Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 25 May 2022
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Measurable Brain and Cognitive Reserve: The Implication of Neuroimaging Biomarkers in the Normal Aging Process View all 11 articles

Age-Related Differences in the Neural Processing of Idioms: A Positive Perspective

  • 1Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 2Graduate Institute of Brain and Mind Sciences, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 3Neurobiology and Cognitive Science Center, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 4Center for Artificial Intelligence and Advanced Robotics, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 5Graduate Institute of Biomedical Sciences, China Medical University, Taichung, Taiwan
  • 6Taiwan International Graduate Program, Interdisciplinary Neuroscience, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 7Department of Computer Science and Information Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
  • 8Institute of Statistical Science, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan

We examined whether older adults benefit from a larger mental-lexicon size and world knowledge to process idioms, one of few abilities that do not stop developing until later adulthood. Participants viewed four-character sequences presented one at a time that combined to form (1) frequent idioms, (2) infrequent idioms, (3) random sequences, or (4) perceptual controls, and judged whether the four-character sequence was an idiom. Compared to their younger counterparts, older adults had higher accuracy for frequent idioms and equivalent accuracy for infrequent idioms. Compared to random sequences, when processing frequent and infrequent idioms, older adults showed higher activations in brain regions related to sematic representation than younger adults, suggesting that older adults devoted more cognitive resources to processing idioms. Also, higher activations in the articulation-related brain regions indicate that older adults adopted the thinking-aloud strategy in the idiom judgment task. These results suggest re-organized neural computational involvement in older adults’ language representations due to life-long experiences. The current study provides evidence for the alternative view that aging may not necessarily be solely accompanied by decline.

Introduction

Aging has been portrayed as a process of deterioration. Like any developmental process, aging involves various aspects (physiological, cognitive, language, perceptual, motivational, and emotional) of changes. Physical deterioration seems inevitable with increased age from a biological perspective. Previous studies have illustrated age-related decline, including slower reaction time (Williamson et al., 2009), loss of muscular strength (Charlier et al., 2016), and lower cardiovascular efficiency (Freiheit et al., 2010). Thus, unsurprisingly, aging is often associated with frailty, characterized by multiple physiological impairments and dependency (Ferrucci et al., 2006; Kuh, 2007; Weiss et al., 2010).

Even if solely examining aging from a behavioral perspective, it also appears as if older adults always perform “worse” than younger adults, since it has been shown that the abilities gained throughout the first quarter of our lives will only decline in the latter part of development, including sensation (Ulfhake et al., 2002), perception (Faubert, 2002), and cognition (Park et al., 2002). With such a negative societal view on aging, older adults may also acquire negative perspectives on their capabilities, which may prevent them from engaging in social and physical activities (Palacios et al., 2009) and living independently (Coudin and Alexopoulos, 2010). Indeed, many studies have demonstrated the detrimental effects of ageism on older adults’ mental and physical health (Bryant et al., 2016). Moreover, Palacios et al. (2009) also suggested that older adults with less stereotypical views of aging were more likely to be socially and physically engaged. Thus, by changing perspectives on aging, or at least an aspect of the aging process, the whole society may benefit from enhanced older adults’ overall well-being.

The current study aims to provide an alternative perspective and evidence that aging might not necessarily be solely accompanied by decay. Language is one of the functions that develop rapidly in childhood and remains relatively stable from adulthood to later life (Gaskell et al., 2009; Ramscar et al., 2013), especially in terms of lexicon size (Gaskell et al., 2009). However, some studies also suggest that language capacity becomes more limited with age due to sensory and cognitive decline [e.g., Burke and Shafto (2004)]. For instance, compared to younger adults, older adults produced fewer words, took a longer time, and made more errors when performing list generation tasks and object naming tasks (Goral, 2004; Goral et al., 2007). In addition, Chan and Poon (1999) have shown that older adults performed worse than younger adults in the category fluency task, suggesting an alternation of the semantic network in the process of aging (Tomer and Levin, 1993; Kozora and Cullum, 1995). A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study also demonstrated that language lateralization toward the left hemisphere declined after age 25 (Szaflarski et al., 2006). Since language is a heavily lateralized function, such decline was interpreted as an explanation for less efficient communication in old age [but see Chen et al. (2019)].

However, older adults’ decline in language functions can be interpreted differently. A well-researched language-related limitation in later life is the failure to retrieve appropriate words during conversations, known as the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon (Juncos-Rabadán et al., 2010). Such a phenomenon of knowing a term but cannot immediately retrieve it from memory has been observed consistently when people age (Salthouse and Mandell, 2013). Burke et al. (1991) proposed that this phenomenon is due to infrequent word use, which leads to weaker connections between lexical and phonological nodes. However, Ramscar et al. (2013) proposed an alternative explanation. They interpreted this finding that older adults performed poorer on the pair association learning task was due to their larger lexicon size and more prosperous life experience. They suggested that existing knowledge of words might prevent older adults from forming new connections with previously unassociated words, implying that the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon may also result from older adults’ larger vocabulary size. Other studies have also supported the view of Ramscar and colleagues. For example, Bialystok and Luk (2012) found that older adults outperformed their younger peers on similar tasks due to their larger vocabulary size. Additionally, Fiez et al. (1999) also found that older adults know more rare words than younger adults. From this perspective, language-related knowledge increases with age rather than declines.

Here we examined whether and how older adults process idioms differently from younger adults from both behavioral and neural aspects. Idioms are commonly used in daily life, especially in Chinese, as they contain a rich cultural and historic background (Yang et al., 2016) with a stable structure (Liu et al., 2010), and their use is a form of language ability that we continue to acquire from adolescence through adulthood (Chan and Marinellie, 2008). As idioms work as large words or lexical units (Nippold et al., 2007; Conner et al., 2011), older adults should preserve the ability to process idioms with their stabilized or even increased lexicon size. Indeed, Hung and Nippold (2014) showed no age-related decline when tasked with explaining idioms, and 60–69 years old older adults reported greater familiarity and provided better explanations to idioms than 20–29 years old younger adults. Thus, the ability to process idioms varies between older and younger adults, depending on their familiarity with idioms. Despite that some studies have examined the effect of aging on idiom production [e.g., Qualls and Harris (2003) and Hung and Nippold (2014)], those studies might not be generalizable to the Chinese language, given that Chinese is relatively character-based processing, whereas, English idioms are relatively sentence-based. In addition, the Chinese writing system consists of characters taking up a square-shaped space with a nonlinear configuration varied in character structure (Yeh, 2000; Yeh and Li, 2002; Yeh et al., 2003), and the cognitive process underlying Chinese is different from that of English words (Tan et al., 2001). To date, no previous studies have investigated how aging might impact the processing of Chinese idioms, which is one of the novelties of the current study.

