitude with changing target distance, but other signal parameters are also adaptively adjusted. The active control of sonar vocalizations directly impacts what the bat hears and also suggests how the bat perceptually organizes dynamic acoustic information. (2) The bat may perceptually organize echoes from objects at changing positions into streams, which allow the bat to track moving auditory objects over time. In this example, echo delay streams from the insect become most salient when the bat’s sonar repetition rate is high. To build a representation of echo streams, the bat must integrate echo information over time, and its vocal behavior can directly influence perceptual grouping processes. These notions are elaborated in the sections below.

Adaptive Vocal Control in Echolocation: A Window to the Bat’s Auditory Scene

While research over the last several decades has elucidated the echo features that bats use to localize and discriminate sonar targets, there remains an incomplete understanding of the larger problem of auditory scene analysis, namely, how echo features are perceptually organized into representing the natural scene. We propose that the bat’s adaptive echolocation behavior can shed light on this problem.

Below, we consider field and laboratory data on echolocation behavior, showing that bats adaptively control echo direction, delay, frequency, and timing, which the animal can then use to represent a complex auditory scene. These adaptive vocal-motor behaviors give rise to distinct echo features that the bat can use to segregate and group auditory objects into echo streams. Echo streams present a natural dimension for the bat to organize acoustic information in a dynamic and complex acoustic environment. The bat’s adaptive sonar signal design provides a window to the perceptual representations it builds of the environment.

Direction: Bats control the aim of sonar calls to sequentially sample objects at different locations

The bat’s sonar beam can be likened to an auditory flashlight. Beam width constitutes a spatial filter, determining which limited region of space is sampled at a given point in time. The width of the sonar beam constrains the bat’s “field of view.” Different lines of data demonstrate that beam width is under the bat’s dynamic control. Myotis daubentonii emits calls of higher directionality in the wild compared to those produced in the lab. At peak power frequency, 55 kHz, the half amplitude angle measured in the lab was 40°, but only 20° in the wild, where the “sonar view” is narrower, but with longer range due to the higher on-axis intensity, thus sampling further ahead but not as far to the side as in the lab. Beam width may be controlled by adjusting mouth aperture, such that opening the mouth wider results in a narrower beam (Surlykke et al., 2009b). Adjusting call frequency is another way of controlling beam width. Thus, by lowering the frequency by almost an octave, vespertilionid bats increase beam width dramatically in the last phases of the pursuit (Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010). Face morphology also affects beam width and shape. Bats from the families Rhinolophidae and Phyllostomidae emit their calls through the two nostrils. Mouth-emitting bats have simple faces resembling those of other mammals, but nose-emitting bats are characterized by a nose-leaf, a fleshy structure around and above the nostrils. The association between nose-emission and nose-leaf suggests a role for the nose-leaf in sonar beam shaping. Nose-leaf morphology can be quite complicated, but in phyllostomid bats from the New World tropics, with relatively simple lancet-shaped nose-leaves, this fleshy structure around the nostrils seems to restrict the beam mainly in the vertical direction (Vanderelst et al., 2010).

By directing the beam axis towards specific objects, the bat influences which echo information it samples from the environment. Phyllostomid bats may move the nose-leaf to steer the beam independent of head aim to selectively sample echoes from certain objects (Weinbeer and Kalko, 2007). Vespertilionid bats and other mouth emitters control beam direction and thus the region of space they inspect by moving the head. As mentioned earlier, CF bats employ a different strategy to sort echoes from vegetation and insect prey, by listening for Doppler shifts introduced by prey wing movements (Schnitzler and Flieger, 1983; von der Emde and Schnitzler, 1986, 1990; von der Emde and Menne, 1989). These examples serve to illustrate the enormous diversity of bats and indicate that there may be more than one echolocation solution to the perceptual challenges that arise in a complex acoustic scene. The echolocating bat’s sonar adaptations to different habitat and foraging requirements in the wild present a window to the animal’s active analysis of acoustic information in a natural environment and can be used to motivate carefully designed laboratory studies.

Laboratory studies of sonar emission patterns of the big brown bat, E. fuscus, show that sonar beams are broad enough to collect echo information from objects within a 60–90° cone (Hartley and Suthers, 1989), which could enable simultaneous inspection of several objects in the frontal plane (Ghose and Moss, 2003). However, the results of a recent study clearly demonstrate for the first time that bats encountering a complex environment shift the directional aim of their sonar beam to accurately and sequentially point the central axis of the sonar beam in the direction of closely spaced objects (Surlykke et al., 2009a). This was determined by taking measurements of the directional aim of the bat’s sonar beam as it performed in a dual task, obstacle avoidance and insect capture. Bats were trained to fly through one of two openings in a fine net. Behind only one of the openings a tethered insect was presented, at variable distances behind the net (see Figure 3A,B). A microphone array was used in this study to reconstruct the sonar beam pattern as the bat inspected the net obstacle and the prey (Figures 3A,C). Changes in the directional aim (acoustic gaze) of sonar calls showed that the bat first inspected the obstacle, and then shifted its gaze to the more distant prey, before negotiating its way past the obstacle. The bat sequentially shifted the axis of its sonar beam to different objects with an accuracy of ∼5° (see Figure 3E). These findings provide indicators that the bat negotiates a complex acoustic environment by shifting its attention (acoustic gaze) sequentially to closely spaced objects. Doppler shift compensation measured in a CF bat also suggest shifts in attention to different walls in a flight room (Hiryu et al., 2005).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Contains schematics illustrating the experimental setup and recording methods in the obstacle avoidance/insect capture task (top). (A) The bat was trained to fly through one of two openings in a net that was stretched across the room (see top left diagram). Behind one of the openings, a food reward (tethered mealworm) was presented. Two high-speed IR sensitive video cameras recorded the positions of the obstacles, prey and the bat’s flight behavior, which were later used for off-line analyses. Microphones on the floor recorded the full bandwidth of the bat’s sonar signals. A microphone array, positioned along three walls, was used to reconstruct the bat’s sonar beam pattern. (B), above, show the bat’s 3D flight path through the net and to intercept the tethered insect, and, below, the corresponding changes in the duration and interval between successive sonar calls. Zero on the x-axes of these plots marks the time when the bat passes through the net opening. More details on the sonar beam reconstruction are presented in the top right panel (C), which shows an overhead view of the flight room and microphone array. The beam pattern is reconstructed from the relative energy of signals recorded at each of the microphones along the three walls. (D) Schematic illustrates how changes in the bat’s call duration determine whether its signals overlap with echoes from objects in the environment. (E) Plots show data collected from a trial in which the bat approaches the net opening on the left. The left plot shows the beam pattern for selected vocalizations as the bat approaches the net. The right plot displays the beam axis for each vocalization, and they are color coded according to the directional aim of the sonar beam as the bat begins its approach to the net, blue for the right edge of the net opening, green for the left edge of the net opening and red for the tethered worm. Note the bat’s sequential scanning of the closely spaced objects in this trial. Adapted from Surlykke et al. (2009a).

Visual animals may engage similar behavioral strategies by moving their eyes to sequentially sample closely spaced objects in a scene (e.g. Kano and Tomonaga, 2009). We therefore propose that the components of scene analysis detailed here for the echolocating bat apply more generally to the analysis of natural scenes in a broad range of animals that rely on different sensory modalities.

