Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 21 November 2023
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias

Examining the role of participant and study partner report in widely-used classification approaches of mild cognitive impairment in demographically-diverse community dwelling individuals: results from the Einstein aging study

Katherine H. Chang,
Katherine H. Chang1,2*Cuiling Wang,Cuiling Wang3,4Caroline O. Nester,Caroline O. Nester1,2Mindy J. KatzMindy J. Katz4Desiree A. Byrd,Desiree A. Byrd1,2Richard B. Lipton,,Richard B. Lipton3,4,5Laura A. Rabin,,,
Laura A. Rabin1,2,4,6*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Queens College, City University of New York (CUNY), Queens, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, The Graduate Center, City University of New York (CUNY), New York, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, United States
  • 4Saul R. Korey Department of Neurology, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, United States
  • 6Department of Psychology, Brooklyn College, City University of New York (CUNY), Brooklyn, NY, United States

Objective: The role of subjective cognitive concerns (SCC) as a diagnostic criterion for MCI remains uncertain and limits the development of a universally (or widely)-accepted MCI definition. The optimal MCI definition should define an at-risk state and accurately predict the development of incident dementia. Questions remain about operationalization of definitions of self- and informant-reported SCCs and their individual and joint associations with incident dementia.

Methods: The present study included Einstein Aging Study participants who were non-Hispanic White or Black, free of dementia at enrollment, had follow-up, and completed neuropsychological tests and self-reported SCC at enrollment to determine MCI status. Informant-reported SCC at baseline were assessed via the CERAD clinical history questionnaire. Self-reported SCC were measured using the CERAD, items from the EAS Health Self-Assessment, and the single memory item from the Geriatric Depression Scale. Cox proportional hazards models examined the association of different operationalizations of SCC with Petersen and Jak/Bondi MCI definitions on the risk of dementia, further controlling for age, sex, education, and race/ethnicity. Time-dependent sensitivity and specificity at specific time points for each definition, and Youden’s index were calculated as an accuracy measure. Cox proportional hazards models were also used to evaluate the associations of combinations of self- and informant-reported SCC with the risk of incident dementia.

Results: 91% of the sample endorsed at least one SCC. Youden’s index showed that not including SCC in either Jak/Bondi or Petersen classifications had the best balance between sensitivity and specificity across follow-up. A subset of individuals with informants, on average, had a lower proportion of non-Hispanic Blacks and 94% endorsed at least one self-reported SCC. Both informant-reported and self-reported SCC were significantly associated with incident dementia.

Conclusion: Our findings suggest that the SCC criterion may not improve the predictive validity for dementia when included in widely-employed definitions of MCI. Consistent with some prior research, informant-reported SCC was more related to risk of incident dementia than self-reported SCC. Given that requiring informant report as a diagnostic criterion may unintentionally exclude health disparate groups, additional consideration is needed to determine how best to utilize informant-report in MCI diagnosis.

1 Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) represents an intermediate stage of cognitive impairment, intended to identify the transitional phase between normal aging and dementia (Petersen et al., 1999; Winblad et al., 2004; Petersen et al., 2014; Scharre, 2019). When the concept of MCI was first introduced, documentation of memory decline was largely drawn from cognitive concerns expressed during the clinical interview, with informant reports of decline and objective neuropsychological data used to corroborate abnormal memory performance (Petersen et al., 1999). Currently, subjective cognitive concern (SCC) is a core criterion for the most widely-used definition of MCI in clinical and research settings (Winblad et al., 2004; Kelley and Petersen, 2007; Albert et al., 2011; Jack et al., 2018; Kasper et al., 2020; Mayo Clinic, 2020; UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences, 2022). For other prodromal dementia conditions (e.g., subjective cognitive decline, motoric cognitive risk syndrome), research has examined the assessment of SCC in a more systematic, psychometrically validated manner with some studies using formal SCC screens including cognitive domains beyond memory (i.e., executive functioning, language, attention/concentration, visuospatial navigation, etc.) (Rabin et al., 2020; Diaz-Galvan et al., 2021; Nester et al., 2021; Wasef et al., 2021). However, the measurement of SCC, especially as a criterion for MCI classification, lacks standardization (Stephan et al., 2013; Rabin et al., 2015; Molinuevo et al., 2017).

There are a multitude of studies on mild cognitive impairment (more than 18,000 articles) with highly variable approaches for (1) how to measure SCC (i.e., clinical interview vs. questionnaire; single- vs. multiple-item questionnaire; “homegrown” questionnaires vs. standardized/validated questionnaires; assessing multiple domains vs. only memory; capturing current ability versus change); (2) optimal sources of the SCC report (i.e., self-, informant-, clinician-report); (3) whether corroboration from informant is required; or (4) how to capture and quantify SCC (i.e., any concern, use of a cutoff score or percentage, median or average score, age-appropriate normative scores) (Stephan et al., 2013; Rabin et al., 2015). Further systematic review of how SCC has been measured for the diagnosis of MCI is warranted but beyond the scope of this paper. Supplementary Table 1 provides a sampling to demonstrate the extensive variability in the field but is not intended to be exhaustive. A large proportion of studies list SCC as a criterion for MCI diagnosis, citing Petersen/Winblad’s original definition, but do not provide specific information on how SCC was defined. Some studies do not include SCC as a criterion. Others describe the measure(s) used but do not provide information on the nature of the measurement, source of information, or precise operational definitions of SCC. In studies using SCC questionnaires, items also vary in their referents (relative to previous state personal cognitive status or age-matched mates), time periods (now, over the past year, over the past 5 years), and response option formats (e.g., dichotomous yes/no vs. Likert-like scales for severity, frequency, degree of change).

The original rationale for the inclusion of SCC as a criterion for MCI diagnosis was to identify individuals undergoing cognitive change. Objective cognitive tests measure status, but unless baseline measures are available, they do not capture change. At baseline, cognitive tests may flag individuals with long-standing cognitive difficulties (e.g., learning disabilities, neurodevelopmental disorders, etc.) as impaired (Petersen, 2004). For individuals with above average intellectual abilities, cross-sectional cognitive tests may not capture early stages of cognitive decline if scores fall within normal limits. Despite the compelling rationale, the predictive validity of SCC for incident dementia remains uncertain. For the original Petersen criteria, SCC needed to be self-reported (Petersen et al., 1999), requiring an individual to possess a level of awareness and a willingness to acknowledge memory changes. Though self-reported SCC are associated with objective cognitive performance, they are also associated with depression and anxiety (Buckley et al., 2013; Edmonds et al., 2014; Ryu et al., 2016; Yates et al., 2017; Topiwala et al., 2021; Scholz and Donders, 2022), resulting in overreporting of SCC from cognitively unimpaired individuals (Yates et al., 2017; Edmonds et al., 2018). Many studies show that individuals with MCI demonstrate anosognosia or poor awareness of their own cognitive and functional deficits (Vogel et al., 2004; Edmonds et al., 2014; Fragkiadaki et al., 2016; Gerretsen et al., 2017; Bastin et al., 2021; Ilardi et al., 2021), resulting in underreporting of cognitive changes (Edmonds et al., 2018; Ryu et al., 2019). Further, anosognosia independently predicts conversion from MCI to dementia and is associated with biomarkers, such as reduced brain metabolism (Gerretsen et al., 2017), suggesting that the absence of anosognosia (i.e., insight and SCC) may actually be clinically useful to identify individuals who are unlikely to convert from MCI to dementia (Gerretsen et al., 2017; Bastin et al., 2021). This appears to be contradictory to SCC being a core criterion for defining MCI, and as such, some research efforts support the removal of SCC (Lenehan et al., 2012; Edmonds et al., 2014, 2018; Hackett et al., 2020) on the basis that they do not meaningfully contribute to diagnosis or may even contribute to misdiagnosis of MCI (Vogel et al., 2004; Edmonds et al., 2014; Fragkiadaki et al., 2016; Ilardi et al., 2021). Consistent with this approach and with the various problematic aspects of requiring SCC as a criterion in MCI, Jak, Bondi, and colleagues introduced an approach to MCI classification that uses comprehensive neuropsychological criteria without SCC (Jak et al., 2009; Bondi et al., 2014).

