Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 16 March 2023
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs in a targeted stepping task are similar in young and older adults

Wouter MuijresWouter Muijres1Sylvie ArnalsteenSylvie Arnalsteen1Cas DaenensCas Daenens1Maarten Afschrift,&#x;Maarten Afschrift2,3Friedl De Groote
&#x;Friedl De Groote1*
  • 1Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 2Department of Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 3Robotics, Automation and Mechatronics, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Introduction: Stepping accuracy, speed, and stability are lower in older compared to young adults. Lower stepping performance in older adults may be due to larger accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs because of reduced ability to simultaneously fulfill these task-level goals. Our goal was to evaluate whether trade-offs are larger in older compared to young adults in a targeted stepping task. Since sensorimotor function declines with age, our secondary goal was to evaluate whether poorer sensorimotor function was associated with larger trade-offs.

Methods: Twenty-five young (median 22 years old) and 25 older (median 70 years old) adults stepped into projected targets in conditions with various levels of accuracy, speed, and stability requirements. We determined trade-offs as the change in performance, i.e., foot placement error, step duration, and mediolateral center of pressure path length, between each of these conditions and a control condition. To assess age-related differences in the magnitude of trade-offs, we compared the change in performance between age groups. Associations between trade-offs and measures of sensorimotor function were tested using correlations.

Results: We found an accuracy-speed and an accuracy-stability trade-off in both young and older adults, but trade-offs were not different between young and older adults. Inter-subject differences in sensorimotor function could not explain inter-subject differences in trade-offs.

Conclusion: Age-related differences in the ability to combine task-level goals do not explain why older adults stepped less accurate and less stable than young adults. However, lower stability combined with an age-independent accuracy-stability trade-off could explain lower accuracy in older adults.

1. Introduction

One out of three adults aged over 65 years old falls at least once a year (Blake et al., 1988; Salvà et al., 2004; Van Schooten, 2014), often leading to injuries (33% minor and 15% serious) and even deaths (Van Schooten, 2014; WHO, 2018). The most common cause of falling is incorrect body weight shifting (Robinovitch et al., 2013), which are a prerequisite to lift the swing foot of the ground when taking a step (Lyon and Day, 2005; Bancroft and Day, 2016). In young adults, pre-step weight shifts have been found to determine stepping accuracy and speed (Luchies et al., 2002; Bancroft and Day, 2016). Incorrect weight shifts may thus challenge balance by reducing the ability for timely and accurate foot placement to maintain balance. When the task is to step into a target, stepping accuracy may come at the cost of stability as restraining foot placement reduces available balance strategies to non-stepping strategies (Hof, 2008; Yiou et al., 2017). In addition, there is a trade-off between speed and accuracy. Healthy young adults step slower when they are required to step more accurate (Drury and Woolley, 1995; Roerdink et al., 2021). Compared to young adults, older adults step slower (Patla et al., 1993; Luchies et al., 2002; St George et al., 2007; Kurz et al., 2013) and less accurate (Lindemann et al., 2013; Caetano et al., 2016; Sun et al., 2017). Older adults may have more trouble than young adults to combine different stepping goals resulting in a larger decrease in performance on one task-level goal (e.g., accuracy) when the demands on another task-level goal are increased (e.g., speed or stability). The primary aim of our pre-registered study was to determine whether trade-offs between stepping speed, accuracy, and stability are larger in older compared to young adults.

Stepping accuracy, speed, and stability are reduced in older compared to young adults. Older adults step less accurate to targets during walking (Lindemann et al., 2013; Caetano et al., 2016). In gait initiation, older adults position their foot less accurate when stepping towards targets that move laterally and late during step initiation (Sun et al., 2017). In addition to lower accuracy, older adults are slower in forward stepping (Luchies et al., 2002; Kurz et al., 2013) and in a choice stepping task, where the target could appear in multiple positions (Patla et al., 1993; Luchies et al., 2002; St George et al., 2007). Stepping stability is lower in older than in young adults. More specifically, pre-step mediolateral center of pressure fluctuations and end-of-step mediolateral center of mass displacement are higher in older compared to young adults (Kurz et al., 2013; Singer et al., 2013). However, to our knowledge, age-related changes in stability have not been associated with speed or accuracy in targeted stepping.

Decreased stepping accuracy, speed, and stability in older adults may result from the inability to simultaneously fulfill these three task level goals. The trade-off between speed and accuracy has been well documented for targeted stepping (Drury and Woolley, 1995; Reynolds and Day, 2005). In young adults, foot positioning was slower when demands on accuracy increased, such as smaller targets or larger movement amplitudes (Drury and Woolley, 1995). The other way around, foot placement accuracy decreased with increasing speed in targeted stepping (Reynolds and Day, 2005). However, it is unclear whether there is an effect of age on the speed-accuracy trade-off. The accuracy-stability trade-off has been studied less extensively (Nonnekes et al., 2010; van der Veen et al., 2020). External balance support, that reduces balance control requirements during a stepping task, improves stepping accuracy (Nonnekes et al., 2010; van der Veen et al., 2020). It is however unclear if aging influences the accuracy-stability trade-off. The existence of a speed-stability trade-off in older adults has been suggested but has not been tested. For example, Luchies et al. (2002) and Patla et al. (1993) observed that older adults performed slower postural movements and larger weight shifts in preparation of a step and interpreted this as a strategy to increase stability at the cost of speed. Here, we systematically studied the effect of age on the trade-offs between speed, accuracy, and stability by manipulating stepping conditions.

