Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 03 July 2024
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Age-related reduction in anxiety and neural encoding of negative emotional memory

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 2St. Luke’s Medical Center, Quezon City, Metro Manila, Philippines
  • 3VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, United States
  • 4Department of Medicine, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 5Department of Neuroscience, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 6Wu Tsai Institute, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States

Introduction: Older adults experience less anxiety. We examined how memory of negative emotional images varied with age and may reflect age-related differences in anxiety.

Methods: Fifty-one adults, age 22-80 years, underwent imaging with a memory task where negative and neutral images were displayed pseudo-randomly. They were queried post-scan about the images inter-mixed with an equal number of images never displayed. Sensitivity (d’) and reporting bias (Z-score of false alarm rate; Z[FAR]) were quantified with signal detection theory.

Results: Age was negatively correlated with both Spielberg State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) state score and d’ (negative – neutral) and positively with Z[FAR] (negative – neutral). However, STAI score and d’ or Z[FAR] (negative – neutral) were not significantly correlated. In whole-brain regression, STAI score was correlated with higher activity of the right middle/superior temporal gyri/temporal parietal junction (MTG/STG/TPJ) for “negative correct – incorrect” – “neutral correct – incorrect” trials. Further, the MTG/STG/TPJ activity (β) was also negatively correlated with age. Mediation analyses supported a complete mediation model of age → less anxiety → less MTG/STG/TPJ β.

Discussion: Together, the findings demonstrated age-related changes in negative emotional memory and how age-related reduction in anxiety is reflected in diminished temporoparietal cortical activities during encoding of negative emotional memory.

1 Introduction

Old versus younger adults focus more on positive than negative stimuli in emotion processing – an age-related positivity effect (Mather and Carstensen, 2005). For instance, older adults exhibited an attentional bias away from negative emotional faces in a dot probe task, while younger adults did not present such a bias (Mather and Carstensen, 2003). Similarly, in another study using dot-probe task, older and young adults were more responsive to training to direct their gaze toward positive and negative emotional pictures, respectively (Isaacowitz and Choi, 2011). Older adults in a “bad” mood looked towards positive and away from negative faces, while younger adults demonstrated mood-congruent gaze (Isaacowitz et al., 2008). Confirmed in a meta-analysis of 100 studies (Reed et al., 2014), the positivity effect is often interpreted within the socioemotional selectivity theory where shorter time horizon prompts a shift towards positive emotions as one ages.

Further, emotion influences memory, an effect that may vary across the lifespan. As one grows older, emotion becomes relatively more salient (Carstensen and Turk-Charles, 1994) and older adults appear to encode and recall emotion-laden information as well as their younger counterparts, despite an overall decline in cognitive capacity (Mather, 2004), likely because the core emotion processing circuit suffer less age-related changes (Mather, 2016).

Considering age-related positivity in emotional memory, an earlier study demonstrated evidence in support of positivity (Charles et al., 2003). However, other studies showed that emotionally negative as compared to positive events are remembered in greater contextual details in both young and old, and the accuracy of recall did not vary with age (Kensinger et al., 2002, 2007a). In a more recent work, Stam and colleagues demonstrated enhanced memory for negative (angry) versus neutral faces in young but not older adults, with the latter showing shorter eye fixation on angry versus neutral faces, possibly indicating avoidance of negative emotional stimuli (Stam et al., 2022). In another study, young and older adults had similarly enhanced memory for fearful relative to neutral faces and consistent amygdala and hippocampus activations, though young and older adults showed higher activations in the right amygdala/bilateral hippocampus and in the insula/prefrontal cortices, respectively, during fearful versus neutral face encoding (Fischer et al., 2010). Kensinger and Schacter noted poor recognition memory in older than young adults for both the negative and neutral images (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008). However, both age groups exhibited better recognition for negative versus neutral images (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008).

Imaging studies likewise presented less than consistent results. For instance, the aforementioned study noted no age differences in the neural correlates of negative versus neutral images encoding (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008). Both young and older adults showed ventromedial and dorsomedial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex as well as hippocampus activation during the encoding of negative or positive versus neutral pictures (Addis et al., 2010). However, collapsing across emotional and neutral pictures, older and young adults each exhibited more pronounced frontal and temporal cortical activity (Addis et al., 2010). Another study noted greater negative versus neutral picture memory in both young and older adults, but overall reduced memory enhancement of regional activities for negative versus neutral pictures recalled in older versus young adults (St Jacques et al., 2009). The latter study also noted differential activity during encoding of negative versus neutral pictures between the age groups – young versus older adults had greater activity in superior/middle frontal gyrus, hippocampus/parahippocampal gyrus, visual cortex and left amygdala, whereas older versus young adults had greater activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and right amygdala (St Jacques et al., 2009). Thus, the extant findings appear to vary in the effects of age on emotional memory and the underlying neural processes, perhaps because of the characteristics of the emotional stimuli (e.g., face vs. non-face) and demand (e.g., instruction about the need to recall), of the memory task (Mather and Carstensen, 2005). Here, we revisit the issue of positivity effect and assessed potential age-related differences in the neural correlates of emotional memory.

A state of heightened worry or fear, anxiety can impact the way individuals remember negative information. For instance, individuals with higher anxiety demonstrated better performance in recalling threatening relative to non-threatening words, and the difference was associated with state anxiety scores (Reidy and Richards, 1997). Words of negative versus positive emotions or learned in negative versus neutral emotional context appeared better remembered in individuals of high versus those of low anxiety (Lee and Fernandes, 2018; Yu et al., 2018; Hakamata et al., 2022). Individuals with general anxiety disorder compared to controls had less activation in the middle cingulate gyrus and precentral gyrus during negative word encoding (Park et al., 2022). In monozygotic twins of high versus low risk for anxiety, encoding of correctly recognized negative words versus baseline led to enhanced amygdala and occipital cortical, and diminished hippocampal activity (Wolfensberger et al., 2008). Thus, anxiety may impact encoding of emotional stimuli. However, it is not clear how anxiety affects age-related variations in emotional memory encoding. For instance, older adults may show a general positivity bias in memory, while anxiety could potentially modulate and/or reflect this bias, with accompanying neural processes of emotion memory.

Here we tested these effects in 51 participants 22–80 years of age. Participants were engaged in a memory task where emotional and neutral stimuli were displayed pseudo-randomly during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and queried post-fMRI whether they remembered seeing the stimuli presented during imaging along with a same number of new stimuli never presented. Individuals’ anxiety was assessed using the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, 1989). We calculated encoding sensitivity according to signal detection theory (SDT) as correct recognition controlling for false-positive or bias, the tendency in identifying a stimulus as present that was actually not presented (Yu et al., 2018). According to the age-related positivity effect, we hypothesized diminishing sensitivity in encoding negative relative to neutral emotional stimuli with older age and explored the corresponding age-related neural correlates during encoding. Next, we would also examine the neural processes of individual anxiety during emotion memory encoding and identified shared correlates of age and anxiety. With the shared correlates, we would employ mediation analyses to examine how age, anxiety, and the neural correlates of negative emotional memory were inter-related. As the participants did not undergo fMRI while they were queried about the images, the current study could not address the neural correlates of memory recall or of inter-subject variation in reporting bias.