Chinese idioms require character processing that involves orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing (Wu et al., 2012). Using fMRI to directly examine which components might vary with age has several advantages to tackle how aging influences Chinese idiom processing. First, the task-based fMRI allows us to examine whether different neural substrates are in charge of the processing of idioms across different age groups, which can help us determine if older and younger adults recruit different brain areas in processing idioms; Second, fMRI allows us to investigate whether there is any re-organized neural processing in older adults’ brain, where some brain regions might show more robust activation for older adults and positively correlate with behavioral performance. In comparison, the same pattern might be absent in younger adults.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Since no previous studies have investigated age differences in processing Chinese idioms, we referenced a relevant study, Hung and Nippold (2014), to calculate the required sample size. In comparing familiarity with idioms across age groups, η2 = 0.29 was found. According to G*Power ver. 3 (Faul et al., 2007), 34 participants (17 participants in each group) were required to detect age differences with power = 0.95. We recruited nearly 60% more participants than needed to verify our results to be more conservative. Hence, 28 older adults [mean age (SD) = 67.0 (4.96) years, age range 59–82 years old, male/female 9/18; mean years of education (SD) = 14.9 (3.21) years] and 30 younger adults [mean age (SD) = 23.2 (3.23) years, age range 19–31 years old, male/female 14/16; mean years of education (SD) = 15.9 (1.86) years] were recruited. All were right-handed Taiwanese with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and had Montreal Cognitive Assessment [MoCA; Nasreddine et al. (2005)] scores above or equal to 26 points. All participants gave written informed consent before participating. One older adult was excluded because of his excessive head movement in translation (>3 mm) during fMRI scanning, making the total number of older adults 27. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at the National Taiwan University (201611HS004).

Stimuli and Procedure

Stimuli were presented using E-prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Toolbox, Sharpsburg, PA, United States) on a Windows PC. Participants viewed the stimuli via a head-mounted display in the scanner with 800 × 600 pixels resolution and 60 Hz refresh rate (Resonance Technology Inc., Northridge, CA, United States). Chinese idioms consisted of four characters (white, 4° × 4°) were presented serially one at a time at the center of a black background (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sample experimental stimuli of the idiom judgment task. Four conditions were used for the target phase in this study, including Frequent Idiom (e.g., “亡羊補牢” is translated as “When the sheep run away, then you mend the sheepfold,” and means “To figure out a way to remedy the problem and prevent further loss”), Infrequent Idiom (e.g., “削足適履” is translated as “Cut off a piece of your feet to fit the size of the shoe” and means “To have inflexible thinking”), Random (character sequences with no meaning), and Perceptual Control (non-word sequences). For each trial, the four-character stimuli were presented sequentially after which a response display containing a question mark appeared, during which participants responded whether the quartets constituted an idiom or not.

Four conditions were manipulated: frequent idiom, infrequent idiom, random, and perceptual control. Frequent and infrequent idioms were idioms of high and low frequency [mean (SD): 92.6 (49.66) vs. 6.84 (2.44) per million words; t(24) = 8.88, p < 0.001], respectively. There were 25 frequent and 25 infrequent idioms adapted from the materials used in Zhou et al. (2016). In the random condition, non-idiom four-character Chinese strings were generated randomly, and these characters did not overlap with the characters of frequent and infrequent idioms. In the perceptual control condition, non-word character stimuli were created by rearranging Chinese characters’ strokes to form quartets of meaningless character-like stimuli. For the response cue display, a question mark (white, 4° × 2°) was presented at the same location as the Chinese characters.

There were 25 trials in each condition and 100 trials in total, equally distributed across five functional runs in this event-related fMRI experiment. There were five trials for each of the four conditions within a run, with the conditions presented in random order within each run. In each trial (Figure 1), during the target display, one of the characters of the four-character idiom was presented sequentially for 500 ms. After the quartet was completed, the response display with a question mark was presented for a maximum duration of 2,000 ms. Participants were instructed to judge whether the word sequence presented was an idiom or not during the response phase with assigned buttons as quickly and accurately as possible. After a response, the question mark would disappear, and a black background would appear until the entire 2000 ms period was over. A blank screen was then presented with the inter-trial interval (ITI) jittered between 3 and 4.5 s.

After the idiom judgment task, participants completed (1) the vocabulary test, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV), and (2) the reading habits questionnaire based on Acheson et al. (2008). In the vocabulary test, 33 cards containing a Chinese two- or three-character word were shown to the participants, and they were asked to explain the meaning of each word. The reading habits questionnaire included author recognition and magazine recognition. Participants were asked to indicate from an author and magazine list the items they could recognize. The total score of these two tasks (author recognition and magazine recognition) indicates participants’ reading habits.

fMRI Data Acquisition and Analysis

Scanning was conducted on a 3T Magnetom Prisma scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 20-channel head coil at the Imaging Center for Integrated Body, Mind, and Culture Research of National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan. For each participant, functional images were recorded using a gradient echo-planar imaging sequence with TR 2,000 ms, TE 32 ms, flip angle 87°, field of view 220 mm × 220 mm, voxel size 3.43 mm × 3.43 mm × 4.00 mm, 33 axial slices, and 390 scans. Axial slices were aligned parallel to the anterior-posterior commissural axis and placed for whole-brain coverage. We also acquired a T1-weighted Magnetization Prepared RApid Gradient Echo (MP-RAGE) sequence with TR 2,000 ms, TE 22.8 ms, flip angle 8°, field of view 256 mm × 256 mm, voxel size 1.00 mm isotropic, and 192 sagittal slices for registration and normalization to standardized template space.

