Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 17 November 2023
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias

Somatic comorbidity and the progression of cognitive impairment

  • 1Faculty of Medicine, Medical University of Gdańsk, Gdańsk, Poland
  • 2Department of Health Sciences, Pomeranian University in Słupsk, Słupsk, Poland
  • 3Institute of Psychology, University of Gdańsk, Gdańsk, Poland
  • 4Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical University of Gdańsk, Gdańsk, Poland
  • 5Institute of Pedagogy and Languages, University of Applied Sciences in Elbląg, Elbląg, Poland

Background: There are usually multiple factors underlying dementia in old age. Somatic comorbidity is one important element that influences the progression of cognitive impairment.

Objective: The goal of this study was to assess the relationship between the progression of cognitive impairment and the presence and severity of comorbidities based on a four-year observation.

Material: Out of 128 patients from the Clinic for Outpatients in Gdansk, who were recruited into the study based on the criteria of the Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment, a total of 93 participants completed the four-year observation. Only the data from participants who completed the full period of observations were analysed. The mean age of the group was M = 75.93 (SD = 9.43). The level of progression of cognitive impairment was measured using the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale – Sum of Boxes, the severity of comorbidities was measured using the modified Cumulative Illness Rating Scale, and, additionally, at the time of inclusion in the study, participants were assessed using the MMSE scale and the Activity Scale, and sociodemographic data were collected. The Generalized Estimating Equations method was employed to fit a marginal model for analyzing the data collected in a repeated measures design. The tested model elucidated the role of the overall severity of comorbidities in explaining the progression of cognitive impairment, while controlling for everyday activity and basic demographic variables.

Results: During the four-year observation, a significant decline in cognitive function (B = 1.86, p < 0.01) was observed in the examined sample. The statistical analysis revealed that individuals with higher overall severity of comorbidities exhibited significantly more pronounced progression of cognitive impairment over time. Regarding particular comorbidities, metabolic diseases were found to be associated with a poorer prognosis (rho = 0.41, p < 0.05). Furthermore, a time physical activity interaction was identified as predicting cognitive impairment, indicating that individuals who were more physically active at the beginning of the study exhibited significantly less pronounced progression of cognitive impairment over the course of the 4 years.

Conclusion: This study suggests the important roles of comorbidities and physical activity for the prognosis of mild cognitive impairment.

1 Introduction

As the mean population age increases, so does the prevalence of dementia; this is already a serious socio-economic problem, being a burden for both healthcare systems and the caregivers of those affected. Dementia is usually preceded by a period of impaired cognitive functioning, which can be of different lengths in different people (Bookheimer et al., 2000; Bidzan and Bidzan, 2014; Cooper et al., 2019; Edmonds et al., 2019). A decrease in cognitive fitness is associated with the risk of onset of fully symptomatic dementia in a relatively short period of time. This risk is especially serious if the impairment deteriorates, taking the form of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Bidzan and Bidzan, 2014; Pąchalska et al., 2015; Bidzan et al., 2017; Cooper et al., 2019). MCI is defined as when cognitive function (usually memory) is impaired, but functionality is preserved (i.e., a person with MCI has no significant impairment in their abilities to perform tasks of daily living), in the absence of medical disturbances. The most recent characterization of MCI includes the following criteria: (1) a subjective sense of the presence of memory disorder (2) intact ability to carry out everyday activities, such as meal preparation; (3) preserved general cognitive abilities; (4) objective evidence of a memory deficit; and (5) the absence of dementia (Kempf et al., 2016; Livingston et al., 2020).

The yearly prevalence of onset of dementia in individuals with MCI is significantly higher than among the general population, varying between 4.9% and 9.6% (Mitchell and Shiri-Feshki, 2009; Bidzan et al., 2017). The severity of further progression of cognitive impairment depends on many factors: in addition to age, these also include family history (Fratiglioni et al., 1993; Bidzan et al., 2017; Treder-Rochna, 2020) and genetic factors such as the ε4 allele of Apolipoprotein E (Gylys et al., 2010; Jóźwik et al., 2012; Livingston et al., 2020). However, only genetic factors and age are non-modifiable – other factors can be modified through interventions. This is why efforts should focus on identifying potentially modifiable factors which could accelerate the process of cognitive deterioration. Around 35%–40% of all dementia cases are attributable to modifiable risk factors (Kempf et al., 2016; Livingston et al., 2017, 2020). A growing body of evidence supports the 12 potentially modifiable risk factors for dementia proposed by the 2017 Lancet Commission on dementia prevention, intervention, and care: low education level, hypertension, hearing impairment, smoking, obesity, depression, physical inactivity, diabetes, low social contact, excessive alcohol consumption, traumatic brain injury, and air pollution (Livingston et al., 2020).