Target distance: Bats control call intensity and duration to sample objects along the range axis

Bat sonar operates in three-dimensional space, and the bat’s echolocation behavior influences the information it gathers along the range axis. In particular, the bat makes adjustments in the intensity and duration of calls to sample echoes from targets at different distances, i.e. higher intensity and longer duration for more distant objects.

Call intensity

Emitted intensity directly impacts the sonar operating range. The operating range of echolocation varies with species and habitat, and refers to the distance over which echoes return to the bat’s ears at levels that are above the animal’s detection threshold. Emitted intensity directly influences a bat’s operating range, since adequate sound energy must impinge upon objects to return an audible echo. Bats that fly high in open space may be able to detect echoes from the ground many tens of meters away, due to the large reflection surface of the ground, which is not a point target (Jung et al., 2007). Ultrasound attenuates rapidly with distance (Lawrence and Simmons, 1982), and to collect audible echoes from small insects, some bats produce echolocation calls with source levels (at 0.1 m from the mouth) up to ca. 140 dB SPL (Surlykke and Kalko, 2008), a finding that demonstrates call intensities emitted in the wild are much greater than previously believed (Griffin, 1958). Furthermore, bat sonar call intensities depend on habitat, with the highest intensities being emitted by bats hunting in uncluttered space (Holderied et al., 2005; Surlykke and Kalko, 2008) whereas bats hunting closer to clutter produce lower intensities (Kingston et al., 2003; Brinkløv et al., 2010). The lowering of emitted intensity in the lab parallels other acoustic changes in sonar signals, for example, shorter calls with broader bandwidths (Surlykke and Moss, 2000), indicating that a collection of call parameter adjustments aid sonar operation in more cluttered environments (Brinkløv et al., 2010). Clutter from more distant objects is reduced by the attenuation of call intensity, and echo returns becomes less noisy (Johnson et al., 2008).

Call duration and interval

The duration and timing of the sonar calls are also tied to sonar range. Holderied et al. (2005) showed that call duration increased with emitted intensity in E. bottae, an open air forager, and they estimated a maximum detection range of 21 m for large objects. Thus, the longer the range, the more intense and the longer the call. The same relation between duration and intensity was shown for E. fuscus in the lab (Møhl and Surlykke, 1989). In general, bats emitting FM signals avoid overlap between their outgoing sounds and the returning echoes (Figure 3D), and they wait until the echo has been received before producing the next sonar call, hence “placing” the echo in a window between the end of one pulse and the beginning of the next. Holderied and von Helversen (2003) report that the interval between successive sonar calls fits an estimate of maximum detection range for 11 European bat species, and they suggest that this match ensures receipt of a prey echo before producing the next call. The same timing control of signals is observed in echolocating dolphins (Au, 1993), indicating that this is a general strategy for echolocating animals to avoid call-echo assignment confusions.

As a bat inspects an object, it adjusts the duration of its call to avoid overlap between sonar pulses and echo returns from the target of interest (see Figure 3D). Such vocal-motor adjustments in call duration provide an indirect measure of where the bat is attending along the distance axis. This is illustrated in data from laboratory studies. For example, in a target discrimination task, the big brown bat adjusted the duration of calls to the distance of the object it was inspecting (Falk et al., 2010). The bat was trained in this study to discriminate between two small (16 mm diameter) spheres, one smooth (S+) and the other textured (S−). The bat received a food reward for tapping S+ as it flew by. When the bat flew towards the S-sphere, it initially decreased the duration of its calls to avoid overlap with echoes from the object. However, before flying past this non-rewarded target, the bat increased the duration of its calls, experiencing pulse-echo overlap, suggesting that it had shifted its attention to a more distant rewarded target (S+). The data from this study indicate that the bat adjusted its calls to sample echo information from different objects at changing distances. Similarly, in the obstacle avoidance/insect capture task described above, the bat made adjustments in call duration as it approached the net, shortening its calls to avoid pulse-net echo overlap (Surlykke et al., 2009a). When the bat was close to the net and had planned its path through the opening, but before flying through, it increased the duration of its calls, tolerating pulse-net echo overlap. Directional aim of the sonar beam provided an independent measure of where the bat was focusing its sound and thus confirmed that call duration, was adjusted to the more distant worm (Figure 3E). Data from these studies demonstrate that the big brown bat makes vocal-motor adjustments to shift its acoustic gaze to sequentially sample different objects along the range axis.

Shortening a broadband signal is typical of bats negotiating more cluttered environments. A broadband, brief signal allows for more precise time determination of echo return (Surlykke, 1992, Figure 4), which makes it easier to discriminate between prey and background. Field results supporting this notion showed, based on duration, frequency, and bandwidth, that the clutter-adapted M. septentrionalis has a shorter maximum sonar operating range than M. lucifugus, a species known to forage in a variety of habitats but mainly in uncluttered areas (Broders et al., 2004). Myotis nattereri emits calls of extremely broad bandwidth, with a downward FM-sweep from 130 to 30 kHz, i.e. spanning two octaves, and is able to forage within a few cm of clutter (Ratcliffe and Dawson, 2003; Siemers and Schnitzler, 2004).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Shows the ranging accuracy for approach signals and search type signals in big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus. Two bats were trained in a psychophysical experiment, using a Yes/No procedure in a phantom target simulator. When the echo was a short broadband approach signal the bats could determine echo delay with around 85 μs accuracy, corresponding to a range difference of 1.5 cm, whereas long narrow band search type signals as echoes resulted in a ranging accuracy 10 times as long, more than 800 μs or 15 cm.

In this section, we have provided several examples of sonar call duration and intensity adjustments to objects at different distances, which contribute to the bat’s perception of the natural scene. Auditory scene analysis invokes shifts in attention to object features and locations, and discretely sampled information must be ultimately integrated over time and space (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). The data summarized here show that the bat adapts the amplitude and temporal features of its calls to inspect objects at different distances, and serve to illustrate how this animal’s actions play directly into its representation of a complex environment.

Frequency: Bats control call frequency to sort echoes in a complex environment

When bats forage in cluttered environments, where a single sonar call results in a cascade of sonar echoes, the animal may experience ambiguity about the delays of echoes associated with a given sonar call. As noted above, bats typically adjust the intervals between successive calls to avoid such ambiguity, waiting for echo returns before producing the next sonar call. However, echolocating bats sometimes encounter complex environments that prevent sorting of calls and echoes by pulse interval (PI) adjustments alone. Hiryu et al. (2010) discovered that big brown bats, E. fuscus, flying through an array of echo reflecting obstacles made frequency adjustments between alternating sonar calls, presumably to tag time dispersed echoes with a given sonar call by using echo frequency information. This result suggests that bats may treat the cascade of echoes following each sonar vocalization as representing one complete view of the auditory scene. If the integrity of one view of the acoustic scene is compromised by overlap of one echo cascade with the next, the bat changes its call frequencies to create the conditions for segregating echoes associated with a given sonar vocalization, thus providing strong evidence for the bat’s use of frequency cues to sort information about a complex acoustic scene.

When bats forage together in groups, they face additional challenges, namely to sort echoes from their own signals from the signals and echoes of neighboring bats. The call design within a species is much more similar than across species, which would seem to create severe problems for acoustic orientation and prey detection in proximity of conspecifics. However, field data show that a number of species, e.g. Pipistrellus pygmaeus, M. daubentonii, Rhinopoma hardwickei (Habersetzer, 1981) Tadarida brasiliensis (Gillam et al., 2007) or Noctilio sp. (Barak and Yom-Tov, 1989) hunt in groups, perhaps due to food abundance or water availability. Sound recordings from a group of Noctilio leporinus and N. albiventris in Panama (cited in Moss and Surlykke, 2001) reveal a chaos of sound, where it would appear to be absolutely impossible to detect prey echoes. However, the bats feed successfully under these acoustically challenging conditions.