Brain image data preprocessing and statistical analysis were performed using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom). For each participant, functional volumes were realigned with unwarping to the first volume to correct for head motion and slice correction. Structural T1 images were then registered to the functional images and then segmented and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template space using the Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration Through Exponentiated Lie algebra [DARTEL; Ashburner (2007)] procedure. T1 deformation parameters from the DARTEL procedure were then applied to the functional images with spatial smoothing using a 3D 8 mm Gaussian kernel.

First-level analysis of each participant’s fMRI data was conducted using a General Linear Model (GLM) with canonical hemodynamic functions. The GLM included four predictors corresponding to the four conditions (frequent idioms, infrequent idioms, random, and perceptual control) in the idiom judgment task, and the six head movements of each run were added to the model as regressors. Thus, first-level GLMs yielded whole-brain voxel-wise estimates of each participant’s neural responses to the four contrasts, including (1) idiom (frequent and infrequent idioms) vs. random, (2) frequent idiom vs. random, (3) infrequent idiom vs. random, and (4) idiom vs. perceptual control. These whole-brain neural response estimates were then submitted to second-level analysis.

Second-level analysis of the group-wise whole-brain neural response estimates was conducted using a two-sample t-test and multiple regressions to compare the contrast images of older and younger adults. The images were set at p < 0.001 (uncorrected) in all two-sample t-tests and multiple regression results. The cluster-wise correct False Discovery Rate (FDR) q < 0.05 was used in two-sample t-tests.

The Region Of Interest (ROI) mask was used in the multiple regression. The choice of ROIs was based on the overlapping brain regions between whole-brain neural results of idiom vs. random conditions and the semantically related areas (temporal and frontal regions) as suggested in the literature [e.g., Booth et al. (2002)]. The mask image of ROIs was produced by using XjView.1

Results

Behavioral Performance

Age differences in demographics, results of the vocabulary test score, and reading habits questionnaire are shown in Table 1. Two younger and two older participants did not complete the post-test tasks (vocabulary test and reading habits questionnaires) and thus were not included for further post-test analysis, but their data for other analyses were included. As shown in Table 1, there were no significant differences in gender [x2(1, N = 57) = 1.05, p = 0.306], education level [t(55) = 1.47, p = 0.148], vocabulary test score [t(51) = −0.02, p = 0.986], or reading habit score [t(51) = −0.83, p = 0.411] for older and younger adults except for their difference in age [t(55) = −39.88, p < 0.001].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Basic demographics and reading skills of younger and older participants (Standard deviations in parentheses).

Mean accuracies for the idiom judgment task are shown in Figure 2. Two types of trials were excluded from further analysis: trials in which participants responded before the response display and trials without responses. A two-way mixed repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied on judgment responses with Age (younger and older) as the between-subjects variable and Condition (frequent, infrequent, random, and perceptual control) as the within-subjects variable. The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Condition [F(3,165) = 6.04, MSE = 0.001, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.099] but not Age [F(1,55) = 3.43, MSE = 0.002, p = 0.069, ηp2 = 0.059]. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons corrected using the Šidák method showed that accuracy was higher for the perceptual control (99.9%) than the frequent (98.3%) [t(56) = 4.71, p < 0.001], infrequent (98.3%) [t(56) = 4.18, p = 0.001], and random (97.1%) [t(56) = 3.05, p = 0.007] conditions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Accuracy (including hits for frequent and infrequent idioms, and correct rejection for random and perceptual control) for younger and older adults in the idiom judgment task across four conditions. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean. Note that the standard error of older adults’ perceptual control was zero. **denotes p < 0.01, and *denotes p < 0.05.

The interaction effect of Age x Condition was significant [F(3,165) = 9.90 MSE = 0.001, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.152]. Simple main effect tests corrected by the Šidák method showed that older adults had higher accuracy for perceptual control than the infrequent idioms [100.0 vs. 97.9%, t(26) = 3.85, p = 0.004] and random [100.0 vs. 94.3%, t(26) = 3.16, p = 0.024] conditions. In contrast, younger adults showed lower accuracy for frequent idioms than the perceptual control [97.3 vs. 99.9%, t(29) = −4.56, p = 0.001] and random conditions [97.3 vs. 99.6%, t(29) = −3.83, p = 0.004]. These results indicate that for both age groups perceptual control (scrambled characters) could be correctly judged compared to other conditions consisting of characters. However, older adults tended to misjudge random sequences as idioms more often than younger adults, which was further confirmed by the result that accuracy for the random condition was lower for older adults than younger adults [94.3 vs. 99.6%, t(55) = −3.07, p = 0.012]. Most important and relevant to our hypothesis was the result that accuracy for frequent idioms was higher for older adults than younger adults [99.4 vs. 97.3%, t(55) = 3.03, p = 0.016]. No other contrasts achieved statistical significance (ps < 0.05).

We also calculated individual d’ scores for the recognition of frequent and infrequent idioms. The d’ scores were acquired by subtracting the Z scores of the false alarm rates from those of the hit rates. The estimation of the d’ scores is as follows:

d Z A c c u r a c y i d i o m - Z ( 1 - A c c u r a c y r a n d o m ) (1)

Correct identification of the frequent idioms was used as the hit rate in the frequent idiom condition, and that of the infrequent idioms was used as the hit rate in the infrequent idiom condition. These two conditions shared the same false alarm rate from the erroneous identification as idioms when they were random sequences in the random condition.

Two-sample t-test comparisons of the mean d’ scores across age groups yielded no significant difference between older and younger adults in the frequent idiom condition [4.09 vs. 4.25, t(55) = −1.17, p = 0.246] but significantly lower d’ for older than younger adults in the infrequent idiom condition [3.90 vs. 4.43, t(55) = −3.44, p = 0.001]. In addition, we calculated individual β scores that indicate the bias to respond with one judgment more than the other. β larger than 1 indicates a tendency for “non-idiom” responses, whereas β smaller than 1 indicates a tendency for “idiom” responses. The calculation of β is as follows:

β = exp { d × - 1 2 [ Z A c c u r a c y i d i o m + Z ( 1 - A c c u r a c y r a n d o m ) ] } (2)

Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed that the distribution of β is not normally distributed (all ps < 0.001), so the non-parametric test was used. Mann-Whitney U-Tests showed lower β for older relative to younger adults in the frequent idiom condition [z = 2.68, p = 0.007], but no age difference for β in the infrequent idiom condition [z = 1.65, p = 0.098] (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean d’ and β estimates of response biases in older and younger adults.