Stepping accuracy, speed, and stability trade-offs may be larger in older than in young adults due to sensorimotor decline. Sensorimotor function declines with age resulting in higher motor noise (Singh et al., 2013) and reduced sensory function (Hurley et al., 1998; Owsley, 2011; Zalewski, 2015). The speed-accuracy trade-off has been suggested to emerge from motor noise (Harris and Wolpert, 1998) therefore age-related increases in motor noise may induce a larger speed-accuracy trade-off. As sensorimotor function is decreased in older adults, this effect may be larger in older compared to young adults. In addition, sensorimotor decline has been repeatedly associated with balance deficits (Ivers et al., 1998; Lord and Menz, 2000; Lord and Fitzpatrick, 2001; De Boer et al., 2004; Freeman et al., 2007). Indeed, results from our lab suggest that increased sensorimotor noise can explain age-related alterations in reactive standing balance (Afschrift et al., 2018). Age-related decreases in sensorimotor function may thus affect the ability to meet accuracy, speed, and stability goals simultaneously. The secondary aim of this study was therefore to explore the relationship between age-related decline in sensorimotor function and task-level goal trade-offs.

Summarized, our confirmatory hypothesis was that accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs would be larger in older compared to young adults. Moreover, our exploratory hypothesis was that larger accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs would be associated with poorer sensorimotor function. To test these hypotheses, we studied stepping into a target in young and older adults in a range of conditions with different accuracy, speed, and stability demands. For each condition, we measured accuracy, speed, and stability performance. We defined task-level goal trade-offs as the change in stepping performance for one task level goal (e.g., speed) as an effect of increasing the demands for the other task level goals (e.g., accuracy or stability). Additionally, we quantified sensorimotor function and performed a correlation analysis to test relationships between sensorimotor outcomes and task-level goal trade-offs.

2. Methods and protocol

2.1. Subjects

Twenty-five healthy young (median = 22, range = [20, 29] years, 12 females) and 25 healthy older adults (median = 70, range = [67, 89] years, 13 females) participated in the study, see Table 1. Prior to participation, subjects completed the Fall Efficacy Scale-International (FES-I) and the MoCA (Montreal Cognitive Assessment). Subjects were excluded when they were suffering from severe obesity (BMI > 30), severe orthostatic hypotension, acute neuromuscular injuries in the last 6 months, and orthopedic, neurological, cardiac, vestibular, or severe visual impairments. Subjects who were unable to initiate gait without a walking aid or took medication that negatively affected balance or gait were excluded from the experiment. The experimental protocol was approved by the ethical committee of UZ Leuven (number S64605) and subjects signed informed consent.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of study population.

2.2. Protocol

2.2.1. Step initiation experiment

Subjects performed a stepping task in which they stepped into projected targets starting from quiet standing (Figure 1B). Targets were constructed in D-Flow (Motekforce link, Amsterdam, Netherlands) and projected on an instrumented split belt treadmill that did not move during the experiment (M-Gait, Motekforce link, Amsterdam, Netherlands). Target size and location were determined based on the subject’s body dimensions. To assess performance trade-offs, accuracy, speed, and stability requirements were altered by manipulating target location and size or altering the instructions in four conditions.

Control condition. This condition served as the reference condition. Subjects were asked to step at comfortable pace into rectangular targets with width and length equal to the width and length of the subject’s foot. Step width and length were set equal to spina iliaca anterior superior distance (pelvic width) and 0.5 times the distance between lateral malleolus and trochanter major (i.e., leg length), respectively.

Accuracy condition. The accuracy condition was 2 and 5.1 cm larger in width and length, respectively. The demand on stepping accuracy was thus increased in the control condition with respect to the accuracy condition.

Speed condition. Demand on speed was increased with respect to the control condition by instructing the subject to step as fast as possible.

Stability condition. Demand on stability was increased with respect to the control condition by reducing the base of support at the end of the step by reducing step width to 0.5 times pelvic width.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Graphical impression of the experimental protocol. The effect of increasing accuracy, speed, and stability demands on stepping was tested by assessing the change in stepping outcomes as a result of reducing target size, increasing speed, and decreasing the base of support (panel A top to bottom). The horizontal arrows in panel (A) indicate in which conditions demand was increased. Subjects were asked to step into targets projected from standstill (panel B). In panel (A,B), starting targets (blue), the stepping leg target (purple), and the trailing leg target (yellow) are positioned as for a right dominant subject. Panel (C) shows marker placement to determine positioning of the foot with respect to the targets. Markers were placed on the posterior apex, lateral and medial aspects of the heel, head of metatarsal 1 and 5, and on the nail of the first toe (indicated by the arrow).

Subjects were instructed (1) to distribute their weight equally over their two feet before stepping to prevent anticipatory weight shifts, (2) to step into the targets with the dominant side first as soon as the targets appeared, (3) not to reposition their feet after placing the feet in the targets, (4) to step as accurate as possible at a comfortable stepping speed except for when explicitly being told to step as fast as possible (speed condition), and (5) to try to stand still as quickly as possible after finalizing the step.

Before the measurement started, subjects performed each condition one time to get acquainted with the experimental procedure. The order of the control, accuracy, speed, and stability conditions was randomized during the measurements.

2.2.2. Sensorimotor function

We collected measures of motor variability, proprioception, and reliance on sensory information during standing.