However, it needs to be noted that the processes that lead to the histopathological changes which are the basis of dementia begin many years, if not decades, before the clinical onset of the condition (Nutaitis et al., 2019).

Individuals with dementia are found to have many concomitant conditions. On average, such patients have diagnoses of two to eight other conditions (Schubert et al., 2006). These conditions can disrupt a patient’s functioning and make caregiving more difficult (Chen et al., 2018). They may also influence the severity of the progression of dementia-related impairment (Cooper et al., 2015). Epidemiological studies have found that the occurrence of conditions such as hypertension, diabetes, and hyperlipidemia in middle age is associated with a higher risk of Alzheimer’s-type dementia as well as vascular dementia later on in life (Reijmer et al., 2012; Baumgart et al., 2015; Kempf et al., 2016). In addition to cardiovascular factors, conditions of the respiratory system, the liver (cirrhosis), and kidneys may also be risk factors (Chen et al., 2018). While the association between cardiovascular factors in middle age and the risk of dementia seems well documented, their impact at later stages of life requires further study.

2 Methods

2.1 Study design

In order to investigate the progression of cognitive impairment, a prospective observational study with repeated measures was carried out. Over the course of the four-year study, a psychiatrist observed and analysed a cohort of patients, refraining from intervening or imposing any experimental treatments. The principal aim of the study was to acquire information on the course of cognitive impairment advancement of cognitive impairment and to uncover potential correlations between cognitive impairment and somatic comorbidity assessed at the study’s outset. A battery of tests and a demographic questionnaire were administered during the initial visits, once the participants had consented to engaging in the study. After 4 years, measurement of the outcome variable – cognitive impairment – was repeated.

In this study design, researchers refrain from active interventions, allowing the accumulation of real-world data within a more naturalistic context. Nevertheless, the lack of controlled interventions makes it challenging to establish cause-and-effect relationships, as other confounding variables may influence the observed outcomes (Hammoudeh et al., 2018).

This research project was reviewed and approved by the Bioethics Committee (decision no. NKBBN/279/2014) at the Medical University of Gdansk, Poland.

2.2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Participants were enrolled in the study based on the following criteria.

2.2.1 Inclusion criteria

(1) Voluntary agreement to participate; (2) diagnosis of amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) according to the Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment criteria, i.e., (a) evident cognitive decline self-reported by the participant or reported by a caregiver, confirmed by objective cognitive impairment assessment, and/or observable cognitive decline over time through objective examination (Winblad et al., 2004); (b) ability to perform basic daily activities preserved, with minimal impairment in complex activities; (c) and not currently undergoing psychiatric pharmacotherapy during recruitment; (3) presence of an individual from the participant’s daily environment who lives with them or visits them several times weekly and consents to participate as an informant; (4) global score of 0.5 on the Clinical Dementia Rating (Global CDR); and (5) Mini-Mental State Examination score between 24 and 30 (Folstein et al., 1975).

2.2.2 Exclusion criteria

(1) Diagnosis of dementia, regardless of etiology, according to the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013); (2) current or past diagnosis of any of the following conditions: mood disorders, schizophrenia, alcoholism, drug or substance dependence, epilepsy, Parkinson’s disease, or any intellectual disability; (3) present impaired consciousness, or motor, visual, or hearing impairment significantly hindering the ability to complete tasks and assessments in the clinical scales used in the study; and (4) withdrawal of consent to participate at any stage of the study.