Recent reports suggest that SCC can be assessed by an individual or knowledgeable informant, including clinicians, friends, or family members (Albert et al., 2011; Tangalos and Petersen, 2018). Some research supports that informant-reported SCC or mutual report from both the individual and informant better correlate with an individual’s objective cognitive performance and may better predict progression to dementia than only self-reported SCC (Tierney et al., 1996; Rabin et al., 2012; Gifford et al., 2015; Edmonds et al., 2018; Numbers et al., 2023; Peng et al., 2023). Some investigators address the differences in self- and informant-report with a discrepancy score that may correlate well with objective cognitive impairment (Edmonds et al., 2018) and is a validated approach to measure anosognosia (Starkstein et al., 2006)—which may be clinically useful to improve identification of progression from MCI to dementia. However, informant-reported SCC still has its limitations and can be affected by factors, including type and quality of relationship, frequency of contact, expectations, and affective states of both individual and/or informant (Jessen et al., 2014; Hackett et al., 2020). For example, there may be limited opportunity to notice changes in cognition for older adults who are socially isolated or do not see their families or physicians except for rare occasions.

Whether to include SCC as a criterion for MCI depends critically on its predictive validity for incident dementia. Much of the research on this topic has been carried out in samples of predominantly highly educated, non-Hispanic White older adults, limiting the generalizability of this work to more diverse populations. The current study investigates three operational definitions of self-reported concerns and two definitions of informant-reported concerns about cognition to determine whether their inclusion in two widely-used MCI definitions (i.e., Petersen and Jak/Bondi) add to the predictive validity for incident dementia. Using a more representative cohort of older adults than is typical for aging studies, we additionally investigated discrepancy patterns between self- and informant-reported SCC and their association with incident dementia. Based on prior research (Buckley et al., 2013; Edmonds et al., 2014; Ryu et al., 2016; Yates et al., 2017; Topiwala et al., 2021), we predict that the inclusion of any SCC endorsement (compared to no SCC or a high average cutoff) as a criterion for MCI diagnosis will be the least predictive of incident dementia. Beyond this, there is no prior literature that can specifically guide hypotheses for other operational definitions of SCC as it pertains to the diagnosis of MCI. As such, this work will be exploratory and is an important step forward toward standardizing SCC measurement in MCI.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

The EAS is a longitudinal study of community-residing individuals from the Bronx NY, which is a racially and ethnically diverse urban setting (Katz et al., 2012). Details about study recruitment have been described elsewhere (Katz et al., 2012). In brief, participants were systematically recruited using Bronx County Voter Registration lists. Individuals were mailed introductory letters and given a telephone screen to determine study eligibility. Those who met preliminary eligibility criteria were invited for further in-person evaluations. In-person assessments were conducted annually and included comprehensive neurological, medical, psychosocial, and neuropsychological evaluations. All protocols were approved by the Einstein Institutional Review Board (IRB) and written informed consent was obtained at the initial clinic visit. Inclusion criteria were age 70 and above, resident of Bronx, NY, noninstitutionalized, and English speaking. Exclusion criteria at baseline included severe audiovisual, physical impairments, or active psychiatric symptomatology, which may interfere with the ability to complete assessments (Katz et al., 2012). Participants eligible for the present study were enrolled between October 1993 and June 2016, had at least one annual follow-up, and were free of dementia at baseline and first follow-up visit.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Neuropsychological assessment

Participants completed standardized neuropsychological testing at baseline and all annual follow-up visits (Katz et al., 2012). Five cognitive domains were used for MCI diagnosis: memory, attention, executive functioning, language, and visuospatial functioning, with two tests included in each domain. The memory domain included the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT) (Buschke, 1984) and the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Logical Memory I subtest (WMS-R-LMI) (Wechsler, 1987). Attention/processing speed was measured using the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (Wechsler, 1997) and the Trail Making Test, part A (Reitan, 1958). Executive functioning tests included Trail Making Test, part B (Reitan, 1958) and the Letter Fluency “FAS” task (Spreen and Strauss, 2006). Language was measured with the Category Fluency task (animals, vegetables, fruits) (Rosen, 1980) and the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al., 1983). Visuospatial functioning tests included the Block Design and Digit Symbol subtest from the WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997).

Normative data were calculated using local norms derived by cognitively unimpaired individuals in the sample. Participants were classified as CU if they had: (1) no significant SCC, measured by no endorsement of any item on three SCC questionnaires (detailed below); (2) unimpaired self-reported ADL as measured by the IADL Lawton Brody Scale (Lawton and Brody, 1969); and (3) cognitive functioning within normal limits as defined by having two out of three of the following at baseline: (i) a global score of 0 (“normal cognition”) on the CDR® Dementia Staging Instrument (CDR®, Morris, 1997), (ii) a score of 3 or lower on the Blessed Information–Memory–Concentration Test (BIMC; Blessed et al., 1968), or (iii) a score of 5 or greater on the Memory Impairment Screen (MIS; Buschke et al., 1999).

2.2.2 Subjective cognition assessment

2.2.2.1 Self

Twenty-two items derived from three questionnaires were used to assess self-perceived cognitive functioning: 17 items from the CERAD clinical history questionnaire (Morris et al., 1989), a yes/no/do not know rating scale of current functioning in several cognitive domains; four items from the EAS Health Self-Assessment (HSA; Derby et al., 2013) that inquired about current memory problems and changes in memory compared to 1 to 10 years prior to the baseline assessment (ordinal data 3 to 4 response options); and the dichotomous memory item from the short form of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): “Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?” (Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986).

2.2.2.2 Informant

Informant perceptions of participants’ cognition were collected from 17 corresponding items from the CERAD (informant form), a yes/no/do not know rating scale of participants’ current cognitive functioning.

2.3 MCI classification and operationalization of SCC

MCI classifications were made using two widely-used MCI criteria: (1) updated Petersen criteria (Winblad et al., 2004; Artero et al., 2006) requiring (a) objective memory impairment (>1.5 SD below the age-, sex-, education-, and race/ethnicity-adjusted mean); (b) subjective memory impairment operationalized as any SCC indicated by self- or informant-report (measures discussed below); (c) absence of functional decline as measured by the IADL Lawton Brody scale (Lawton and Brody, 1969); and (d) no diagnosis of dementia and (2) Jak/Bondi comprehensive neuropsychological criteria (Bondi et al., 2014) requiring (a) one low score (>1 SD below the age-, sex-, education-, and race/ethnicity-adjusted mean) on both measures within at least one cognitive domain; or (b) at least one low score (>1 SD below the age-, sex-, education-, and race/ethnicity-adjusted mean) across at least two cognitive domains.

Table 1 depicts the operational definitions of SCC that were included as a criterion in MCI classifications. The definitions using self-reported SCC were as follows: (1) no inclusion of SCC; (2) any concern (at least one SCC question endorsed); and (3) a high average cutoff (Q3) of concern. Two definitions of informant-reported SCC were as follows: (1) any informant-reported concern (at least one item on the informant CERAD questionnaire was endorsed) and (2) a high average cutoff (Q3) on the informant CERAD. Of note, because Petersen MCI classification already contains a criterion for SCC, this criterion was removed for the “no inclusion of SCC” condition.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Operational definitions of self-reported SCC.