Motor variability was expressed as the variability in torque during a torque matching task (Singh et al., 2012). Dominant knee and ankle torque was recorded by a Biodex device at 1 kHz (Biodex Medical systems, Shirley NY, United States). The knee was tested while subjects were seated in upright position with the knee 90° flexed and the ankle slightly plantar flexed. The ankle was tested with the back rest slightly tilted backwards (85°), with upper leg support flexing the hip in 45° and the ankle in neutral position (Supplementary Figure 3). The joint’s center of rotation was aligned with that of the Biodex arm. Subjects were asked to perform three maximum voluntary contractions (MVC). The peak moment over these three trials represented MVC in following conditions. Consequently, subjects were asked to produce a torque of 15% MVC, 20% MVC, and 30 Nm for 15 s guided by visual feedback on a computer monitor. A blue line represented the target torque, a red line represented the produced torque, and a yellow block marked the 15 s-time window. Each condition was repeated three times with at least 30 s rest between trials.

Knee sensory function was assessed using a position matching task. Subjects were seated (approximately 90° knee flexion and 20° ankle plantar flexion) during the test. The examiner imposed target joint angles by passively moving the non-dominant knee. Subjects were then asked to match the examiner-imposed angle with the dominant leg and to indicate the moment at which both the dominant and non-dominant joint angles matched by pressing a hand-held trigger that sent a pulse to the recording system. Target knee joint angles were 10° or 20° flexion. Each target angle was measured 10 times. Joint angles were computed from marker trajectories recorded using motion capture (see below).

Contributions of vestibular, visual, and proprioceptive information to maintain upright posture was assessed by the Sensory Organization Test on the NeuroCom Smart Balance Master (NeuroCom International, Inc., Clackamas, OR, United States). Before testing, the subject was positioned on the Balance Master in agreement with device instructions and asked to stand as still as possible. Consequently, postural sway was measured in six different conditions while standing upright: (1) unperturbed, (2) eyes closed, (3) sway-referenced movement of the visual environment (i.e., visual environment moved along with subject such that visual information was reduced), (4) postural sway-referenced movement of the platform (i.e., platform moved such that the subject’s ankle angle remained quasi constant and proprioceptive information was reduced), (5) eyes closed with postural sway-referenced movement of the platform, and (6) postural sway-referenced movement of the visual environment and the platform. Every condition consisted of three 20 s trials.

2.2.3. Collection of kinetic and kinematic data

During the stepping and joint position matching tasks, 13 Vicon cameras (Vicon, Oxford Metrics, Oxford, England) measured the trajectories of markers placed on upper legs, lower legs, and feet (Supplementary Figure 1) at 100 Hz. Six markers on each foot registered foot placement with respect to the targets, see Figure 1C. Ground reaction forces under both legs were sampled at 1 kHz by the instrumented treadmill.

2.3. Data analysis

2.3.1. Kinetic and kinematic data

Markers trajectories and ground reaction forces were processed in MATLAB (R2021a, MathWorks, Natick, United States). After labelling marker data in Vicon’s Nexus 2.11, periods of missing marker data shorter than 1 s were interpolated. Ground reaction force and marker data were filtered using a 3rd order low pass bidirectional Butterworth filter with a 20 Hz cut-off frequency.

Kinematics were estimated using OpenSim 4.1 (Delp et al., 2007). First, a generic model (gait2392) was scaled to the subject’s dimensions based on a static trial using OpenSim’s Scaling tool. Then, joint angle trajectories for the proprioception trials were estimated using OpenSim’s Inverse Kinematics tool.

2.3.2. Stepping outcomes

We used foot placement error to quantify stepping accuracy, step duration to quantify stepping speed, and mediolateral center of pressure path length (COPpathml) to quantify stepping stability.

Foot placement error. When all six markers on the feet were inside the target, foot placement error was 0. Otherwise, we computed the maximal distance between the foot markers and the target edge in the mediolateral and anteroposterior direction (Figure 1C). Foot placement error was then calculated as the Euclidean norm of the mediolateral and anteroposterior foot placement errors and averaged over both legs. Foot placement error was evaluated over the foot flat phase, i.e., trailing limb toe-off to foot landing for the stepping leg and trailing limb foot landing to trailing foot weight acceptance for the trailing leg. For trailing foot weight acceptance, see the last paragraph of this section.

Step duration. The duration of a step was computed as the time between target appearance and trailing foot weight acceptance.

Mediolateral center of pressure path length. COPpathml was calculated over the 1 s period after non-dominant (trailing) leg foot landing. The COPpathml was normalized to pelvic width to be able to compare between individuals. COPpathml quantified the subject’s ability to stabilize posture after the step according to the instruction to stand still as quickly as possible after finalizing the step.

Stepping events were determined based on ground reaction force data. A threshold of 30 N on the vertical component of the ground reaction force was used to detect toe-off and foot landing events. Finally, we defined trailing foot weight acceptance as the moment the vertical ground reaction force increased over 10% of the subject’s body weight in the period after non-dominant foot landing. To represent stepping outcomes for each condition, we calculated the median over five repetitions per condition.

2.3.3. Trade-offs between task-level goals

We computed performance trade-offs between task-level goals by calculating the difference in stepping outcomes between the condition in which we altered stepping requirements and the control condition. For example, we evaluated the change in step duration (T) between the speed and control condition to evaluate the effect of increasing demand, see Figure 1A second row.

Δ T = T speed T control .

Next, we tested the effect of increasing speed on accuracy and stability by calculating the change in foot placement error ( ϵ ) and COPpathml between the speed and control conditions:

Δ ϵ = ϵ speed ϵ control ,
Δ CoPpath ml = COPpath ml speed COPpath ml control .