2 Participants and methods

2.1 Participants and clinical assessments

We determined the sample size based on previous studies that assessed the effects of aging on emotional memory encoding, with n = 30–37 participants (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008; St Jacques et al., 2009; Addis et al., 2010). We screened 70 volunteers from the greater New Haven, Connecticut area. The inclusion/exclusion criteria included (1) all participants needed to be physically healthy, with no major medical conditions and score ≥ 27 in Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE); (2) those with current use of prescription medications or with a history of head injury or neurological illness were excluded; (3) those who met criteria of current or history of Axis I disorders according to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al., 1996) or reported current use of illicit substances or tested positive for cocaine, methamphetamine, opioids, marijuana, barbiturates, or benzodiazepines were excluded. Post-screening, we invited 62 volunteers for study participation. Among the 62, 2 were excluded because of MMSE score < 27, suggesting cognitive impairment (Salis et al., 2023), 4 could not complete the MR scan, and 5 with insufficient number of events in each fMRI conditions (see Section 2.4). Thus, we presented the data from 51 healthy adults (23 women) 22 to 80 years of age (Table 1). These participants were assessed with the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, 1989). The STAI state score ranged from 20 to 63 with a mean ± SD of 32.80 ± 10.54 in the current sample. Men and women did not differ in STAI state score (t = 1.33, p = 0.189), and differed marginally in age (t = 2.00, p = 0.050). The Human Investigation Committee at Yale School of Medicine approved the study procedures. All participants signed an informed consent prior to the study.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information.

2.2 Behavioral paradigm and performance metrics

We scanned participants during encoding of negative emotional and neutral pictures selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) image set (Figure 1). Prior to MRI and again at the beginning of task during MRI, participants were instructed to remember the pictures and that they would be tested for how well they did after the scan. Forty negative emotional (normative valence = 2.72 ± 0.83; normative arousal = 5.67 ± 0.73) and 40 neutral (normative valence = 5.26 ± 0.89; normative arousal = 3.83 ± 0.83) pictures were presented in pseudorandomized order each for 4 s, with an intertrial interval (blank screen) 8 to 16 s in duration to provide jittering of the stimuli. The total task duration was approximately 8 min. We presented the task in two sessions, each with a different set of pictures.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Behavioral paradigm. Forty emotional and 40 neutral pictures were obtained from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) image set and presented in pseudorandomized order for 4 s each with an intertrial interval (ITI: blank screen) of a variable duration, ranging between 8 and 16 s to provide jitter. The total task duration was approximately 8 min. We presented the task in two sessions, each with a different set of images. Following the MR scan, participants were shown the same along with an equal number of new images and asked whether the images were presented before in the scanner or not.

The task was presented using a Dell Desktop (Windows 10; NVIDIA GeForce GT 1030, resolution: 1024 × 768; active signal resolution: 1024 × 768; refresh rate: 60 Hz) with Presentation Software (version 22.1 Build 04.30.21 by Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.). The stimuli were projected onto an Optoma projector positioned at the front of the magnet bore opening, approximately 150 cm from the subject’s eyes. Subjects viewed this display through an inverted mirror placed on the head coil, positioned directly above their eyes.

Following the MR scan, participants were presented a picture one at a time from the entire set of pictures shown, interleaved with an equal number of new pictures (new negative picture normative valence = 2.72 ± 0.83, new negative picture normative arousal = 5.67 ± 0.73; new neutral picture normative valence = 5.34 ± 1.11, new neutral picture normative arousal = 3.86 ± 0.82) never shown during the MR scan, and queried whether the pictures were presented before. Participants answered yes or no to each picture.

According to signal detection theory (SDT) (Yu et al., 2018), the four possible outcomes for each trial/picture were: hits, misses, false alarm (FA), and correct rejection (CR). A hit and miss each represent an accurate and failed recognition that the picture was presented during the MR scan. An FA is false recognition of a picture never presented as presented and CR is correct recognition that such a picture was not presented. We computed the probability of hit rate (HR) and false alarm rate (FAR) as follows: P(HR) = n(hit)/[n(hit) + n(miss)]; P(FAR) = n(FA)/[n(FA) + n(CR)]. The sensitivity (d’) and bias were calculated as d’ = ZHR − ZFAR; bias = ZFAR, where ZHR and ZFAR are the Z-score transforms, based on the normal distribution, of the P(HR) and P(FAR). We computed the Z-scores on the basis of negative and neutral trials combined, so the d’ and bias (ZFAR) could be contrasted between negative and neutral trials in statistical analyses.

2.3 Imaging protocol and data pre-processing

Brain images were collected using multiband imaging with a 3-Tesla MR scanner (Siemens Trio, Erlangen, Germany). Conventional T1-weighted spin echo sagittal anatomical images were acquired for slice localization. Anatomical 3D MPRAGE image were next obtained with spin echo imaging in the axial plane parallel to the AC–PC line with TR = 1900 ms, TE = 2.52 ms, bandwidth = 170 Hz/pixel, field of view = 250 × 250 mm, matrix = 256 × 256, 176 slices with slice thickness = 1 mm and no gap. Functional, blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals were acquired with a single-shot gradient echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence in two-sessions. Fifty-one axial slices parallel to the AC–PC line covering the whole brain were acquired with TR = 1,000 ms, TE = 30 ms, bandwidth = 2,290 Hz/pixel, flip angle = 62°, field of view = 210 × 210 mm, matrix = 84 × 84, 51 slices with slice thickness = 2.5 mm and no gap, 480 volumes, and multiband acceleration factor = 3. Images from the first ten TRs at the beginning of each scan were discarded to ensure that only BOLD signals in steady-state equilibrium between RF pulsing and relaxation were included in data analyses.

Data were analyzed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, Welcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, United States), following our published routines (Chaudhary et al., 2023b). Images of each individual subject were first realigned (motion corrected) and corrected for slice timing. A mean functional image volume was constructed for each subject per run from the realigned image volumes. These mean images were co-registered with the high-resolution structural image and segmented for normalization with affine registration followed by nonlinear transformation. The normalization parameters determined for the structure volume were then applied to the corresponding functional image volumes for each subject. The resampled voxel size is 2.5 × 2.5 × 2.5 mm3. Finally, the images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm at Full Width at Half Maximum.

2.4 Imaging data: group analyses

We separated the negative and neutral trials according to the answers – “yes, I have seen the image” or “no, I have not seen the image” – to post-scan queries and categorized trials as negative-correct, negative-incorrect, neutral-correct, and neutral-incorrect. A statistical analytical design was constructed for each individual subject, using a general linear model (GLM) with the onsets of the event for each trial convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) and with the temporal derivatives of the canonical HRF and entered as regressors in the model (Friston et al., 1994). Realignment parameters in all six dimensions were also entered in the model. Serial autocorrelation caused by aliased cardiovascular and respiratory effects was corrected by a first-degree autoregressive or AR (1) model. The GLM estimated the component of variance explained by each of the regressors.

