Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 22 July 2022
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Physical Exercise for Age-Related Neuromusculoskeletal Disorders View all 36 articles

Exploring the effects of peripheral sensibility on visuospatial and postural capacities during goal-directed movements in long-term Tai Chi practitioners

\r\nZhufeng ShaoZhufeng Shao1Li LiLi Li2Min MaoMin Mao3Wei SunWei Sun1Cui ZhangCui Zhang4Qipeng Song*Qipeng Song1*
  • 1College of Sports and Health, Shandong Sport University, Jinan, China
  • 2Department of Health Sciences and Kinesiology, Georgia Southern University, Statesboro, GA, United States
  • 3School of Nursing and Rehabilitation, Cheeloo College of Medicine, Shandong University, Jinan, China
  • 4Laboratory of Biomechanics, Shandong Institute of Sport Science, Jinan, China

Background: Falls are directly related to visuospatial ability and postural stability. Perturbations of upper body movements pose a challenge to older adults and may cause falls. This study investigated visuospatial ability and postural stability during goal-directed upper body movements between the Tai Chi and control groups and tried to connect them with their sensations.

Materials and methods: Thirty-seven older adults were recruited to perform the touch (TT) and blind touch (BTT) tasks. The target positioning error (TPE), ankle proprioception, tactile sensation, time to stabilization (TTS), and maximum displacement (Dmax) of the center of pressure trajectory were compared between the groups during the tasks. The relationships of visuospatial ability and postural stability to proprioception and tactile sensation were investigated.

Results: Dmax in the mediolateral (DmaxML) direction decreased during BTT compared to TT among the Tai Chi group but not the control group. Compared to the control group, less Dmax in the anterio-posterior (DmaxAP) direction, and shorter TTS in AP/ML (TTSAP/TTSML) directions were observed among the Tai Chi group. Compared to TT, DmaxAP decreased during the BTT. The Tai Chi group had less TPE in the vertical (TPEV) direction and in three-dimensional space. Among the Tai Chi group, TPEV, TTSML, and DmaxAP were correlated to their proprioception during plantarflexion; TTSAP was correlated to tactile sensation at the great toe during the TT and BTT; DmaxAP was correlated to tactile sensation at the great toe during the TT. Among the control group, TTSML was correlated to ankle proprioception during dorsiflexion and plantarflexion during the BTT.

Conclusion: Long-term Tai Chi practitioners exhibited superior visuospatial ability and postural stability during goal-directed upper body movements, which was associated with sensitive proprioception and tactile sensation.

Introduction

Falls in older adults pose a severe problem and are the fifth leading cause of death, following cancer, heart disease, stroke, and respiratory diseases (Canada, 2011). Standing with goal-directed upper body movements (reaching and fitting) is an important activity in daily life and is closely associated with motor, sensory, or cognitive functions among older adults (Hyndman et al., 2002; Pan et al., 2016). Goal-directed upper body reaching movements require the visuospatial ability of individuals (Hondzinski et al., 2010), and the perturbations of upper body movements under such tasks challenge the postural stability among older adults (Pan et al., 2016).

Falls are directly related to visuospatial ability (Naslund, 2010) and postural stability (Liu et al., 2012), especially during goal-directed upper body movements. Visuospatial ability is often used in describing how the mind organizes and understands two- and three-dimensional spaces (Pinker, 1984) and is one of the essential abilities in daily life (Martin et al., 2009). Poor visuospatial ability is associated with a greater risk of falls among older adults (Naslund, 2010). The target positioning error (TPE) measured during a blind touch task (BTT) could be used as an important indicator for evaluating visuospatial ability (McIntyre et al., 2000), indicated by a distance between the participant’s pointed position by memory and the original target position after the target was removed. Less TPE indicated better visuospatial ability (McIntyre et al., 2000). Postural stability was often used to reflect an individual’s ability to control their posture during locomotion (Kang and Dingwell, 2006). A proper postural control strategy was needed to restore the body’s stability when needed (Haddad et al., 2012). Greater postural stability enhanced the ability to resist perturbations, thereby reducing the risk of falls (Latash et al., 2010). Postural stability could be reflected by time to stabilization (TTS) (Ross and Guskiewicz, 2003, 2004; Fransz et al., 2015) and maximum displacement (Dmax) of the center of pressure (COP) trajectory during goal-directed upper body movements (Prieto et al., 1996; Pan et al., 2016). Shorter TTS (Fransz et al., 2015; Sherrington et al., 2019) or the less Dmax indicated better postural stability and reduction in fall risk (Prieto et al., 1996; Pan et al., 2016).

A few types of physical activities are effective in reducing fall risk among older adults (Bangsbo et al., 2019). As a traditional Chinese multi-genre fitness exercise, Tai Chi might be an option for improving visuospatial ability and postural stability. Visuospatial ability (Nemoto et al., 2020) and postural stability (Ghai et al., 2017) requires combinations of cognition and movement coordination. Tai Chi encompasses motor (a series of body movements) and mental (mind concentration) elements (Wayne et al., 2014; Song et al., 2018), and has been proven to improve motor and cognitive functions among older adults (Solianik et al., 2021). Further, practicing Tai Chi improved the sensitivity of proprioception and tactile sensation (Sun et al., 2015; Hu et al., 2021), which were positively related to visuospatial ability (Hondzinski et al., 2010) and postural stability (Zhang et al., 2015). Moreover, Tai Chi involves upper body movements supported by lower extremities to maintain postural stability, similar to the goal-directed upper body movements. Therefore, visuospatial ability and postural stability during goal-directed upper body movements may benefit from Tai Chi practice.