Δ Foot placement error and Δ COPpath ml represent the speed-accuracy and speed-stability trade-offs, respectively. We similarly evaluated the effect of increasing stability and accuracy demands.

2.3.4. Sensorimotor function outcomes

Torque variability. Torques measured from the Biodex were interpolated for missing values after which the signal was filtered using a 5th order bidirectional Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 25 Hz. Consequently, torque variability was estimated as the coefficient of variance over a 5 s time window, between 5 and 10 s after the start of the trial, for each joint and torque level separately. Per condition, torque variability for the ankle and knee was represented as the median coefficient of variance over trials. In the subsequent statistical analysis, we averaged the median coefficient of variance over conditions to obtain one value for the ankle and knee.

Proprioceptive accuracy and precision. Joint angle time series were averaged over the 0.1 s period after the trigger pulse was recorded. The joint matching error between the two knees was then calculated as the difference between the imposed and matched angle per trial. Finally, the mean and standard deviation in joint matching error over 20 trials were calculated to represent proprioceptive accuracy and precision.

Somatosensory (SOM), visual (VIS), vestibular (VEST), and visual preference (PREF) scores. For each of the six sensory organization test conditions, an equilibrium score EQi with i referring to the condition was computed. Equilibrium scores quantify postural stability by comparing the measured sway against the theoretical sway limit of 12.5° (Chaudhry et al., 2004). These equilibrium scores were combined to obtain four outcomes describing the subject’s reliance on sensory modalities for postural stability (Faraldo-García et al., 2012): SOM =  E Q 2 E Q 1 , VIS =  E Q 4 E Q 1 , VEST =  E Q 5 E Q 1 , and PREF =  E Q 3 + E Q 6 E Q 2 + E Q 5 . Where the VIS score measures the ability to use visual information to maintain quite standing and the PREF score measures the ability to suppress visual information that is incongruent with the body’s spatial orientation.

2.4. Statistical analysis

To determine whether trade-offs between task-level goals in stepping were larger in older compared to young adults, the six trade-off measures were compared between age groups using Multivariable Analysis of Variance (MANOVA). We considered stepping performance trade-offs as separate variables in the MANOVA. Consequently, we identified differences between age groups by means of two-tailed pairwise comparison. We corrected for the effect of multiple comparisons based on the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. Test outcomes were considered significant below an alpha value of 0.05.

Finally, we explored the hypothesis that age-related differences in task-level goal trade-offs were associated with decreases in sensorimotor function. Before calculating correlations, data was mean corrected for each group. When statistical assumptions were met, we tested relationships using Pearson correlations. Otherwise, we used Spearman’s rho ( ρ s p e a r m a n ) to assess relationships. As in this part of the analysis we tested our exploratory hypothesis, we corrected correlations for multiple comparisons for proprioception, torque fluctuation, and sensory organization test outcomes separately. One older subject was removed from the analysis of proprioceptive and force fluctuation data as this subject was unable to comply with the measurement protocol.

The statistical analysis was carried out in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria), version 4.1.3 (R Core Team, 2022).

2.4.1. Preregistration

Hypotheses, the experimental protocol, data processing, and analysis steps were preregistered on the Open Science Framework website (Muijres et al., 2022a). Deviations from the preregistered documents are described in the Supplementary material.

3. Results

3.1. Preregistered outcomes

We found trade-offs between task-level goals as we increased stepping demands (V = 0.35, F (6,43) = 3.9, p < 0.01) but these were not different between young and older adults. Foot placement error increased with increasing demand on speed and stability (median = 0.43 cm, interquartile range (iqr) = 1.25 cm, p = 0.03 and median = 0.47 cm, iqr = 0.88 cm, p < 0.01 respectively, Figures 2B,C). MANOVA test results indicated that trade-offs were different between young and older adults (V = 0.25, F (6,43) = 2.41, p = 0.04). However, after performing pairwise tests with correction for multiple comparisons, we did not find a difference between groups.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Stepping outcomes with increasing task-demands. Circles and triangles represent individual data points for young and older adults, respectively. Plots show the change in foot placement error, step duration, and mediolateral center of pressure path length (in the rows in top-to-bottom order) as a result of increasing accuracy, speed, and stability demands (in the columns in left-to-right order). Panels (A,E,I), show changes in task-level goal performance (i.e., stepping outcomes) on task-goals in which demand was increased. These plots thus show the effectiveness of increasing task demands. E.g. panel (E) shows whether increasing demand on speed successfully decreased step duration. Panels (B–H) show the change in stepping performance resulting from task-level goal trade-offs, i.e., the extent to which increased demand on one task goal leads to decreased performance on the others. E.g. panel (B) illustrates the change in foot placement error resulting from increasing speed, i.e., the speed-accuracy trade-off. *Indicates when increasing stepping demands resulted in a change in stepping outcomes that was statistically significant different from zero after correcting for multiple comparisons.

We found few correlations between task-level goal trade-offs and sensorimotor function. Changes in mediolateral COP path length with increasing demand on speed correlated with SOM score ( ρ spearman  = 0.47, p = 0.01). Correlations between trade-offs and other sensorimotor function outcomes (i.e., knee and ankle torque variability, knee joint matching error and standard deviation matching error, and other sensory ratios) did not reach significance after correction for multiple comparison.