2.3 Participants

A cohort of amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) patients was enrolled at the Clinic for Outpatients in Gdansk, Poland. A total of 128 participants with diagnoses of amnestic MCI were eligible for this study. The recruitment for the study lasted from the end of 2015 through 2016, and each individual was observed for the 4 years after their enrolment in the study. All data were collected as a part of a routine clinical visit. We excluded 35 participants who, for various reasons, could not be re-examined after 4 years. Thus, the study sample consisted of 93 participants (71 women and 22 men) with diagnosed MCI. At the start of the study, the average age of the participants was M = 75.93 (SD = 9.43). They varied in terms of the number of years of formal education (M = 11.77, SD = 4.25). Over the course of the study, 18 participants received a diagnosis of dementia.

2.4 Measures

2.4.1 Primary outcome: progression of cognitive impairment

The Clinical Dementia Rating Scale – Sum of Boxes (CDR-SB) developed by Hughes et al. (1982) was used for evaluating the progression of cognitive impairment. The CDR-SB consists of six items covering the main domains of cognitive functioning: memory, orientation, judgment and problem solving, community affairs, home and hobbies, and personal care. Each domain is rated by an interviewer on a five point behaviorally anchored scale (0 = no cognitive impairment, 0.5 = questionable or very mild dementia, 1 = mild dementia, 2 = moderate dementia, and 3 = severe dementia). The overall CDR-BS score (ranging from 0 to 18) is obtained by summing each of the six domain scores. In the current study, the CDR-SB scores were collected twice: first during the initial examination and then during the follow-up after 4 years. Both results were based on information collected from the patient and caregiver (informant). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for both measurements in the present study was 0.78, indicating a satisfactory level of reliability.

2.4.2 Cognitive functioning: initial assessment

The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), developed by Folstein et al. (1975), Polish adaptation by Stańczak (2010), was used to assess cognitive functioning. The MMSE is a 30-item tool that includes tests of orientation, attention and calculation, memory, language, and visual–spatial skills. Participants’ responses are scored “1” if correct or “0” if incorrect. The Polish version of the MMSE has been shown to have high internal consistency in diverse clinical samples (Stańczak, 2010).

2.4.3 Somatic comorbidity: initial assessment

The modified Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (CIRS) was used to assess somatic comorbidity (Salvi et al., 2008). The modified CIRS includes 15 categories that assess physical impairment. Each category applies to a relatively independent body system and is rated on a five-point scale (from 1 = no impairment to that organ/system to 5 = impairment is life threatening). However, previous studies have shown that results for the CIRS categories have skewed distributions, with the mode being the lowest value for all items (Cova et al., 2016). Therefore, the items were dichotomized (0 = no physical impairment, 1 = physical impairment diagnosed), making it less likely to result in biased estimates than more unbalanced scales (Pedhazur and Schmelkin, 1991). The overall CIRS score was obtained by summing each of the 15 individual system scores (using the original values before dichotomization).

2.4.4 Sociodemographic variables and living activities

The participants were interviewed and asked about their number of years of formal education, sex, and age. To assess the participants’ living activities, the Activity Scale (Bidzan et al., 2016) was used, which is our own modification of the scale by Christensen and Mackinnon (1993). It consists of 13 items that measure the intensity of three types of activity: intellectual (five items; e.g., reading books and magazines or doing crosswords), physical (three items; e.g., sports and recreation, such as bicycling, skiing, aerobics, or gymnastics), and social (five items; e.g., playing games or having a good time with others). Each activity was rated on a four-point frequency scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (very often). The score for each subscale was computed by summing up the points scored on items that pertain to a given type of activity.

2.5 Statistical analyses

We employed the Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) method as proposed by Liang and Zeger (1986) and Ballinger (2004). This method is particularly suitable for generating regression estimates when dealing with repeated measures involving non-normally distributed outcome variables. The tested model provided insights into the association between overall severity of comorbidities and the progression of cognitive impairment. This analysis was performed while accounting for three categories of daily living activities – intellectual, physical, and social – as well as relevant demographic variables, including age, sex, and years of education. Furthermore, in line with the approach of Wang et al. (2016), we integrated more recent modified variance estimators into the model fitting procedure. This choice was driven by the goal of enhancing the small-sample performance of the model, thereby contributing to the robustness of our findings.

3 Results

The main characteristics of the participants’ cognitive functioning and somatic impairment are presented in Table 1. A more in-depth examination of the data revealed that hypertension (38%), along with cardiac (48%) and metabolic (36%) impairment, were prevalent within the studied sample. The frequencies of physical and social activity were moderate; the frequency of intellectual activity was rather low.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the main variables examined in the study.