As individual ages, their visuospatial ability and postural stability decline rapidly, twice as fast as the decline in memory (Murre et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2019). Therefore, a suitable exercise that delays or reverses a decline in visuospatial ability and postural stability among older adults is urgently needed. Although the relationship of visuospatial ability and postural stability to sensations has been investigated, the different roles of its two main components, proprioception, and tactile sensation, have yet been fully understood. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to investigate the benefits of Tai Chi practice for visuospatial ability and postural stability during goal-directed upper body movements and their relationship to practitioners’ visuospatial ability and postural stability were related to their proprioception and tactile sensations. It is hypothesis that 1. compared to the control group, Tai Chi practitioners have better visuospatial ability and postural stability; 2. The visuospatial ability and postural stability are positively correlated with the sensitivity of proprioception and tactile sensation.

Materials and methods

The study design is analytical cross-sectional.

Participants

An a priori power analysis (G*Power Version 3.1) indicated that a minimum of 15 participants was needed in each group to obtain an alpha level of 0.05 and a beta level of 0.80 based on a previous report, in which the TTS was compared after the matched bias (2.75 ± 0.85) or unmatched bias (3.76 ± 1.01) exercises (Tulloch et al., 2012). The current study recruited 37 older adults aged 65∼77 years through flyers, leaflets, and advocacy from local communities (Table 1). The inclusion criteria were as follows: age ≥ 65 years, long-term Tai Chi practice experience (at least four times per week, 1 h each time, for more than 5 years) for the Tai Chi group, and absence of regular exercise (total exercise time less than 1 h per week in the past 3 years) for the control group. The exclusion criteria were movement disorders or nervous system diseases, recent lower extremity and dominant arm surgery, cardiovascular pathologies, diabetes or hepatorenal syndrome, coordination function disorders, peripheral neuritis, Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores < 24. All participants were right arm dominant, defined by the outstretched hand to reach an object (Haddad et al., 2008). All the participants signed informed consent forms before the formal test. The project was approved by the Ethics Committee of Shandong Sports University (2020108) and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Basic information of the participants in Tai Chi and control groups.

Testing protocol

Touch task and blind touch task

Each participant wore experimental shoes provided by the laboratory (Flattie, Qingdao Luzhong Co. Ltd., Qingdao, China). A reflective marker (Marker-1) was attached to a metal bar (height = 2 m, diameter = 1.5 cm) with a solid base; another reflective marker (Marker-2) was attached to the tip of the index finger of a participant’s dominant arm. The metal bar was removable, and the height of Maker-1 was adjustable. A force plate (AMTI 600*900, AMTI Inc., Watertown, MA, United States) was used to collect force data at 1,000 Hz. A 12-camera motion analysis system (Vicon, Oxford Metrics, Yarnton, England) was used to collect the markers’ three-dimensional data at 100 Hz. The force plate and motion analysis system were collected via the Vicon system with internal synchronization.

The location of the marker-1 is adjustable. The horizontal location could be adjusted by moving the bar forward or backward, the vertical location could be adjusted by attaching the marker to the high or low part of the bar. The height of Marker-1 was adjusted to the participant’s shoulder joint height with 1.3 times the dominant arm length to the shoulder joint horizontally. The metal bar location was adjusted to allow participants to stand on the center of the force plate. Each participant was asked to complete 2 tasks in a fixed order, namely, touch task (TT) and BTT. During the TT (Figure 1A), each participant stood quietly on the force plate for 30 s for stabilization. When the command “start” was given, the participants raised their dominant arms and used Marker-2 to touch Marker-1 at a comfortable speed, then brought their arms back to the initial position and stood still for another 30 s. During the BTT (Figure 1B), the metal bar with Marker-1 was removed from sight manually about 1 s before individual movement, and the participants could see the process of moving out (Hondzinski et al., 2010). The participants raised their dominant arms and used Marker-2 to point to the remembered position of Marker-1, and then return to a stable standing position as soon as possible. Five trials were performed for each task. Rests were taken between each round of data collection for as long as requested by the participant (Hondzinski et al., 2010). Before data collection, the participants had 10 min to familiarize the test protocol, and their height, weight, dominant arm length, and shoulder height were recorded. The protocol was similar to a previous study, in which the effects of aging and sensory deficits were examined (Hondzinski et al., 2010).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The illustration about the actual testing protocol. (A) Touch task; (B) blind touch task; (C) proprioception test; (D) tactile sensation tests.

Proprioception test

The proprioception threshold at the ankle joint of the dominant leg was assessed using a proprioception test device (AP-II, Sunny Co. Ltd., Jinan, China) (Figure 1C). Good test-retest reliability (ICC value, 0.74∼0.94) for the device has been reported previously (Sun et al., 2015). The dominant leg was defined as the preferred leg for kicking a football in the lab. The minimum angular motion that the patient can detect during ankle dorsiflexion/plantarflexion was collected by using the proprioception test device. The device consists of a box and a platform that can rotate within the frontal and sagittal planes. Two electric motors drive the platform at an angular velocity of 0.4°/s (Song et al., 2021). The movement of the platform can be stopped at any time by a hand switch controlled by the participants. An electronic goniometer in the device recorded the angular displacement of the platform. Each participant was seated on a height-adjustable chair with the foot placed on the platform. During the ankle proprioception test, the knee and hip joints were flexed at 90°, and the leg was perpendicular to the surface of the platform when the platform was placed in a horizontal position. Approximately 50% of the participant’s lower extremity weight was rested on the platform using the thigh cuff suspension system to control unwanted sensory cues from the contact between the platform and the plantar surface of the foot. The participant sat with their eyes closed and wore headphones with light music playing to eliminate potential environmental visual and auditory stimulation. The participant was instructed to concentrate on their foot and press the hand switch to stop the movement of the platform when they could sense motion, followed by identification of the rotation direction. The motor was operated to rotate with a random time interval ranging from 2 to 10 s after an indication to start a trial. At least five trials were performed for each direction to reduce random measurement errors.