3.2. Unregistered outcomes

The accuracy, speed and stability conditions successfully increased demand on stepping requirements (V = 0.97, F(3,46) = 575.06, p < 0.001). Foot placement errors increased when target sizes decreased (median = 1.73 cm, iqr = 0.49 cm, p < 0.001), step duration decreased when subjects were instructed to step faster (median = −0.55 s, iqr = 0.30 s, p < 0.001), and mediolateral COP path length decreased when step width in the target position decreased (median = −0.31 a.u., iqr = 0.14 a.u., p < 0.001; Figures 2A,E,I). These results indicate that stepping conditions successfully challenged accuracy, speed, and stability.

Stepping performance was poorer in older than in young adults. Foot placement error was larger in older compared to young individuals in the control and stability condition. In the control condition, median foot placement error was 2.41 cm (iqr = 0.61 cm) in older and 1.88 cm (iqr = 0.69 cm) in younger adults, p = 0.03. In the stability condition, median foot placement error was 2.80 cm (iqr = 0.93 cm) in older and 2.26 cm (iqr = 0.77 cm) in younger adults, p = 0.03. Also in the stability condition, mediolateral COP path length was larger in older (median = 0.43 a.u., iqr = 0.14 a.u.) compared to young adults (median = 0.38 a.u., iqr = 0.06 a.u.), p = 0.03 (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Significant differences in stepping outcomes between young and older adults. Between group differences in foot placement error in the control condition [Panel (A)], and mediolateral center of pressure path length and foot placement error in the stability condition [Panel (B,C)] with individual data points for young (circle) and older adults (triangles). The group median and interquartile range are represented by the red circle and whiskers.

Knee joint position matching error and the standard deviation of the matching error were not different between young and older adults (Table 2). Additionally, the coefficients of variance in torque production for the knee and the ankle were not different between young and older adults (Table 2). Two of the four scores on the sensory organization test were lower in older adults. The VIS score was lower in older (median = 0.80) compared to young adults (median = 0.86), p < 0.01, indicating that reducing proprioceptive information induced a larger increase in sway in older than in young adults. VEST score was also lower in older (median = 0.57) compared to young adults (median = 0.72), p < 0.01, indicating that reducing proprioceptive and visual information induced a larger increase in sway in older than in young adults.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Between group differences in sensorimotor outcomes.

Raw data and processing scripts were published for replication and reuse (Muijres et al., 2022b).

4. Discussion

We found no differences in the magnitude of the accuracy-speed-stability trade-off between young and older adults although stepping performance was worse in older adults. Older adults stepped less accurate than young adults in the control and stability condition, indicating clear differences between groups. In the stability condition, older adults also showed more center of pressure movements at the end of the step, suggesting that they needed more balance correcting movements than young adults when stability requirements were high. As we did not find differences in accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs between groups, reduced stepping accuracy and stability in older adults was not caused by a reduced ability to combine different task-level goals in the stepping task. We thus reject our confirmatory hypothesis that larger task-level goal trade-offs contribute to decreased performance in older adults. Additionally, sensorimotor function could not explain inter-individual differences in task-level goal trade-offs. We therefore also reject the hypothesis that poorer sensorimotor function is related to larger trade-offs between stepping accuracy, speed, and stability. Nevertheless, we found that older adults have similar speed-accuracy and accuracy-stability trade-offs to young adults. Due to the accuracy-stability trade-off, a reduced ability to control balance may have led to less accurate steps in older adults.

The observed speed-accuracy trade-off across age groups is partly in line with previous findings. We observed that foot placement error increased when asking participants to step as fast as possible compared to stepping at self-selected speed. Reynolds and Day (2005) found a similar increase in foot placement errors when giving young adults 300 ms (fast) instead of 600 ms (slow) to complete their step. However, our observation, that stepping speed did not decrease when increasing accuracy demands, contrasts a previous study by Drury and Woolley (1995). They found a decrease in speed when reducing target size. These contrasting findings may be due to differences in experimental conditions. First, the difference in stepping accuracy requirements between conditions was smaller in our study (approx. 20%) than in the study of Drury and Woolley (approx. 300%). Second, we imposed stepping accuracy less stringently than Drury and Woolley. Drury and Woolley requested subjects to repeat the step in case of a foot placement error (Drury and Woolley, 1995).

The observed accuracy-stability trade-off across age groups suggests a conflict between stepping to the target and foot placement to control balance. Similar to our findings, Nonnekes et al. (2010) found a decrease in stepping accuracy in stroke survivors and age matched controls when the base of support at the end of the step was decreased in a stepping task. This indicates that reducing the base of support at the end of the step causes a conflict between stepping to the target and foot placement to control balance. In the same study, Nonnekes et al. found that stepping accuracy increased when individuals, and especially stroke survivors, used crutches that increased their base of support during the stepping task. This effect is also seen in walking where the use of crutches improved foot placement accuracy in both young and older adults (van der Veen et al., 2020). Hence, conditions that improve balance, either through a wider step as in our study or upper limb support as reported in literature, improve stepping accuracy, probably by reducing the need for foot placement as a balance control strategy.

Reduced ability to control balance may have led to less accurate steps in older adults. Several observations seem to indicate that our group of older adults had a reduced ability to control balance. First, mediolateral COP path length was larger in older adults when the base of support at the end of the step was decreased with respect to the control condition. Second, older adults scored lower on the sensory organization test conditions in which the platform was sway referenced, i.e., a lower score on EQ4, EQ5, and EQ6 (Supplementary Table 1). Given the accuracy-stability trade-off (in both young and older adults), lower stability in older than young adults may have been the origin of less accurate stepping in the stability condition. Possibly, reduced accuracy in the control condition may be attributed to reduced stability. We did not observe any differences in stability in the control condition between young and older adults based on COP path length at the end of the step, but our stability measure might not have captured all age-related differences in stability. In general, it is hard to determine stability in experiments that did not cause a loss of balance (Bruijn et al., 2013).