The utilization of the Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) method provided insight into the dynamic nature of cognitive impairment throughout the study period. By accounting for the nature of the repeated measures data, the GEE model was able to identify trends and associations within the dataset. Table 2 shows regression estimates encompassing both primary and controlled variables, elucidating their roles in predicting cognitive impairment.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regression estimates for tested models using Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE).

Notably, the outcomes of Model 1 highlighted a statistically significant correlation between the passage of time and cognitive impairment: overall, within the studied cohort, cognitive impairment worsened after 4 years (B = 1.86, p < 0.01). Meanwhile, Model 2 yielded valuable insights indicating that the initial overall severity of comorbidities and physical activity were significantly intertwined with cognitive impairment: participants with higher overall severity of comorbidities exhibited more pronounced cognitive impairment (B = 0.24, p < 0.01), whereas individuals displaying heightened levels of physical activity experienced relatively less cognitive impairment (B = −0.16, p < 0.01). The participants’ gender was found to be an additional influential predictor of cognitive impairment: women exhibited more substantial cognitive impairment (B = 0.77, p < 0.05). Lastly, the results from Model 3 indicated that individuals with elevated overall severity of comorbidities demonstrated a notably more pronounced advancement of cognitive impairment as time progressed (interaction between Cumulative Illness and Time: B = 0.28, p < 0.05). Additionally, the analysis also highlighted that individuals with higher levels of physical activity displayed a significantly attenuated progression of cognitive impairment across the four-year observation period (interaction between Physical Activity and Time: B = −0.23, p < 0.05).

In relation to specific comorbidities, Spearman’s rho correlation with cognitive impairment progression (ΔCDR-SB) was computed for each disease category. Only metabolic diseases exhibited a significant association with cognitive impairment progression, with a correlation coefficient of rho = 0.41 (p < 0.05).

4 Discussion

Our research supports the association between being affected by somatic conditions in old age and faster progression of impairment of cognitive functions. This association pertained to cumulative comorbidity (the overall score on the CIRS scale). When distinct categories on the scale were analysed separately, only the metabolic conditions were significantly associated with poorer outcomes with regards to cognitive functions.

The most represented conditions in the “metabolic” category in the current study were type II diabetes and dyslipidemias (hypercholesterolemia): the first was present in 24 participants and the second in 16 participants. The presence of diabetes together with amnestic mild cognitive impairment has been found to be associated with a significant increase in conversion to dementia, especially the Alzheimer’s type (Li et al., 2011, 2012). Other, later studies indicate that even just increased fasting glucose (above 100 mg/dL in individuals with MCI) is a risk factor for conversion to dementia, especially of the Alzheimer’s type, independently of whether or not amnestic-type impairments are the dominant ones (Morris et al., 2014). Recent metaanalyses support the existence of a strong relationship between having diabetes and faster progression of cognitive impairment in individuals who already show some impairment (i.e., who are diagnosed with MCI) (Cooper et al., 2015). This is in line with our results. The proposed mechanisms behind this include not only purely vascular processes but also the influence of diabetes on other neurobiological processes (Mushtaq et al., 2014; Baumgart et al., 2015). It must be noted that insulin plays a role in many important relevant processes, not least in the growth of nerve cells, the functioning of glial cells, energetic homeostasis of the brain, oxidative stress, and inflammation processes in the central nervous system (Blázquez et al., 2014; Prasad et al., 2014). The role of insulin in the maintenance of the blood–brain barrier might also be key: according to some theories, impairment of the blood–brain barrier is associated with the triggering of the so-called amyloid cascade through the initiation of inflammatory processes in the brain (Jóźwik et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2018).

The role of lipid metabolism disorders, especially those pertaining to cholesterol metabolism, in dementias have been the subject of many studies (McGuinness et al., 2009; Mushtaq et al., 2014; Baumgart et al., 2015; Pąchalska et al., 2015). Results regarding the role of cholesterol (and, more broadly, lipid metabolism) are often contradictory (McGuinness et al., 2009). It is generally accepted that there is a link between higher cholesterol levels in middle age and the risk of developing dementia at a later age (Solomon et al., 2009). On the other hand, there is much variability in the results of studies on older individuals (Gorelick et al., 2011). The current study analysed both diabetes and hypercholesterolemia as one category (“metabolic”) and thus it is impossible to say which of these factors played a bigger role in the progression of cognitive impairment.