A recent laboratory study investigated strategies used by echolocating animals to reduce interference from conspecifics by placing pairs of big brown bats in a situation where they competed for a single prey item (Chiu et al., 2009). This laboratory study used high-speed 3-D video and microphone array recordings that permitted unambiguous assignment of calls to the individual vocalizing bat. The results showed that the big brown bat made adjustments in the spectral characteristics of its calls when it flew with conspecifics, and the magnitude of these adjustments depended on the baseline similarity of calls produced by the individual bats when flying alone (Figure 5). Bats that produced sonar calls with similar baseline signal design made larger adjustments in their sonar calls than those bats whose baseline call designs were already dissimilar. Field recordings from the same species showed frequency adjustments of up to 8 kHz, when two individuals flew closely together (Surlykke and Moss, 2000). Bates et al. (2008) demonstrated that frequency adjustments of paired big brown bats can aid in target detection. It is noteworthy that free-tailed bats, Tadarida brasiliensis, can prevent mutual interference by avoiding emission of sounds at the same time (Jarvis et al., 2010). Also, Gillam et al. (2007) reported that free-tailed bats changed emitted call frequency in response to signal playbacks in the field by 3–4 kHz, corroborating Habersetzer’s (1981) suggestion that the frequency shifts in Rhinopoma hardwickei were jamming avoidance responses (see Table 1). These findings imply that frequency features of sonar calls produced by different bats aid each individual in segregating echoes of its own sonar vocalizations from the acoustic signals of neighboring bats (Chiu et al., 2009). Distinct frequency components of an individual’s calls could be used by the bat to hear out the signals of interest (echoes from its own calls) from background (calls and echoes from other bats).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Call adjustments for frequency streaming in Eptesicus fuscus. Upper left photo illustrating bats vocalizing in close proximity. Photo taken by Jessica Nelson, and image assembled by Chen Chiu. Upper right plots adjustment in call frequency as a function of baseline call separation across bat pairs. Bats with similar baseline calls made larger adjustments in the end frequency the FM sweep of their calls than those with different baseline calls. Bottom panel shows raw sonar signal recording segment from two bats flying together in close proximity. Call assignment to the vocalizing bat could be made by combining three microphone recordings and 3D video position data. Adapted from Chiu et al. (2009).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Changes in emitted frequency (maximum adjustment of fundamental in kHz) when bats hunt in groups or highly cluttered environments.

The same laboratory study of bats foraging in pairs led to the surprising discovery that echolocating bats sometimes go silent. The prevalence of silent behavior depended on the spatial separation of the bats as they flew together and also on the baseline similarity of their calls when they flew alone, suggesting that silence is at least in part a jamming avoidance response. In addition, the trailing bat tended to go silent, raising the possibility that it used the vocalizations and echoes from the leading bat to orient (Chiu et al., 2008). Silent behavior in bats suggests possible connections between scene analysis by echolocation in bats and other animals that listen passively to the natural soundscape (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Silent behavior for jamming avoidance in Eptesicus fuscus. (A) shows the prevalence of silent behavior (no calls over a minimum of 200 ms) as a function of baseline call similarity in bat pairs. For bat pairs that showed greater call similarity in baseline trials when they flew alone calls (% DFA classification lower), there was more silent behavior than for bat pairs that showed baseline call dissimilarity calls (% DFA classification higher). (B) shows % silent behavior for five individual bats, each paired with two different bats. When paired with a bat whose baseline call similarity was comparatively high (low DFA, white bars), bats consistently exhibited more silent behavior than when paired with a bat whose baseline call similarity was comparatively low (high DFA, black bars). Adapted from Chiu et al. (2008).

The problem of jamming is not restricted to conspecifics. Sympatric bats and other ultrasound emitting animals also contribute to the complexity of the soundscape. In the tropics, where bat density and diversity is particularly high, the variety of call design across species is pronounced. Many bats emit search calls with alternating dominant frequency (e.g. Emballonuridae; Jung et al., 2007), and some bats (e.g. Cormura brevirostris, Molossus molossus) produce even more sophisticated calls with three or more different tones (Guillén-Servent and Ibáñez, 2007; Surlykke and Kalko, 2008). A range of fast and high flying, aerial hawking bats regularly alternate peak frequencies between subsequent calls during search flight, as is known for the European noctule Nyctalus noctula with its characteristic “plip-plop” search calls. The significance of this behavior remains controversial (Kingston et al., 2003). Frequency changes may help bats to effectively sort and assign echoes from their own calls in a complex environment, where ambiguity about call-echo assignment can arise. Another advantage of alternating calls is that the “time stamp” of each call increases maximum detection range by marking calls to discriminate between echoes of successive calls (Jung et al., 2007). The emballonurid bat, Saccopteryx bilineata, is well known for emitting calls alternating between 43 and 47 kHz (Jung et al., 2007). However, it may skip the low frequency and only emit high frequency calls, apparently in situations when in transit between the roost and hunting ground. The frequency alternation always occurs in hunting situations, which further supports the notion that frequency alternation is adaptive for separating target and background echoes (Ratcliffe et al., 2010).

These observations point to the high importance of frequency as a cue in echolocating bats for separating and tracking auditory objects. The bat may listen in on a restricted frequency band to separate its own calls and echoes from those of other echolocating bats. In technical radar “frequency hopping” (Jankiraman, 2007) is used to reduce jamming by rapidly switching the frequency of the transmitted energy, and detecting only that frequency during the receiving time window. A broad range of stimulus features may be used to sample information from the natural environment for the analysis of natural scenes. Data presented in this section provide examples of active adjustments of sonar call spectral features by bats foraging in the presence of conspecifics and other sympatric bats. Such vocal-motor adjustments may allow the bat to perceptually segregate echoes from its sonar calls from the signals of neighboring bats.

Timing: Bats control call intervals to group and segregate echoes

Echolocation calls are discrete signals that yield brief acoustic snapshots of the environment. Because of the interrupted nature of sonar calls and echoes, bats must integrate echo information over time to build up a representation of an auditory scene and track dynamic objects. Does the temporal patterning of bat vocalizations yield insights to the information-processing strategies and integration windows for representing auditory objects in the natural scene? In human listening experiments, the perception of auditory streams depends not only on the spectral features of acoustic signals but also on the temporal spacing between stimuli (Bregman, 1990). For example, when a human listener is presented with a series of tones that alternate between high and low frequencies at a low temporal rate, the subject hears out the individual tones. However, when the rate of presentation is increased, the listener begins to perceive two separate sounds streams, one low frequency and the other high frequency. The perception of auditory streams, in this example, depends on the frequency separation of high and low tones and the temporal interval between successive sounds (Bregman and Campbell, 1971). A similar phenomenon occurs in visual motion perception: When neighboring lights flash slowly and sequentially along the perimeter of a marquis, a viewer perceives each flash as a separate visual event; however, if neighboring lights flash sequentially at a higher rate, the viewer perceives a stream of light that moves along the perimeter. The perception of visual movement in this example depends on the spatial separation of the neighboring lights and the time interval between successive flashes (Körte, 1915).