Older adults had the same preferred stepping speed as young adults, which is in contrast to previous observations. For example, older adults have repeatedly been observed to step slower than young adults in a choice stepping task, in which subjects step to targets that appear in multiple locations (Patla et al., 1993; Halliday et al., 1998; Polcyn et al., 1998; Luchies et al., 2002). In these earlier studies, authors hypothesized that older adults increase the time in the weight shifting phase because they prioritize stability over stepping speed (Patla et al., 1993; Luchies et al., 2002). Although that weight shifting was a prerequisite in this study’s stepping task, older adults were not slower than young adults. We did not find a speed-stability trade-off in both age groups indicating that both young and older adults are able to increase speed without compromising balance. A difference in cognitive load between paradigms may explain the different findings. The cognitive load is higher in a choice stepping task where the target direction is variable in contrast to our stepping task where the target always appeared in front of the subject. Increased cognitive load was previously found to amplify age-related differences in stepping (St George et al., 2007). It might therefore be useful to study whether age influences the speed-stability trade-off in a task with higher cognitive load.

Inter-subject variability in trade-offs cannot be explained by variability in sensorimotor function, as we found few associations between sensorimotor outcomes and trade-offs. We explored 48 potential associations between trade-offs and sensorimotor function and found only one positive association between the change in mediolateral COP path length with increasing speed and the somatosensory score on the sensory organization test. Individuals with a low somatosensory score, i.e., low equilibrium score in the eyes closed relative to the eyes open condition, depend more on visual information to stand still. Hence, the association between COP path and somatosensory score would indicate that individuals who relied more on visual information for postural balance reduced mediolateral COP path length more when speed increased. However, it is questionable whether further research could confirm this relationship as we only partially accounted for multiple comparisons, i.e., only within proprioception, torque variability, and sensory organization test outcomes separately. Since we only find one relationship among the many relationships we tested, we conclude that variability in trade-offs were poorly explained by sensorimotor outcomes.

We did not find differences in proprioception between age groups, in agreement with a recent study (van der Kruk et al., 2022) but contrasting a series of other studies (Skinner et al., 1984; Kaplan et al., 1985; Petrella et al., 1997). We used a common test of proprioception (Goble et al., 2009) but acknowledge that assessing proprioception is challenging. It might be useful to assess other aspects of proprioception in future studies. For example, motion and dynamic position sense were found to be lower in older than in young adults (Goble et al., 2009). Moreover, it might be worthwhile to first explore the relation between accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs in fall-prone older adults, which have been shown to have proprioceptive deficits (Lord and Fitzpatrick, 2001; Pijnappels et al., 2010).

Task-level goal trade-offs and sensorimotor deficits may be larger in fall-prone compared to healthy older adults. In stroke survivors, increasing the base of support resulted in larger improvements in stepping accuracy than in age matched healthy controls (Nonnekes et al., 2010). This suggests that accuracy-stability trade-offs may be larger in individuals with balance deficits. Moreover, it has been suggested that the lower stepping speed in fall prone older adults compared to healthy older adults may be due to a larger stability-speed trade-off in fall-prone older individuals (Tisserand et al., 2016). Yet, this remains to be investigated. If accuracy-speed-stability tradeoffs are indeed larger in fall prone compared to healthy older adults, testing for relationships between sensorimotor function and trade-offs may shed light on potential mechanisms. Sensorimotor function, and in particular proprioception, was found to predict falls (Lord and Fitzpatrick, 2001; Pijnappels et al., 2010) indicating that the relationship between stepping stability and sensorimotor function may be stronger in fall-prone compared to healthy older adults.

5. Conclusion

Older adults stepped less accurate and less stable than young adults but we did not find any age-related differences in the ability to simultaneously fulfill different task-level goals that could have contributed to this reduced performance in older adults. Yet, we observed an age-independent accuracy-stability trade-off, which might have led to the lower accuracy in older adults as a trade-off for prioritizing stability. Our exploratory analysis suggests that inter-subject variability in accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs could not be explained by variability in sensorimotor function. Future work should investigate whether accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs are altered in fall-prone as compared to healthy older adults.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://doi.org/10.48804/6GRWWE.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee Research UZ/KU Leuven. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

FG, MA, and WM developed the study concept. FG, MA, SA, CD, and WM designed the experiments. After the design of the study FG, MA, and WM preregistered the study, interpreted the results, wrote, and edited the manuscript. SA, CD, and WM acquired the data. WM processed and analyzed the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by ‘Fonds Wetenschappelijk onderzoek’ (FWO), Research foundation Flanders. The project was funded by a FWO research project (G088420N) received by FG and a FWO fellowship (1SF7322N) received by WM.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all subjects for participating in the experiments. During the experiments we received support from master students. In particular, we would like to thank Dries De Moor and Michiel Follon for their support during the experiments. We received support from Steffen Fieuws (Leuven Biostatistics and Statistical Bioinformatics Centre) on the design of the statistical analysis. The authors would like to thank Jente Willaert for reading the manuscript and providing feedback.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2023.1130707/full#supplementary-material