Following previous studies of memory encoding (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008; Spaniol et al., 2009; St Jacques et al., 2009; Fischer et al., 2010; Clemens et al., 2015; Dennis and Turney, 2018), we contrasted (negative-correct – negative-incorrect) versus (neutral-correct – neutral-incorrect) to identify the neural correlates specific to successful encoding of negative versus neutral emotional images. This contrast allows assessment of a direct association between brain-activity and performance specific to negative emotion memory (Dolcos et al., 2012). All subjects had 7 or more events in each of the 4 conditions (negative-correct, negative-incorrect, neutral-correct – neutral-incorrect), ensuring a sufficient number of events in each condition to observe a significant effect (Fischer et al., 2010).

To identify age-related and anxiety-related correlates, we performed a whole-brain linear regression of the contrast “(negative-correct – negative-incorrect) – (neutral-correct – neutral-incorrect)” on age and STAI state anxiety score, respectively. We evaluated the results at voxel p < 0.005, uncorrected in combination with cluster p < 0.05, FWE-corrected. For the clusters that showed a significant correlation with age, we checked for the correlation of their activities (β’s) with STAI state score. Likewise, for the clusters that showed a significant correlation with STAI state score, we checked for the correlation of their β’s with age.

2.5 Mediation analyses

For the clusters with activities of “(negative-correct – negative-incorrect) – (neutral-correct – neutral-incorrect)” showing a significant correlation with both age and STAI state score, we performed mediation analyses to characterize the inter-relationships of these variables (See Results). We performed mediation analyses as with our previous studies (Chaudhary et al., 2023a,b) and described in the Supplemental methods. The analysis was performed with package ‘Medsem’ (Mehmetoglu, 2018) in Stata: Statistical software for data science (StataCorp LLC).

3 Results

3.1 Age, behavioral performance, and clinical characteristics

The mean ± standard deviation (SD) of hit rate (HR), false alarm rate (FAR), sensitivity d’, and bias Z(FAR) are shown in Figure 2A. Participants showed higher bias or Z(FAR) (t = 2.43, p = 0.019, paired t-test) and indistinguishable sensitivity d’ (t = 1.05, p = 0.296, paired t test) in recognizing negative versus neutral emotional images.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Hit rate or HR (%), false alarm rate or FAR (%), sensitivity d’, and bias Z(FAR) of negative and neutral images shown in bar plots of mean ± SD; (B) d’ (negative - neutral) and Z(FAR) (negative - neutral) were significantly correlated with age, but not with anxiety, with sex as covariate. Note: the data points in scatterplots represent values of the residuals (res.).

Across subjects, the differences in Z(FAR) [negative Z(FAR) – neutral Z(FAR); r = 0.31, p = 0.025] and in d’ (negative d’ – neutral d’; r = −0.44, p = 0.001) in recalling negative versus neutral images were both significantly correlated with age, with sex as a covariate (Figure 2B). Thus, age was associated with higher bias but lower sensitivity in recognizing negative relative to neutral images.

Participants showed a STAI state score of 32.8 ± 10.5 (mean ± SD). STAI state score was significantly and negatively correlated with age, with sex as a covariate (r = −0.52, p < 0.001). STAI state score was not significantly correlated with the difference in d’ (negative d’ – neutral d’; r = 0.15, p = 0.309) or with the difference in Z(FAR) [negative Z(FAR) − negative Z(FAR); r = −0.11, p = 0.452] in a linear regression with sex as a covariate (Figure 2B). Thus, individual variation in state anxiety was not significantly associated with the bias or sensitivity in recognizing negative emotional relative to neutral images.

3.2 Neural correlates of emotional memory

To identify the correlates specific to negative emotional memory, we contrasted “negative-correct – negative-incorrect” with “neutral-correct – neutral-incorrect” and showed the regional activities in Figure 3A. Two clusters each of the left insula/inferior precentral gyrus/superior temporal gyrus (INS/iPrCG/STG; x = −43, y = −1, z = 18, Z = 3.95, 335 voxels) and right superior temporal gyrus/temporo-parietal junction (STG/TPJ; x = 43, y = −34, z = 6, Z = 3.86, 424 voxels) showed lower activation during correctly versus incorrectly recognized emotional images, as compared to the same of neutral images. At the same threshold, no regional activities were significant for the reverse contrast. The mean brain activations for all relevant contrasts are presented in the Supplementary Figures S1, S2 and Supplementary Tables S1, S2.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Regional correlates of “negative_correct - negative_incorrect” (neg_corr - neg_incorr) versus “neutral_correct - neutral_incorrect” (neu_corr - neu_incorr). Color bars show voxel T value of (neg_corr - neg_incorr) < (neu_corr - neu_incorr); the reverse contrast did not show any significant voxels. (B,C) Each shows the regional correlates of age and anxiety regression on (neg_corr - neg_incorr) – (neu_corr - neu_incorr). Warm/cool color bar each shows the T values of voxels in positive/negative correlation. Voxel p < 0.005, uncorrected.

3.3 Age- and anxiety-related neural correlates of emotional memory

To identify age-related correlates of negative emotional memory, we performed a whole brain regression of the contrast “(negative_correct - negative_incorrect) – (neutral_correct - neutral_incorrect)” on age, with sex as a covariate, and showed the regional activities in Figure 3B. Only the cluster that included the superior parietal cortex and precuneus (SPC/PCu) met cluster p < 0.05, FWE-corrected (x = 3, y = −59, z = 53, Z = 3.72, 437 voxels). We extracted the β value of “(negative_correct - negative_incorrect) – (neutral_correct - neutral_incorrect)” of the SPC/PCu cluster for testing in mediation analysis, and correlation with clinical variables. As expected, the β value of right SPC/PCu was significantly and negatively correlated with age, with sex as a covariate (r = −0.49, p < 0.001; Figure 4A). The β value of the SPC/PCu cluster was also significantly and positively correlated with anxiety score (r = 0.33, p = 0.018; Figure 4B), and with d’(negative-neutral) (r = 0.35, p = 0.013), with sex as a covariate. We did not test for correlation with the reporting bias Z(FAR) as these did not represent neural activities during memory encoding.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. SPC/PCu β (A,B) and MTG/STG/TPJ β (C,D) were significantly correlated with age and anxiety, with sex as covariate; (E,F) significant mediation models with SPC/PCu β or MTG/STG/TPJ β, age and state. Scatterplots present data in residuals (res.). The two values noted for each mediation path represent the coefficient and p value, respectively. Shaded panel highlighted the only mediation model that was significant when age was considered solely as an independent variable.

To identify anxiety-related correlates of negative emotional memory, we performed a whole brain regression of the difference in sensitivity “(negative_correct - negative_incorrect) – (neutral_correct - neutral_incorrect)” on STAI score, with sex as a covariate, and showed the regional activities in Figure 3C. Four clusters met cluster p < 0.05, FWE-corrected: the left parahippocampal gyrus and part of the cerebellar cortex (x = −23, y = −56, z = −12, Z = 4.15, 1,541 voxels), right middle/superior temporal gyri and temporo-parietal junction (MTG/STG/TPJ; x = 58, y = −44, z = 1, Z = 4.19, 626 voxels), cuneus and precuneus (x = −3, y = −79, z = 48, Z = 3.83, 472 voxels), and the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas (x = −13, y = 12, z = 51, Z = 4.00, 418 voxels). Of these four clusters, only the right MTG/STG/TPJ showed a β contrast in significant correlation with age, with sex as a covariate (r = −0.33, p = 0.018; p’s > 0.07 for other clusters) (Figure 4C). Right MTG/STG/TPJ β (r = 0.32, p = 0.022) also correlated positively with d’ (negative-neutral) with sex as a covariate.