An additional analysis was conducted with individuals with informants to examine the discrepancy between self-reported and informant-reported SCC using the CERAD questionnaire. Using low levels (i.e., below the cut-off of Q3) of self- or informant-reported SCC as reference, the following combinations were used to reflect patterns of discrepancy: (1) high levels of both self- and informant-reported SCC; (2) high levels of informant-reported SCC and low self-reported SCC; and (3) high levels of self-reported SCC and low informant-reported SCC (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Combinations of self- and informant-report on the CERAD questionnaire.

2.3.1 Dementia

Incident dementia diagnosis at follow-up was the main outcome. Broadly consistent with DSM-IV for major neurocognitive disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), participants were classified as having incident dementia if all of the following criteria are met: (1) there was substantial cognitive impairment on objective measures—that is, scores at least 1.5 standard deviations below the age-adjusted mean; (2) the participant or study informant reported changes in cognitive function; (3) there was functional decline determined at a case conference based on information from self or informant report, impairment scores on the IADL Lawton Brody Scale (Lawton and Brody, 1969), and clinical evaluation; and (4) cognitive impairment was not better explained by the effects of a substance or medication.

2.4 Demographic and clinical characteristics

Demographic information from the EAS included self-reported race/ethnicity as defined by the U.S. Census Bureau in 1994 (categorized to: non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black), number of years of education, sex, and age. Subclinical symptoms of depression were assessed using the GDS short form, excluding the single memory item (Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986).

2.5 Data analysis

The time to event was defined as the time between the baseline clinic visit and the date of dementia diagnosis or the final follow-up visit when the participants were known without dementia. To evaluate the associations of five operationalizations of SCC into the MCI definitions at baseline with the risk of incident dementia, Cox proportional hazards models were applied. Because the cumulative dementia disease status is time-dependent and time to dementia can be censored, time-dependent receiver operating characteristic (ROC) based on the Cox model was used to evaluate the discriminative ability of each MCI for dementia incidence within a given time period. Specifically, the time-dependent sensitivity and specificity for cumulative disease incidence within a time interval t were defined as

Se(t)=P(X=1|Tt)=(1S(t|X=1))P(X=1)1S(t)
Sp(t)=P(X=0|T>t)=S(t|X=0)P(X=0)S(t)

Where X is MCI status at baseline, T is time to dementia, S(t|X=x), x =0,1, is the conditional survival probability given X, S(t)=S(t|X=1)P(X=1)+S(t|X=0)P(X=0) is the overall survival probability. They were estimated using estimates of the survival probabilities from the Cox model and estimate of the prevalence of MCI. Youden’s index, Se(t)+Sp(t)1, was then obtained as a compromised accuracy measure. Results at 2-, 3-, 5-, and 7-years follow-up, along with the numbers of participants at risk at the time, were reported.

Additional models controlling for covariates including age, sex, education, race/ethnicity, and depressive symptoms at baseline were also applied. Cox proportional hazards models were also used to evaluate the associations of combinations of self- and informant-reported SCC with the risk of incident dementia. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, N.C.).

3 Results

3.1 Overview

At baseline (N = 1,097), participants’ age ranged from 70 to 100 (mean = 78.6 ± 5.4) years, the sample was 62.4% female and educational achievement averaged 13.7 ± 3.5 years, with 45.1% obtaining 12 years or fewer years of education. Notably, nearly one fifth of participants (18.9%) did not complete high school, highlighting the educational diversity within the sample. Most participants identified as White (70.6%), though Black participants were well-represented (29.4%). During up to 19.6 years of follow-up (mean 4.5 years, median 3.3 years), 124 individuals developed incident dementia. As shown in Table 3, those who developed dementia during follow-up were significantly older and had fewer years of education at baseline. The groups did not differ in number of depressive symptoms, sex, race/ethnicity, or follow-up time.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Baseline descriptive characteristics of the sample by dementia status.

3.2 Relationship of self- and informant-reported SCC and risk of incident dementia

Table 4 shows the baseline descriptive characteristics for individuals with informants versus no informants. Individuals with informants, on average, had a lower proportion of non-Hispanic Blacks (25.6% versus 35.4%, p = 0.001) and a higher proportion endorsed at least one self-reported SCC (93.9% versus 87.2%, p < 0.001). Among those with informants, high average cutoff for informant-reported SCC (HR = 2.52, p = 0.003) was slightly more strongly related to incident dementia than self-reported SCC using the same questionnaire (HR = 2.03, p = 0.005), but both were significantly associated, controlling for baseline age, sex, education, and race/ethnicity (Table 5).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Baseline descriptive characteristics of the sample by informant status.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Estimates of associations of self-reported SCC and informant-reported SCC on incident dementia, adjusting for age, education, sex, and race/ethnicity.

Table 6 shows the association of combined categories of having high levels of self- and informant-report SCC (using the same questionnaire) with risk of dementia. Using low self- and informant-reported SCC as the reference, the risk of dementia was higher among those with high informant-reported SCC and high self-reported SCC (HR = 5.14, p < 0.0001) and those with high informant-reported SCC but low self-reported SCC (HR = 2.32, p = 0.017). The risk of dementia was also higher, though not significant, in those with high self-reported SCC but low informant-reported SCC (HR = 1.85, p = 0.086).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Estimates of combinations of self-reported SCC and informant-reported SCC on incident dementia, adjusting for age, education, sex, and race/ethnicity.

3.3 Time dependent ROC of MCI defined with and without SCC ascertained from the participant

Notably, 91% of the entire sample endorsed at least one self-reported SCC. Comparisons of time-dependent ROC results for various operational definitions of self-reported SCC (not including SCC, any SCC, and high average, Q3 cutoff) as a criterion in both Jak/Bondi and Petersen MCI classifications, at 2-, 3-, 5-, and 7-years of follow-up and numbers of participants at risk at the time are shown in Tables 7, 8, respectively. Youden’s index, which is determined by the sum of sensitivity and specificity, was highest in all cases for definitions that did not include SCC for both the Jak/Bondi and the Petersen classifications after 2-, 3-, 5-, and 7-years of follow-up. Though inclusion of SCC improved specificity, those gains were more than offset by loss of sensitivity.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Sensitivity, specificity, and Youden’s index for various operationalization of self-reported SCC in Jak/Bondi MCI at baseline for dementia after 2 to 7 years of follow-up.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Sensitivity, specificity, and Youden’s index for various operationalization of self-reported SCC in Petersen MCI at baseline for dementia after 2 to 7 years of follow-up.

3.4 Time dependent ROC of MCI definition with and without SCC ascertained from the participant or informant

In the subset of the sample with informants available, 94% endorsed at least one self-reported SCC. Comparisons of time-dependent ROC results for various operational definitions of self-reported SCC (not including SCC, any SCC, and high average cutoff) and informant-reported SCC (any SCC and high-average cutoff) as a criterion in both Jak/Bondi and Petersen MCI classifications are shown in Tables 9, 10, respectively. Youden’s index was highest for definitions that included any self-reported SCC for both the Jak/Bondi and the Petersen classifications after 2-, 3-, 5-, and 7-years of follow-up, followed very closely by definitions that did not include any SCC. Notably, the difference between Youden’s indices for these two definitions was less than 0.008 or less, suggesting minimal benefit of including SCC compared to no inclusion. Though inclusion of informant-reported SCC improved specificity, the gains were again more than offset by the drastically decreased sensitivity.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Sensitivity, specificity, and Youden’s index for various operationalization of self- and informant-reported SCC in Jak/Bondi MCI at baseline for dementia after 2 to 7 years of follow-up.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Sensitivity, specificity, and Youden’s index for various operationalization of self-and informant-reported SCC in Petersen MCI at baseline for dementia after 2 to 7 years of follow-up.