References

Afschrift, M., de Groote, F., Verschueren, S., and Jonkers, I. (2018). Increased sensory noise and not muscle weakness explains changes in non-stepping postural responses following stance perturbations in healthy elderly. Gait Posture 59, 122–127. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bancroft, M. J., and Day, B. L. (2016). The throw-and-catch model of human gait: evidence from coupling of pre-step postural activity and step location. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:635. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, A. J., Morgan, K., Bendall, M. J., Dallosso, H., Ebrahim, S. B., Arie, T. H., et al. (1988). Falls by elderly people at home: prevalence and associated factors. Age Ageing 17, 365–372. doi: 10.1093/ageing/17.6.365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruijn, S. M., Meijer, O. G., Beek, P. J., and van Dieën, J. H. (2013). Assessing the stability of human locomotion: a review of current measures. J. R. Soc. Interface 10:20120999. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2012.0999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caetano, M. J. D., Lord, S. R., Schoene, D., Pelicioni, P. H. S., Sturnieks, D. L., and Menant, J. C. (2016). Age-related changes in gait adaptability in response to unpredictable obstacles and stepping targets. Gait Posture 46, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudhry, H., Findley, T., Quigley, K. S., Bukiet, B., Ji, Z., Sims, T., et al. (2004). Measures of postural stability. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 41, 713–720. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2003.09.0140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Boer, M. R., Pluijm, S. M. F., Lips, P., Moll, A. C., Völker-Dieben, H. J., Deeg, D. J. H., et al. (2004). Different aspects of visual impairment as risk factors for falls and fractures in older men and women. J. Bone Miner. Res. 19, 1539–1547. doi: 10.1359/JBMR.040504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delp, S. L., Anderson, F. C., Arnold, A. S., Loan, P., Habib, A., John, C. T., et al. (2007). OpenSim: open-source software to create and analyze dynamic simulations of movement. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 54, 1940–1950. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2007.901024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drury, C. G., and Woolley, S. M. (1995). Visually-controlled leg movements embedded in a walking task. Ergonomics 38, 714–722. doi: 10.1080/00140139508925143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraldo-García, A., Santos-Pérez, S., Crujeiras-Casais, R., Labella-Caballero, T., and Soto-Varela, A. (2012). Influence of age and gender in the sensory analysis of balance control. Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 269, 673–677. doi: 10.1007/s00405-011-1707-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, E. E., Mun~oz, B., Rubin, G., and West, S. K. (2007). Visual field loss increases the risk of falls in older adults: the Salisbury eye evaluation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 48, 4445–4450. doi: 10.1167/IOVS.07-0326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goble, D. J., Coxon, J. P., Wenderoth, N., van Impe, A., and Swinnen, S. P. (2009). Proprioceptive sensibility in the elderly: degeneration, functional consequences and plastic-adaptive processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 271–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, S. E., Winter, D. A., Frank, J. S., Patla, A. E., and Prince, F. (1998). The initiation of gait in young, elderly, and Parkinson’s disease subjects. Gait Posture 8, 8–14. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(98)00020-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C. M., and Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780–784. doi: 10.1038/29528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, A. L. (2008). The “extrapolated center of mass” concept suggests a simple control of balance in walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 27, 112–125. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley, M. V., Rees, J., and Newham, D. J. (1998). Quadriceps function, proprioceptive acuity and functional performance in healthy young, middle-aged and elderly subjects. Age Ageing 27, 55–62. doi: 10.1093/ageing/27.1.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivers, R. Q., Cumming, R. G., Mitchell, P., and Attebo, K. (1998). Visual impairment and falls in older adults: the Blue Mountains eye study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 46, 58–64. doi: 10.1111/J.1532-5415.1998.TB01014.X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, F. S., Nixon, J. E., Reitz, M., Rindfleish, L., and Tucker, J. (1985). Age-related changes in proprioception and sensation of joint position. Acta Orthop. 56, 72–74. doi: 10.3109/17453678508992984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurz, I., Berezowski, E., and Melzer, I. (2013). Frontal plane instability following rapid voluntary stepping: effects of age and a concurrent cognitive task. J. Gerontol. A 68, 1402–1408. doi: 10.1093/GERONA/GLT040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindemann, U., Klenk, J., Becker, C., and Moe-Nilssen, R. (2013). Assessment of adaptive walking performance. Med. Eng. Phys. 35, 217–220. doi: 10.1016/J.MEDENGPHY.2012.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S. R., and Fitzpatrick, R. C. (2001). Choice stepping reaction time: a composite measure of falls risk in older people. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 56, M627–M632. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.10.M627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S. R., and Menz, H. B. (2000). Visual contributions to postural stability in older adults. Gerontology 46, 306–310. doi: 10.1159/000022182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luchies, C. W., Schiffman, J., Richards, L. G., Thompson, M. R., Bazuin, D., and DeYoung, A. J. (2002). Effects of age, step direction, and reaction condition on the ability to step quickly. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 57, M246–M249. doi: 10.1093/gerona/57.4.M246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyon, I. N., and Day, B. L. (2005). Predictive control of body mass trajectory in a two-step sequence. Exp. Brain Res. 161, 193–200. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2058-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muijres, W., Afschrift, M., and De Groote, F. (2022a). What is the effect of ageing on task-level goal performance trade-offs and is there a relationship with sensorimotor noise? doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/XKA53

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muijres, W., Arnalsteen, S., Daenens, C., Afschrift, M., and De Groote, F. (2022b). Replication data for: accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs in a targeted stepping task are similar in young and older adults. KU Leuven RDR. doi: 10.48804/6GRWWE