Tactile sensation test

The dominant foot’s tactile sensation was tested with the participants while lying supine on the treatment table with a set of Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments (six piece foot kit, North Coast Medical, Inc., Morgan Hill, CA, United States) (Figure 1D), which showed good test-retest reliability (ICC value, 0.83∼0.86) (Collins et al., 2010). Monofilaments of 6 different sizes used in this study were 2.83, 3.61, 4.31, 4.56, 5.07, and 6.65 that applies 0.07, 0.4, 2, 4, 10, and 300 grams of force when being pressed into a C-shape (bent 90°). The filament size was log10 (10 × force in milligrams). The filaments were applied to the skin on the bases of the great toe, 1st and 5th metatarsals, arch, and heel in random order (Song et al., 2021). These touches were performed for 1 s and with two repetitions. Randomized null-stimuli were added to ensure that the participants could not anticipate the application of the filaments. Plantar sensitivity was determined by the initial application of the thin filaments, progressing to the thicker filaments until the participants were able to detect the touch (Song et al., 2021). The participants were asked to provide a verbal response about the localization of the area tested when they perceived the stimulation. The sensitivity threshold was determined by the minimum monofilament gauge detected correctly. A less sensitivity threshold indicates better plantar tactile sensation.

Data reduction

Force plate data were used in calculating ground reaction force (GRF) and Dmax of COP trajectory. COP was measured in the anterior-posterior (AP) and mediolateral (ML) directions. The GRF and COP data were filtered using a lowpass fourth-order Butterworth digital filter with a cut-off frequency of 50 Hz (Pan et al., 2016). The 20 s GRF data after the Marker-2 detached from the Marker-1 (or detached from the position of the removed Marker-1) were used in calculating the TTS. Two time-windows of the last 10 s (10∼15 s, 15∼20 s) of the AP and ML components of the GRF were analyzed. The windows with the smallest absolute GRF range for the AP and ML components were regarded as the optimal range of variation values (Ross and Guskiewicz, 2004). The 20 s COP data were collected from each participant after they began to move Marker-2 to Marker-1 (or removed Marker-1) (Prieto et al., 1996; Pan et al., 2016). The hand movement onset was taken as the moment when hand velocity exceeds 5% of the maximal hand speed at the beginning of the movement (Kubicki et al., 2012). The hand movement offset was calculated when the hand velocity fell below 5% of the maximal hand speed (the maximum speed of the Marker M2) at the end of the movement (Kubicki et al., 2012). Marker-2 position data were filtered with a 5 Hz fourth-order lowpass Butterworth filter (Hondzinski et al., 2010) and used in calculating TPE in the AP, ML, and vertical directions and three-dimensional space.

Variables

The TPE was calculated in the EXCEL as the distance between a participant’s pointed position by memory (Marker-2) and the position of Marker-1 before it was removed. The TPE in 3D space is the 3D spatial distance between the marker 2 and the position of Marker-1 before it was removed. Final Marker-2 location was determined as the average of five frames after the movement offset (Hondzinski et al., 2010). TTS was defined as the time from Marker-2 detached from Marker-1 until the body regains stability, i.e., the starting moment when the smoothed GRF was within the optimal range of variation values for at least 0.5 s (Tulloch et al., 2012). The DmaxAP/DmaxML was defined as the maximum displacement (maximum-minimum) of COP trajectory in the AP/ML direction.

Data analysis

All statistical analysis was conducted using the SPSS software package (26.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Descriptive analysis results were presented as mean ± standard error in TTS, proprioception, and tactile sensation. The normality of all variables was tested using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Independent sample t-test (normally) or the Mann–Whitney U (non-normally) test was used to analyze TPE, proprioception, and tactile sensation thresholds. Two-way analysis of variance with repeated measures (normally) or Scheirer-Ray-Hare test (non-normally) was used to determine differences in TTS and Dmax of COP trajectory.

A Bonferroni-adjusted post-hoc analysis was conducted when significant Group-by-Task interaction was detected. Partial eta squared (η2p) was used to represent the effect size of the main effect and interaction of the two-way analysis of variance. The thresholds for η2p were as follows: <0.06, small; 0.06∼0.14, moderate; >0.14, large (Pierce et al., 2004). Cohen’s d was used to represent the effect size of the post-hoc pair comparison. The thresholds for Cohen’ d were as follows: <0.20, trivial; 0.20∼0.50, small; 0.51∼0.80, medium; >0.80, large (Cohen et al., 1988). Pearson (normally) or Spearman (non-normally) correlations were used for testing the relationships of visuospatial ability and postural stability to proprioception and tactile sensation. The thresholds for the correlation coefficient (r) were as follows: <0.10, trivial; 0.10∼0.30, weak; 0.31∼0.50, moderate; >0.50, strong (Cohen, 1988). A Type I error rate of less than 0.05 was used as an indication of statistical significance.