For reaction time (RT) data, trials that were two standard deviations above the mean RT and RTs of incorrect responses were excluded from the analysis. As above, we applied a two-way mixed repeated-measures ANOVA with Age (younger and older) and Condition (frequent, infrequent, random, and perceptual control) as independent variables. This analysis yielded main effects of Age [F(1,55) = 26.7, MSE = 42,061, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.327], Condition [F(3,165) = 31.0, MSE = 3,154, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.361], and Age by Condition interaction [F(3,165) = 3.51, MSE = 3,154, p = 0.017, ηp2 = 0.060]. Post-hoc pairwise analyses corrected by using the Šidák method revealed faster RTs in younger than older adults (340 vs. 480 ms), and RTs of both frequent (372 ms) and infrequent (377 ms) conditions were faster than RTs of random (448 ms) and perceptual control (444 ms) conditions (all ps < 0.001). There were no differences between RTs for frequent and infrequent idioms, as well as random idioms and perceptual control non-words. For the interaction effect, younger adults had numerically faster RTs in the random condition than in the perceptual control condition [358 vs. 376 ms], and reversed result was found in older adults [539 vs. 512 ms]. However, the simple main effect test corrected by using the Šidák method could not obtain any statistically significant difference between the two conditions for either younger [t(29) = 2.20, p = 0.195] or older adults [t(26) = −0.92, p = 0.934].

Neuroimaging Data

We used whole-brain two-sample t-tests to evaluate brain areas in which older adults showed different contrast responses from younger adults across conditions (Figure 3 and Table 3). For idioms (frequent and infrequent included), relative to random characters, older adults engaged higher neural responses than younger adults in the left superior temporal gyrus (STG), bilateral insula, right postcentral gyrus (PostCG), left superior frontal gyrus (SFG), and right middle temporal gyrus (MTG). Considering the two types of idioms separately, for frequent idioms relative to random characters, older adults showed higher activation than younger adults in the right supplementary motor area (SMA), left STG, and left paracentral lobule (PCL). For infrequent idioms relative to random characters, older adults showed higher activation than younger adults in the right MTG, left PostCG, and left STG. In addition, for random characters relative to non-word characters (perceptual control), older adults engaged higher neural responses in the left SFG. However, no higher activations across conditions were found in older compared to younger adults.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Whole-brain statistical contrast maps overlaid on 3D rendered template brains. Contrast maps depict brain areas in which the contrast responses for older adults were higher than those for younger adults. Voxel-wise statistical significance was set at cluster-wise corrected q < 0.05 False Discovery Rate (FDR). L, left; R, right; FDRc, FDR-corrected cluster threshold; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area; PostCG, postcentral gyrus; PCL, paracentral lobule.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Peak activation details of brain areas with higher contrast responses in older than younger adults.

Based on the above whole-brain contrast of the idiom vs. random condition, ROIs were defined for the right MTG, left SFG, and left STG (see Materials and Methods). We then evaluated how ROI neural responses in younger and older adults were associated with behavioral performance during the idiom judgment task to see if there were compensatory activations or reorganization in older adults. Individual neural contrast response estimates were extracted from these ROIs and submitted to multiple regression analyses with d’ scores, which shows the behavioral performance of the idiom judgment task without the response bias in the frequent and infrequent conditions with age groups as predictors. The idiom words (frequent or infrequent) relative to random characters and frequent idioms relative to infrequent idioms are included as contrast pairs in the multiple comparison corrections to clarify how the compensatory/reorganization system works on the language function of word regularity (idiom words vs. random characters) and the frequency of idioms (frequent vs. infrequent). The reported areas of activation of the ROI analysis were significant using p < 0.05 Familywise Error (FWE) correction with the three predefined ROIs.

Region of interest analysis showed correlation trends in which greater neural responses to infrequent idiom than random conditions correlated with higher d’ scores in older adults. Specifically, a positive correlation between the brain activation in the infrequent condition (contrasting with the random condition) and d’ was found in the left SFG (PFWE–corr = 0.011, voxel size = 97) in older adults (r = 0.747, p < 0.001; shown in Figure 4), whereas no such correlation was found in younger adults (r = 0.025, p = 0.895). We further confirmed this by applying a regression analysis using age and d’ as independent variables to predict the brain activation in the left SFG. A significant interaction of age and d’ was found, t(53) = −2.11, p = 0.040, verifying the greater brain-behavior associations in the left SFG for older than younger adults (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Associations between individual infrequent d’ scores in the infrequent idiom vs. random condition and brain contrast responses in younger and older adults in the left Superior Frontal Gyrus showed significant whole-brain age difference in the neural-behavior correlation.

Discussion

This study showed differences between older and younger participants in behavioral and imaging data. We discuss the implications of these differences below.

Behavioral Results of Idiom Processing

On the behavioral level, older adults showed higher accuracy for frequently seen idioms than younger adults. The results here thus indicate how experience benefits older adults, allowing them to be better than younger adults in making judgments on frequent idioms. No difference in the accuracy of infrequent idioms was found between older and younger adults, indicating the same level of performance in judging the less familiar idioms between the two age groups. Together, this supports our hypothesis that older adults preserve the language function in terms of idiom processing and can even perform better with frequent idioms than their younger counterparts.

However, by taking d’ as the dependent variable, we found no age differences in the sensitivity of idiom judgment with frequent idioms and lower sensitivity for older adults with infrequent idioms. Closer scrutiny further indicates that the average d’ for older adults (4.08 and 3.9 for frequent and infrequent idioms, respectively) was not low compared to the d’ in younger adults (4.25 and 4.43 for frequent and infrequent idioms, respectively); this can further be verified by the similar accuracies (>97%) in infrequent idioms across age groups. It is to be noted that as the calculation of d’ scores (of both frequent and infrequent idioms) involves the performance in random sequences (false alarm by mistaking these for idioms), how older and younger adults performed differently in this random condition is critical. Given that the d’ measure depends on the responses to the random sequences which differed between older and younger adults, the accuracy of idiom judgment as mentioned above could better serve as a main behavioral proxy and the d’ as an additional measure to reveal the results from different aspects.