4 Discussion

Whether or not to include SCC as a criterion in the classification of MCI is an uncertainty in the field—but one with important implications. A research-driven approach to the inclusion, exclusion, and/or specific operationalization would be ideal to address this question; however, relatively little attention has been given to standardizing and optimizing the SCC criterion. Moreover, research examining MCI criteria in diverse and/or health disparate populations is limited, which not only restricts the generalizability of the construct, but also upholds health inequities in aging populations (Turner et al., 2022). The present study investigated three operational definitions of self-reported SCC and two operational definitions of informant-reported SCC, within the classification of MCI, in a demographically diverse cohort of older adults. Overall, results indicated that not including self-reported SCC in either Jak/Bondi or Petersen MCI classifications provided the best balance between sensitivity and specificity for incident dementia, across 2-, 3-, 5-, and 7-years of follow-up. In a subset of the sample that had informants available, not including self-reported SCC was comparable to definitions that included any self-reported SCC.

Results are consistent with a growing literature that has questioned the utility of SCC as a criterion in MCI classification, primarily on the basis that SCC may contribute to the misdiagnosis of MCI (Mitchell, 2008; Lenehan et al., 2012; Edmonds et al., 2014; Yates et al., 2017; Edmonds et al., 2018). Self-perceived SCC are present across the lifespan and typically increase with age, with approximately 11% of adults aged 45–64 years endorsing SCC and growing to 50–80% of those over the age of 70 (Mitchell, 2008; Taylor et al., 2018; CDC, 2019; Jessen et al., 2020). Extensive variability in methods of SCC assessment for aging populations (see Stephan et al., 2013; Rabin et al., 2015; Molinuevo et al., 2017) likely contributes to the difference in the prevalence of SCC between studies and highlights the need for further research toward standardizing SCC measurement. In addition to inconsistency in operationalization, there can be challenges in disentangling the numerous demographic, psychosocial, and health related factors that complicate the relationship between SCC, objective cognition, and neurodegeneration. Emerging research shows inconsistent associations between SCC and objective memory in racial/ethnic minoritized individuals (Sims et al., 2011; Jackson et al., 2017; Hughes et al., 2020; Rodriguez et al., 2021; Chapman et al., 2023; Katz et al., 2023). SCC reporting may differ (1) among racial/ethnic groups; (2) among those with different levels of formal educational attainment; or (3) due to sociocultural factors that impact the perception subjective experience, and meaning of cognitive changes (Sayegh and Knight, 2013; CDC, 2019; Hughes et al., 2020; Dilworth-Anderson and Gibson, 2022; Katz et al., 2023). Overall, additional research investigating how cultural factors such as level of education, language status, or cultural norms and values impact an individual’s understanding of their own cognitive status and how they endorse cognitive concerns. These cultural differences provide an added layer of complexity to measuring SCC in diverse individuals and using it as a reliable criterion for MCI.

Further contributing to the instability of SCC as a diagnostic criterion, cognitive concerns may evolve as a result of biopsychosocial variables, such as mood, somatic conditions, chronic pain, medication use, sleep disorders, psychosocial stress, personality factors, and/or normal aging (Bassett and Folstein, 1993; Comijs et al., 2002; Mitchell, 2008; Elfgren et al., 2010; Yates et al., 2017; Jessen et al., 2020; Jenkins et al., 2021; Scholz and Donders, 2022) and resolution of these factors may impact the course of SCC over time (Glodzik-Sobanska et al., 2007; Vestberg et al., 2010). On the other hand, subsets of older adults who endorse persistent SCC but do not show cognitive impairment on sensitive neuropsychological measures (i.e., do not meet criteria for MCI) are more likely to demonstrate objective cognitive decline in the future and are at higher risk for ultimately developing dementia (Treves et al., 2005; Jessen et al., 2014; Luck et al., 2015; Molinuevo et al., 2017; Rabin et al., 2017). It is notable, however, that many individuals who meet criteria for subjective cognitive decline (SCD), do not ultimately progress to dementia (Jessen et al., 2020) and the association of SCD with incident MCI is also not clearly understood (Warren et al., 2022). In the clinical context, a careful evaluation of SCC is essential to identify and treat any plausibly modifiable contributors and to identify older adults in need of additional work-up. Thus, as a clinical tool, SCC may be valuable during preclinical phases (i.e., when an individual’s insight remains intact and prior to objective cognitive impairment) or when the clinical question is to rule out dementia (i.e., when increased specificity is needed). However, in research settings or when individuals progress to more overt cognitive impairment, the complex, multifactorial contributors to SCC may cloud the predictive validity for dementia when SCC is included in MCI definitions.

Although the association between SCC and dementia risk is complex, our results showed both self- and informant-reported SCC was related to risk of dementia. Additionally, the risk of dementia was significantly higher among those with high levels of informant-reported SCC, regardless of the level of self-reported SCC, but the risk of dementia was not as high in those with high levels of self-reported SCC but low informant-reported SCC. This corroborates previous research showing that informant-reported SCC or mutual report better correlate with an individual’s objective cognitive performance and better predicts progression to dementia than only self-reported SCC (Tierney et al., 1996; Rabin et al., 2012; Gifford et al., 2015; Edmonds et al., 2018; Peng et al., 2023). Self-awareness of cognitive deficits and the discrepancy between self- and informant-report have a U-shaped distributions. Self-reported cognitive concerns tend to be elevated relative to informant-reported concerns in SCD (i.e., high discrepancy, Rami et al., 2014; Ryu et al., 2019), become more aligned in early MCI (i.e., low discrepancy, Rabin et al., 2017; Edmonds et al., 2018; Ryu et al., 2019), and become more discrepant as anosognosia develops in late MCI and dementia (Edmonds et al., 2014; Rueda et al., 2014; Rabin et al., 2017). Given the possible fluctuation in self-reported SCC and self-awareness of cognitive and adaptive functioning, this may explain why utilizing SCC was not additive to predicting incident dementia in our sample.

Importantly, although collateral information about an older adult’s daily functioning can be of great benefit in the clinical setting, requiring the presence of an informant may, in fact, hamper ADRD research efforts. For example, excluding older adults without informants effectively translates to excluding socially isolated older adults, who may be at greater risk for dementia (Huang et al., 2023). In addition to having more social support, older adults who have available informants may be demographically different in other ways (e.g., fewer medical comorbidities, better quality of life, etc., Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2013; Portacolone et al., 2018). Of note, the EAS recruits from the Bronx, NY, a county with one of the most diverse U.S.-born older adult populations in New York and some of the poorest health metrics. Only 62% of our sample had informants available, and this subset had a higher proportion of non-Hispanic Whites, suggesting an unintended selection bias for studies that exclude those without study partners. To promote health equality, it is necessary to examine diagnostic criteria and direct research efforts to capture the intended at-risk populations.

Several study limitations warrant mention. Only a small subset of participants was joined by their informants, and there was a limited number of incident dementia cases. Additional studies in larger samples are needed. Although non-Hispanic Blacks were well represented, the population of Hispanic older adults or those of other racial/ethnic groups were insufficient to include in the analyses. Additionally, the self- and informant-report SCC questionnaires were limited in their coverage of cognitive domains and not all measures were available for both participants and their informants. As EAS has evolved, we have given much more consideration to the measurement of SCC and have newly implemented an expanded version of the widely-used Cognitive Change Index (CCI-40, Nester et al., 2022). Future research could expand on this current study utilizing standardized measures of SCC and exploring their utility in other health disparate groups.