Results

Of the 37 participants, 18 were included in the Tai Chi group (female = 4, male = 14, age = 69.7 ± 3.9 years, weight = 70.6 ± 11.0 kg, height = 1.68 ± 0.08 m, BMI = 24.8 ± 2.9 kg/m2, MMSE scores = 28.83 ± 1.01, Tai Chi experience = 10.6 ± 5.6 years), and 19 were included in the control group (female = 5, male = 14, age = 68.2 ± 3.3 years, weight = 71.4 ± 12.4 kg, height = 1.65 ± 0.08 m, BMI = 26.2 ± 3.1 kg/m2, MMSE scores = 27.11 ± 2.38). The basic information of each participant in Tai Chi or control groups is shown in Table 1. Independent t-tests showed no significant differences in age, weight, height, and BMI between the groups.

The Shapiro-Wilk test showed that most variables were normally distributed, except the TPE in the ML and vertical directions and tactile sensation thresholds.

The TPE between Marker-1 and Marker-2 is shown in Figure 2. The bar chart with error lines represented the mean and standard error of the TPE among the Tai Chi and control groups. Compared with the control group, the Tai Chi group had significantly less TPE in the vertical direction (TPEV; Tai Chi group: 20.3 ± 3.9 mm; control group: 34.9 ± 4.4 mm, p = 0.019, d = 0.848) and in three-dimensional space (Tai Chi group: 61.0 ± 6.9 mm; control group: 101.3 ± 10.3 mm, p = 0.003, d = 1.065) during the BTT.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Target positioning error (TPE) between the finger marker and the removed bar marker in the AP, ML, vertical directions, and in three-dimensional space during the blind touch task (BTT). AP, anterior-posterior; ML, mediolateral.

The descriptive statistics and subgroup comparisons of the TTS are presented in Table 2. No Group-by-Task interaction was observed. Significant group effects were detected in TTS in the AP (TTSAP; p = 0.001, η2p = 0.458) and ML (TTSML; p < 0.001, η2p = 0.557) directions. The control group took a longer time to be stabilized compared to the Tai Chi group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Time to stabilization (TTS) in the anterior-posterior (AP) and mediolateral (ML) directions during the touch task (TT) and blind touch task (BTT).

The descriptive statistics and subgroup comparisons of Dmax of COP trajectory in the AP (DmaxAP) and ML (DmaxML) directions were presented in Figure 3. Significant Group-by-Task interactions were detected in DmaxML (p = 0.025, η2p = 0.278). Pairwise comparisons showed that DmaxML direction was larger during the TT than during the BTT among the Tai Chi group (p = 0.001, d = 0.975); And less during the TT (p = 0.037, d = 0.498) and BTT (p < 0.001, d = 1.364) among the Tai Chi group compared with the control group. Significant group and task effects were detected in DmaxAP (p = 0.002, η2p = 0.477; and p = 0.015, η2p = 0.316), which was larger among the control group and TT than the Tai Chi group and BTT.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Dmax in the AP and ML directions during the TT and BTT. AP, anterior-posterior; BTT, blind touch task; Dmax, maximum displacement; ML, mediolateral; TT, touch task.

The differences in ankle proprioception and tactile sensation thresholds between the TC and the control groups are presented in Table 3. Participants in the Tai Chi group had less ankle proprioception thresholds during both dorsiflexion (p = 0.029, d = 0.886) and plantarflexion (p = 0.012, d = 1.025), and less tactile sensation thresholds at the great toe (p = 0.015, d = 0.932), arch (p = 0.019, d = 0.855) and heel (p = 0.027, d = 0.930), compared to people in the control group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Proprioception and tactile sensation thresholds.

The relationships between TPE, TTS, Dmax and proprioception/tactile sensation thresholds are presented in Table 4. Among the Tai Chi group, sensitive ankle proprioception during plantarflexion was strongly correlated to less TPEV (r = –0.515, p = 0.029), TTSML (r = 0.587, p = 0.01) and DmaxAP during the TT (r = –0.629, p = 0.005). Sensitive tactile sensation at the great toe was strongly correlated to less TTSAP during the TT (r = 0.550, p = 0.018) and BTT (r = 0.564, p = 0.015), and moderately correlated to DmaxAP during the TT (r = –0.468, p = 0.05); Among the control group, sensitive ankle proprioceptions during dorsiflexion (r = 0.492, p = 0.033) and plantarflexion (r = 0.519, p = 0.021) were moderately to strongly correlated to less TTSml during the BTT.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Relationship of TPE, TTS, Dmax to proprioception and tactile sensation.

Discussion

This study investigated the differences in visuospatial ability and postural stability between the Tai Chi practitioners with more than 5 years’ experience and their controls during the TT and BTT. The relationship between visuospatial ability and postural stability with proprioception and the tactile sensation was also explored. The results supported our hypotheses. The TPE was significantly less, the time to recover from an unstable state was significantly shorter, and the Dmax was significantly less among the Tai Chi group compared to the control group. The Tai Chi group decreased DmaxML during the BTT compared to TT; Compared to the control group, the Tai Chi group was more sensitive in proprioception and tactile sensation.