It seems that older adults might not be able to correctly judge whether the random sequence was an idiom or not, as lower accuracy was found for older adults than younger ones. However, these results could also be due to older adults’ better world knowledge (Spreng and Turner, 2019), which requires a serial search through memory to accumulate more evidence to reject the random sequence as an idiom. Lee et al. (2018) have suggested that answering “no” is more challenging to assess and that answering “no” may require much more evidence in comparison to answering “yes.” Therefore, given older adults’ higher world knowledge, the more accumulated evidence is needed to form a rejection, which could have caused performance accuracy to be lower in the random sequence than in younger adults.

The finding of higher response bias (β) for younger adults in the idiom judgment for frequent idioms suggests that younger adults were more conservative in making judgments regarding frequent idioms. The result here also indicates the benefit that older adults acquire because of their more considerable world knowledge, enabling them to make more liberal judgments of the frequent idioms than younger adults. Yet, the age difference in bias was absent in the infrequent idiom condition, indicating that both age groups had the same criterion for judging infrequent idioms.

Despite the differences in Chinese and English writing systems, the current results align with Hung and Nippold (2014), who did not find age-related declines in idiom processing of English. On the other hand, Hung and Nippold (2014) found better performance in explaining idioms in individuals over 60 years older than younger adults in their twenties. Additionally, Qualls and Harris (2003) also found better performances in idiom interpretation in African-American older adults than younger adults when controlling for working memory capacity and reading comprehension ability. Therefore, the superiority or maintenance of language functions in idiom processing for older adults is not only applied to the English language system but can also be generalized to Chinese and different ethnicities, as shown in the current study.

Higher Brain Activation in Older Adults Compared to Younger Adults

Compared to younger adults, older adults engaged in higher neural responses to idioms (both frequent and infrequent) relative to scrambled characters (i.e., the random condition) in the bilateral frontotemporal areas and the medial frontoparietal regions. This may indicate that older adults exert more cognitive resources in processing idioms than younger adults. For example, the SFG is in charge of working memory processing (Rypma et al., 1999). In addition, the STG is involved in the processing of tonal representations of Chinese characters (Tan et al., 2001) and is associated with the perception of intonation in speech prosody (Chang et al., 2010). The MTG is engaged in the extensive processing of spatial information in the Chinese language (Xiong et al., 2000) as a form of semantic representation (Booth et al., 2002, 2006). Higher activations in the insula and SMA were also found in older adults compared to younger adults. The activation in the SMA can be served as an index of motoric representation or articulatory rehearsal of vocally phonological information (Kuo et al., 2004) and the grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (Fiez et al., 1999). The insula has also been shown to mediate the motor aspect of speech production and articulatory control (Cereda et al., 2002; Oh et al., 2014). The higher activations in these brain regions suggest that older adults might recruit more resources to process idioms than younger adults. Older adults might also adopt the thinking-aloud strategy during the judgment of Chinese idioms as higher activations were found in several articulation-related areas, which might allow them to reduce cognitive load during the task (Smagorinsky, 1989). Together, we have shown that older adults adopted different strategies for judging Chinese idioms from a neuroimaging perspective.

We did not find higher activations in the fusiform gyrus (FG) and left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in older compared to younger adults. Both the FG and left IFG are highly associated with language processing, either in English or Chinese (Tan et al., 2005). In Chinese, the FG is related to the orthographic organization of Chinese (Bolger et al., 2005; Tan et al., 2005). As for the left IFG, previous studies have shown that the anterior and posterior part of the IFG is in charge of processing semantic and phonological information, respectively [see Wu et al. (2012) for review]. In addition, the IFG is associated with the processing of semantic integration in a sentence-level structure (Zhu et al., 2009). The absence of age differences in these two regions might indicate that older and younger adults recruited the same resources in processing orthographic organization and semantic integration across characters in the task. Although null results and inverse inference should be interpreted carefully, we consider this to be proper since the recognition of Chinese characters in skilled Chinese readers is based on orthographic constituents (Chen, 1996), which can easily be done by both younger and older adults in the current study. In addition, Atas et al. (2014) have shown that the temporal integration of symbols could be learned with trial-by-trial feedback. The learning of temporal regularity could even take place without visual awareness. Hence, with more experience in using idioms, older adults should have the ability to integrate temporal semantic information at the same level as younger adults.

When random vs. perceptual control conditions were compared, older participants recruited more high-level brain resources in the SFG than younger participants. No other brain regions showed significantly higher contrast responses in older than younger participants. These results are consistent with the behavior results: older adults made more effort during idiom processing, even with the random sequence characters. Moreover, a greater specific engagement of infrequent idioms relative to scrambled words across similar brain regions for semantic processing (left SFG) positively correlated with the task performance of older participants. These results suggest that when the brain regions correlated with idiom processing were active, older adults performed equally well as younger adults. Therefore, the activation in the SFG could be served as a form of compensatory mechanism (Cabeza et al., 2002), as the positive correlation between brain and performance was only found in older adults. Alternatively, given the higher accuracy of recognizing frequent idioms and higher brain activations for older adults than younger ones, this may not just be a form of compensation (which would imply a presence of a deficit) but a reorganization with behavioral advantages. Future studies that more specifically distinguish linguistic operations involved in processing idioms, which arguably are quite complex, are required to better evaluate the roles of the above brain regions as reflecting compensatory or re-organized neuro-computational involvement in older adult language representations.

Current Views on Aging

By presenting the current study, we aim to combat the general stereotype regarding the decline of aging. While it is undeniable that certain capacity limitations are associated with the increase of age, there are potentially severe consequences when aging is evaluated solely from a negative perspective. With a view that aging is associated exclusively with deterioration, older adults may be perceived prejudicially and treated discriminatively even though most older adults do not experience frailty (McPherson, 2004). These negative beliefs from the public, the media, and policies seemed to influence older adults negatively. For example, age-related public policies, such as mandatory retirement age, can affect older adults’ sense of self and how others regard them (Hendricks, 2004). Many studies [e.g., Hausdorff et al. (1999); Chrisler et al. (2016), and Ng et al. (2016)] demonstrated that older individuals’ health and behaviors might be negatively affected in self-fulfilling ways as they embodied age-related stereotypes. Bai et al. (2016) also illustrated that those older adults with stronger beliefs about being a burden to their family have a higher risk of depression. Older individuals are affected physically and mentally by age-related negative stereotypes. They may require more social and healthcare support in a society where age is not respected, reinforcing the idea that older adults are weak or burdensome. This phenomenon was noted by Butler (2009), who coined the term ageism for negative stereotypes and discriminative acts against older adults. Older adults were greatly valued for their experience, knowledge, and institutional memory in the past. In Chinese culture, older adults have been regarded as the source of wisdom for their lifelong experiences. It has been considered virtuous for the younger generation to respect the older generation (Laidlaw et al., 2010).

