Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 11 May 2022
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias

Investigating the Association Between Polygenic Risk Scores for Alzheimer’s Disease With Cognitive Performance and Intrinsic Functional Connectivity in Healthy Adults

Updated
  • 1Faculty of Biomedical Sciences, Università della Svizzera Italiana, Lugano, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
  • 3Department of Gerontopsychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine, and Psychotherapy, Pfalzklinikum, Klingenmünster, Germany
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Hospital, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany
  • 5Institute of General Practice, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany

Background: Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology is present many years before the onset of clinical symptoms. AD dementia cannot be treated. Timely and early detection of people at risk of developing AD is key for primary and secondary prevention. Moreover, understanding the underlying pathology that is present in the earliest stages of AD, and the genetic predisposition to that might contribute to the development of targeted disease-modifying treatments.

Objectives: In this study, we aimed to explore whether genetic disposition to AD in asymptomatic individuals is associated with altered intrinsic functional connectivity as well as cognitive performance on neuropsychological tests.

Methods: We examined 136 cognitively healthy adults (old group: mean age = 69.32, SD = 4.23; young group: mean age = 31.34, SD = 13.12). All participants had undergone resting-state functional magnetic resonance imagining (fMRI), DNA genotyping to ascertain polygenic risk scores (PRS), and neuropsychological testing for global cognition, working memory, verbal fluency, and executive functions.

Results: Two-step hierarchical regression analysis revealed that higher PRS was significantly associated with lower scores in working memory tasks [Letter Number Span: ΔR2 = 0.077 (p < 0.05); Spatial Span: ΔR2 = 0.072 (p < 0.05)] in older adults (>60 years). PRS did not show significant modulations of the intrinsic functional connectivity of the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) with other regions of interest in the brain that are affected in AD.

Conclusion: Allele polymorphisms may modify the effect of other AD risk factors. This potential modulation warrants further investigations, particularly in cognitively healthy adults.

Background

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disorder that is characterized by a progressive decline in cognitive function (McKhann et al., 2011). The neuropathological hallmark of AD in the brain is the presence of extracellular Aβ amyloid plaques and intracellular neurofibrillary tangles (NFT; Braak and Braak, 1991). Previous evidence shows that specific biomarker abnormalities consistent with these neuropathological changes are detectable years before the commencement of clinical symptoms (Jack et al., 2010; Sperling et al., 2011). The United States National Institute on Aging – Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) proposed a biological definition of AD, allowing for the diagnosis of AD in the presence of β-amyloid and neurofibrillary tau in cognitively healthy elders (Jack et al., 2010, 2018; Albert et al., 2011; McKhann et al., 2011; Sperling et al., 2011). Because irreversible cognitive dysfunction is caused by neuronal cell death, network dysfunction and eventual neurodegeneration, it is critical to identify people at risk before this occurs. In order to design disease-modifying drugs for AD, it is widely accepted that treatments should be administered as early as possible before clinical symptoms have appeared, and ideally, earlier than the start of neuronal damage (Jessen et al., 2014, 2018). The NIA-AA criteria are not meant for clinical uses, but only for research purposes and in clinical research settings.

Resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) may be a viable biomarker to detect altered intrinsic functional connectivity in people at risk of AD (Sorg et al., 2007). Rs-fMRI shows the intrinsic functional connectivity between brain regions at rest when no task is being performed. Previous evidence suggests that AD may be conceived as a disconnection syndrome, both structural and functional (Delbeuck et al., 2003; Sorg et al., 2007). The Default Mode Network (DMN), a set of brain regions that shows functional activity during rest, is one of the most widely studied functional networks in AD (Raichle et al., 2001; Krajcovicova et al., 2014). The Posterior Cingulate Cortex (PCC) is the posterior anatomical/computational hub in the DMN and brain in general (Hagmann et al., 2008; Greicius et al., 2009). It is suggested that the neuropathological changes and the resulting structural lesions in the brain may be associated with alteration in intrinsic brain activity in AD in the DMN (Buckner and Vincent, 2007). Previous studies show that the PCC is especially susceptible to the deposition of amyloid plaques in AD (Sperling et al., 2009; Mormino et al., 2011). Evidence from fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) studies demonstrate diminished resting state glucose metabolism in the PCC of patients with early AD or MCI as well as in cognitively healthy older adults at risk of AD (Ishii et al., 2003; Buckner et al., 2005). This might reflect a possible hypometabolism or synaptic dysfunction in this region (Fessel, 2021). Previous studies investigating the functional connectivity using functional MRI observed decreased connectivity between the PCC and the hippocampus, both in MCI and AD (Greicius et al., 2004; Krajcovicova et al., 2014). On the other hand, increased connectivity is observed in the anterior DMN and in the hippocampal-medial prefrontal and the frontoparietal connectivity in similar groups (Zhang et al., 2009; Zarei et al., 2013). However, alterations in the DMN alone could only differentiate between healthy controls and people with AD but not between the different prodromal stages of AD where cognitive performance may be still preserved (Teipel et al., 2018). Because of its important role in the DMN and its vulnerability toward AD pathology, the PCC is an ideal candidate region for investigating associations between intrinsic functional connectivity and its relation to other biomarkers and risk factors of AD.

Besides alterations in intrinsic functional connectivity, there are several candidate genes that constitute another frontier for early detection of people at risk of AD. The Apolipoprotein E gene on chromosome 19 is the most commonly associated genetic risk factor for late-onset AD (LOAD), and its ε4 allele is most commonly associated with LOAD (Belloy et al., 2019; Chaudhury et al., 2019). Previous research suggests that people with MCI due to AD who were carriers of ApoE ε4 allele indeed showed altered functional connectivity as well as lower cognitive performance compared to healthy controls (Wang et al., 2015; Harrison et al., 2016). In an earlier study that investigated the relationship between resting state connectivity and genetic risk, carriers of the ApoE ε4 allele were found to have a higher activation across several cortical regions (Bookheimer et al., 2000). However, the study only looked at carriers of AD-related, ApoE variants, and investigated connectivity alterations on task-based fMRI where participants were asked to perform a memory-activation task that is sensitive to the identification of neuropathological changes in the medical temporal lobe (MTL) structures (Bookheimer et al., 2000). Nonetheless, despite its strong genetic association with AD, clinical trials for disease-modifying treatments targeting the amyloid cascade pathway and focusing on carriers of ApoE ε4 did not yield successful results yet (Yiannopoulou et al., 2019; Serrano-Pozo et al., 2021). Moreover, the relationship between specific alterations of functional connectivity on rs-fMRI should not be attributed to a single gene and further investigation of the impact of other genetic variants should be considered (Harrison and Bookheimer, 2016).

This has directed the focus on investigating other pathways and other possible genetic variants associated with AD. Genome Wide Association Studies (GWAS) have identified several single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that are associated with an increased risk of developing AD in late life (Baker and Escott-Price, 2020). These include CLU, PICALM, and CR1 as well as BIN1, ABCA7, and EPHA1 (Harrison and Bookheimer, 2016). Homozygous carriers of CLU, for example, show a stronger magnitude of intrinsic functional connectivity compared to non-carriers (Zhang et al., 2015). A suggested approach would be to investigate the effect of these SNPs, combined in a polygenic risk score (PRS), as a biomarker to reliably detect an elevated risk of developing AD already in its earliest stages. A PRS is a method to predict the genetic susceptibility of an individual to a specific disease by its summarized genetic risk for the disease based on previous evidence, and that can be used for clinical prediction rules in conjunction with the clinical history and physical examination. However, consistent evidence on the effect of other genetic variants, calculated in a PRS, on intrinsic functional connectivity in cognitively healthy older adults at risk of AD is still lacking. Therefore, the association between alterations in functional connectivity and a PRS that combines the summed and weighted risk of several genetic variants in one metric would increase the prediction power for people at higher risk of developing AD dementia.