To our knowledge, this was the first longitudinal study directly comparing five specific operational definitions of self- or informant-reported SCC in a diverse community-based sample. Our findings suggest that not including SCC, across different MCI definitions, provides the best balance of sensitivity and specificity in terms of predicting incident dementia in our sample of diverse individuals. Although the original rationale was to avoid prescribing a diagnosis of MCI to individuals with long-standing cognitive difficulties (e.g., learning disabilities) (Petersen, 2004), the extremely high prevalence of self-reported SCC in our sample warrants consideration as to whether this criterion is diagnostically additive and/or should be removed from MCI classifications. Consistent with prior research, informant-reported SCC was more strongly related to risk of incident dementia than self-perceived cognitive concerns. However, requiring informant reports in diagnostic criteria for MCI or as inclusion criteria in research settings may unintentionally exclude health disparate groups and/or those at elevated risk for dementia. Additional consideration is needed to determine how best to utilize informant-report in MCI screening and diagnosis. Taken together, these results provide valuable information for researchers and clinicians in their understanding of self- and informant-reported SCC and their relation to MCI classifications and incident dementia in diverse community-based populations. Importantly, this research can serve as a springboard for future efforts (e.g., pooling data) and to stimulate increased discussion among experts to arrive at an optimal MCI definition that promotes diagnostic accuracy while also being mindful of factors that may uphold health inequalities in aging populations.

Data availability statement

The terms of consent for research participants stipulate that an individual’s data can only be shared outside of the EAS investigators group if the group has reviewed and approved the proposed secondary use of the data. This consent applies regardless of whether data has been de-identified. Access is mediated via a standardized request process via Mindy Katz, mindy.katz@einsteinmed.edu.

Ethics statement

All protocols were approved by the Einstein Institutional Review Board (IRB). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/kin was obtained at the initial clinic visit.

Author contributions

KC and LR are the corresponding authors and guarantors of this manuscript and contributed to the conception and the design of the study. MK and CW organized the database. CW performed the statistical analysis and assisted with interpretation of results. KC, in collaboration with the co-authors, wrote the first draft of the manuscript, incorporated all levels of feedback from co-authors, and prepared the manuscript for submission. RL, DB, and CN provided expert guidance and reviewed this manuscript and revisions. The project was funded by successful project grants secured by MK and RL. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging at the National Institutes of Health (grant number P01 AG03949), the Leonard and Sylvia Marx Foundation, and the Czap Foundation.

Acknowledgments

We thank the EAS participants, investigators, and staff for their contribution to this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor RA declared a shared research group, the Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (part of Alzheimer’s Association ISTAART), with the author LR at the time of review.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2023.1221768/full#supplementary-material

References

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

Google Scholar

Artero, S., Petersen, R., Touchon, J., and Ritchie, K. (2006). Revised criteria for mild cognitive impairment: validation within a longitudinal population study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22, 465–470. doi: 10.1159/000096287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baars, M. A. E., van Boxtel, M. P. J., and Jolles, J. (2010). Migraine does not affect cognitive decline: results from the Maastricht aging study. Headache J. Head Face Pain 50, 176–184. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2009.01572.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balogh, R., Imre, N., Gosztolya, G., Hoffmann, L., Pákáski, M., and Kálmán, J. (2022). The role of silence in verbal fluency tasks – a new approach for the detection of mild cognitive impairment. J Inter Neuropsychol Soc 29, 46–58. doi: 10.1017/s1355617721001454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassett, S. S., and Folstein, M. F. (1993). Memory complaint, memory performance, and psychiatric diagnosis: a community study. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 6, 105–111. doi: 10.1177/089198879300600207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastin, C., Giacomelli, F., Miévis, F., Lemaire, C., Guillaume, B., and Salmon, E. (2021). Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment: lack of awareness of memory difficulties characterizes prodromal Alzheimer's disease. Front. Psych. 12:e631518. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.631518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., and Roth, M. (1968). The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br. J. Psychiatry 114, 797–811. doi: 10.1192/bjp.114.512.797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bondi, M. W., Edmonds, E. C., Jak, A. J., Clark, L. R., Delano-Wood, L., McDonald, C. R., et al. (2014). Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and progression rates. J. Alzheimers Dis. 42, 275–289. doi: 10.3233/jad-140276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadhouse, K. M., Winks, N. J., and Summers, M. J. (2021). Fronto-temporal functional disconnection precedes hippocampal atrophy in clinically confirmed multi-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. EXCLI J 20, 1458–1473. doi: 10.17179/excli2021-4191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckley, R., Saling, M. M., Ames, D., Rowe, C. C., Lautenschlager, N. T., Macaulay, S. L., et al. (2013). Factors affecting subjective memory complaints in the AIBL aging study: biomarkers, memory, affect, and age. Int. Psychogeriatr. 25, 1307–1315. doi: 10.1017/S1041610213000665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschke, H. (1984). Cued recall in amnesia. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 6, 433–440. doi: 10.1080/01688638408401233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschke, H., Kuslansky, G., Katz, M., Stewart, W. F., Sliwinski, M. J., Eckholdt, H. M., et al. (1999). Screening for dementia with the memory impairment screen. Neurology 52, 231–238. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.2.231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., and Cacioppo, S. (2013). Older adults reporting social isolation or loneliness show poorer cognitive function 4 years later. Evid Based Nurs 17, 59–60. doi: 10.1136/eb-2013-101379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CDC. (2019). Subjective cognitive decline - a public health issue. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/aging/data/subjective-cognitive-decline-brief.html (Accessed April 15, 2023).