The TPE provided the following key data: 1. The Tai Chi and control groups did not reach Marker-2 far enough to the original position of Marker-1 during the BTT; 2. Less TPE in the vertical direction and three-dimensional space were detected during the BTT among the Tai Chi group, compared to the control group; 3. The TPE was correlated to ankle plantarflexion proprioception among the Tai Chi group. The further the practitioner stretched, the more forward the center of gravity was, and the greater disturbance of postural stability was. Therefore, those practitioners selected a more conservative strategy to maintain their postural stability during the BTT and prevent falls. TPE was correlated to participants’ gaze direction (Kennedy and Inglis, 2002; Meyer et al., 2004) and sensation (Liu et al., 2012). The effects of Tai Chi practice on gaze stability (McGibbon et al., 2004), proprioception, and tactile sensation (Hu et al., 2021) had been proven, and this study further indicated that the proprioception among the Tai Chi group was correlated to the TPE. Among the Tai Chi group, the TPE was correlated to ankle proprioception during plantarflexion, but not dorsiflexion. During the tasks, the participants needed to lean forward to touch the marker, and ankle plantarflexors, such as soleus and gastrocnemius, were stretched. The proprioceptive receptors in these muscles provide spatial and temporal afferent information to maintain postural stability (Hogervorst and Brand, 1998). In the plantarflexion proprioceptive test, the same muscles were stretched and the sensitivity of proprioception was also determined by the function of the proprioceptive receptors in these muscles (Hogervorst and Brand, 1998). It is reasonable to indicate that Tai Chi practitioners used their better proprioception to reduce their TPE. The less TPE reflected the higher positioning accuracy of the Tai Chi group.

The results of TTS showed that 1. The Tai Chi group had shorter TTS than the control group; 2. TTS was correlated to proprioception and tactile sensation among the Tai Chi group and correlated to proprioception only among the control group. 3. TTS was correlated with proprioception during the BTT among the control group. TTS was defined as the time that a participant returns to a stable state from an unstable condition (Ross and Guskiewicz, 2003, 2004; Fransz et al., 2015) and could be used in evaluating postural stability (Ross and Guskiewicz, 2003). A shorter TTS indicated improved posture stability and low fall risk (Fransz et al., 2015; Sherrington et al., 2019). Individuals with functional ankle instability had longer TTS than the healthy controllers, and long TTS might be caused by poor neuromuscular functions (Ross and Guskiewicz, 2004). Our study pointed out that both proprioception and tactile sensation were correlated to TTS. Ross and Guskiewicz (2004) supported the relationship by showing that ankle joint proprioception was highly correlated to an individual’s TTS. Konradsen and Ravn (1991) suggested that proprioceptive defects at the ankle joint weaken the reflex contraction of the muscles that stabilize the body. This is the first study that revealed the relationship between tactile sensation and TTS to the best of our knowledge. Song et al. (2021) indicated that tactile sensation was only correlated to postural control when body movements were restricted. During the TT and BTT, older adults fixed their feet on the ground, so their body movements were relatively limited. In this circumstance, the position of the plantar COP changes as the subtle variation of GRFs, the perception of forces under the feet during the stance could be used to generate an internal estimate of the body center of mass location (Meyer et al., 2004). Tactile sensation afferents could provide valuable feedback to the central nervous system regarding ankle torque production, weight transfer, and limb loading (Meyer et al., 2004). From this viewpoint, a better tactile sensation might help to reduce the TTS among the Tai Chi group. In addition, this study showed that only the tactile sensation at the great toe was correlated to TTS. The tactile sensation is different between foot sole sites due to the notable differences in cutaneous receptor distribution, firing characteristics (Kennedy and Inglis, 2002), and the mechanical properties of the skin, like its hardness and thickness (Strzalkowski et al., 2015). Previous studies showed that the arch and great toe were thin and soft plantar regions (Strzalkowski et al., 2015) with better sensitivity, which was correlated to static balance control (Song et al., 2021). In our study, only tactile sensation at the great toe, rather than at the arch, was correlated with the postural stability. As one of the areas with the highest pressure during walking, great toe is subjected to much greater pressure loads than the arch (Mao et al., 2006b). It could be inferred that more pressure signals at the great toe could be transmitted to the central nervous system, so the correlation of TTS to tactile sensation at the great toe was detected, rather than at the arch. In our study, only the tactile sensation at the great toe, but not the arch, was correlated with postural stability. As one of the areas with high pressure during walking, the great toe is subjected to a much higher weight load than the arch (Mao et al., 2006b). It can be inferred that more pressure signals at the great toe could be transmitted to the central nervous system, so that significantly correlation of TTS with tactile sensation at the big toe was detected, but not at the arch. Among the control group, TTS was correlated with proprioception during the BTT, but not TT. Proprioception and vision can be compensated for each other by sensory weighting in the central nervous system (Rand et al., 2013), so the participants may rely more on proprioception to maintain postural stability during the BTT, where there was less visual information.