The aim of this study was to explore whether a higher PRS in cognitively healthy adults is associated with modulation of intrinsic functional connectivity between the PCC and other regions of interest (ROIs) in the brain. We examined this association using PRS and rs-fMRI data in a cohort of healthy adults. We also aimed to examine the association of PRS with cognitive performance in validated neuropsychological tests. We hypothesized that individuals with a higher PRS show altered intrinsic functional connectivity between the PCC and other brain regions that are implicated in AD (i.e., medio-temporal lobe, MTL) as well as lower cognitive performance.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We examined a subset of 136 cognitively healthy participants that met the inclusion criteria for the purpose of this analysis (having rs-FMRI data, PRS, and cognitive scores available). The sample was divided into two groups with the young group including participants aged 60 years and younger (n = 80). 55% of the young group were females (n = 44). The mean age of the young group was 31.34 (SD = 13.12). The old group included those who were above 60 years of age (n = 56). The mean age for the old group was 69.32 (SD = 4.23). 60.71% of the old group were females (n = 34). Participants were drawn from a cohort of participants from the project B4 of the Neuronal Coordination – Research Focus Frankfurt (NeFF) titled “Funktionelle und strukturelle neuronale Diskonnektion als Grundlage früher episodischer Gedächtnisstörungen der Alzheimer-Krankheit” (“Functional and structural neuronal disconnection as a basis/prerequisite for early neuronal memory dysfunction in Alzheimer’s Disease”) (Matura et al., 2014a,b, 2016, 2020, 2021). The project was performed at the Laboratory for Neuroimaging of the Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy at the Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany. The methodology of this project has been extensively described in previous publications (Matura et al., 2014a,b, 2016, 2020, 2021). None of the participants had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Eligible participants were selected based on the presence of a PRS and fMRI data. Sociodemographic variables of the included participants were ApoE status, age, gender, education, family history of Alzheimer’s disease dementia, handedness, weight, height, Body Mass Index (BMI), and smoking status. For the purpose of the current study, we only analyzed the association of PRS, in younger and older participants, with cognitive performance and intrinsic functional connectivity with the PCC as a seed region. The Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Goethe University Frankfurt approved the study, and all subjects signed a written informed consent. The study was undertaken in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) (Rickham, 1964).

Neuropsychological Measures

We used the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) to assess general cognition (Folstein et al., 1975), and the German version of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) for verbal learning and short-term memory (Delis et al., 1987; Niemann et al., 2008). Furthermore, measures of working memory and attention were obtained using the Trail Making Test – Part A (Spreen and Strauss, 1998), Letter Number Span (Gold, 1997), and the spatial Span test of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1997). Verbal fluency was tested using two subsets of the CERAD-NP (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease) – the semantic fluency and phonemic fluency tests (Morris et al., 1988). Finally, we used the Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q) to assess subjective memory decline, (Crook et al., 1992).

Magnetic Resonance Imagining Hardware and Procedure

All details about the study design and methods have been previously reported (Matura et al., 2014a,b, 2016, 2020, 2021). All MR images were acquired using a Trio 3-T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a standard head coil for radiofrequency transmission and signal reception. Participants were outfitted with protective earplugs to reduce scanner noise. For T1-weighted structural brain imaging, an optimized 3D modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform sequence (Deichmann et al., 2004) with the following parameters was conducted: acquisition matrix = 256 × 256, repetition time (TR) = 7.92 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.48 ms, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, 176 slices and 1.0-mm slice thickness. Functional resting state images were acquired using a blood oxygen level-dependent-sensitive echo-planar imaging sequence comprising the following parameters: 300 volumes, voxel size: 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, 30 slices, slice thickness = 3 mm, distance factor = 20%, flip angle = 90°, and FOV = 192 mm. Resting state measurements were part of a larger fMRI study on episodic memory. For the resting state measurements, all participants were instructed to keep their eyes open, to lie still, not to engage in any speech, to think of nothing special and to look at a white fixation cross-presented in the center of the visual field during the whole scan procedure.

Resting-State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imagining Data Analysis

To analyze the resting state functional data, we used the Connectivity (CONN) Toolbox (CONN functional connectivity toolbox, 2012). CONN is an open-source Matlab/SPM-based cross-platform software for the computation, display, and analysis of functional connectivity Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fcMRI). CONN is used to analyze resting state data (rs-fMRI) as well as task-related designs. We first imported the raw/partially processed Digital Imaging and Communications in Medicine (DICOM) functional and anatomical files into the CONN graphical user interface (GUI). We then ran the default pre-processing pipeline (direct normalization to MNI-space) in CONN’s GUI. The pipeline performs the following steps:

– Functional realignment using SPM12 realign and unwrap procedure (Andersson et al., 2001).