The interval between successive echolocation calls directly impacts the timing of echo returns, with consequences for sorting and tracking sonar objects in a dynamic environment. Literature on bat echolocation behavior typically describes a continuous and regular decrease in pulse interval with a reduction in target range (e.g. Nachtigall and Moore, 1988). However, as detailed below, the decrease may not be as regular as generally characterized. The temporal patterning often contains periods of stable call production, embedded in sequences with decreasing call intervals. As noted above, humans report that the perception of auditory streams depends on the timing of acoustic stimuli (Bregman, 1990), and by extension, the bat’s temporal control over echo returns would be expected to influence the animal’s perceptual representation of objects comprising the natural scene. Therefore, quantitative analysis of the temporal characteristics of sonar calls produced by bats in complex auditory environments provides insight to the information that allows the bat to segregate, group, and track echoes from different objects over time and space.

Analysis of sounds produced by the bat E. fuscus as it forages provides an example of how the sound repetition rate does not change continuously over time; rather it remains relatively high and stable over extended periods; during the approach phase, the sound repetition rate may plateau at around 50–60 Hz, interrupted by longer PI gaps, for time periods as long as 200 ms (Moss et al., 2006; see Figure 7 below). The grouping of calls over time periods exceeding a wingbeat cycle demonstrates that the link between respiration and call emission (Suthers et al., 1972) may be broken, even though coupling call emission to wing beat saves energy (Waters and Wong, 2007). Thus, we infer that when the rhythm is broken, the bat makes adjustments in sonar call timing in response to perceptual demands for echo streaming. The breaks in pulse production serve to open up a temporal window for the bat to listen for echoes from more distant objects before producing the next sonar vocalization. The bat may also need the longer intervals between sound groups to integrate echo sequences and update motor behaviors (Wilson and Moss, 2003). The stable periods of sound repetition rate (sonar “strobe groups”) occur when the bat is selecting a target, changing the direction of its flight path and in proximity to obstacles (Moss et al., 2006).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Temporal adjustments of sonar calls in bats foraging in the lab (left, Eptesicus fuscus) and in the field (right, Macrophyllum macrophyllum). Both examples illustrate “sonar strobing” behavior, i.e. the production of sound groups with relatively stable intervals, as they approach prey. The far left panels are taken from typical laboratory trials and show 3D flight paths of bats as they take tethered insects in the vicinity of vegetation. The adjacent panels show overhead views of the same trials. Arrows indicate the direction of the flight path. The numbers in the upper left corner of each plot indicate the distance between the tethered insect and the vegetation, shown in green. Flight segments that contained “sonar strobe groups” are displayed in red. The pulse intervals for each of these four trials are plotted, and calls that fit the criterion for “sonar strobe groups” (<5% variation in PI) are circled in red. Adapted from Moss et al. (2006). Right, sonar strobing behavior taken from field recordings of M. macrophyllum. SC search calls, AC approach calls, TG terminal group, CM capture of mealworm. Adapted from Weinbeer and Kalko (2007).

Grouping of sounds has been reported from field studies of a number of bats from different families e.g. Vespertilionidae M. nattereri (Melcón et al., 2007), E. fuscus (Surlykke and Moss, 2000), Phyllostomidae (Weinbeer and Kalko, 2007), Rhinolophidae (Schnitzler, 1968). Also, small Vespertilionid bats from Malaysia hunting close to clutter emit groups of 2–15 pulses at high pulse repetition rates (37 ± 105 Hz) of high frequency and very broad bandwidth. The longest pulse groups and highest within-group repetition rates have been recorded in the most maneuverable, Kerivoula pellucida, of the nine studied Kerivoulinae and Murininae species, supporting the hypothesis that inter-call interval indicates the degree of clutter tolerance (Kingston et al., 1999).

We hypothesize that stable signal repetition rates have immediate consequences for the bat’s perception of space (Moss and Surlykke, 2001; Moss et al., 2006). Neural recordings from the midbrain of the awake bat show that sonar “strobe groups” influence spatial-temporal response profiles of neurons in the bat auditory system that are hypothesized to play a role in coding sonar target distance. Specifically, a class of echo-delay tuned neurons in the bat superior colliculus exhibit facilitated responses and narrow tuning to pulse-echo pairs presented at stable PIs. However, the echo-delay tuning collapses when stimulus intervals are jittered, even by as little as 20–30% (Gifford and Moss, 2005; Ulanovsky and Moss, 2008). This finding suggests that activity of neurons responsive to echo delay can be gated by the temporal stability of successive sonar calls, which in turn may influence the animal’s perception of targets along the range axis.

The bat’s adjustments of sonar signal repetition rate and duration are tied to target range; however, echolocation parameters also depend on the bat’s azimuth and elevation relative to a selected prey item, and most importantly, its plan of attack. For example, when a bat approaches an insect, flies past it and returns to intercept it, the temporal patterning of the animal’s sonar signals are distinctly different from those produced by a bat that flies directly to intercept the insect (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). Thus, the temporal patterning of the bat’s echolocation signals provides explicit data on the motor commands that feed directly back to the auditory system for spatially-guided behavior. The temporal clustering of calls into groups may also reveal the time window over which echolocating bats integrate pulse-echo information to build up a representation of the auditory scene.

In summary, echolocating bats produce sounds in groups, and the bat may use the collection of echoes from such sound groups to process and update acoustic information gathered from the environment. Given the importance of temporal patterning to the perceptual organization of sound patterns in human listeners, we propose that the sound groups contribute to the bat’s perceptual organization of echo streams from a spatially complex environment.

Discussion

Several themes emerged from our review that we discuss below, both in the context of scene analysis by echolocation and in other animal systems. We propose that the components of scene analysis detailed here for the echolocating bat apply more generally to the analysis of natural scenes in a broad range of animals that rely on hearing, as well as other senses.

The bat’s active sonar system offers indirect access to its perceptual world, which then presents a special opportunity to tap into the processes supporting the analysis of natural auditory scenes. The features of a bat’s sonar signals have a direct impact on the echo information available to its acoustic imaging system. In turn, the bat’s perception of objects in the environment influences its motor behavior. Therefore, the bat’s adaptive sonar behavior can shed light on its perception of auditory objects in a natural scene and how its control over sonar signals can contribute directly to perceptual grouping and segregation of dynamic sound streams.

Research from field and laboratory studies demonstrate the bat’s control over the frequency, timing, and direction of sonar calls, which leads us to propose that these parameters are used by the bat to segregate and track auditory objects in a dynamic environment. Figure 8 summarizes in schematic form the finding that bats echolocating in a complex environment adjust the frequency and/or direction of sonar vocalizations to stabilize these parameters in echo returns. By maintaining some constancy in sound frequency (Figure 8A) and direction (Figure 8B), the bat may be able to hear out auditory streams from selected objects in the midst of echoes from background targets and signals from other bats. As a bat flies towards a target, echo delay necessarily changes, and the bat must track coherent patterns of object distance changes over time. In the case of changing echo delay (Figure 8C), the bat may be able to hear out streams of echo delay that are shortening in a predictable temporal pattern, which depends on the angle between the bat’s flight direction and the object.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Schematic illustrating the active adjustments that bats make in call frequency (A), beam aim (B) and signal duration (C) that can aide in the segregation and streaming of acoustic information along the perceptual dimensions of pitch, direction, and distance.