It is somewhat surprising that no relationship was observed for hypertension, given that its role in the progression of cognitive impairment seems well documented (Reijmer et al., 2012; Baumgart et al., 2015). Results suggest that every reduction of either systolic or diastolic pressure by 10 mm hg is associated with a marked decrease in the risk of onset of dementia (Ravaglia et al., 2006). Moreover, treatment with diuretic agents has also been found to be associated with a decrease in this risk (Yasar et al., 2013). Nevertheless, in our sample, hypertension did not differentiate groups. One explanation for this may be found in newer studies which suggest that while hypertension observed around the age of 50 can be considered a risk factor for dementia, moderately increased blood pressure in older individuals is a protective factor (Duron and Hanon, 2008; Corrada et al., 2017). According to some reports, blood pressure starts to gradually decrease several years before the onset of dementia (Kahonen-Vare et al., 2004). Moreover, hypertension was a very frequent diagnosis in the studied population and including it as a dichotomized variable in our statistical model could have influenced the result. However, it should also be noted that there are reports that indicate that the influence of increased blood pressure is most pronounced when the cognitive impairment affects areas other than memory (Hansson et al., 2006).

Another variable which also differentiated individuals in terms of the severity of the progression of cognitive impairment was physical activity. Numerous studies have previously indicated that it is a factor beneficial for the prognosis of cognitive impairment (Baumgart et al., 2015; Bidzan et al., 2016; Miyawaki et al., 2017; Lipnicki et al., 2019).

On the other hand, many randomized intervention studies have failed to support the relationship between increased physical activity and slower progression of cognitive impairment (Sink et al., 2015; Andrieu et al., 2017). Research by Sabia et al. (2017) suggests that physical activity itself is not actually a protective factor, but rather decreased physical activity is an effect of the ongoing neurodegenerative process. This could be indicated by a relationship between decreased physical activity and faster progression of dementia processes in the period directly preceding the clinical manifestation of dementia, while levels of activity in earlier periods are not related to risk levels (Sabia et al., 2017). Physical activity should be considered in the context of potential risk factors for not only dementia, but also a number of other conditions, including cardio-vascular ones (which were the subject of the study). It is to be expected that comorbidities limit one’s levels of physical activity. Research by Daimiel et al. (2020) found that physical fitness, rather than physical activity itself, was associated with levels of cognitive functioning. Physical fitness was not the subject of analysis in the current study; however, it would be reasonable to assume that the presence of comorbidities could have also decreased participants’ physical activity.

4.1 Limitations

The lack of detailed analysis of the comorbidities, especially in terms of when they occurred, could be considered a limitation of this study. Previous literature discussing the relationship between dementia and, in particular, cardiovascular conditions has stressed their differential importance depending on the age at which they occur (Duron and Hanon, 2008). It can be expected that conditions which appear in old age have a different impact on the progression of cognitive impairment than those which are present already in middle age.

Another important limitation is the lack of detailed analysis of the treatments received by participants, both during and before observation – only the treatment being received at the time of enrollment in the study was recorded.

4.2 Strength of this study

The strength of this research is that we followed up for 4 years and assessed the relationship with and conversion to dementia of MCI. As previous studies have shown, metabolic diseases (DM and dyslipidemia) and physical activities are associated with progression of cognitive impairment. This investigation confirmed those findings using MCI participants living in Poland, and it may be useful for future study in this field.

5 Conclusion

The study was based on a four-year-long observation of individuals diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment. The results suggest poorer prognosis in terms of the progression of cognitive impairment in individuals with more comorbidities, especially metabolic conditions. This supports the particular importance of the prevention and treatment of somatic illness for decreasing the prevalence of dementia. Further studies should analyse the types of treatments being undertaken for such comorbidities and their effectiveness in individuals with mild cognitive impairment.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Bioethics Committee at the Medical University of Gdansk, Poland. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

LB, IB-B, MB-W, MB, MO, and PJ: conceptualization, formal analysis, and writing—original draft. LB, IB-B, MB-W, and MB: data curation and investigation. LB: funding acquisition and supervision. LB, PJ, and MO: methodology. LB and MB: project administration. LB, MB, and PJ: writing—review & editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The work was supported by the publishing support program of the University of Gdansk.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Padraic Coughlan for proofreading the article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing

Google Scholar

Andrieu, S., Guyonnet, S., Coley, N., Cantet, C., Bonnefoy, M., Bordes, S., et al. (2017). Effect of long-term omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation with or without multidomain intervention on cognitive function in elderly adults with memory complaints (MAPT): a randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 16, 377–389. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30040-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballinger, G. A. (2004). Using generalized estimating equations for longitudinal data analysis. Organ. Res. Methods 7, 127–150. doi: 10.1177/1094428104263672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H., and Johns, H. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 718–726. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidzan, M., and Bidzan, L. (2014). Neurobehavioral manifestation in early period of Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Psychiatr. Pol. 48, 319–330.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bidzan, M., Bidzan, L., and Bidzan-Bluma, I. (2017). Neuropsychiatric symptoms and faster progression of cognitive impairments as predictors of risk of conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Arch. Med. Sci. 5, 1168–1177. doi: 10.5114/aoms.2017.68943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidzan, L., Bidzan, M., and Pąchalska, M. (2016). The effects of intellectual, physical, and social activity on further prognosis in mild cognitive impairment. Med. Sci. Monit. 22, 2551–2560. doi: 10.12659/MSM.899004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blázquez, E., Velázquez, E., Hurtado-Carneiro, V., and Ruiz-Albusac, J. M. (2014). Insulin in the brain: its pathophysiological implications for states related with central insulin resistance, type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Endocrinol. 5:161. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookheimer, S. Y., Strojwas, M. H., Cohen, M. S., Saunders, A. M., Pericak-Vance, M. A., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2000). Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer's disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 343, 450–456. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200008173430701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P. H., Cheng, S. J., Lin, H. C., Lee, C. Y., and Chou, C. H. (2018). Risk factors for the progression of mild cognitive impairment in different types of neurodegenerative disorders. Behav. Neurol. 2018:6929732. doi: 10.1155/2018/6929732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, H., and Mackinnon, A. (1993). The association between mental, social and physical activity and cognitive performance in young and old subjects. Age Ageing 22, 175–182. doi: 10.1093/ageing/22.3.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, C., Sommerlad, A., Lyketsos, C. G., and Livingston, G. (2015). Modifiable predictors of dementia in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Insanity 172, 323–334. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14070878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, S., Thomas, K. R., Weigand, A. J., Wong, C. G., Edmonds, E. C., and Bondi, M. W. (2019). P 1-309: neuropsychiatric symptoms predict conversion to mild cognitive impairment in cognitively normal older adults. Alzheimers Dement. 15, P361–P362. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.06.864

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrada, M. M., Hayden, K. M., Paganini-Hill, A., Bullain, S. S., DeMoss, J., Aguirre, C., et al. (2017). Age of onset of hypertension and risk of dementia in the oldest-old: the 90+ study. Alzheimers Dement. 13, 103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cova, I., Clerici, F., Rossi, A., Cucumo, V., Ghiretti, R., Maggiore, L., et al. (2016). Weight loss predicts progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. PLoS One 11:e0151710. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daimiel, L., Martínez-González, M. A., Corella, D., Salas-Salvadó, J., Schröder, H., Vioque, J., et al. (2020). Physical fitness and physical activity association with cognitive function and quality of life: baseline cross-sectional analysis of the PREDIMED-plus trial. Sci. Rep. 10, 3472–3412. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59458-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duron, E., and Hanon, O. (2008). Hypertension, cognitive decline and dementia. Arch. Cardiovasc. Dis. 101, 181–189. doi: 10.1016/S1875-2136(08)71801-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmonds, E. C., McDonald, C. R., Marshall, A., Thomas, K. R., Eppig, J., Weigand, A. J., et al. (2019). Early versus late MCI: improved MCI staging using a neuropsychological approach. Alzheimers Dement. 15, 699–708. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fratiglioni, L., Ahlbom, A., Viitanen, M., and Winblad, B. (1993). Risk factors for late-onset Alzheimer's disease: a population-based, case-control study. Ann. Neurol. 33, 258–266. doi: 10.1002/ana.410330306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorelick, P. B., Scuteri, A., Black, S. E., Decarli, C., Greenberg, S. M., Iadecola, C., et al. (2011). Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 42, 2672–2713. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gylys, K. H., Arold, S. P., Henkins, K. M., Miller, C. A., Vinters, H. V., Cole, G. M., et al. (2010). Apoliprotein-E is increased in Alzheimer's disease synapses and associates with amyloid beta. Alzheimers Dement. 6:S152. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.05.473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammoudeh, S., Gadelhaq, W., and Janahi, I. (2018). “Prospective cohort studies in medical research” in Cohort studies in health sciences. ed. R. M. Barría (London: InTech)