– Correction of temporal misalignment of slices of functional data using SPM12 slice-timing correction (STC) procedure (Henson et al., 1999).

– Outlier identification from the observed global BOLD signal and the amount of subject-motion in the scanner.

– Direct segmentation and normalization into standard MNI space and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF tissue classes using SPM12 unified segmentation and normalization procedure (Ashburner and Friston, 2005).

– Functional smoothing using spatial convolution with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full width half maximum (FWHM).

After pre-processing was completed, we ran the default denoising pipeline in CONN. The pipeline performs two general steps: linear regression of potential confounding effects in the BOLD signal, and temporal band-pass filtering. Once completed, we evaluated the effect of denoising by assessing the CONN Quality Control Plots. These plots provide a visualization of the distribution of functional connectivity values (FC) between randomly selected pairs of points within the brain before and after denoising. After denoising, FC distributions showed approximately centered distributions, with small but noticeable larger tails in the positive side, and considerably reduced inter-session and inter-subject variability.

Selection of Seed Region

To analyze DMN connectivity, we used a seed region-based approach. Because we were specifically interested in DMN resting state activity and whether any alteration is associated with the PRS we investigated the intrinsic functional connectivity of a region anatomically co-localized with the major posterior hub of the DMN, the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). We also explored the connectivity of the PCC as the region-of-Interest (ROI) with other ROIs in the brain that are commonly affected by AD pathology such as the bilateral hippocampus, bilateral anterior and posterior parahippocampus, and bilateral amygdala.

DNA Extraction, Genome-Wide Genotyping and Polygenic Risk Scores Calculation

DNA was extracted from whole-blood samples. The DNA extraction and genotyping process were conducted at bio.logis laboratories (Frankfurt am Main, Germany). DNA was genotyped on the Infinium Global Screen Array (GSA) with multi-disease drop in (MD) covering in total ca. 700K SNPs per person at Broad Institute, Cambridge, MA, United States. For PRS calculation, 177 persons were originally considered, after quality control measures using PLINK v1.9 (Chang et al., 2015) regarding relatedness and missingness per individual (<0.1), 142 participants remained for further analysis and 136 met the inclusion criteria mentioned above and were included in this study. Regarding SNP quality, SNPs were filtered excluding minor allele frequencies (MAF < 0.01) and genotyping missing rate per marker (<0.05), deviations from Hardy Weinberg Equilibrium (1 × e–6) and deviations from heterozygosity. A principal component and ancestry analysis revealed that the sample is of predominantly CEU ancestry.

Polygenic risk scores were calculated using the PRSice software version 2.3.1.e with default options (clump-kb 250, clump-p 1.0, clump r2 0.1 stat BETA) (Choi and O’Reilly, 2019). The summary statistics stage 1 from the International Genomics of Alzheimer’s Project (IGAP) were used (Kunkle et al., 2019) and subjected to INFO score filtering (INFO > 0.8). Neither the present study sample nor the IGAP sample show any overlap. The ApoE region (Chr19:45,116,911–46,318,605) was excluded from the analysis due to its reported large effect size. PRS values with p-threshold (pT) of 0.1 were used for further statistical analysis since this pT explains the highest variance and provides the best prediction accuracy according to a study conducted by Leonenko et al. (2021). We performed the analysis using PRS that excluded ApoE region as the main explanatory variable. To enhance readability, we refer to this variable as PRS in this manuscript.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses for the association between PRS and neuropsychological test scores were conducted using RStudio (RStudio Team, 2020). Cognitive scores and age followed a bimodal distribution. Therefore, after conducting the analysis of cognitive performance in the whole sample, we repeated the analysis for young and old groups separately. To investigate the relationship between PRS and cognitive performance, two step hierarchical regression analyses were performed with different cognitive test scores as dependent variables in the whole sample and for the old and young age group separately. ApoE status, age, gender, and education were entered at stage one of the regression to control for these influencing variables. The PRS was entered at stage two to determine the true correlation between PRS and cognitive performance controlling for the effect of potential influencing factors. For linear regression analysis, a positive beta value reflected a positive relationship while a negative beta value reflected an inverse relationship between the variables of interest.