The Role of Action in Perception

Bat echolocation highlights the importance of action to perception, as this animal’s motor behaviors give rise to the very stimuli that it uses to guide behaviors. It is important to note, however, that action influences perception, not only in echolocating bats, but in a variety of animal systems that rely on different modalities for sensing their environments. Some senses are referred to as active, because the animal detects stimuli that result from its own production of energy. This applies to echolocation in bats and toothed whales that produce and process sound energy to perceive objects in their surroundings, and also to electrolocation in African mormyrid and South American gymnotiform fishes that generate electric fields to orient in murky waters. The dynamic adjustment of signal production by active sensing animals has immediate influence on the stimuli they can use to monitor their perceptual worlds.

The link between action and perception may be less obvious in so-called passive sensing animals that rely on environmental energy to perceive their surroundings, but it is no less important. For example, vision involves active processes to seek out task-relevant visual information. Visual input is determined by gaze control, modulated by head movements, fixation-saccade cycles of eye movements, and accommodation. Active gaze control has been demonstrated in visual animals as different as jumping spiders (Tarsitano and Andrew, 1999), stalk-eyed flies (Ribak et al., 2009), zebra finches (Eckmeier, 2008), and humans (Henderson, 2003).

Perception also depends on motor adjustments in other “passive” sensory modalities. Acoustic cues for passive sound localization is enhanced by head movements that bring the sound source into auditory midline, as has been convincingly demonstrated in behavioral experiments with barn owls (Knudsen et al., 1979; Konishi and Knudsen, 1979), and cats (Tollin and Populin, 2005). Acoustically orienting robots (TeleHead) confirmed that dynamic cues produced by head movements play important roles in auditory localization (Toshima and Aoki, 2006). Similarly, the control of perception through action is essential for animals relying mainly on olfaction (sniffing, Bensafi et al., 2003), whisking (movement of whiskers Metha et al., 2007), and touch (Catania and Remple, 2004). The echolocating bat is a valuable model for studying the role of action in perception, not because it is unique, but because its dynamic call modifications provides us with direct access to the link between action and perception.

Integration

Echolocating bats experience a dynamic acoustic environment as they fly. The bat’s own movement, combined with movement of insect prey, results in time-varying echo parameters that the bat must track over time. To accomplish basic tasks like insect capture, the bat must integrate brief “acoustic snapshots” of objects in the environment to build a representation of a natural scene. An insectivorous bat must also anticipate its point of contact with prey to prepare appropriate movements for target interception with the wing or interfemoral membrane. These perceptual and behavioral requirements suggest that sequential analysis of acoustic stimuli is central to the bat’s perception of its natural scene.

Psychophysical experiments have demonstrated that echolocating bats can integrate acoustic information that arrives sequentially over time (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). In human listening experiments, the integration of acoustic stimuli into perceptual streams depends critically on the interval between successive sounds (Bregman, 1990), and we argue, based on the adaptive changes of temporal patterning in sonar calls, that a similar perceptual phenomenon operates in bats. Thus, the production of sonar sound groups, recorded in both the laboratory and the field (e.g. Kingston et al., 1999; Moss et al., 2006; Weinbeer and Kalko, 2007) reflects the bat’s control over echo streams. Since the bat actively adjusts the timing of its signals, we infer that the intervals selected by the animal under different environmental and task conditions contribute to its perceptual segregation and integration of echo returns over time.

Visual animals with well-developed fovea move their eyes to scan and fixate objects in the environment (Land and Hayhoe, 2001). Sequential fixations are interrupted by saccades, during which visual perception is suppressed (Volkmann et al., 1978). Thus, high-resolution snapshots of visual information must be integrated over time to perceive a continuous and stable world, as reported by human viewers. The agile flight of echolocating bats in cluttered environments (Fenton, 1990; Kingston et al., 1999; Ratcliffe and Dawson, 2003; Siemers and Schnitzler, 2004) shows that they can operate by listening to stroboscopic echo returns, and this leads us to hypothesize that bats integrate dynamic and interrupted sensory information to represent complex natural scenes. This hypothesis is bolstered by laboratory studies with simplified stimuli (Moss and Surlykke, 2001).

The Role of Attention, Learning and Memory in Perception

Attention, learning, and memory contribute to the analysis of natural scenes, as these cognitive processes allow an animal to efficiently manage the sensory load from a complex and noisy environment (e.g. Henderson and Hollingworth, 1999; Knudsen, 2007; Walker et al., 2008). In laboratory studies reviewed in this paper, we present several examples of the bat’s adaptive control of the sonar beam aim and range (e.g. Falk et al., 2010; Ghose and Moss, 2006; Ghose et al., 2006; Surlykke et al., 2009a; Falk et al., 2010), which directly influence the direction and amplitude of echo returns from a limited region in space. Since the animal’s beam directing behavior determines the echo information it samples, we propose that the bat’s “acoustic gaze” provides an indicator of its attention to objects in space, similar to the relation between foveation and spatial attention in visual animals. In other words, the bat’s sonar beam may be a physical manifestation of the animal’s “spotlight of attention” (Broadbent, 1958), by allocating information-processing resources to a restricted region of a complex and noisy natural scene.

Learning and memory can also reduce the information-processing load on the bat’s sonar imaging system. Both anecdotal reports and experimental findings provide documentation that bats performing routine tasks in familiar environments rely on memory in favor of echo information to orient and navigate. Changes in familiar environments sometimes lead to mistakes by the bat, as it fails to listen to echoes that could guide appropriate behavior. For example, Griffin (1958) describes bats, returning to the roost after an evening’s hunt, crashing into a newly erected cave barricade even though it reflected strong echoes. Griffin coins this mishap the “Andrea Doria Effect,” because the bats seemed to ignore important information from echo returns to guide their behavior and instead favored spatial memory. More recently, Jensen et al. (2005) conducted a laboratory study to investigate the bat’s use of an acoustic landmark to guide spatial navigation. Bats learned to associate a landmark with passage through a net and could reliably navigate the obstacle when the landmark and net opening were moved together. However, on catch trials, when the landmark and net opening were moved to an unfamiliar configuration, the bat crashed into the echo-reflecting net at a location adjacent to the landmark, where the animal had come to anticipate an opening. These findings suggest that spatial orientation in a complex environment can be aided by learning and memory and this only fails when (often artificial) changes occur in the environment.

Summary

Echolocating bats actively control the features of their sonar calls in response to information gathered from the environment, and we hypothesize that active adjustments in sonar signal design play directly into bat perception of complex and dynamic acoustic scenes. This hypothesis cannot be tested using phenomenological reports, as in many studies of human auditory scene analysis. Instead, objective measures of the bat’s adaptive behaviors provide us with a window to the animal’s perception. Here, we have reviewed field and laboratory studies of bat sonar behavior that demonstrate adjustments in call direction, duration, intensity, timing, and frequency, and we propose that these signal parameters are fundamental to the perceptual grouping and segregation of auditory objects in a complex environment.