3.4 Mediation analysis of age, anxiety, and neural correlates of negative emotional memory

As presented in the previous section, we observed 3-way pair-wise correlations among age, STAI score, and the β “(negative_correct - neutral_correct) – (negative_incorrect - neutral_incorrect)” of the SPC/PCu identified from age regression (Figures 4A,B) and of the right MTG/STG/TPJ identified from STAI score regression (Figures 4C,D). We ran mediation analyses with sex as a covariate to further investigate the inter-relationship amongst these clinical and neural measures. For each cluster, we tested all six models and evaluated the results at a corrected p < 0.05/6 = 0.0083. It’s important to note that, while all six mediation models were tested, our focus was specifically on models with age as the independent variable. For SPC/PCu, a significant mediation was noted in model β → age → anxiety (Figure 4E), whereas for the right MTG/STG/TPJ, mediation analyses showed a significant model with age → anxiety → β value (Figure 4F), both with sex as a covariate. The statistics of all other models are shown in Supplementary Tables S3A,B. If we considered solely models where age served as an independent variable, only the model age → anxiety → MTG/STG/TPJ β was significant.

4 Discussion

We observed that age was associated with both higher reporting bias but lower sensitivity in recognizing negative versus neutral images. That is, older versus young people were more likely to have incorrectly reported recognizing negative versus neutral images, although they fared worse in encoding and/or retrieving memory. Age was also negatively correlated with anxiety. However, across participants STAI anxiety score was not correlated with either reporting bias or sensitivity in recognizing negative versus neutral images. These findings suggest that, with correction for reporting bias, age is associated both with impaired encoding/retrieval of negative versus neutral emotional images and with diminished anxiety. However, this impairment in negative emotional memory cannot explain the age-related changes in anxiety.

Whole-brain regression showed that activity of the right superior parietal cortex/precuneus (SPC/PCu) and right middle/superior temporal gyri (MTG/STG) and temporoparietal junction (TPJ) during successful encoding of negative versus neutral images decreased and increased with age and anxiety, respectively. Further, anxiety mediated the relationship between age and MTG/STG/TPJ activity, suggesting that the temporo-parietal cortical activities reflect the consequence of an anxiety state. We discussed the main findings below.

4.1 Negative emotional memory

Negative emotions can enhance both the accuracy (Kensinger, 2007; Mather and Sutherland, 2011) and/or subjective feelings of vividness or confidence for retrieved memory (Dougal and Rotello, 2007; Phelps and Sharot, 2008). Previous studies observed higher sensitivity in recalling negative versus neutral images (Anderson et al., 2006; Rimmele et al., 2011; Xie and Zhang, 2017; Yeh et al., 2021). Here, we did not observe a significant difference in sensitivity in recalling negative versus neutral images. On the other hand, we observed higher reporting bias in recalling negative versus neutral images. Indeed, while recollection was more accurate for negative than neutral scenes (Anderson et al., 2006; Yeh et al., 2021), Rimmele et al. noted enhanced subjective feeling of remembering negative versus neutral scenes without actually remembering the scene details (Rimmele et al., 2011). The other study noted item memory as enhanced under negative versus neutral conditions was also associated with elevated subjective feelings of remembering the conditions (Xie and Zhang, 2017). Together, these findings suggest the critical importance of accounting for reporting bias in evaluating the accuracy of recall.

4.2 Age and negative emotional memory

We noted age-associated reduction in sensitivity d’ in recognizing negative emotions, consistent with some (Charles et al., 2003; Leigland et al., 2004; Kensinger and Schacter, 2008; Leal et al., 2015; Stam et al., 2022) but not other (Kensinger et al., 2002, 2007a,b; Fischer et al., 2010) studies. Notably, Fischer et al., 2010 did not correct for bias when evaluating response accuracy. With bias accounted for, the memory performance would trend similarly to our findings (Hit-false alarm rate for negative emotional (fearful face): young = 0.35 ± 0.15, old = 0.24 ± 0.19; and for neutral face: young = 0.26 ± 0.19, old = 0.17 ± 0.14) (Fischer et al., 2010), suggesting an increase in false alarms with age (as we discuss next). In addition to reporting bias, a number of other factors may influence age-related differences in encoding accuracy. For instance, Kensinger et al., noted that in studies where participants passively viewed but were not instructed to memorize the pictures, young and older adults performed equally in memory (Kensinger et al., 2002, 2007a). However, in studies that required encoding, younger adults performed better than older adults (Kensinger et al., 2007b). Thus, the differences in study design and the role of active versus passive engagement in encoding may contribute to the observed discrepancies in age-related memory performance across studies. Further, although age was significantly associated with lower levels of anxiety, STAI scores were not significantly correlated with d’. This finding suggests that age-related reduction in negative emotional memory is unlikely to account for age-related reduction in anxiety. Together with the earlier reports, the present findings are consistent with age-related positivity in emotional memory, which, however, cannot explain the changes in state anxiety in this sample.

We found that age was associated with higher reporting bias during recall of negative versus neutral images. Compared to young adults, older adults have objectively less accurate event memory, but over-estimated event vividness (Folville et al., 2020, 2022). Age-related cognitive changes may have imposed on older people to rely more on familiarity-based monitoring, which along with reduced inhibition of irrelevant information, induces false memories (Devitt and Schacter, 2016). However, how emotion affects such an age-related tendency toward false memories is still not clear. Gallo et al., noted false memory to be higher in response to emotional than to neutral pictures, with older versus young adults showing higher frequency of false memory of emotional pictures (Gallo et al., 2009). In another study with emotional/neutral words presented during encoding and new emotional/neutral lure words during retrieval, both young and older adults performed equally and showed less false memory of emotional versus neutral words (Kensinger and Corkin, 2004). A possible explanation for the discrepant findings concerns the stimuli, with studies using images but not those using words showing an age effect, although depending on the context in which lure words were presented, word stimuli could elicit false memories (Garry and Wade, 2005; Smith and Hunt, 2020).