To examine the association of PRS and connectivity values of the included participants, we performed an analysis of the whole group using multiple linear regression in the CONN Toolbox and controlled for ApoE status, age, gender and education. Functional connectivity values at rest between the PCC as the seed region and other ROIs were encoded as the dependant variable. To verify the results, the same analysis was conducted in R studio, using the beta values of connectivity between the PCC and the ROIs as the dependant variables, PRS as the explanatory variable, and controlled for ApoE status, age, gender and education.

Results

Participants’ Demographic Characteristics

Table 1 describes the demographic characteristics and neuropsychological tests scores of the included participants. The final sample size was 136 participants. 57% of all participants were female (n = 78). The mean age of the young group was 31.34 years (SD = 13.12) while for the old group the mean age was 69.32 years (SD = 4.23).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological tests scores of the study sample.

Relationship Between Polygenic Risk Scores and Intrinsic Connectivity

After controlling for ApoE status, age, gender and education, seed-based correlation and ROI-to-ROI analysis of a pooled group of both young and old adults (n = 136) revealed no significant association between the PRS and intrinsic functional connectivity of the PCC with other ROIs (q-FDR > 0.05). More specifically, we looked at the connectivity of the PCC with largely implicated regions in AD pathology that are critical for episodic and spatial memory [i.e., the medial temporal lobe (MTL)] and whose functional connectivity is also altered in early AD (Cutsuridis and Yoshida, 2017; Berron et al., 2020). Our results did not show a significant association between the connectivity of the PCC and bilateral hippocampus in participants with PRS (right: beta = 53.80, q-FDR = 0.930829; left: beta = −54.82, q-FDR = 0.927068). Moreover, there was no significant association of the connectivity between both the right posterior parahippocampus (beta = −109.68, q-FDR = 0.920784) and the left posterior parahippocampus (beta = 79.07, q-FDR = 0.920784) and the PCC with individual PRS. We furthermore explored alterations of connectivity of the PCC with the amygdala and its association with PRS and could neither find significant results for the right amygdala (beta = 36.25, q-FDR = 0.985057), nor for the left amygdala (beta = 65.50, q-FDR = 0.985057).

To verify the results, we extracted the beta connectivity values of the PCC and the above-mentioned regions and examined their association with the respective PRS of each individual in RStudio using multiple linear regression. This additional analysis did not yield any significant association between the PRS and PCC connectivity with other ROIs, neither in the young nor in the old group (p > 0.05).

Relationship Between Polygenic Risk Scores and Cognitive Performance of Young and Old Groups

The two-step hierarchical regression revealed PRS to be a significant predictor for cognitive performance in Letter Number Span and Spatial Span in elderly individuals (>60 years). The regression model with Letter Number Span as dependent variable revealed that at stage one, gender, age, education, and ApoE status did not contribute significantly to the regression model [F(4,50) = 2.06, p = 0.10] and accounted for 14.2% of the variation in Letter Number Span scores. Introducing the PRS ratio explained an additional 7.7% of variation in Letter Number Span Scores and this change in R2 was significant [F(5,49) = 2.74, p = 0.029]. Together the five independent variables accounted for 21.9% of the variance in Letter Number Span scores (see Table 2). The regression model with Spatial Span as dependent variable revealed that at stage one, gender, age, education, and ApoE status did not contribute significantly to the regression model [F(4,50) = 2.42, p = 0.06] and accounted for 16.2% of the variation in Spatial Span scores. Introducing the PRS explained an additional 7.2% of variation in Spatial Span Scores and this change in R2 was significant [F(5,49) = 3.00, p = 0.019]. Together the five independent variables accounted for 23.4% of the variance in Spatial Span scores (see Table 3). The association between PRS and performance in working memory tests (Letter Number Span and Spatial Span) in the old group is demonstrated in Figure 1. Statistical analyses with the whole group did not reveal any significant associations between PRS and cognitive performance. Furthermore, there were no significant associations between PRS and cognitive performance in the group of young individuals.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis for variables predicting Letter Number Span performance.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis for variables predicting Spatial Span performance.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Association between the Polygenic Risk Score (PRS) for AD and performance in working memory tests (Letter Number Span and Spatial Span). The red line demonstrates that a higher genetic risk for AD, reflected in a higher PRS, is associated with decreased working memory capacity, reflected in lower scores, in both tests.