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nonnekes, J. H., Talelli, P., de Niet, M., Reynolds, R. F., Weerdesteyn, V., and Day, B. L. (2010). Deficits underlying impaired visually triggered step adjustments in mildly affected stroke patients. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 24, 393–400. doi: 10.1177/1545968309348317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owsley, C. (2011). ‘Aging and vision’, vision research. NIH public. Access 51, 1610–1622. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patla, A. E., Frank, J. S., Winter, D. A., Rietdyk, S., Prentice, S., and Prasad, S. (1993). Age-related changes in balance control system: initiation of stepping. Clin. Biomech. 8, 179–184. doi: 10.1016/0268-0033(93)90012-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrella, R. J., Lattanzio, P. J., and Nelson, M. G. (1997). Effect of age and activity on knee joint proprioception. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 76, 235–241. doi: 10.1097/00002060-199705000-00015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pijnappels, M., Delbaere, K., Sturnieks, D. L., and Lord, S. R. (2010). The association between choice stepping reaction time and falls in older adults-a path analysis model. Age Ageing 39, 99–104. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afp200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polcyn, A. F., Lipsitz, L. A., Kerrigan, D. C., and Collins, J. J. (1998). Age-related changes in the initiation of gait: degradation of central mechanisms for momentum generation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 79, 1582–1589. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90425-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2022) ‘R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing ’. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Reynolds, R. F., and Day, B. L. (2005). Visual guidance of the human foot during a step. J. Physiol. 569, 677–684. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.095869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinovitch, S. N., Feldman, F., Yang, Y., Schonnop, R., Leung, P. M., Sarraf, T., et al. (2013). Video capture of the circumstances of falls in elderly people residing in long-term care: an observational study. Lancet 381, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61263-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roerdink, M., Geerse, D. J., Peper, C., and Lieke, E. (2021). Haste makes waste: the tradeoff between walking speed and target-stepping accuracy. Gait Posture 85, 110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2021.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvà, A., Bolíbar, I., Pera, G., and Arias, C. (2004). Incidence and consequences of falls among elderly people living in the community. Med. Clin. 122, 172–176. doi: 10.1016/s0025-7753(04)74184-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, J. C., Prentice, S. D., and McIlroy, W. E. (2013). Age-related changes in mediolateral dynamic stability control during volitional stepping. Gait Posture 38, 679–683. doi: 10.1016/J.GAITPOST.2013.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, N. B., König, N., Arampatzis, A., Heller, M. O., and Taylor, W. R. (2012). Extreme levels of noise constitute a key neuromuscular deficit in the elderly. PLoS One 7:e48449. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, N. B., König, N., Arampatzis, A., and Taylor, W. R. (2013). Age-related modifications to the magnitude and periodicity of neuromuscular noise. PLoS One 8, 2–7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, H. B., Barrack, R. L., and Cook, S. D. (1984). Age related decline in proprioception. Clin. Orthop. Relat. Res. 184, pp. 208???211, –211. doi: 10.1097/00003086-198404000-00035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St George, R. J., Fitzpatrick, R. C., Rogers, M. W., and Lord, S. R. (2007). Choice stepping response and transfer times: effects of age, fall risk, and secondary tasks. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 62, 537–542. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.5.537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, R., Cui, C., and Shea, J. B. (2017). Aging effect on step adjustments and stability control in visually perturbed gait initiation. Gait Posture 58, 268–273. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tisserand, R., Robert, T., Chabaud, P., Bonnefoy, M., and Chèze, L. (2016). Elderly fallers enhance dynamic stability through anticipatory postural adjustments during a choice stepping reaction time. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:613. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kruk, E., Strutton, P., Koizia, L. J., Fertleman, M., Reilly, P., and Bull, A. M. J. (2022). Why do older adults stand-up differently to young adults?: investigation of compensatory movement strategies in sit-to-walk, npj. Aging 8, 1–19. doi: 10.1038/S41514-022-00094-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Veen, S. M., Hammerbeck, U., and Hollands, K. L. (2020). How accuracy of foot-placement is affected by the size of the base of support and crutch support in stroke survivors and healthy adults. Gait Posture 76, 224–230. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Schooten, K. S. (2014). Predicting falls: Amount and quality of daily-life gait as risk factors, thesis.

Google Scholar

WHO (2018). Falls. Available at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/falls (Accessed June 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

Yiou, E., Caderby, T., Delafontaine, A., Fourcade, P., and Honeine, J. L. (2017). Balance control during gait initiation: state-of-the-art and research perspectives. World J. Orthopaed. 8, 815–828. doi: 10.5312/wjo.v8.i11.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalewski, C. K. (2015). Aging of the human vestibular system. Semin. Hear. 36, 175–196. doi: 10.1055/s-0035-1555120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: task requirements, postural control, voluntary stepping, locomotion, motor control

Citation: Muijres W, Arnalsteen S, Daenens C, Afschrift M and De Groote F (2023) Accuracy-speed-stability trade-offs in a targeted stepping task are similar in young and older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1130707. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1130707

Received: 23 December 2022; Accepted: 24 February 2023;
Published: 16 March 2023.

Edited by:

Rubem C. A. Guedes, Federal University of Pernambuco, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Irene Sintini, Mayo Clinic, United States
Carolina Rodrigues Alves Silveira, Lawson Health Research Institute, Canada

Copyright © 2023 Muijres, Arnalsteen, Daenens, Afschrift and De Groote. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Friedl De Groote, friedl.degroote@kuleuven.be

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.