Grouping and segregation of auditory objects is a general problem of auditory scene analysis that all hearing animals must solve. When a bat echolocates in a complex environment, a single vocalization results in a cascade of echoes from objects at different directions and distances. By the time a flying bat produces a subsequent vocalization, the relative position of these objects changes, creating a new “acoustic snapshot” of the environment. Grouping echoes returning from different objects across calls invokes sequential scene analysis processes, which may give rise to distinct perceptual streams. Parsing information carried by overlapping echoes from different objects invokes simultaneous scene analysis processes. The bat’s sequential analysis of echo returns enables target tracking, while its simultaneous analysis enables figure-ground segregation. Target tracking and figure-ground segregation are commonly viewed as tasks of the visual system, and this parallel with echolocation suggests that comparative studies across animal systems and modalities may help to deepen our understanding of natural scene analysis.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

C.F. Moss and A. Surlykke gratefully acknowledge the generous support of the Institute of Advanced Study in Berlin, which allowed them the time and space to write this review. We would also like to thank Wei Xian for assistance with data analysis and figure preparation. Melville Wohgemuth contributed the top panel to Figure 1. Collection of experimental data reported in this manuscript was supported by NSF grant, “Active Sensing for Three-Dimensional Auditory Localization,” NIBIB grant, “CRCNS: Innovative technologies inspired by biosonar” to CFM, NIDCD P-30 grant in “Comparative and Evolutionary Biology of Hearing” (R. Dooling and A. Popper, Co-PIs) and the Danish Natural Science Research Council to AS. Discussions with Albert Bregman, Chen Chiu, Michael Lewicki, Bruno Olshausen motivated some of the ideas presented in this review. We are grateful for comments from Shihab Shamma, Melville Wohlgemuth, James Simmons, and Khaleel Razak, who helped us to improve the manuscript.

References

Au, W. W. L. (1993). The Sonar of Dolphins. New York: Springer.

Aytekin, M., Grassi, E., Sahota, M., and Moss, C. F. (2004). The bat head-related transfer function reveals binaural cues for sound.

Barak, Y., and Yom-Tov, Y. (1989). The advantage of group hunting to Kuhl’s pipistrelle bat Pipistrellus kuhli (Microchiroptera). J. Zool. 219, 670–675.

CrossRef Full Text

Bates, M. E., Stamper, S. A., and Simmons, J. A. (2008). Jamming avoidance response of big brown bats in target detection. J. Exp. Biol. 211, 106–113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Batteau, D. W. (1967). The role of the pinna in human localization. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 1011, 158–180.

CrossRef Full Text

Bensafi, M., Porter, J., Pouliot, S., Mainland, J., Johnson, B., Zelano, C., Young, N., Bremner, E., Aframian, D., Khan, R., and Sobel, N. (2003). Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1142–1144.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blauert, J. (1996). Spatial Hearing. The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Bregman, A. S., and Campbell, J. (1971). Primary auditory stream segregation and perception of order in rapid sequences of tones. J. Exp. Psychol. 89, 244–249.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brinkløv, S., Kalko, E. K. V., and Surlykke, A. (2010). Dynamic adjustment of biosonar intensity to habitat clutter in the bat Macrophyllum macrophyllum (Phyllostomidae). Behav.Ecol. Sociobiol. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-0998-9.

CrossRef Full Text

Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and Communication. London: Pergamon Press.

Broders, H. G., Findlay, C. S., and Zheng, L. (2004). Effect of clutter on echolocation call structure of Myotis septentrionalis and M. lucifugus. J. Mammal. 85, 273–281.

CrossRef Full Text

Bruns, V. (1980). “Structural adaptation in the cochlea of the horseshoe bat for the analsyis of long CF-FM echolocation signals,” in Animal Sonar Systems, eds R. G. Busnel and J. F. Fish (New York: Plenum Press), 867–869.

Catania, K. C., and Remple, F. E. (2004). Tactile foveation in the star-nosed mole. Brain Behav. Evol. 63, 1–12.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chiu, C., Xian, W., and Moss, C. F. (2008). Flying in silence: echolocating bats cease vocalizing to avoid sonar jamming. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 13115–13120.

CrossRef Full Text

Chiu, C., Xian, W., and Moss, C. F. (2009). Adaptive echolocation behavior in bats for the analysis of auditory scenes. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 1392–1404.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Covey, E., and Casseday, J. H. (1999). Timing in the auditory system of the bat. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 61, 457–476.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eckmeier, D., Geurten, B. R. H., Kress, D., Mertes, M., Kern, R., Egelhaaf, M., and Bischof, H.-J. (2008). Gaze Strategy in the Free Flying Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata). PLoS One 3, e3956.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Falk, B., Williams, T., Aytekin, M., and Moss, C. F. (2010). Adaptive behavior for texture discrimination by the free-flying big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Comp. Physiol., under review.

Fenton, M. B. (1990). The foraging behaviour and ecology of animal-eating bats. Can. J. Zool. 68, 411–422.

CrossRef Full Text

Ghose, K., Horiuchi, T. K., Krishnaprasad, P. S., and Moss, C. F. (2006). Echolocating bats use a nearly time-optimal strategy to intercept prey. PLoS Biol. 4, 865–873.

CrossRef Full Text

Ghose, K., and Moss, C. F. (2003). The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 114, 1120–1131.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ghose, K., and Moss, C. F. (2006). Steering by hearing: a bat’s acoustic gaze is linked to its flight motor output by a delayed, adaptive linear law. J. Neurosci. 26, 1704–1710.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gifford, G., and Moss, C. F. (2005). Spatial Tuning of Auditory Neurons in the Superior Colliculus of the Echolocating Bat Depends on Pulse Interval. (Abstract Number 164.23). Washington, D.C.: Thirty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience.

Gillam, E. H., Ulanovsky, N., and McCracken, G. F. (2007). Rapid jamming avoidance in biosonar. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 274, 651–660.

CrossRef Full Text

Griffin, D. R. (1958). Listening in the Dark, 2nd Edn., 1986 Cornell University. New York: Yale University Press.

Griffin, D. R., Friend, J. H., and Webster, F. A. (1965). Target discrimination by the echolocation of bats. J. Exp. Biol. 158, 155–168.

Griffin, D. R., Webster, F. A., and Michael, C. R. (1960). The echolocation of flying insects by bats. Anim. Behav. 8, 141–154.

CrossRef Full Text

Grinnell, A. D., and Grinnell, V. S. (1965). Neural correlates of vetical localization by echo locating bats. J. Physiol. 181, 830–851.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Guillén-Servent, A., and Ibáñez, C. (2007). Unusual echolocation behavior in a small molossid bat, Molossops temminckii, that forages near background clutter. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 1599–1613.

CrossRef Full Text

Habersetzer, J. (1981). Adaptive echolocation sounds in the bat Rhinopoma hardwickei. J. Comp. Physiol. A 144, 559–566.

CrossRef Full Text

Habersetzer, J., and Vogler, B. (1983). Discrimination of surface-structured targets by the echolocating bat Myotis myotis during flight. J. Comp. Physiol. A 152, 275–282.

CrossRef Full Text

Hartley, D. J., and Suthers, R. A. (1989). The sound emission pattern of the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 1348–1351.

CrossRef Full Text

Helversen, D. v., and Helversen, O. v. (1999). Acoustic guide in bat- pollinated flower. Nature 398, 759–760.

CrossRef Full Text

Henderson, J. M. (2003). Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 7, 498–504.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Henderson, J. M., and Hollingworth, A. (1999). High level scene perception. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50, 243–271.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hiryu, S., Bates, M. E., Simmons, J. A., and Riquimaroux, H. (2010). FM echolocating bats shift frequencies to avoid broadcast-echo ambiguity in clutter. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. (published online ahead of print March 29, 2010). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000429107.