Google Scholar

Chandler, J. M., Rentz, D. M., Zagar, A., Kim, Y., Schwartz, R. L., and Fillit, H. (2023). Disease progression and costs at the 3-year follow-up of the GERAS-US study. Alzheimer’s Dementia 15:e12430. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, S., Rentería, M. A., Dworkin, A., Garriga, S. M., Barker, M. S., Avila-Rieger, J., et al. (2023). Association of subjective cognitive decline with progression to dementia in a cognitively unimpaired multiracial community sample. Neurology 100, e1020–e1027. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000201658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Magnotta, V. A., Duff, K., Boles Ponto, L. L., and Schultz, S. K. (2006). Donepezil effects on cerebral blood flow in older adults with mild cognitive deficits. J Neuropsy Clinical Neurosci 18, 178–185. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2006.18.2.178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comijs, H. C., Deeg, D. J., Dik, M. G., Twisk, J. W., and Jonker, C. (2002). Memory complaints; the association with psycho-affective and health problems and the role of personality characteristics. A 6-year follow-up study. J. Affect. Disord. 72, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00453-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derby, C. A., Burns, L. C., Wang, C., Katz, M. J., Zimmerman, M. E., L'Italien, G., et al. (2013). Screening for predementia AD: time-dependent operating characteristics of episodic memory tests. Neurology 80, 1307–1314. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0b013e31828ab2c9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Simone, M. S., Rodini, M., De Tollis, M., Caltagirone, C., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2023). Exploring mechanisms that affect retrograde memory for public events in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: A longitudinal update. J Neuropsyc 10.1111/jnp.12317. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diaz-Galvan, P., Ferreira, D., Cedres, N. F., Falahati, F., Hernández-Cabrera, J. A., Ames, D., et al. (2021). Comparing different approaches for operationalizing subjective cognitive decline: impact on syndromic and biomarker profiles. Sci. Rep. 11:4356. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83428-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dicks, E., Tijms, B. M., ten Kate, M., Gouw, A. A., Benedictus, M. R., Teunissen, C. E., et al. (2018). Gray matter network measures are associated with cognitive decline in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 61, 198–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dilworth-Anderson, P., and Gibson, B. E. (2022). The cultural influence of values, norms, meanings, and perceptions in understanding dementia in ethnic minorities. Alzh. Dis. Assoc. Disord. 16, S56–S63. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200200002-00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmonds, E. C., Delano-Wood, L., Galasko, D. R., Salmon, D. P., and Bondi, M. W. (2014). Subjective cognitive complaints contribute to misdiagnosis of mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 836–847. doi: 10.1017/s135561771400068x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmonds, E. C., Weigand, A. J., Thomas, K. R., Eppig, J., Delano-Wood, L., Galasko, D. R., et al. (2018). Increasing inaccuracy of self-reported subjective cognitive complaints over 24 months in empirically derived subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 24, 842–853. doi: 10.1017/s1355617718000486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elfgren, C., Gustafson, L., Vestberg, S., and Passant, U. (2010). Subjective memory complaints, neuropsychological performance and psychiatric variables in memory clinic attendees: a 3-year follow-up study. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 51, e110–e114. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2010.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facal, D., Guàrdia-Olmos, J., Pereiro, A. X., Lojo-Seoane, C., Peró, M., and Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2019). Using an overlapping time interval strategy to study diagnostic instability in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Brain Sci 9:242. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9090242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fragkiadaki, S., Kontaxopoulou, D., Beratis, I. N., Andronas, N., Economou, A., Yannis, G., et al. (2016). Self-awareness of cognitive efficiency: differences between healthy elderly and patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 38, 1144–1157. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1198469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganguli, M., Snitz, B. E., Saxton, J. A., Chang, C.-C. H., Lee, C.-W., Vander Bilt, J., et al. (2011b). Outcomes of mild cognitive impairment by definition. Arc Neurol 68. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, S. (2004). Pharmacotherapy of mild cognitive impairment. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 6, 391–395. doi: 10.31887/dcns.2004.6.4/sgauthier

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerretsen, P., Chung, J. K., Shah, P., Plitman, E., Iwata, Y., Caravaggio, F., et al. (2017). Anosognosia is an independent predictor of conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease and is associated with reduced brain metabolism. J. Clin. Psychiatry 78, e1187–e1196. doi: 10.4088/jcp.16m11367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford, K. A., Liu, D., Hohman, T. J., Xu, M., Han, X., Romano, R. R. 3rd, et al. (2015). A mutual self- and informant-report of cognitive complaint correlates with neuropathological outcomes in mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One 10:e0141831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glodzik-Sobanska, L., Reisberg, B., De Santi, S., Babb, J. S., Pirraglia, E., Rich, K. E., et al. (2007). Subjective memory complaints: presence, severity, and future outcome in normal older subjects. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 24, 177–184. doi: 10.1159/000105604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackett, K., Mis, R., Drabick, D. A. G., and Giovannetti, T. (2020). Informant reporting in mild cognitive impairment: sources of discrepancy on the functional activities questionnaire. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 26, 503–514. doi: 10.1017/s1355617719001449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, L., Jia, J., Xing, Y., and Han, Y. (2021). An application study-subjective cognitive decline Questionnaire9 in detecting mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Aging Mental Healthz 26, 2014–2021. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1980860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, A. R., Roth, D. L., Cidav, T., Chung, S. E., Amjad, H., Thorpe, R. J., et al. (2023). Social isolation and 9-year dementia risk in community-dwelling Medicare beneficiaries in the United States. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 71, 765–773. doi: 10.1111/jgs.18140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, T. M., Winkey, K., Sachs, B. C., Fitzpatrick, A. L., Craft, S., Baker, L. D., et al. (2020). Subjective cognitive impairment differs by racial/ethnic groups and sociodemographic factors: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis and alzheimer’s disease (mesa-mind). Alzheimers Dement. 16:e040429. doi: 10.1002/alz.040429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilardi, C. R., Chieffi, S., Iachini, T., and Iavarone, A. (2021). Neuropsychology of posteromedial parietal cortex and conversion factors from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: systematic search and state-of-the-art review. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 289–307. doi: 10.1007/s40520-021-01930-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., et al. (2018). Nia-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 14, 535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. D., Rentz, D. M., Aghjayan, S. L., Buckley, R. F., Meneide, T. F., Sperling, R. A., et al. (2017). Subjective cognitive concerns are associated with objective memory performance in Caucasian but not African-American persons. Age Ageing 46, 988–993. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afx077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jak, A. J., Bondi, M. W., Delano-Wood, L., Wierenga, C., Corey-Bloom, J., Salmon, D. P., et al. (2009). Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 17, 368–375. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkins, A., Tree, J., and Tales, A. (2021). Distinct profile differences in subjective cognitive decline in the general public are associated with metacognition, negative affective symptoms, neuroticism, stress, and poor quality of life. J. Alzheimers Dis. 80, 1231–1242. doi: 10.3233/jad-200882

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., Buckley, R. F., van der Flier, W. M., Han, Y., Molinuevo, J. L., et al. (2020). The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 19, 271–278. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(19)30368-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jester, D. J., Andel, R., Cechová, K., Laczó, J., Lerch, O., Marková, H., et al. (2020). Cognitive phenotypes of older adults with subjective cognitive decline and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: The Czech Brain Aging Study. J Inter Neuropsychol Soc 27, 329–342. doi: 10.1017/s1355617720001046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juncos-Rabadán, O., Pereiro, A. X., Facal, D., Lojo, C., Caamaño, J. A., Sueiro, J., et al. (2013). Prevalence and correlates of mild cognitive impairment in adults aged over 50 years with subjective cognitive complaints in primary care centers. Geriat GerontInter 14, 667–673. doi: 10.1111/ggi.12157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E.F., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston naming test (2nd). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

Google Scholar

Kasper, S., Bancher, C., Eckert, A., Förstl, H., Frölich, L., Hort, J., et al. (2020). Management of mild cognitive impairment (MCI): the need for national and international guidelines. World J. Biol. Psych. 21, 579–594. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2019.1696473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, M. J., Lipton, R. B., Hall, C. B., Zimmerman, M. E., Sanders, A. E., Verghese, J., et al. (2012). Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and Alzheimer dementia in blacks and whites. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 26, 335–343. doi: 10.1097/wad.0b013e31823dbcfc

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, M. J., Qin, J., Wang, C., and Rabin, L. (2023). Subjective cognitive concerns differ by race/ethnicity among Einstein aging study (EAS) participants: implications for assessment and interpretation of self-perceived cognitive functioning. Alzheimers Dement. 19:e065943. doi: 10.1002/alz.065943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, B. J., and Petersen, R. C. (2007). Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurol. Clin. 25, 577–v. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2007.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinsella, G. J., Mullaly, E. R. E., Ong, B., Burton, C., Price, S., Phillips, M., et al. (2009). Early intervention for mild cognitive impairment: A randomised controlled trial. J Neurol Neurosur Psych 80, 730–736. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.148346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klekociuk, S. Z., and Summers, M. J. (2013). The self-fulfilling prophecy of episodic memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment: Do episodic memory deficits identified at classification remain evident when later examined with different memory tests? Neurol Res Inter 2013, 1–6. doi: 10.1155/2013/437013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knapstad, M. K., Steihaug, O. M., Aaslund, M. K., Nakling, A., Naterstad, I. F., Fladby, T., et al. (2019). Reduced walking speed in subjective and mild cognitive impairment: A cross-sectional study. J Geriat Physic Therapy 42. doi: 10.1519/jpt.0000000000000157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwok, T., Wu, Y., Lee, J., Lee, R., Yung, C. Y., Choi, G., et al. (2020). A randomized placebo-controlled trial of using B vitamins to prevent cognitive decline in older mild cognitive impairment patients. Clinical Nutr 39, 2399–2405. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2019.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, M. P., and Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist 9, 179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_part_1.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. T. C., Fung, A. W. T., Richards, M., Chan, W. C., Chiu, H. F. K., Lee, R. S. Y., et al. (2021). Risk of incident dementia varies with different onset and courses of depression. J Affective Dis 282, 915–920. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenehan, M. E., Klekociuk, S. Z., and Summers, M. J. (2012). Absence of a relationship between subjective memory complaint and objective memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment (MCI): is it time to abandon subjective memory complaint as an MCI diagnostic criterion? Int. Psychogeriatr. 24, 1505–1514. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000695