Discussion

In a cohort of cognitively healthy adults, we conducted an exploratory analysis to investigate the association between the genetic risk for AD reflected in a PRS and intrinsic functional connectivity. Our findings demonstrate that PRS did not have a significant predictive effect on the intrinsic functional connectivity of the PCC with other regions in the brain that are implicated in AD. In addition, we investigated the effect of PRS on cognitive performance. Our results show that a higher PRS seems to have a significant association with working memory performance in participants older than 60 years. In this group, a higher PRS was significantly associated with worse performance in tests of working memory capacity. In the next paragraphs, we aim to contextualize our findings, and to interpret their implication in practical terms as well as account for the limitations of the present study.

Previous evidence suggests that the DMN and the PCC connectivity are greatly disrupted in AD by amyloid deposition in DMN regions (Greicius et al., 2004; Sperling et al., 2009; Beason-Held, 2011; Binnewijzend et al., 2012; Damoiseaux et al., 2012; Palmqvist et al., 2017). Altered functional connectivity of the DMN has also been observed in cognitively healthy ApoE ε4 carriers as well as in patients with MCI or AD (Lambert et al., 2013). Nonetheless, clinical trials targeting the amyloid cascade hypothesis have shown inconsistent results so far (Yiannopoulou et al., 2019; Walsh et al., 2021). Anti-amyloid monoclonal antibody treatments such as the recently approved Aducanumab showed reduction in amyloid load in people with MCI or early AD (Sevigny et al., 2016; Office of the Commissioner, 2021; Walsh et al., 2021). However, the clinical benefits (i.e., reversal or slowing of cognitive decline) of this drug are yet to be confirmed in further post-approval clinical trials (Office of the Commissioner, 2021). The previous inconsistent results of clinical trials have directed research into looking for other potential genetic risk factors that are linked to AD. We first investigated whether an elevated genetic risk for AD in healthy persons, reflected in a polygenic risk score that excludes the ApoE region, may have similar alterations in intrinsic functional connectivity to that observed in patients with MCI and AD. This was done by calculating a PRS based on Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) that are significantly associated with AD. Results from our sample could not demonstrate a significant association between individual PRS and intrinsic functional connectivity of the PCC and other regions in the brain, namely bilateral hippocampus, bilateral anterior and posterior parahippocampus, and bilateral amygdala.

Secondly, we looked at the effect of PRS on cognitive performance. We found that a higher PRS was negatively associated with performance in tests of working memory. However, this was true only in participants who were older than 60 years. Working memory is mostly described as a system that maintains and stores information in the short term. Contents within working memory are actively held and manipulated over a limited period of time (Baddeley, 2000). Information is stored between 15 and 30 s in working memory, although this time can be substantially prolonged when the information is constantly rehearsed (Baddeley, 2000). Following Baddeley’s (2000) model, working memory can be subdivided by two main components: the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad. The phonological loop stores phonological information and prevents its decay by continuously articulating its contents. The visuo-spatial sketchpad handles visual and spatial information. Working memory facilitates planning, comprehension, reasoning, and problem-solving (Baddeley, 2005). Previous studies showed that working memory is sensitive to AD pathology (Saunders and Summers, 2011) and that tests of working memory such as Letter Number Span can reliably differentiate healthy controls from patients with MCI or AD (Kessels et al., 2011). Some findings suggest a possible effect of age on working memory capacity with significant decline starting at the age of 60 years (Dobbs and Rule, 1989). A higher genetic risk for AD might amplify the age-related decline. Overall, there is increasing evidence that a higher genetic risk of AD reflected in a higher PRS is associated with a faster decline in memory (Ge et al., 2018; Gustavson et al., 2022). Ge et al. (2018) investigated the influence of a genetic risk for AD on Amyloid β (Aβ) accumulation in the brain, cognitive performance, and hippocampal volume over the course of 2 years. The study found that higher polygenic risk was strongly associated with higher rates of decline in memory and executive function. This was shown primarily in individuals with a cerebral accumulation of Aβ. Our finding of a negative association of PRS and memory performance in elderly participants, but not in younger individuals, could be due to the fact that there was some degree of Aβ accumulation in the brains of the elderly individuals that was not present in the younger group. Cerebral Aβ accumulation associated with Alzheimer’s Disease usually starts later in life, around the age of 60–70 years (Bilgel et al., 2016). Since we only found an association between PRS and working memory performance in the older group (<60 years), a possible explanation could be the prevalence of cerebral Aβ in this group, which may be contributing to the association between working memory performance and PRS (Dobbs and Rule, 1989). However, since we do not have information on cerebral Aβ accumulation for the participants of our study, this hypothesis remains speculative.