The outcomes of the Dmax of COP trajectory showed that: 1. The Tai Chi group showed significantly less Dmax in the AP direction during the TT and BTT compared with the control group. 2. The Tai Chi and control groups showed significantly less Dmax in the AP direction during the BTT than during the TT. 3. Significant Group-by-Task interaction showed that the Tai Chi group had decreased Dmax in the ML direction during BTT than during TT, while the control group did not have similar changes. 4. Dmax was correlated to proprioception and tactile sensation among the Tai Chi group. One of the previous studies supported our first discovery by indicating that the Tai Chi group showed significantly less postural sway than the brisk walking and control groups during goal-directed upper body movements (Pan et al., 2016). The better postural control could explain the less Dmax (Hondzinski et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2011; Hu et al., 2021; Song et al., 2021). The less Dmax in the AP direction during the BTT may be explained by the fact that the Tai Chi and control groups reached their hands less forward and produced a small postural sway in the AP direction during the BTT. The Group-by-Task interaction in the Dmax indicated that the Tai Chi group increased their postural stability in the ML direction during BTT compared to that during TT. Compared with normal walking, Tai Chi has more variety movements and greater plantar loading in the ML direction (Mao et al., 2006a), so it is not surprising that the Tai Chi group showed better postural stability in the ML direction. Compared to TT, the effects of vision on postural control were reduced during the BTT because the target was removed, and individuals relied more on sensations to compensate for the decreased vision effects. The outcomes of this and previous studies well-documented that proprioception and tactile sensation were better among the Tai Chi group (Li et al., 2019; Hu et al., 2021). It was reasonable to assume that the Tai Chi group took advantage of their better sensations to control their movements more precisely and decreased the disturbance in the ML direction during BTT than during TT.

The differences in proprioception and tactile sensation between the Tai Chi and control groups and the relationships of proprioception and tactile sensation to visuospatial ability and postural stability showed that: 1. Most of the proprioception and tactile sensation thresholds were significantly less among the Tai Chi group than among the control group; 2. Compared with the control group, more correlations of proprioception to visuospatial ability and postural stability were detected among the Tai Chi group; and 3. No correlations of tactile sensation to visuospatial ability and postural stability were detected among the control group. The positive effects of Tai Chi exercise on proprioception and tactile sensation had been extensively investigated and well-explained previously (Hondzinski et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2011; Hu et al., 2021). We investigated the relationship of proprioception and tactile sensation to visuospatial ability and postural stability. The proprioceptive receptors in and around joints provided important spatial and temporal afferent information regarding the positions and movements of body segments and information between body segments in space (Hogervorst and Brand, 1998; Relph and Herrington, 2016). Proprioceptive receptors afferent information was transmitted to the central nervous system, and these receptors, in turn, were organized and managed in various high-order areas. Therefore, proprioception had an important role in visuospatial ability and postural stability. One study measured the postural sway in 74 healthy subjects from different age groups. It reported that all groups were more dependent on proprioception than on other sensations to maintain balance control (Colledge et al., 1994). It is reasonable to assume that individuals prefer proprioception over vision to precisely control their movements. Hence, the Tai Chi group relied more on their better proprioception and performed better during upper body movements. Moreover, our outcomes indicated that proprioception was only correlated to postural stability during BTT, but not during TT.

In this study, the tactile sensation was better among the Tai Chi group. One previous study supported our observations and further indicated that Tai Chi intervention significantly improved tactile sensation among older adults by altering ring the plasticity of the sensory-motor system to increase somatosensory information from the plantar sensory (Hondzinski et al., 2010). Skin receptors in the foot sole are sensitive to contact pressure and potential changes in the distribution of pressure, provide important information about the body’s status with respect to the supporting surface (Kennedy and Inglis, 2002). Tai Chi practitioners used their better tactile sensation, felt the slight changes of plantar pressure distribution, and finally improved their visuospatial ability and postural stability. This study confirmed the relationship of visuospatial ability and postural stability to proprioception and tactile sensation. This finding has clinical implication. Exercises that enhance sensations should be included in the exercise prescriptions for fall prevention among older adults.

This study has several limitations. First, considering the size of the sample and the heterogeneity in the number of years of practicing Tai Chi, it could be specified that it is an exploratory study. Second, this is an analytical cross-sectional study, rather than a randomized controlled trial, only one measurement of the study variables was made, and no variables were assessed over time, that might cause some participant selection bias. Therefore, future studies should be designed by randomized controlled trials. Third, this study only examined the relationship of proprioception and tactile sensation with the visuospatial ability and postural stability, other contributors, such as the central nervous system, visual, vestibular, or cognitive functions, could also influence them. Fourth, some observations of this study could not to be explained by our limited understanding, e.g., only proprioception during plantarflexion, instead of during dorsiflexion, was correlated to visuospatial ability and postural stability among the Tai Chi group, and only proprioception during the BTT, instead of during the TT, was correlated to postural stability among the control group. Therefore, further investigations can be designed based on our new and innovative report.

Conclusion

During goal-directed upper body movements, long-term Tai Chi practitioners exhibited superior visuospatial ability and superior postural stability, along with better proprioception and tactile sensation. Compared with those of the control group, Tai Chi practitioners’ better sensations enhanced their superior visuospatial ability and postural stability.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Sports Science Ethics Committee of Shandong Sport University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

ZS participated in the design of the study and contributed to the data collection, reduction, and analysis. LL and QS participated in the design of the study. WS participated in the design of the study and contributed to the data collection. MM contributed to the data reduction and analysis. CZ contributed to the data analysis and interpretation of results. All authors contributed to the manuscript writing, read and approved the final version of the manuscript, and agreed with the order of presentation of the authors.

Funding

This study was supported by Shandong Province Young Innovative Talent Introduction and Cultivation Program (2019-183) and National Natural Science Foundation of China (12102235).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Xinrui Zhang, Xinheng Che, Bo Gao, Qi Wang, Yaya Pang, Xiaoli Ma, Jingwen Wang, and Xiaohui Duan, the postgraduate students from Shandong Sport University, for conducting the experiments and acquiring the data for this manuscript.