4.3 Anxiety and negative emotional memory

We did not observe a significant association between sensitivity or reporting bias with anxiety, in-line with earlier reports that did not find any differences in recall in high versus low anxiety individuals (Wolfensberger et al., 2008; van Tol et al., 2012), and in contrast with other reports of better recall of negative versus neutral emotional stimuli in high anxiety individuals (Reidy and Richards, 1997; Lee and Fernandes, 2018; Yu et al., 2018; Hakamata et al., 2022; note that only Lee and Fearnandes and Yu et al. controlled for reporting bias in evaluating accuracy in recall). Exposure to stress prior to encoding enhances encoding of emotional versus neutral stimuli (Payne et al., 2007; Hoscheidt et al., 2014). On the other hand, negative emotional states can disrupt memories; a recent meta-analysis showed that negative emotions elevated recall failures in a working memory task and that the effects were moderated by participants’ self-reported severity of negative life experiences (Xie et al., 2022). Other studies with image pairs-negative and neutral images presented in pairs of both neutral, both negative, or mixed; noted varied impacts of context on memory (Kensinger and Schacter, 2006; Bisby et al., 2016; Ritchey et al., 2019). While we cannot currently explain the absence of a behavioral association between anxiety and memory, future studies may explore these explanations. Further, despite the lack of a behavioral association, we observed a correlation between regional encoding of negative emotion and anxiety, consistent with previous studies (Wolfensberger et al., 2008; van Tol et al., 2012). This is probably because encoding may be more sensitive to anxiety, while retrieval may share a complex relationship with anxiety (Gagnon and Wagner, 2016).

4.4 Age- and anxiety- related neural correlates of negative emotional memory

Age and anxiety correlated negatively and positively, respectively, with right MTG/STG/TPJ activity during “(negative correct > incorrect) – (neutral correct > incorrect)” trials, and anxiety mediated the association between age and MTG/STG/TPJ activity. The MTG has been implicated in successful encoding of emotional memory (Murty et al., 2010; Dahlgren et al., 2020; Riegel et al., 2022). The STG is involved in social cognition (Allison et al., 2000) and emotion processing (Tseng et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2022), and the TPJ in differentiating old and new memory in memory updating (Simon et al., 2017). Previous studies have noted either no age-related changes (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008) or reduced MTG (hippocampal) activity (St Jacques et al., 2009; Fischer et al., 2010) during correct versus incorrect negative versus neutral encoding in older adults. Our findings are consistent with the study that used scenes (St Jacques et al., 2009), but not those that used objects (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008; Addis et al., 2010). This discrepancy may be due to different stimuli engaging different brain regions during processing (Preckel et al., 2019). In anxiety disorders, previous literature supported structural/functional deficits of the temporal lobe. For instance, relative to healthy participants, individuals (15–55 years age) with anxiety disorders demonstrated lower left MTG/STG volumes (van Tol et al., 2010). Another study of young adolescents noted greater right MTG volume in individuals scoring higher in social anxiety (Wang et al., 2021). A meta-analysis of structural and resting-state functional connectivity identified altered amygdala-MTG/STG connectivity in anxiety disorders (Cao et al., 2021). Exposure to stressful versus neutral stimuli led to higher magnetoencephalographic activity in the medial temporal lobe, especially during memory encoding (Heinbockel et al., 2021). Collectively, diminished anxiety with age may dampen negative emotion encoding in MTG.

Age and anxiety also correlated negatively and positively, respectively, with SPC/PCu activity in the contrast of “(negative correct > incorrect) – (neutral correct > incorrect)” trials. Along with the MTG, the parietal cortex is implicated in threat detection and encoding as well as hypervigilance and excessive monitoring of environment in the pathophysiology of anxiety (Carlsson et al., 2006). Higher SPC activity during anticipation of an unpredictable aversive event was associated with subjective anxiety scores in healthy participants (Hasler et al., 2007). Individuals with social anxiety disorder demonstrated higher SPC activity in processing threatening versus neutral visual scenes (Goldin et al., 2009; Heitmann et al., 2016). As the hub of default mode network and in support of self-reflection and self-focused thinking (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), the precuneus is also implicated in anxiety. The precuneus showed elevated amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in individuals with social anxiety as compared to controls (Yuan et al., 2018). An earlier meta-analysis noted precuneus hyperactivity during processing of negative versus neutral (and positive) emotional stimuli in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (Etkin and Wager, 2007). Importantly, a broad literature highlighted age-related changes in precuneus activity in a wide range of behavioral tasks. For instance, while watching a video and responding to each meaningful event, which may be important for episodic memory retrieval, the PCu amongst other posterior medial brain regions showed higher activity and this increase in activity became less prominent with age (Reagh et al., 2020). Our findings are thus consistent with this literature. Further, the SPC/Pcu activity was positively correlated with both anxiety score and negative versus neutral d’ (sensitivity), though mediation models failed to substantiate the relationship amongst [age, SPC/PCu β, and d’] (data not shown) or amongst [age, SPC/PCu, and anxiety] (model in support of SPC/PCu β → age → anxiety; Figure 4E). A larger sample size is needed to revisit this issue.

4.5 Reduced temporoparietal cortical and insula activity in negative emotional memory

Averaging across all participants, we observed lower left insula/iPrCG/STG and right STG/TPJ responses to successfully versus unsuccessfully remembered negative versus neutral images. The insula supports visceral/interoceptive, sensorimotor, and homeostatic processing as well as emotional awareness and response selection (Nieuwenhuys, 2012). In the context of memory encoding, a recent animal study noted enhanced consolidation of object memory by the basolateral amygdala through suppression of insular cortical activity, suggesting a possible role of the insula in allocating resources for memory (Chen et al., 2022). The STG is involved in social cognition (Allison et al., 2000) and emotion processing (Tseng et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2022), and the TPJ in differentiating old and new memory in memory updating (Simon et al., 2017). A previous study noted greater STG activation during successful versus unsuccessful encoding of emotional (negative + positive) versus neutral images (Mickley Steinmetz and Kensinger, 2009). A meta-analysis observed enhanced STG activity during the encoding and PrCG activity during the retrieval of emotional items or neutral items encoded in an emotional context (Dahlgren et al., 2020). In our study, we observed an overall reduction in activity, which may be attributed to the inclusion of subjects across the lifespan. Notably, a previous study that similarly encompassed subjects across the lifespan reported reduced activity in the PrCG and STG during the processing of emotional compared to neutral items (Daley et al., 2020). Nevertheless, further investigation of this issue is warranted.