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leng, M., Yin, H., Zhang, P., Jia, Y., Hu, M., Li, G., et al. (2020). Sleep quality and health-related quality of life in older people with subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer disease. J Nerv Men Dis 208, 387–396. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000001137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Jia, S., Zhou, Z., Jin, Y., Zhang, X., Hou, C., et al. (2018). The role of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA) and its memory tasks for detecting mild cognitive impairment. Neurol Sci 39, 1029–1034. doi: 10.1007/s10072-018-3319-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, T., Luppa, M., Matschinger, H., Jessen, F., Angermeyer, M. C., and Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2015). Incident subjective memory complaints and the risk of subsequent dementia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 131, 290–296. doi: 10.1111/acps.12328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayo Clinic. (2020). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Mayo Clinic Available at: https://www.mayoclinic.org/diseases-conditions/mild-cognitive-impairment/diagnosis-treatment/drc-20354583 (Accessed July 30, 2022).

Google Scholar

Mazzeo, S., Padiglioni, S., Bagnoli, S., Bracco, L., Nacmias, B., Sorbi, S., et al. (2019). The dual role of cognitive reserve in subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment: A 7-year follow-up study. J Neur 266, 487–497. doi: 10.1007/s00415-018-9164-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. J. (2008). Is it time to separate subjective cognitive complaints from the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment? Age Ageing 37, 497–499. doi: 10.1092/ageing/afn147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molinuevo, J. L., Rabin, L. A., Amariglio, R., Buckley, R., Dubois, B., Ellis, K. A., et al. (2017). Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheim. Dement. 13, 296–311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.09.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C. (1997). Clinical dementia rating: a reliable and valid diagnostic and staging measure for dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int. Psychogeriatr. 9, 173–176. doi: 10.1017/s1041610297004870

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., et al. (1989). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 39, 1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nester, C., Ayers, E., Rabin, L. A., and Verghese, J. (2021). Non-memory subjective cognitive concerns predict incident motoric cognitive risk syndrome. Eur. J. Neurol. 27, 1146–1154. doi: 10.1111/ene.14271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nester, C., Mogle, J., Katz, M., Saykin, A., Lipton, R., and Rabin, L. A. (2022). The cognitive change index supplement (CCI-40): broadening the assessment of subjective cognitive concerns in demographically diverse community dwelling older adults. [Abstract]. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.18702.23364,

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosheny, R. L., Jin, C., Neuhaus, J., Insel, P. S., Mackin, R. S., and Weiner, M. W. (2019). Study partner-reported decline identifies cognitive decline and dementia risk. Anna Clin Transl Neurol 6, 2448–2459. doi: 10.1002/acn3.50938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Numbers, K., Lam, B. C., Crawford, J. D., Kochan, N. A., Sachdev, P. S., and Brodaty, H. (2023). Longitudinal changes in participant and informant reports of subjective cognitive complaints are associated with dementia risk. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1044807. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1044807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S.-J., Lee, J.-E., Lee, K.-S., and Kim, J.-S. (2018). Comparison of odor identification among amnestic and non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive decline, and early Alzheimer’s dementia. Neurol Sci 39, 557–564. doi: 10.1007/s10072-018-3261-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, S. W., Wang, C. Y., Lin, S. Y., Lee, Y. L., Lin, Y. C., Lin, Y. J., et al. (2023). Subjective cognitive complaints: comparing the relation between self-reported versus informant-reported subjective cognitive complaints and cognitive performances in cognitive unimpaired, mild cognitive impairment, and populations with dementia. J. Prev Alzheimers Dis. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2023.47

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Caracciolo, B., Brayne, C., Gauthier, S., Jelic, V., and Fratiglioni, L. (2014). Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J. Intern. Med. 275, 214–228. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 56:303. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portacolone, E., Rubinstein, R. L., Covinsky, K. E., Halpern, J., and Johnson, J. K. (2018). The precarity of older adults living alone with cognitive impairment. The Gerontologist 59, 271–280. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potter, G. G., Plassman, B. L., Burke, J. R., Kabeto, M. U., Langa, K. M., Llewellyn, D. J., et al. (2009). Cognitive performance and informant reports in the diagnosis of cognitive impairment and dementia in African Americans and whites. Alzhei Demen 5, 445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.04.1234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pusil, S., López, M. E., Cuesta, P., Bruña, R., Pereda, E., and Maestú, F. (2019). Hypersynchronization in mild cognitive impairment: the ‘X’ model. Brain 142, 3936–3950. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L.A., Mogle, J., Katz, M.J., and Sliwinski, M.J. (2020). Do repeated ambulatory cognitive assessments enhance the relation between objective and subjective cognition in non-demented community dwelling older adults? Results from the Einstein aging study [Conference Presentation]. Alzheimer’s Association International Conference 2020, Amsterdam, Netherlands.

Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Smart, C. M., and Amariglio, R. E. (2017). Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's Disease. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 13, 369–396. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Smart, C. M., Crane, P., Amariglio, R. E., Berman, L. M., Boada, M., et al. (2015). Subjective cognitive decline in older adults: an overview of self-report measures used across 19 international research studies. J. Alzheimers Dis. 48, S63–S86. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Wang, C., Katz, M. J., Derby, C. A., Buschke, H., and Lipton, R. B. (2012). Predicting Alzheimer's disease: neuropsychological tests, self-reports, and informant reports of cognitive difficulties. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 1128–1134. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.03956.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rami, L., Mollica, M. A., García-Sanchez, C., Saldaña, J., Sanchez, B., Sala, I., et al. (2014). The subjective cognitive decline questionnaire (SCD-Q): a validation study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 41, 453–466. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, S., Brenes, G., and Marsh, A. P. (2002). Memory enhancement training for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary study. Aging Mental Health 6, 5–11. doi: 10.1080/13607860120101077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, D., Ayers, E., Weiss, E. F., and Verghese, J. (2021). Cross-cultural comparisons of subjective cognitive complaints in a diverse primary care population. J. Alzheimers Dis. 81, 545–555. doi: 10.3233/jad-201399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, W. G. (1980). Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2, 135–146. doi: 10.1080/01688638008403788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, A. D., Lau, K. M., Saito, N., Harvey, D., Risacher, S. L., Aisen, P. S., et al. (2014). Self-rated and informant-rated everyday function in comparison to objective markers of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 1080–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryu, S. Y., Kim, A., Kim, S. Y., Park, K. W., Park, K. H., Youn, Y. C., et al. (2019). Self- and informant-reported cognitive functioning and awareness in subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, and very mild Alzheimer disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 35, 91–98. doi: 10.1002/gps.5224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryu, S. Y., Lee, S. B., Kim, T. W., and Lee, T. J. (2016). Subjective memory complaints, depressive symptoms, and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 487–494. doi: 10.1017/s1041610215001945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayegh, P., and Knight, B. G. (2013). Cross-cultural differences in dementia: the sociocultural health belief model. Int. Psychogeriatr. 25, 517–530. doi: 10.1017/s104161021200213x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharre, D. W. (2019). Preclinical, prodromal, and dementia stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Pract. Neurol., 36–47.