Google Scholar

Hansson, O., Zetterberg, H., Buchhave, P., Londos, E., Blennow, K., and Minthon, L. (2006). Association between CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer’s disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a follow-up study. Lancet Neurol. 5, 228–234. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(06)70355-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C., Berg, L., Danziger, W., Coben, L., and Martin, R. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 140, 566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jóźwik, A., Landowski, J., Bidzan, L., Fülop, T., Bryl, E., and Witkowski, J. M. (2012). Beta-amyloid peptides enhance the proliferative response of activated CD4+ CD28+ lymphocytes from Alzheimer disease patients and from healthy elderly. PLoS One 7:e33276. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahonen-Vare, M., Brunni-Hakala, S., Lindroos, M., Pitkala, K., Strandberg, T., and Tilvis, R. (2004). Left ventricular hypertrophy and blood pressure as predictors of cognitive decline in old age. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 16, 147–152. doi: 10.1007/bf03324544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempf, C., Schwarz, L. R., Desai, A. K., and Perkinson, M. A. (2016). “Cognitive impairment, dementia, and occupational therapy interventions” in Occupational therapy with aging adults. eds. K. Frank Barney, C. Emerita, and M. A. Perkinson (Maryland Heights, MO: Mosby), 245–265.

Google Scholar

Li, L., Wang, Y., Yan, J., Chen, Y., Zhou, R., Yi, X., et al. (2012). Clinical predictors of cognitive decline in patients with mild cognitive impairment: the Chongqing aging study. J. Neurol. 259, 1303–1311. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6342-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Wang, Y. J., Zhang, M., Xu, Z. Q., Gao, C. Y., Fang, C. Q., et al. (2011). Vascular risk factors promote conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease. Neurology 76, 1485–1491. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318217e7a4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, K.-Y., and Zeger, S. L. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika 73, 13–22. doi: 10.1093/biomet/73.1.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipnicki, D. M., Makkar, S. R., Crawford, J. D., Thalamuthu, A., Kochan, N. A., Lima-Costa, M. F., et al. (2019). Determinants of cognitive performance and decline in 20 diverse ethno-regional groups: a COSMIC collaboration cohort study. PLoS Med. 16:e1002853. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Sommerlad, A., Orgeta, V., Costafreda, S. G., Huntley, J., Ames, D., et al. (2017). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet 390, 2673–2734. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuinness, B., Craig, D., Bullock, R., and Passmore, P. (2009). Statins for the prevention of dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4:CD003160. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003160.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. J., and Shiri-Feshki, M. (2009). Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia–meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 119, 252–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01326.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyawaki, C. E., Bouldin, E. D., Kumar, G. S., and McGuire, L. C. (2017). Associations between physical activity and cognitive functioning among middle-aged and older adults. J. Nutr. Health Aging 21, 637–647. doi: 10.1007/s12603-016-0835-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. K., Vidoni, E. D., Honea, R. A., and Burns, J. M. (2014). Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative. Impaired glycemia increases disease progression in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 585–589. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mushtaq, G., Khan, J. A., and Kamal, M. A. (2014). Biological mechanisms linking Alzheimer’s disease and type-2 diabetes mellitus. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 13, 1192–1201. doi: 10.2174/1871527313666140917114537

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nutaitis, A. C., Tharwani, S. D., Serra, M. C., Goldstein, F. C., Zhao, L., Sher, S. S., et al. (2019). Diet as a risk factor for cognitive decline in African Americans and Caucasians with a parental history of Alzheimer’s disease: a cross-sectional pilot study dietary patterns. JPAD 6, 50–55. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2018.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pąchalska, M., Bidzan, L., Bidzan, M., and Góral-Półrola, J. (2015). Vascular factors and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer disease. Med. Sci. Monit. 21, 3483–3489. doi: 10.12659/msm.894550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedhazur, E. J., and Schmelkin, L. P. Measurement, design, & analysis: an integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, (1991).