4.6 Limitations and conclusions of the study

A number of limitations need to be considered. First, we focused here only on negative and neutral emotional memory. Investigating the neural processes of positive emotion processing will better our understanding of the mechanisms of “positivity effect” and age- related reduction in anxiety. It is also important to note that, whereas the great majority of studies employed the contrast “negative correct - neutral correct” to identify the correlates of memory, we considered incorrect trials in the contrast to control for the perceptual processes, as with several earlier studies (Kensinger and Schacter, 2008; Spaniol et al., 2009; St Jacques et al., 2009; Fischer et al., 2010; Clemens et al., 2015; Dennis and Turney, 2018). The specific contrasts used would dictate the results and may account for the discrepancy between the current and some of the previous findings. Second, we did not scan subjects during the retrieval phase. Previous studies have shown common and distinct regions involved in encoding and retrieval (Prince et al., 2005; Dahlgren et al., 2020), both of which may be altered by age (Srokova et al., 2022) and anxiety (Wolfensberger et al., 2008). Imaging data collected during recall would help in elucidating the neural correlates of reporting bias and how neural processes of encoding and recall may be related. Third, the STAI state score ranged from 20 to 63 in the current sample. Participants with higher state score are needed to better understand the effects of anxiety as well as the impact of age on anxiety, with the caveat that most people with the highest state scores have a clinical diagnosis of anxiety and depressive disorders and typically receive mediations for their conditions. Our sample size is moderate; studies of a larger sample may allow us to investigate the mediation models with an adequate power and sex differences in the roles of emotional memory in supporting age-related reduction in anxiety. Finally, not all older adults display reduced anxiety. Increased anxiety is evident in older adults with subjective cognitive decline (SCD) (Liew, 2020), mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Devier et al., 2009; Li et al., 2014; Klimova et al., 2015), and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) (Devier et al., 2009). Along with anxiety, these cohorts also display emotion processing dysfunction. For instance, individuals with AD display impaired identification of negative facial emotions, inconsistent emotional memory benefit (Brueckner and Moritz, 2009; Klein-Koerkamp et al., 2012; Mistridis et al., 2013; Cárdenas et al., 2021), and inconsistent positivity effect (Goodkind et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2015). People with MCI exhibit impaired negative emotion recognition in people with MCI (Elferink et al., 2015; Morellini et al., 2022); and those with SCD show altered facial emotion processing(Perez et al., 2022) and emotion dysregulation (Mah et al., 2021). Further, anxiety represents a risk factor of cognitive decline in the elderly (Pietrzak et al., 2012; Santabárbara et al., 2020; Gracia-García et al., 2023). Thus, impaired emotion processing and anxiety may be linked to early cognitive changes. Our study cohort includes typically healthy, cognitively intact participants without severe anxiety. Thus, the findings represent healthy aging. Future studies should target older adults with high anxiety and cognitive impairments.

In conclusion, age is associated with lower levels of anxiety and reduced sensitivity but higher reporting bias in recognizing negative emotions. The right middle temporal gyrus/temporal parietal junction/superior temporal gyrus (MTG/TPJ/STG) show activity (β) during “negative correct – incorrect” – “neutral correct – incorrect” trials each in positive and negative correlation with anxiety and with age. Mediation analyses supported a complete mediation model of age → less anxiety → less MTG/TPJ/STG. Together, the findings demonstrate age-related changes in negative emotional memory and how age-related reduction in anxiety is reflected in diminished temporal–parietal cortical activities during encoding of negative emotional memory.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Human Investigation Committee at Yale School of Medicine. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SC: Formal analysis, Project administration, Writing – original draft. SZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. YC: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. JD: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. HC: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. C-SL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The current study was supported by NIH grants R21AG067024 (C-SL), R01AG072893 (C-SL), R01CA218501 (HC), as well as a VA Merit Award CX001301 (HC). The NIH and VA are otherwise not responsible for the design of the study or data analyses and interpretation or in the decision to publish these findings.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2024.1375435/full#supplementary-material

References

Addis, D. R., Leclerc, C. M., Muscatell, K. A., and Kensinger, E. A. (2010). There are age-related changes in neural connectivity during the encoding of positive, but not negative, information. Cortex 46, 425–433. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, T., Puce, A., and McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 267–278. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01501-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, A. K., Yamaguchi, Y., Grabski, W., and Lacka, D. (2006). Emotional memories are not all created equal: evidence for selective memory enhancement. Learn. Memory 13, 711–718. doi: 10.1101/lm.388906

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisby, J. A., Horner, A. J., Hørlyck, L. D., and Burgess, N. (2016). Opposing effects of negative emotion on amygdalar and hippocampal memory for items and associations. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 981–990. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brueckner, K., and Moritz, S. (2009). Emotional valence and semantic relatedness differentially influence false recognition in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and healthy elderly. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 268–276. doi: 10.1017/S135561770909047X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, J., Huang, Y., Hodges, S. A., Meshberg, N., and Kong, J. (2021). Identify potential neuroimaging-based scalp acupuncture and neuromodulation targets for anxiety. Brain Sci. Adv. 7, 97–111. doi: 10.26599/BSA.2021.9050011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cárdenas, J., Blanca, M. J., Carvajal, F., Rubio, S., and Pedraza, C. (2021). Emotional processing in healthy ageing, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:2770. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052770

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlsson, K., Andersson, J., Petrovic, P., Petersson, K. M., Ohman, A., and Ingvar, M. (2006). Predictability modulates the affective and sensory-discriminative neural processing of pain. NeuroImage 32, 1804–1814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., and Turk-Charles, S. (1994). The salience of emotion across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 9, 259–264. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.2.259

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, S. T., Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 310–324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudhary, S., Chen, Y., Zhornitsky, S., Le, T. M., Zhang, S., Chao, H. H., et al. (2023a). The effects of age on the severity of problem drinking: mediating effects of positive alcohol expectancy and neural correlates. Addict. Biol. 28:e13278. doi: 10.1111/adb.13278

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudhary, S., Zhang, S., Zhornitsky, S., Chen, Y., Chao, H. H., and Li, C.-S. R. (2023b). Age-related reduction in trait anxiety: behavioral and neural evidence of automaticity in negative facial emotion processing. NeuroImage 276:120207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120207

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y.-F., Song, Q., Colucci, P., Maltese, F., Siller-Pérez, C., Prins, K., et al. (2022). Basolateral amygdala activation enhances object recognition memory by inhibiting anterior insular cortex activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 119:e2203680119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2203680119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemens, B., Regenbogen, C., Koch, K., Backes, V., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., Pauly, K., et al. (2015). Incidental memory encoding assessed with signal detection theory and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:305. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlgren, K., Ferris, C., and Hamann, S. (2020). Neural correlates of successful emotional episodic encoding and retrieval: an SDM meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 143:107495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107495

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daley, R. T., Bowen, H. J., Fields, E. C., Parisi, K. R., Gutchess, A., and Kensinger, E. A. (2020). Neural mechanisms supporting emotional and self-referential information processing and encoding in older and younger adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 405–421. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, N. A., and Turney, I. C. (2018). The influence of perceptual similarity and individual differences on false memories in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 62, 221–230. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.10.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devier, D. J., Pelton, G. H., Tabert, M. H., Liu, X., Cuasay, K., Eisenstadt, R., et al. (2009). The impact of anxiety on conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 1335–1342. doi: 10.1002/gps.2263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devitt, A. L., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). False memories with age: neural and cognitive underpinnings. Neuropsychologia 91, 346–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., Denkova, E., and Dolcos, S. (2012). Neural correlates of emotional memories: a review of evidence from brain imaging studies. Psychologia 55, 80–111. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2012.80

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dougal, S., and Rotello, C. M. (2007). “Remembering” emotional words is based on response bias, not recollection. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 423–429. doi: 10.3758/BF03194083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elferink, M. W.-O., van Tilborg, I., and Kessels, R. P. C. (2015). Perception of emotions in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s dementia: does intensity matter? Transl. Neurosci. 6, 139–149. doi: 10.1515/tnsci-2015-0013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., and Wager, T. D. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1476–1488. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J. (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Google Scholar

Fischer, H., Nyberg, L., and Bäckman, L. (2010). Age-related differences in brain regions supporting successful encoding of emotional faces. Cortex 46, 490–497. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folville, A., Bahri, M. A., Delhaye, E., Salmon, E., D’Argembeau, A., and Bastin, C. (2020). Age-related differences in the neural correlates of vivid remembering. NeuroImage 206:116336. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116336