Google Scholar

Scherder, E. J. A., Van Paasschen, J., Deijen, J.-B., Van Der Knokke, S., Orlebeke, J. F. K., Burgers, I., et al. (2003). Physical activity and executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment. Aging Mental Health 9, 272–280. doi: 10.1080/13607860500089930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, M. P., and Donders, J. (2022). Cognitive complaints in older adults: relationships between self and informant report, objective test performance, and symptoms of depression. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 1–16. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2022.2144617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheikh, J. I., and Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Geriatric depression scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin. Gerontol. 5, 165–173. doi: 10.1300/J018v05n01-09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheikh, F., Ismail, Z., Mortby, M. E., Barber, P., Cieslak, A., Fischer, K., et al. (2017). Prevalence of mild behavioral impairment in mild cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive decline, and its association with caregiver burden. Inter Psychog 30, 233–244. doi: 10.1017/s104161021700151x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibilano, E., Brunetti, A., Buongiorno, D., Lassi, M., Grippo, A., Bessi, V., et al. (2023). An attention-based deep learning approach for the classification of subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment using resting-state EEG. J Neural Eng 20:016048. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/acb96e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sims, R. C., Whitfield, K. E., Ayotte, B. J., Gamaldo, A. A., Edwards, C. L., and Allaire, J. C. (2011). Subjective memory in older African Americans. Exp. Aging Res. 37, 220–240. doi: 10.1080/0361073x.2011.555640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. D., Smith, S. M., de Jager, C. A., Whitbread, P., Johnston, C., Agacinski, G., et al. (2010). Homocysteine-lowering by B vitamins slows the rate of accelerated brain atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. PloS one 5:e12244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Starkstein, S. E., Jorge, R., Mizrahi, R., and Robinson, R. G. (2006). A diagnostic formulation for anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 77, 719–725. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.085373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, C. M., Minett, B., Pagett, T., Siervo, E., Brayne, M., and Mckeith, I. G. (2013). Diagnosing mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in clinical trials: a systematic review. BMJ Open 3:e001909. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangalos, E. G., and Petersen, R. C. (2018). Mild cognitive impairment in geriatrics. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 34, 563–589. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2018.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, C. A., Bouldin, E. D., and McGuire, L. C. (2018). Subjective cognitive decline among adults aged ≥ 45 years – United States, 2016-2016. MMWR Morb. Mort. Week. Rep. 67, 753–757. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6727a1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thal, L. J., Ferris, S. H., Kirby, L., Block, G. A., Lines, C. R., Yuen, E., et al. (2005). A randomized, double-blind, study of rofecoxib in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychop 30, 1204–1215. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tierney, M. C., Szalai, J. P., Snow, W. G., and Fisher, R. H. (1996). The prediction of Alzheimer disease. The role of patient and informant perceptions of cognitive deficits. Arch. Neurol. 53, 423–427. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1996.00550050053023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Topiwala, A., Suri, S., Allan, C., Zsoldos, E., Filippini, N., Sexton, C. E., et al. (2021). Subjective cognitive complaints given in questionnaire: relationship with brain structure, cognitive performance, and self-reported depressive symptoms in a 25-year retrospective cohort study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 217–226. doi: 10.1016/jagp.2020.007.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treves, T. A., Verchovsky, R., Klimovitzky, S., and Korczyn, A. D. (2005). Incidence of dementia in patients with subjective memory complaints. Int. Psychogeriatr. 17, 265–273. doi: 10.1017/s1041610205001596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C.-L., Ukropec, J., Ukropcová, B., and Pai, M.-C. (2018). An acute bout of aerobic or strength exercise specifically modifies circulating exerkine levels and neurocognitive functions in elderly individuals with mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage: Clinical 17, 272–284. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.10.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, B. E., Steinberg, J. R., Weeks, B. T., Rodriguez, F., and Cullen, M. R. (2022). Race/ethnicity reporting and representation in US clinical trials: a cohort study. Lancet Reg. Health Am. 11:100252. doi: 10.1016/j.lana.2022.100252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences. (2022). A healthcare provider’s guide to mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Mild Cogn. Impair. Available at: https://memory.ucsf.edu/sites/memory.ucsf.edu/files/wysiwyg/UCSF_MCI_Providers_7-13-17.pdf (Accessed July 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

van Uffelen, J. G., Hopman-Rock, M., Paw, C. A., and van Mechelen, W. (2005). Protocol for Project FACT: A randomised controlled trial on the effect of a walking program and vitamin B supplementation on the rate of cognitive decline and psychosocial wellbeing in older adults with mild cognitive impairment [ISRCTN19227688]. BMC Geriatrics 5:18. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-5-18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, S., Passant, U., and Elfgren, C. (2010). Stability in the clinical characteristics of patients with memory complaints. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 50, e26–e30. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2009.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, A., Stokholm, J., Gade, A., Andersen, B. B., Hejl, A. M., and Waldemar, G. (2004). Awareness of deficits in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: DO MCI patients have impaired insight? Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 17, 181–187. doi: 10.1159/000076354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S. M., Han, K., Kim, N. Y., Um, Y. H., Kang, D. W., Na, H. R., et al. (2021). Late-life depression, subjective cognitive decline, and their additive risk in incidence of dementia: a Nationwide longitudinal study. PLoS One 16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, S. L., Reid, E., Whitfield, P., and Moustafa, A. A. (2022). Subjective memory complaints as a predictor of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Discover Psychol. 2. doi: 10.1007/s44202-022-00031-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wasef, S., Laksono, I., Kapoor, P., Tang-Wei, D., Gold, D., Saripella, A., et al. (2021). Screening for subjective cognitive decline in the elderly via subjective cognitive complaints and informant-reported questionnaires: a systematic review. BMC Anesthesiol. 21, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12871-021-01493-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1987). WMS-R: Wechsler memory scale-revised. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Administration and scoring manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L. O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment - beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J. Intern. Med. 256, 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yates, J.A., Clare, L., and Woods, R.T., & MRC CFAS (2017). Subjective memory complaints, mood and MCI: a follow-up study. Aging Ment. Health, 21, 313–321.

Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective cognitive concerns, mild cognitive impairment, informant report, study partner, aging, longitudinal

Citation: Chang KH, Wang C, Nester CO, Katz MJ, Byrd DA, Lipton RB and Rabin LA (2023) Examining the role of participant and study partner report in widely-used classification approaches of mild cognitive impairment in demographically-diverse community dwelling individuals: results from the Einstein aging study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1221768. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1221768

Received: 12 May 2023; Accepted: 29 September 2023;
Published: 21 November 2023.

Edited by:

Rebecca Amariglio, Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Catherine Munro, Harvard Medical School, United States
Dubbelman Mark Anton, Harvard Medical School, United States
Robinson Talia, Harvard Medical School, United States
Rory Boyle, Harvard Medical School, United States

Copyright © 2023 Chang, Wang, Nester, Katz, Byrd, Lipton and Rabin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katherine H. Chang, kchang@gradcenter.cuny.edu; Laura A. Rabin, lrabin@brooklyn.cuny.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.