CrossRef Full Text

Hiryu, S., Katsura, K., Lin, L.-K., Riquimaroux, H., and Watanabe, Y. (2005). Doppler-shift compensation in the Taiwanese leaf-nosed bat (Hipposideros terasensis) recorded with a telemetry microphone system during flight. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 118, 3927–3933.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holderied, M. W., and Helversen, O. v. (2003). Echolocation range and wingbeat period match in aerial-hawking bats. Proc.R. Soc. Lond.B 270, 2293–2299.

CrossRef Full Text

Holderied, M. W., Korine, C., Fenton, M. B., Parsons, S., Robson, S., and Jones, G. (2005). Echolocation call intensity in the aerial hawking bat Eptesicus bottae (Vespertilionidae) studied using stereo videogrammetry. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 1321–1327.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jakobsen, L., and Surlykke, A. (2010). Vespertilionid bats control the width of their biosonar sound beam dynamically during prey pursuit. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci. (in press).

CrossRef Full Text

Jankiraman, M. (2007). Design of multi-frequency CW radars. Raleigh, NC: SciTech Publishing.

Jarvis, J., Bohn, K. M., Tressler, J., and Smotherman, M. (2010). A mechanism for antiphonal echolocation by free-tailed bats. Anim. Behav. 79, 787–796.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jen, P. H.-S., and Suga, N. (1976). Coordinated activities of middle-ear and laryngeal muscles in echolocating bats. Science 191, 950–952.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jensen, M. E., Moss, C. F., and Surlykke, A. (2005). Echolocating bats can use acoustic landmarks for spatial orientation. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 4399–4410.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, M., Hickmott, L. S., Aguilar Soto, N., and Madsen, P. T. (2008). Echolocation behaviour adapted to prey in foraging Blainville’s beaked whale (Mesoplodon densirostris). Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 275, 133–139.

CrossRef Full Text

Jung, K., Kalko, E. K. V., and Helversen, O. v. (2007). Echolocation calls in Central American emballonurid bats: signal design and call frequency alternation. J. Zool. 272, 125–137.

CrossRef Full Text

Kalko, E. K. V., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1993). Plasticity in echolocation signals of European pipistrelle bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and prey detection. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33, 415–428.

CrossRef Full Text

Kano, F., and Tomonaga, M. (2009). How chimpanzees look at pictures: a comparative eye-tracking study. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 276, 1949–1955.

CrossRef Full Text

Kick, S. A., and Simmons, J. A. (1984). Automatic gain control in bats sonar receiver and the neuroethology of echolocation. J. Neuroscience. 4, 2725–2737.

Kingston, T., Jones, G., and Akbar, Z. (2003). Alternation of echolocation calls in 5 species of aerial-feeding insectivorous bats from Malaysia. J. Mammal. 84, 205–215.

CrossRef Full Text

Kingston, T., Jones, G., Akbar, Z., and Kunz, T. H. (1999). Echolocation signal design in Kerivoulinae and Murininae (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) from Malaysia. J. Zool. (Lond.) 249, 359–374.

CrossRef Full Text

Knudsen, E. (2007). Fundamental components of attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 57–78.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knudsen, E. I., Blasdel, G. G., and Konishi, M. (1979). Sound localization by the barn owl (Tyto alba) measured with the search coil technique. J. Comp. Physiol. A 133, 1–11.

CrossRef Full Text

Konishi, M., and Knudsen, E. I. (1979). The oilbird: hearing and echolocation. Science 204, 425–427.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Körte, A. (1915). Kinomatoscopische Untersuchungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie der Sinnesorgane 72, 193–296.

Kössl, M., and Vater, M. (1995). “Cochlear structure and function in bats,” in Hearing by Bats, Vol. 5, eds A. N. Popper and R. R. Fay (Berlin, London, NewYork: Springer), 191–234.

Land, M., and Hayhoe, M. (2001). In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? Vision Res. 41, 3559–3565.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Long, G. R., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1975). Behavioural audiograms from the bat Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. J. Comp. Physiol. A 100, 211–219.

CrossRef Full Text

Lawrence, B. D., and Simmons, J. A. (1982). Measurements of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 71, 585–590.

CrossRef Full Text

Melcón, M. L., Denzinger, A., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (2007). Aerial hawking and landing: approach behaviour in Natterer’s bats, Myotis nattereri (Kuhl 1818). J. Exp. Biol. 210, 4457–4464.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Menne, D., Kaipf, I., Wagner, I., Ostwald, J., and Schnitzler, H. -U. (1989). Range estimation by echolocation in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: trading of phase versus time cues. Digital filters in auditory physiology. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 2642–2650.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Metha, S. B., Whitmer, D., Figueroa, R., Williams, B. A., and Kleinfeld, D. (2007). Active spatial perception in the vibrissa scanning sensorimotor system. PLoS Biol. 5, 0309–0322. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050015.

CrossRef Full Text

Møhl, B., and Surlykke, A. (1989). Detection of sonar signals in the presence of pulses of masking noise by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 165, 119–124.

CrossRef Full Text

Moss, C. F., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1989). Accuracy of target ranging in echolocating bats: acoustic information processing. J. Comp. Physiol. A 165, 383–393.

CrossRef Full Text

Moss, C. F., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1995). “Behavioral studies of auditory information processing,” in Hearing by Bats, eds A. N. Popper and R. R. Fay (New York: Springer), 87–145.

Moss, C. F., and Surlykke, A. (2001). Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110, 2207–2226.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moss, C. F., Bohn, K., Gilkenson, H., and Surlykke, A. (2006). Active listening for spatial orientation in a complex auditory scene. PLoS Biol. 4, 615–626. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040079.

CrossRef Full Text

Müller, R., and Kuc, R. (2000). Foliage echoes: a probe into the ecological acoustics of bat echolocation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 836–845.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Müller, R., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1999). Acoustic flow perception in cf-bats: properties of the available cues. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 2958–2966.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nachtigall, P. W., and Moore, P. W. B. (eds) (1988). Animal Sonar: Process and Performance. New York: Springer.

Neuweiler, G. (1989). Foraging ecology and audition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 4, 160–166.

CrossRef Full Text

Neuweiler, G., Bruns, V., and Schuller, G. (1980). Ears adapted for the detection of motion, or how echolocating bats have exploited the capacities of the mammalian auditory system. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 741–753.

CrossRef Full Text

Popper, A. N., and Fay, R. R. (eds) (1995). Hearing by Bats. New York: Springer.

Ratcliffe, J. M., and Dawson, J. W. (2003). Flexibility: the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus, and the northern eared bat, M. septentrionalis, both glean and hawk prey. Anim. Behav. 66, 847–856.

CrossRef Full Text

Ratcliffe, J. M., Jakobsen, L., Kalko, E. K. V., and Surlykke, A. (2010). Echolocation in the insectivorous aerial hawking bat, Saccopteryx bilineata when flying close to clutter: relationship between call intensity, duration and frequency alternation. J. Comp. Physiol. A (in review).

CrossRef Full Text

Ribak, G., Egge, A. R., and Swallow, J. G. (2009). Saccadic head rotations during walking in the stalk-eyed fly (Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni). Proc.R.Soc.B 276, 1643–1649.

CrossRef Full Text

Schnitzler, H.-U. (1968). Die Ultraschall-Ortungslaute der Hufeisen-Fledermäuse (Chiroptera-Rhinolophidae) in verschiedenen Orientierungssituationen. Z. Vgl. Physiol. 57, 376–408.