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folville, A., Simons, J. S., D’Argembeau, A., and Bastin, C. (2022). I remember it like it was yesterday: age-related differences in the subjective experience of remembering. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 29, 1223–1245. doi: 10.3758/s13423-021-02048-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J., Poline, J.-P., Frith, C. D., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1994). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 189–210. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020402

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagnon, S. A., and Wagner, A. D. (2016). Acute stress and episodic memory retrieval: neurobiological mechanisms and behavioral consequences. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1369, 55–75. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12996

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallo, D. A., Foster, K. T., and Johnson, E. L. (2009). Elevated false recollection of emotional pictures in young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 24, 981–988. doi: 10.1037/a0017545

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garry, M., and Wade, K. A. (2005). Actually, a picture is worth less than 45 words: narratives produce more false memories than photographs do. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 359–366. doi: 10.3758/BF03196385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin, P. R., Manber, T., Hakimi, S., Canli, T., and Gross, J. J. (2009). Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation during social and physical threat. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 170–180. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodkind, M. S., Sturm, V. E., Ascher, E. A., Shdo, S. M., Miller, B. L., Rankin, K. P., et al. (2015). Emotion recognition in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a new film-based assessment. Emotion 15, 416–427. doi: 10.1037/a0039261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gracia-García, P., Bueno-Notivol, J., Lipnicki, D. M., de la Cámara, C., Lobo, A., and Santabárbara, J. (2023). Clinically significant anxiety as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: results from a 10-year follow-up community study. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 32:e1934. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1934

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hakamata, Y., Mizukami, S., Izawa, S., Okamura, H., Mihara, K., Marusak, H., et al. (2022). Implicit and explicit emotional memory recall in anxiety and depression: role of basolateral amygdala and cortisol-norepinephrine interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology 136:105598. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105598

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasler, G., Fromm, S., Alvarez, R. P., Luckenbaugh, D. A., Drevets, W. C., and Grillon, C. (2007). Cerebral blood flow in immediate and sustained anxiety. J. Neurosci. 27:6313. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5369-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinbockel, H., Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Schneider, T. R., Engel, A. K., and Schwabe, L. (2021). Stress enhances emotional memory-related theta oscillations in the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiol. Stress 15:100383. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100383

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heitmann, C. Y., Feldker, K., Neumeister, P., Zepp, B. M., Peterburs, J., Zwitserlood, P., et al. (2016). Abnormal brain activation and connectivity to standardized disorder-related visual scenes in social anxiety disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1559–1572. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23120

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoscheidt, S. M., LaBar, K. S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W. J., and Nadel, L. (2014). Encoding negative events under stress: high subjective arousal is related to accurate emotional memory despite misinformation exposure. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 112, 237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., and Choi, Y. (2011). The malleability of age-related positive gaze preferences: training to change gaze and mood. Emotion 11, 90–100. doi: 10.1037/a0021551

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., Goren, D., and Wilson, H. R. (2008). Looking while unhappy: mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 19, 848–853. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02167.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A. (2007). Negative emotion enhances memory accuracy: behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 213–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J. H., and Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional memory. Emotion 2, 118–134. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., and Corkin, S. (2004). The effects of emotional content and aging on false memories. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.1.1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., and Schacter, D. L. (2007a). Effects of emotion on memory specificity in young and older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 62, P208–P215. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.4.p208

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., Gutchess, A. H., and Schacter, D. L. (2007b). Effects of aging and encoding instructions on emotion-induced memory trade-offs. Psychol. Aging 22, 781–795. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., and Schacter, D. L. (2006). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. J. Neurosci. 26, 2564–2570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241-05.2006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). Neural processes supporting young and older adults’ emotional memories. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1161–1173. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20080

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein-Koerkamp, Y., Baciu, M., and Hot, P. (2012). Preserved and impaired emotional memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Psychol. 3:331. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimova, B., Maresova, P., Valis, M., Hort, J., and Kuca, K. (2015). Alzheimer’s disease and language impairments: social intervention and medical treatment. Clin. Interv. Aging 10, 1401–1407. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S89714

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leal, S. L., Noche, J. A., and Yassa, M. A. (2015). P3-206: age-related changes in emotional memory and forgetting: gist versus detail. Alzheimers Dement. 11:711. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.06.1578

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C., and Fernandes, M. A. (2018). Emotional encoding context leads to memory bias in individuals with high anxiety. Brain Sci. 8:6. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8010006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leigland, L. A., Schulz, L. E., and Janowsky, J. S. (2004). Age related changes in emotional memory. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 1117–1124. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.10.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Hu, N., Tan, M. S., Yu, J. T., and Tan, L. (2014). Behavioral and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Biomed. Res. Int. 2014:927804. doi: 10.1155/2014/927804

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, T. M. (2020). Subjective cognitive decline, anxiety symptoms, and the risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 12:107. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00673-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W., Li, H., Lin, X., Li, P., Zhu, X., Su, S., et al. (2022). Blunted superior temporal gyrus activity to negative emotional expression after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for late-life depression. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:1001447. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.1001447

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mah, L., Vandermorris, S., Verhoeff, N. P. L. G., and Herrmann, N. (2021). Emotion regulation in mild cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 17:e055842. doi: 10.1002/alz.055842

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M. (2004). “Aging and emotional memory” in Memory and Emotion. eds. D. Reisberg and P. Hertel (New York: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Mather, M. (2016). The affective neuroscience of aging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 213–238. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033540

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol. Sci. 14, 409–415. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01455

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 114–133. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehmetoglu, M. (2018). Medsem: a Stata package for statistical mediation analysis. Int. J. Comput. Econ. Econometr. 8:63. doi: 10.1504/IJCEE.2018.10007883

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., and Kensinger, E. A. (2009). The effects of valence and arousal on the neural activity leading to subsequent memory. Psychophysiology 46, 1190–1199. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00868.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mistridis, P., Taylor, K. I., Kissler, J. M., Monsch, A. U., Kressig, R. W., and Kivisaari, S. L. (2013). Distinct neural systems underlying reduced emotional enhancement for positive and negative stimuli in early Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:939. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00939

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morellini, L., Izzo, A., Rossi, S., Zerboni, G., Rege-Colet, L., Ceroni, M., et al. (2022). Emotion recognition and processing in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 13, 13:1044385. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1044385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murty, V. P., Ritchey, M., Adcock, R. A., and LaBar, K. S. (2010). fMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 48, 3459–3469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuys, R. (2012). The insular cortex: a review. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 123–163. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00007-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S.-E., Kim, Y.-H., Yang, J.-C., and Jeong, G.-W. (2022). Comparative functional connectivity of Core brain regions between implicit and explicit memory tasks underlying negative emotion in general anxiety disorder. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 20, 279–291. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2022.20.2.279

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, J. D., Jackson, E. D., Hoscheidt, S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W. J., and Nadel, L. (2007). Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Learn. Memory 14, 861–868. doi: 10.1101/lm.743507