Google Scholar

Prasad, S., Sajja, R. K., Naik, P., and Cucullo, L. (2014). Diabetes mellitus and blood-brain barrier dysfunction: an overview. J. Pharmacovigil. 2:125. doi: 10.4172/2329-6887.1000125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravaglia, G., Forti, P., Maioli, F., Martelli, M., Servadei, L., Brunetti, N., et al. (2006). Conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: predictive role of mild cognitive impairment subtypes and vascular risk factors. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 21, 51–58. doi: 10.1159/000089515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijmer, Y. D., van den Berg, E., Dekker, J. M., Nijpels, G., Stehouwer, C. D. A., Kappelle, L. J., et al. (2012). Development of vascular risk factors over 15 years in relation to cognition: the Hoorn study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 1426–1433. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04081.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabia, S., Dugravot, A., and Dartigues, J. F. (2017). Physical activity, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia: 28-year follow-up of Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ 357:j2709. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j2709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvi, F., Miller, M. D., Grilli, A., Giorgi, R., Towers, A. L., Morichi, V., et al. (2008). A manual of guidelines to score the modified cumulative illness rating scale and its validation in acute hospitalized elderly patients. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56, 1926–1931. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01935.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubert, C. C., Boustani, M., Callahan, C. M., Perkins, A. J., Carney, C. P., Fox, C., et al. (2006). Comorbidity profile of dementia patients in primary care: are they sicker? J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 54, 104–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.00543.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sink, K. M., Espeland, M. A., Castro, C. M., Church, T., Cohen, R., Dodson, J. A., et al. (2015). Effect of a 24-month physical activity intervention vs health education on cognitive outcomes in sedentary older adults: the LIFE randomized trial. JAMA 314, 781–790. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.9617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon, A., Kåreholt, I., Ngandu, T., Wolozin, B., MacDonald, S. W. S., Winblad, B., et al. (2009). Serum total cholesterol, statins and cognition in non-demented elderly. Neurobiol Aging 30, 1006–1009. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stańczak, J. MMSE Polish normalization. Warszawa, Pracowania Testów Psychologicznych PTP, (2010).

Google Scholar

Treder-Rochna, N. (2020). Adaptation to the disease – the psychological resources of families struggling with multiple sclerosis. Health Psychol. Rep. 8, 136–144. doi: 10.5114/hpr.2020.94722

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M., Kong, L., Li, Z., and Zhang, L. (2016). Covariance estimators for generalized estimating equations (GEE) in longitudinal analysis with small samples. Stat. Med. 35, 1706–1721. doi: 10.1002/sim.6817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L. O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J. Intern. Med. 256, 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasar, S., Xia, J., Yao, W., Furberg, C. D., Xue, Q. L., Mercado, C. I., et al. (2013). Antihypertensive drugs decrease risk of Alzheimer disease: ginkgo evaluation of memory study. Neurology 81, 896–903. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a35228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognition, dementia, MCI, comorbidities, neurosciences, MMSE, CDR-SB, activity

Citation: Bidzan L, Jurek P, Olech M, Bidzan-Wiącek M, Bidzan-Bluma I and Bidzan M (2023) Somatic comorbidity and the progression of cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1219449. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1219449

Received: 09 May 2023; Accepted: 02 November 2023;
Published: 17 November 2023.

Edited by:

Ashuin Kammar-García, Instituto Nacional de Geriatría, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Juan Francisco Flores-Vazquez, Instituto Nacional de Geriatría, Mexico
Barbara Gawda, Marie Curie-Sklodowska University, Poland
Cristian Ramos-Vera, Cesar Vallejo University, Peru

Copyright © 2023 Bidzan, Jurek, Olech, Bidzan-Wiącek, Bidzan-Bluma and Bidzan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mariola Bidzan, bWFyaW9sYS5iaWR6YW5AdWcuZWR1LnBs; Ilona Bidzan-Bluma, aWxvbmEuYmlkemFuLWJsdW1hQHBoZHN0dWQudWcuZWR1LnBs

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.