CrossRef Full Text

Schnitzler, H.-U., and Flieger, E. (1983). Detection of oscillating target movements by echolocation in the greater horseshoe bat. J. Comp. Physiol. A 153, 385–391.

CrossRef Full Text

Schnitzler, H.-U., and Kalko, E. K. V. (1998). “How echolocating bats search and find food,” in Bat. Biology and Conservation, eds T. H. Kunz and P. A. Racey (Washington, London: Smithsonian Institution Press), 183–196.

Schnitzler, H.-U., Moss, C. F., and Denzinger, A. (2003). From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18, 386–394.

CrossRef Full Text

Schnitzler, H.-U., Menne, D., Kober, R., and Heblich, K. (1983). “The acoustical image of fluttering insects in echolocating bats,” in Neuroethology and Behavioral Physiology, eds F. Huber and H. Markl (Springer), 235–250.

Siemers, B. M., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (2004). Echolocation signals reflect niche differentiation in five sympatric congeneric bat species. Nature 429, 457–661.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A. (1973). The resolution of target range by echolocating bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54, 157–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A. (1979). Perception of echo phase information in bat sonar. Science 204, 1336–1338.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Fenton, M. B., and O’Farrell, M. J. (1979). Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. Science 203, 16–21.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Ferragamo, M., Moss, C. F., Stevenson, S., and Altes, R. A. (1990). Discrimination of jittered sonar echoes by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus: the shape of target images in echolocation. J. Comp.Physiol. A, 167, 589–616.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Lavender, W. A., Lavender, B. A., Doroshow, C. A., Kiefer, S. W., Livingston, R., Scallet, A. C., and Crowley, D. E. (1974). Target structure and echo spectral discrimination by echolocating bats. Science 186, 1130–1132.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, N. B., Seymour, K. L., Habersetzer, J., and Gunnell, G. F. (2008). Primitive early eocene bat from Wyoming and the evolution of flight and echolocation. Nature 451, 818–821.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suga, N. (1988). “Auditory neuroethology and speech processing: complex-sound processing by combination-sensitive neurons,” in Auditory Function. Neurobiological Bases of Hearing, eds G. M. Edelman, W. E. Gal, and W. M. Cowan (New York: John Wiley & Sons), 679–720.

Suga, N. (1990). Biosonar and neural computation in bats. Sci. Am. 262, 34–41.

CrossRef Full Text

Surlykke, A. (1992). Target ranging and the role of time-frequency structure of synthetic echoes in big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170, 83–92.

CrossRef Full Text

Surlykke, A., Ghose, K., and Moss, C. F. (2009a). Acoustic scanning of natural scenes by echolocation in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 1011–1020.

CrossRef Full Text

Surlykke, A., Pedersen, S. B., and Jakobsen, L. (2009b). Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 276, 853–860.

CrossRef Full Text

Surlykke, A., and Kalko, E. K. V. (2008). Echolocating bats cry out loud to detect their prey. PLoS ONE 3, e2036(1)–e2036(10). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002036.

CrossRef Full Text

Surlykke, A., and Moss, C. F. (2000). Echolocation behavior of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, in the field and the laboratory. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 2419–2429.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suthers, R. A., Thomas, S. P., and Suthers, B. J. (1972). Respiration, wing-beat andultrasonic pulse emission in an echo-locating bat. J. Exp. Biol. 56, 37–48.

CrossRef Full Text

Tarsitano, M. S., and Andrew, R. (1999). Scanning and route selection in the jumping spider, Portia labiata. Animal Behaviour, 58, 255–265.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thomas, J. T., Moss, C. F., and Vater, M. (eds) (2004). Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Tollin, D. J., and Populin, L. C. (2005). Sound-localization performance in the cat: the effect of restraining the head. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 1223–1234.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Toshima, I., and Aoki, S. (2006). “The effect of head movement on sound localization in an acoustical telepresence robot: TeleHead,” in Intelligent Robots and Systems, IEEE/RSJ International Conference 9–15 October 2006, 872–877.

CrossRef Full Text

Ulanovsky, N., and Moss, C. F. (2008). What the bat’s voice tells the bat’s brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 8491–8498.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vanderelst, D., De May, F., and Peremans, H. (2010). Simulating the morphological feasibility of adaptive beamforming in bats. plos one. (in press).

Verboom, B., Boonman, A. M., and Limpens, H. J. G. A. (1999). Acoustic perception of landscape elements by the pond bat (Myotis dasycneme). J. Zool. 248, 59–66.

CrossRef Full Text

Volkmann, F. C., Riggs, L. A., White, K. D., and Moore, R. K. (1978). Contrast sensitivity during saccadic eye movements. Vision Res. 18, 1193–1199.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

von der Emde, G., and Menne, D. (1989). Discrimination of insect wingbeat-frequencies by the bat Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. J. Comp. Physiol. A 164, 663–671.

CrossRef Full Text

von der Emde, G., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1986). Fluttering target detection in hipposiderid bats. J. Comp. Physiol. A 14, 43–55.

CrossRef Full Text

von der Emde, G., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1990). Classification of insects by echolocating greater horseshoe bats. J. Comp. Physiol. A 167, 423–430.

CrossRef Full Text

Walker, S., Stafford, P., and Davis, G. (2008). Ultra-rapid categorization requires visual attention: scenes with multiple foreground objects. J. Vis. 8, 1–12.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Waters, D. A., and Wong, J. G. (2007). The allocation of energy to echolocation pulses produced by soprano pipistrelles (Pipistrellus pygmaeus) during the wingbeat cycle. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121(Pt 1), 2990–3000.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weinbeer, M., and Kalko, E. K. V. (2007). Ecological niche and phylogeny: the highly complex echolocation behavior of the trawling long-legged bat, Macrophyllum macrophyllum. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 1337–1348.

CrossRef Full Text

Wightman, F. L., and Kistler, D. J. (1989). Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: psychophysical validation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 868–878.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wightman, F. L., and Kistler, D. J. (1997). Monaural soundlocalization revisited. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 101, 1050–1063.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, W. W., and Moss, C. F. (2003). “Sensory-motor behavior of free-flying FM bats during target capture,” in Advances in the Study of Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins, eds J. A. Thomas, C. F. Moss, and M. Vater (Chicago: Chicago University Press).

Wotton, J. M., Haresign, T., Ferragamo, M., and Simmons, J. A. (1996). Sound source elevation and external ear cues influence the discrimination of spectral notches by the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 1764–1776.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yovel, Y., Franz, M. O., Stilz, P., and Schnitzler, H.-U. (2008). Plant classification from bat-like echolocation signals. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4, e1000032. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000032.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: auditory scene analysis, action-perception, hearing, neuroethology, stream segregation

Citation: Moss CF and Surlykke A (2010) Probing the natural scene by echolocation in bats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:33. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00033

Received: 28 February 2010; Paper pending published: 11 April 2010;
Accepted: 20 May 2010; Published online: 05 August 2010

Edited by:

Paul S. Katz, Georgia State University, USA

Reviewed by:

James A. Simmons, Brown University, USA
Khaleel A. Razak, University of California, USA

Copyright: © 2010 Moss and Surlykke. This is an open-access article subject to an exclusive license agreement between the authors and the Frontiers Research Foundation, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original authors and source are credited.

*Correspondence: Cynthia F. Moss, Department of Psychology, Institute for Systems Research, Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA. e-mail: cmoss@psyc.umd.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.