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, V., Garrido-Chaves, R., Zapater-Fajarí, M., Pulopulos, M. M., Barbosa, F., Hidalgo, V., et al. (2022). Deficits in facial emotional valence processing in older people with subjective memory complaints: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Psychophysiology 59:e13989. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13989

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, E. A., and Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 147–152. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00565.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietrzak, R. H., Maruff, P., Woodward, M., Fredrickson, J., Fredrickson, A., Krystal, J. H., et al. (2012). Mild worry symptoms predict decline in learning and memory in healthy older adults: a 2-year prospective cohort study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 266–275. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182107e24

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Preckel, K., Trautwein, F.-M., Paulus, F. M., Kirsch, P., Krach, S., Singer, T., et al. (2019). Neural mechanisms of affective matching across faces and scenes. Sci. Rep. 9:1492. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37163-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prince, S. E., Daselaar, S. M., and Cabeza, R. (2005). Neural correlates of relational memory: successful encoding and retrieval of semantic and perceptual associations. J. Neurosci. 25:1203. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2540-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reagh, Z. M., Delarazan, A. I., Garber, A., and Ranganath, C. (2020). Aging alters neural activity at event boundaries in the hippocampus and posterior medial network. Nat. Commun. 11:3980. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-17713-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, A. E., Chan, L., and Mikels, J. A. (2014). Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychol. Aging 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0035194

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reidy, J., and Richards, A. (1997). Anxiety and memory: a recall bias for threatening words in high anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 531–542. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00001-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riegel, M., Wierzba, M., Wypych, M., Ritchey, M., Jednoróg, K., Grabowska, A., et al. (2022). Distinct medial-temporal lobe mechanisms of encoding and amygdala-mediated memory reinstatement for disgust and fear. NeuroImage 251:118889. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118889

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimmele, U., Davachi, L., Petrov, R., Dougal, S., and Phelps, E. A. (2011). Emotion enhances the subjective feeling of remembering, despite lower accuracy for contextual details. Emotion 11, 553–562. doi: 10.1037/a0024246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritchey, M., Wang, S.-F., Yonelinas, A. P., and Ranganath, C. (2019). Dissociable medial temporal pathways for encoding emotional item and context information. Neuropsychologia 124, 66–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salis, F., Costaggiu, D., and Mandas, A. (2023). Mini-mental state examination: optimal cut-off levels for mild and severe cognitive impairment. Geriatrics 8:12. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics8010012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Santabárbara, J., Lipnicki, D. M., Olaya, B., Villagrasa, B., Bueno-Notivol, J., Nuez, L., et al. (2020). Does anxiety increase the risk of all-cause dementia? An updated Meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. J. Clin. Med. 9:1791. doi: 10.3390/jcm9061791

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, K. C. N. S., Gómez, R. L., Nadel, L., and Scalf, P. E. (2017). Brain correlates of memory reconsolidation: a role for the TPJ. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 142, 154–161. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. E., and Hunt, R. R. (2020). When do pictures reduce false memory? Mem. Cogn. 48, 623–644. doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-00995-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaniol, J., Davidson, P. S. R., Kim, A. S. N., Han, H., Moscovitch, M., and Grady, C. L. (2009). Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: Meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia 47, 1765–1779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1989). State–Trait Anxiety Inventory: A Comprehensive Bibliography Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Srokova, S., Hill, P. F., and Rugg, M. D. (2022). The retrieval-related anterior shift is moderated by age and correlates with memory performance. J. Neurosci. 42:1765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1763-21.2021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

St Jacques, P. L., Dolcos, F., and Cabeza, R. (2009). Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the amygdala for subsequent memory of negative pictures: a network analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Psychol. Sci. 20, 74–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02258.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, D., Colman, L., Vansteelandt, K., Vandenbulcke, M., and Van den Stock, J. (2022). Age effects in emotional memory and associated eye movements. Brain Sci. 12:1719. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12121719

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, W.-L., Thomas, L. A., Harkins, E., Pine, D. S., Leibenluft, E., and Brotman, M. A. (2016). Neural correlates of masked and unmasked face emotion processing in youth with severe mood dysregulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 78–88. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Tol, M.-J., Demenescu, L. R., van der Wee, N. J. A., Kortekaas, R., Marjan, M. A., Boer, J. A., et al. (2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of emotional word encoding and recognition in depression and anxiety disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 593–602. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Tol, M.-J., van der Wee, N. J. A., van den Heuvel, O. A., Nielen, M. M. A., Demenescu, L. R., Aleman, A., et al. (2010). Regional brain volume in depression and anxiety disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 1002–1011. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.121

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Zhao, Y., Wang, X., Yang, X., Cheng, B., Pan, N., et al. (2021). Emotional intelligence mediates the association between middle temporal gyrus gray matter volume and social anxiety in late adolescence. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 30, 1857–1869. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01651-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfensberger, S. P. A., Veltman, D. J., Hoogendijk, W. J. G., De Ruiter, M. B., Boomsma, D. I., and de Geus, E. J. C. (2008). The neural correlates of verbal encoding and retrieval in monozygotic twins at low or high risk for depression and anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 79, 80–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, W., Ye, C., and Zhang, W. (2022). Negative emotion reduces visual working memory recall variability: a meta-analytical review. Emotion 23, 859–871. doi: 10.1037/emo0001139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, W., and Zhang, W. (2017). Negative emotion enhances mnemonic precision and subjective feelings of remembering in visual long-term memory. Cognition 166, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, N., Payne, J. D., Kim, S. Y., Kensinger, E. A., Koen, J. D., and Rose, N. S. (2021). Medial prefrontal cortex has a causal role in selectively enhanced consolidation of emotional memories after a 24-hour delay: a TBS study. J. Neurosci. 41, 6273–6280. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2599-20.2021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, Q., Zhuang, Q., Wang, B., Liu, X., Zhao, G., and Zhang, M. (2018). The effect of anxiety on emotional recognition: evidence from an ERP study. Sci. Rep. 8:16146. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34289-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, C., Zhu, H., Ren, Z., Yuan, M., Gao, M., Zhang, Y., et al. (2018). Precuneus-related regional and network functional deficits in social anxiety disorder: a resting-state functional MRI study. Compr. Psychiatry 82, 22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.12.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, F., Ho, Y. W., and Fung, H. H. (2015). Learning from Normal aging: preserved emotional functioning facilitates adaptation among early Alzheimer’s disease patients. Aging Dis. 6, 208–215. doi: 10.14336/AD.2014.0620

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, anxiety, negative emotion, memory, fMRI

Citation: Chaudhary S, Zhang S, Chen Y, Dominguez JC, Chao HH and Li C-SR (2024) Age-related reduction in anxiety and neural encoding of negative emotional memory. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1375435. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2024.1375435

Received: 23 January 2024; Accepted: 24 June 2024;
Published: 03 July 2024.

Edited by:

Ian M. McDonough, Binghamton University, United States

Reviewed by:

Yang Zhang, Soochow University, China
Holly Hunsberger, Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science, United States

Copyright © 2024 Chaudhary, Zhang, Chen, Dominguez, Chao and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shefali Chaudhary, shefali.chaudhary@yale.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.