References

Bangsbo, J., Blackwell, J., Boraxbekk, C. J., Caserotti, P., Dela, F., Evans, A. B., et al. (2019). Copenhagen Consensus statement 2019: physical activity and ageing. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 856–858. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-100451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canada, S. (2011). Deaths, Estimates, by Province and Territory. Available online at: http://www.statcan.gc.ca/tables-tableaux/sum-som/l01/cst01/hlth36a-eng.htm (accessed January 24, 2022).

Google Scholar

Cohen, J., Cohen, J., Cohen, J. W., Cohen, J., Cohen, J., Cohen, J., et al. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Science. England, UK: Routledge

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, (2nd ed). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

Google Scholar

Colledge, N. R., Cantley, P., Peaston, I., Brash, H., Lewis, S., and Wilson, J. A. (1994). Ageing and balance: the measurement of spontaneous sway by posturography. Gerontology 40, 273–278.

Google Scholar

Collins, S., Visscher, P., De Vet, H. C., Zuurmond, W. W., and Perez, R. S. (2010). Reliability of the Semmes Weinstein Monofilaments to measure cutaneous sensibility in the feet of healthy subjects. Disabil. Rehabil. 32, 2019–2027. doi: 10.3109/09638281003797406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fransz, D. P., Huurnink, A., de Boode, V. A., Kingma, I., and van Dieen, J. H. (2015). Time to stabilization in single leg drop jump landings: an examination of calculation methods and assessment of differences in sample rate, filter settings and trial length on outcome values. Gait Posture 41, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghai, S., Ghai, I., and Effenberg, A. O. (2017). Effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Interv. Aging 12, 557–577. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S125201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haddad, J. M., Claxton, L. J., Keen, R., Berthier, N. E., Riccio, G. E., Hamill, J., et al. (2012). Development of the coordination between posture and manual control. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 111, 286–298.

Google Scholar

Haddad, J. M., Van Emmerik, R. E., Wheat, J. S., and Hamill, J. (2008). Developmental changes in the dynamical structure of postural sway during a precision fitting task. Exp. Brain Res. 190, 431–441.

Google Scholar

Hogervorst, T., and Brand, R. A. (1998). Mechanoreceptors in joint function. J. Bone Jt. Surg. 80, 1365–1378.

Google Scholar

Hondzinski, J. M., Li, L., and Welsch, M. (2010). Age-related and sensory declines offer insight to whole body control during a goal-directed movement. Motor Control 14, 176–194. doi: 10.1123/mcj.14.2.176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, X., Lyu, S., Mao, M., Zhang, J., Sun, W., Zhang, C., et al. (2021). Effects of Eight Methods and Five Steps of Tai Chi Practice on Balance Control Among Older Adults. Motor Control 25, 616–630. doi: 10.1123/mc.2021-0022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyndman, D., Ashburn, A., and Stack, E. (2002). Fall events among people with stroke living in the community: circumstances of falls and characteristics of fallers. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 83, 165–170. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.28030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, H. G., and Dingwell, J. B. (2006). A direct comparison of local dynamic stability during unperturbed standing and walking. Exp. Brain Res. 172, 35–48. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0224-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, P. M., and Inglis, J. T. (2002). Distribution and behaviour of glabrous cutaneous receptors in the human foot sole. J. Physiol. 538, 995–1002. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konradsen, L., and Ravn, J. B. (1991). Prolonged peroneal reaction time in ankle instability. Int. J. Sports Med. 12, 290–292.

Google Scholar

Kubicki, A., Bonnetblanc, F., Petrement, G., Ballay, Y., and Mourey, F. (2012). Delayed postural control during self-generated perturbations in the frail older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 7, 65–75. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S28352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latash, M. L., Levin, M. F., Scholz, J. P., and Schoner, G. (2010). Motor control theories and their applications. Medicina 46, 382–392.

Google Scholar

Lee, A., Bhatt, T., Smith-Ray, R. L., Wang, E., and Pai, Y. C. (2019). Gait Speed and Dynamic Stability Decline Accelerates Only in Late Life: a Cross-sectional Study in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 42, 73–80. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Zhang, S., and Dobson, J. (2019). The contribution of small and large sensory afferents to postural control in patients with peripheral neuropathy. J. Sport Health Sci. 8, 218–227. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2018.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Zhang, X., and Lockhart, T. E. (2012). Fall risk assessments based on postural and dynamic stability using inertial measurement unit. Saf. Health Work 3, 192–198. doi: 10.5491/SHAW.2012.3.3.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mao, D. W., Li, J. X., and Hong, Y. (2006b). The duration and plantar pressure distribution during one-leg stance in Tai Chi exercise. Clin. Biomech. 21, 640–645. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2006.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mao, D. W., Li, J. X., and Hong, Y. (2006a). Plantar pressure distribution during Tai Chi exercise. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 87, 814–820.

Google Scholar

Martin, K., Thomson, R., Blizzard, L., Wood, A., Garry, M., and Srikanth, V. (2009). Visuospatial ability and memory are associated with falls risk in older people: a population-based study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 27, 451–457.

Google Scholar

McGibbon, C. A., Krebs, D. E., Wolf, S. L., Wayne, P. M., Scarborough, D. M., and Parker, S. W. (2004). Tai Chi and vestibular rehabilitation effects on gaze and whole-body stability. J. Vestib. Res. 14, 467–478.