Future Directions

We used Chinese idioms as the stimuli to tackle the issue of age-related differences in language processing as idiom processing is similar to regular phrases or short sentences (Liu et al., 2010). The extraction of a regular or consistent pattern from the environment to expect upcoming stimuli occurs as early as infancy when infants learn to segment sentences into words (Saffran et al., 1996), and prior experience is essential for statistical regularity (Samuelson, 2002; Lany and Gómez, 2008; Salvagio et al., 2015). Chinese idioms have a very high transitional probability between characters and words (e.g., in the idiom “亡羊補牢”, the transitional probability between “亡羊” (“When the sheep run away”) and “補牢” (“then you mend the sheepfold”) is almost 100%). Such an ability to extract statistical regularity and expect upcoming events are crucial for daily lives. Future studies can examine other non-linguistic processing such as older adults’ motion perception and dynamic emotional expression to see whether similar re-organization or compensation also occurs in the aging brain albeit in different neural networks. That is, whether older adults, with rich experience and knowledge, would have better statistical regularity in general, or idiom processing is a special case in language processing to counter the aging brain.

In addition to reading, previous studies on idiom processing in speech [e.g., Liu et al. (2020)] revealed a dynamic network among temporal-parietal-frontal regions and the crosstalk between the dual-stream systems, whereby the ventral system is for comprehension and the dorsal system for articulation in speech (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007). Fei et al. (2020) found reduced activity of STG and IFG in older adults than in younger adults and increased frontal-temporal-parietal functional connectivity that may work to help facilitate idiom processing in older adults. It is worth comparing the difference between the two forms (visual vs. auditory) of idiom processing within the same experimental framework to find the commonality and differences, especially regarding the preservation of performance and the brain’s re-organization in older adults.

Finally, our finding that older adults could correctly judge frequent idioms better than younger adults and had the equivalent performance regarding infrequent idioms may be related to the expertise experience found previously in other domains. For example, experienced radiologists could recognize abnormal X-ray images better than novices, but not for normal X-ray images. Such a finding indicates that selective processing for distinguishable features increases with experience and this may hinder the detection of variations in normal images [Myles-Worsley et al. (1988), see also Evans et al. (2011)], an idea supported by eye-tracking results (Richter et al., 2020). It is likely that the older adults’ higher accuracy for frequent idioms reflects their expertise performance in language processing due to their lifelong experiences. Whether the two have shared underlying mechanisms awaits future studies.

Conclusion

Compared to younger adults, we found better frequent-idiom recognition and higher neural activations in brain regions in charge of language processing and high-level functions for older adults. Older adults may benefit from experiences to help make judgments regarding Chinese idioms, which can be verified by their better (for frequent idioms) or similar performance (for infrequent idioms) in idiom judgments compared to younger adults. Despite previous studies investigating the deficits in semantic processing and reading in the aging population, this is the first study that directly examined the age effect on Chinese idiom processing with the measurement of neural correlates. This study provides another perspective on the aging process; namely, aging is not necessarily accompanied only by declines—instead, the experience accumulated across a lifespan with shifts in processing strategies.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article can be accessed on https://www.myqnapcloud.com/smartshare/608e947 k4l6p7075v536a699_9g6igg0lklnq2p11q5v25723593e4h2g.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethic Committee at the National Taiwan University (201611HS004). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

S-LY: conceptualization, funding acquisition, and supervision. S-LY, JG, and S-HL: data curation. S-HL and JG: formal analysis. S-HL, JG, Y-PC, and S-LY: methodology. S-LY and S-HL: project administration and writing. S-LY, LJ, and AT: resources. S-HL: software. All authors: investigation and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from Taiwan’s Ministry of Science and Technology awarded to S-LY (MOST106-2420-H-002-010-MY3, 110-2410-H-002-130-MY3, MOST110-2410-H-039-005, MOST110-2634-F-002-049, MOST110-2118-M-001-006-MY2, and MOST110-2221-E-182-009-MY2).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the two reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions, and the Imaging Center for Integrated Body, Mind, and Culture Research at National Taiwan University for providing the fMRI scanner. We also thank Chia-Lin Lee for her help in preparing and interpreting the questionnaires, Chii Shyang Kuo for his help in collecting data, and Hsin-Hao Lee for his help in interpreting part of the data.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.alivelearn.net/xjview

References

Acheson, D. J., Wells, J. B., and MacDonald, M. C. (2008). New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 278–289. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.1.278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113.

Google Scholar

Atas, A., Faivre, N., Timmermans, B., Cleeremans, A., and Kouider, S. (2014). Nonconscious learning from crowded sequences. Psychol. Sci. 25, 113–119. doi: 10.1177/0956797613499591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, X., Lai, D. W., and Guo, A. (2016). Ageism and depression: perceptions of older people as a burden in China. J. Soc. Issues 72, 26–46.

Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., and Luk, G. (2012). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 15, 397–401.

Google Scholar

Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A., and Schneider, W. (2005). Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapp. 25, 92–104. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., and Mesulam, M. M. (2002). Modality independence of word comprehension. Human Brain Mapp. 16, 251–261. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Lu, D., Burman, D. D., Chou, T.-L., Jin, Z., Peng, D.-L., et al. (2006). Specialization of phonological and semantic processing in Chinese word reading. Brain Res. 1071, 197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C., Bei, B., Gilson, K.-M., Komiti, A., Jackson, H., and Judd, F. (2016). Antecedents of attitudes to aging: a study of the roles of personality and well-being. Gerontologist 56, 256–265. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, D. M., and Shafto, M. A. (2004). Aging and language production. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 13, 21–24. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301006.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., and Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: what causes word finding failures in young and older adults? J. Memory Lang. 30, 542–579. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(91)90026-g

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, R. N. (2009). Combating ageism. Int. Psychogeriat. 21:211.

Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 17, 1394–1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cereda, C., Ghika, J., Maeder, P., and Bogousslavsky, J. (2002). Strokes restricted to the insular cortex. Neurology 59, 1950–1955. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000038905.75660.bd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A. S., and Poon, M. W. (1999). Performance of 7-to 95-year-old individuals in a Chinese version of the category fluency test. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5, 525–533. doi: 10.1017/s135561779956606x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, Y.-L., and Marinellie, S. A. (2008). Definitions of idioms in preadolescents, adolescents, and adults. J. Psychol. Res. 37, 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s10936-007-9056-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, E. F., Rieger, J. W., Johnson, K., Berger, M. S., Barbaro, N. M., and Knight, R. T. (2010). Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 13:1428.

Google Scholar

Charlier, R., Knaeps, S., Mertens, E., Van Roie, E., Delecluse, C., Lefevre, J., et al. (2016). Age-related decline in muscle mass and muscle function in Flemish Caucasians: a 10-year follow-up. Age 38:36. doi: 10.1007/s11357-016-9900-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P.-H., Wong, J.-S., Lin, W.-T., Tseng, W.-Y. I., Goh, J. O. S., and Lee, C. L. (2019). “Investigating the role of inter-hemispheric communication in age-related increase in right-hemisphere P600 grammaticality effect: a combined ERP and DTI study”. Poster presented at the Eleventh Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language. Helsinki: SNL.

Google Scholar

Chen, Y.-P. (1996). What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: the stroke or the stroke pattern? Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. Sec. A 49, 1024–1043.

Google Scholar

Chrisler, J. C., Barney, A., and Palatino, B. (2016). Ageism can be hazardous to women’s health: ageism, sexism, and stereotypes of older women in the healthcare system. J. Soc. Issues 72, 86–104.

Google Scholar

Conner, P. S., Hyun, J., O’Connor Wells, B., Anema, I., Goral, M., Monereau-Merry, M.-M., et al. (2011). Age-related differences in idiom production in adulthood. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 25, 899–912. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2011.584136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coudin, G., and Alexopoulos, T. (2010). ‘Help me! I’m old!’How negative aging stereotypes create dependency among older adults. Aging ment. Health 14, 516–523. doi: 10.1080/13607861003713182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. K., Cohen, M. A., Tambouret, R., Horowitz, T., Kreindel, E., and Wolfe, J. M. (2011). Does visual expertise improve visual recognition memory? Attent. Percep. Psychophys. 73, 30–35. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0022-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faubert, J. (2002). Visual perception and aging. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 56:164.

Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fei, N., Ge, J., Wang, Y., and Gao, J.-H. (2020). Aging-related differences in the cortical network subserving intelligible speech. Brain Lang. 201:104713. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrucci, L., Mahallati, A., and Simonsickm, E. M. (2006). Frailty and the foolishness of Eos. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 61, 260–261. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.3.260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiez, J. A., Balota, D. A., Raichle, M. E., and Petersen, S. E. (1999). Effects of lexicality, frequency, and spelling-to-sound consistency on the functional anatomy of reading. Neuron 24, 205–218. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80833-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freiheit, E. A., Hogan, D. B., Eliasziw, M., Meekes, M. F., Ghali, W. A., Partlo, L. A., et al. (2010). Development of a frailty index for patients with coronary artery disease. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 58, 1526–1531. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02961.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaskell, M. G., and Ellis, A. W. (2009). Word learning and lexical development across the lifespan. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 3607–3615. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goral, M. (2004). First-language decline in healthy aging: implications for attrition in bilingualism. J. Neurol. 17, 31–52. doi: 10.1016/s0911-6044(03)00052-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goral, M., Spiro, A., Albert, M. L., Obler, L. K., and Connor, L. T. (2007). Change in lexical retrieval skills in adulthood. Ment. Lexic. 2, 215–238. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Levy, B. R., and Wei, J. Y. (1999). The power of ageism on physical function of older persons: reversibility of age-related gait changes. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 47, 1346–1349. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb07437.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendricks, J. (2004). Public policies and old age identity. J. Aging Stud. 18, 245–260. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2004.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, P.-F., and Nippold, M. A. (2014). Idiom understanding in adulthood: examining age-related differences. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 28, 208–221. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2013.850117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juncos-Rabadán, O., Facal, D., Rodríguez, M. S., and Pereiro, A. X. (2010). Lexical knowledge and lexical retrieval in ageing: insights from a tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) study. Lang. Cogn. Proc. 25, 1301–1334.

Google Scholar

Kozora, E., and Cullum, C. M. (1995). Generative naming in normal aging: total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and semantic constraints. Clin. Neuropsychol. 9, 313–320. doi: 10.1080/13854049508400495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuh, D. (2007). A life course approach to healthy aging, frailty, and capability. J. Gerontol. Ser. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 62, 717–721. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.7.717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, W.-J., Yeh, T.-C., Lee, J.-R., Chen, L.-F., Lee, P.-L., Chen, S.-S., et al. (2004). Orthographic and phonological processing of Chinese characters: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 21, 1721–1731. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laidlaw, K., Wang, D., Coelho, C., and Power, M. (2010). Attitudes to ageing and expectations for filial piety across Chinese and British cultures: a pilot exploratory evaluation. Aging Ment. Health 14, 283–292. doi: 10.1080/13607860903483060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lany, J., and Gómez, R. L. (2008). Twelve-month-old infants benefit from prior experience in statistical learning. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1247–1252. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02233.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. L., Ruby, E., Giles, N., and Lau, H. (2018). Cross-domain association in metacognitive efficiency depends on first-order task types. Front. Psychol. 9:2464. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Li, P., Shu, H., Zhang, Q., and Chen, L. (2010). Structure and meaning in Chinese: an ERP study of idioms. J. Neurol. 23, 615–630.