Google Scholar

McIntyre, J., Stratta, F., Droulez, J., and Lacquaniti, F. (2000). Analysis of pointing errors reveals properties of data representations and coordinate transformations within the central nervous system. Neural Comput. 12, 2823–2855. doi: 10.1162/089976600300014746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, P. F., Oddsson, L. I., and De Luca, C. J. (2004). The role of plantar cutaneous sensation in unperturbed stance. Exp. Brain Res. 156, 505–512. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1804-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murre, J. M., Janssen, S. M., Rouw, R., and Meeter, M. (2013). The rise and fall of immediate and delayed memory for verbal and visuospatial information from late childhood to late adulthood. Acta Psychol. 142, 96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naslund, J. (2010). Visuospatial ability in relation to fall risk and dementia. Arch. Neurol. 67:643.

Google Scholar

Nemoto, M., Sasai, H., Yabushita, N., Tsuchiya, K., Hotta, K., Fujita, Y., et al. (2020). A Novel Exercise for Enhancing Visuospatial Ability in Older Adults with Frailty: development, Feasibility, and Effectiveness. Geriatrics 5:29, doi: 10.3390/geriatrics5020029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, J., Liu, C., Zhang, S., and Li, L. (2016). Tai Chi Can Improve Postural Stability as Measured by Resistance to Perturbation Related to Upper Limb Movement among Healthy Older Adults. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2016:9710941. doi: 10.1155/2016/9710941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, C. A., Block, R. A., and Aguinis, H. (2004). Cautionary Note on Reporting Eta-Squared Values from Multifactor ANOVA Designs. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 64, 916–924.

Google Scholar

Pinker, S. (1984). Visual cognition: an introduction. Cognition 18, 1–63.

Google Scholar

Prieto, T. E., Myklebust, J. B., Hoffmann, R. G., Lovett, E. G., and Myklebust, B. M. (1996). Measures of postural steadiness: differences between healthy young and elderly adults. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 43, 956–966. doi: 10.1109/10.532130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rand, M. K., Wang, L., Musseler, J., and Heuer, H. (2013). Vision and proprioception in action monitoring by young and older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 1864–1872. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Relph, N., and Herrington, L. (2016). The effects of knee direction, physical activity and age on knee joint position sense. Knee 23, 393–398.

Google Scholar

Ross, S. E., and Guskiewicz, K. M. (2003). Time to Stabilization: a Method for Analyzing Dynamic Postural Stability. Athl. Ther. Today 8, 37–39.

Google Scholar

Ross, S. E., and Guskiewicz, K. M. (2004). Examination of static and dynamic postural stability in individuals with functionally stable and unstable ankles. Clin. J. Sport Med. 14, 332–338.

Google Scholar

Sherrington, C., Fairhall, N. J., Wallbank, G. K., Tiedemann, A., Michaleff, Z. A., Howard, K., et al. (2019). Exercise for preventing falls in older people living in the community. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 1:CD012424.

Google Scholar

Solianik, R., Mickeviciene, D., Zlibinaite, L., and Cekanauskaite, A. (2021). Tai chi improves psychoemotional state, cognition, and motor learning in older adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. Exp. Gerontol. 150:111363. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2021.111363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, Q., Li, L., Zhang, C., Sun, W., and Mao, D. (2018). Long-term Tai Chi practitioners have superior body stability under dual task condition during stair ascent. Gait Posture 66, 124–129. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, Q., Zhang, X., Mao, M., Sun, W., Zhang, C., Chen, Y., et al. (2021). Relationship of proprioception, cutaneous sensitivity, and muscle strength with the balance control among older adults. J. Sport Health Sci. 10, 585–593.

Google Scholar

Strzalkowski, N. D., Triano, J. J., Lam, C. K., Templeton, C. A., and Bent, L. R. (2015). Thresholds of skin sensitivity are partially influenced by mechanical properties of the skin on the foot sole. Physiol. Rep. 3:e12425. doi: 10.14814/phy2.12425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, W., Song, Q., Yu, B., Zhang, C., and Mao, D. (2015). Test-retest reliability of a new device for assessing ankle joint threshold to detect passive movement in healthy adults. J. Sports Sci. 33, 1667–1674. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2014.1003589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulloch, E., Phillips, C., Sole, G., Carman, A., and Abbott, J. H. (2012). D M A Clinical Pilates Directional-Bias Assessment: reliability and Predictive Validity. J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Ther. 42, 676–687. doi: 10.2519/jospt.2012.3790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wayne, P. M., Walsh, J. N., Taylor-Piliae, R. E., Wells, R. E., Papp, K. V., Donovan, N. J., et al. (2014). Effect of tai chi on cognitive performance in older adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62, 25–39.

Google Scholar

Zhang, C., Mao, D., Riskowski, J. L., and Song, Q. (2011). Strategies of stepping over obstacles: the effects of long-term exercise in older adults. Gait Posture 34, 191–196. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Manor, B., and Li, L. (2015). H-index is important for postural control for people with impaired foot sole sensation. PLoS One 10:e0121847. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: coordination, postural balance, proprioception, tactile sensation, Tai Chi quan

Citation: Shao Z, Li L, Mao M, Sun W, Zhang C and Song Q (2022) Exploring the effects of peripheral sensibility on visuospatial and postural capacities during goal-directed movements in long-term Tai Chi practitioners. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:881972. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.881972

Received: 23 February 2022; Accepted: 27 June 2022;
Published: 22 July 2022.

Edited by:

Yinghui Hua, Fudan University, China

Reviewed by:

Víctor Manuel Mendoza-Núñez, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico
Ka-Chun Siu, University of Nebraska Medical Center, United States

Copyright © 2022 Shao, Li, Mao, Sun, Zhang and Song. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qipeng Song, songqipeng@sdpei.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.