Google Scholar

Liu, Z., Shu, S., Lu, L., Ge, J., and Gao, J.-H. (2020). Spatiotemporal dynamics of predictive. brain mechanisms during speech processing: an MEG study. Brain Lang. 203:104755. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2020.104755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherson, B. D. (2004). Aging as a social process: Canadian perspectives. Don Mills: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Myles-Worsley, M., Johnston, W. A., and Simons, M. A. (1988). The influence of expertise on X-ray image processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cogn. 14, 553–557.

Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, R., Allore, H. G., Monin, J. K., and Levy, B. R. (2016). Retirement as meaningful: positive retirement stereotypes associated with longevity. J. Soc. Issues 72, 69–85. doi: 10.1111/josi.12156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nippold, M. A., Mansfield, T. C., and Billow, J. L. (2007). Peer conflict explanations in children, adolescents, and adults: examining the development of complex syntax. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 16, 179–188. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/022)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, A., Duerden, E. G., and Pang, E. W. (2014). The role of the insula in speech and language processing. Brain Lang. 135, 96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palacios, C. S., Torres, M. T., and Mena, M. B. (2009). Negative aging stereotypes and their relation with psychosocial variables in the elderly population. Arch. Gerontol. Geriat. 48, 385–390. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2008.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., and Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 17:299.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Qualls, C. D., and Harris, J. L. (2003). Age, working memory, figurative language type, and reading ability. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 12, 92–102. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2003/055)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., Love, B., and Baayen, R. H. (2013). Learning is not decline: The mental lexicon as a window into cognition across the lifespan. Ment. Lexic. 8, 450–481.

Google Scholar

Richter, J., Scheiter, K., Eder, T. F., Huettig, F., and Keutel, C. (2020). How massed practice improves visual expertise in reading panoramic radiographs in dental students: an eye tracking study. PLoS One 15:e0243060. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rypma, B., Prabhakaran, V., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., and Gabrieli, J. D. (1999). Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. Neuroimage 9, 216–226. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., and Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., and Mandell, A. R. (2013). Do age-related increases in tip-of-the-tongue experiences signify episodic memory impairments? Psychol. Sci. 24, 2489–2497. doi: 10.1177/0956797613495881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvagio, E., Gomez, R., and Peterson, M. (2015). Is prior experience necessary for 5.5 month-old infants to use the statistical regularity of an unchanging object on an changing background for segmentation? J. Vision 15:338.

Google Scholar

Samuelson, L. K. (2002). Statistical regularities in vocabulary guide language acquisition in connectionist models and 15-20-month-olds. Dev. Psychol. 38:1016. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.6.1016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smagorinsky, P. (1989). The reliability and validity of protocol analysis. Writ. Commun. 6, 463–479.

Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., and Turner, G. R. (2019). The shifting architecture of cognition and brain function in older adulthood. Persp. Psychol. Sci. 14, 523–542. doi: 10.1177/1745691619827511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szaflarski, J. P., Holland, S. K., Schmithorst, V. J., and Byars, A. W. (2006). fMRI study of language lateralization in children and adults. Human Brain Mapp. 27, 202–212. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. H., Laird, A. R., Li, K., and Fox, P. T. (2005). Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapp. 25, 83–91. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. H., Liu, H.-L., Perfetti, C. A., Spinks, J. A., Fox, P. T., and Gao, J.-H. (2001). The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. Neuroimage 13, 836–846. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomer, R., and Levin, B. E. (1993). Differential effects of aging on two verbal fluency tasks. Percep. Motor Skills 76, 465–466. doi: 10.2466/pms.1993.76.2.465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulfhake, B., Bergman, E., and Fundin, B. (2002). Impairment of peripheral sensory innervation in senescence. Auton. Neurosci. 96, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/s1566-0702(01)00368-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, C. O., Hoenig, H. H., Varadhan, R., Simonsick, E. M., and Fried, L. P. (2010). Relationships of cardiac, pulmonary, and muscle reserves and frailty to exercise capacity in older women. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 65, 287–294. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, J. D., Espeland, M., Kritchevsky, S. B., Newman, A. B., King, A. C., Pahor, M., et al. (2009). Changes in cognitive function in a randomized trial of physical activity: results of the lifestyle interventions and independence for elders pilot study. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 688–694. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C.-Y., Ho, M.-H. R., and Chen, S.-H. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. Neuroimage 63, 381–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J., Rao, S., Jerabek, P., Zamarripa, F., Woldorff, M., Lancaster, J., et al. (2000). Intersubject variability in cortical activations during a complex language task. Neuroimage 12, 326–339. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Li, P., Fang, X., Shu, H., Liu, Y., and Chen, L. (2016). Hemispheric involvement in the processing of Chinese idioms: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 87, 12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, S. L. (2000). Structure detection of Chinese characters: Visual search slope as an index of similarity between different-structured characters. Chin. J. Psychol. 42, 191–216.

Google Scholar

Yeh, S. L., and Li, J. L. (2002). Role of structure and component in judgments of visual similarity of Chinese characters. J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percep. Perform. 28:933. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.4.933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, S. L., Li, J. L., Takeuchi, T., Sun, V., and Liu, W. R. (2003). The role of learning experience on the perceptual organization of Chinese characters. Visual Cogn. 10, 729–764.

Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Lee, C.-L., Li, K.-A., Tien, Y.-H., and Yeh, S.-L. (2016). Does temporal integration occur for unrecognizable words in visual crowding? PLoS One 11:e0149355. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Z., Zhang, J. X., Wang, S., Xiao, Z., Huang, J., and Chen, H.-C. (2009). Involvement of left inferior frontal gyrus in sentence-level semantic integration. Neuroimage 47, 756–763. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: idiom, positive aspects of aging, language, experience, functional brain reorgization

Citation: Yeh S-L, Li S-H, Jingling L, Goh JOS, Chao Y-P and Tsai AC (2022) Age-Related Differences in the Neural Processing of Idioms: A Positive Perspective. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:865417. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.865417

Received: 29 January 2022; Accepted: 25 April 2022;
Published: 25 May 2022.

Edited by:

Chu-Chung Huang, East China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Sasa R. Filipovic, University of Belgrade, Serbia
McNeel Gordon Jantzen, Western Washington University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Yeh, Li, Jingling, Goh, Chao and Tsai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Su-Ling Yeh, suling@ntu.edu.tw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.