We conducted our analysis with a PRS that excluded the ApoE region. Previous evidence suggests that PRS excluding ApoE region is able to differentiate between cognitively healthy adults and those with amnestic MCI (who had higher PRS) (Logue et al., 2019). Findings regarding the association of PRS (including ApoE) with cognition have been inconsistent with some studies showing a lack of significant association with overall cognitive performance (Gui et al., 2014; Bressler et al., 2017), while others found PRS to be associated with a decline in working memory in cognitively healthy adults (Andrews et al., 2016) and in global cognition (Mormino et al., 2016). Our findings suggest that a higher genetic risk of AD beyond ApoE status may drive similar mechanisms that lead to cognitive impairment and deficits in working memory (Becker, 1988; Kessels et al., 2011; Saunders and Summers, 2011).

A major limitation of this present study is the small sample size, specifically after stratification of the sample by age. Since both age and cognitive scores followed a bimodal distribution, we decided to conduct the analysis of cognitive performance in two different age groups. We consider our study to be an exploratory study to examine the association of genetic risk with intrinsic connectivity and cognitive performance. Since genetic variables usually have a small effect size, the small sample size could have resulted in negative findings due to a lack of statistical power. With a larger sample size and thus more statistical power we might have been able to also detect an effect of genetic risk for AD on intrinsic functional connectivity. Studies with small sample sizes have the advantage of allowing to investigate such association while using feasible resources (e.g., less of the participants’ time and lower financial costs). Nonetheless, larger studies are required to further examine and confirm results generated from studies with smaller sample size.

Another limitation is that we only investigate the association of the PRS and intrinsic functional connectivity in cognitively healthy adults. We did not compare this cohort of participants to a group of patients with MCI or AD. Therefore, we could not compare the results and identify potential similarities or differences between the two groups. However, our results still provide a considerable insight into the potential role of genetic variants of AD beyond ApoE ε4 on the modulation of cognitive functions. As this was conducted as an exploratory analysis, further investigation, and comparison between cognitively healthy and people with MCI/AD is imperative to compliment the presented findings. Another potential limitation is the unavailability of longitudinal data of the analyzed sub-sample of this cohort. We analyzed the functional connectivity data that was taken at a cross-sectional point in time. Insightful information could be gathered if we follow up this sample and investigate whether there is further significant modulation in the intrinsic functional connectivity or cognitive decline in relation to their PRS. Secondly, carriers of genetic variants associated with AD may not necessarily express the phenotype (i.e., typical clinical symptoms of AD) in their lifetime. Therefore, it is important to track those who have shown a worse performance in working memory tasks and examine whether they started to express clinical symptoms, show alteration in intrinsic functional connectivity, and develop further cognitive decline.

Conclusion

Our results contribute to the growing body of research exploring the complex polygenicity of AD and its association with alterations in functional connectivity at rest and in cognitive functions. Further investigation of the interaction between genetic risk factors and other sociodemographic variables is warranted to understand the epigenetic nature of AD in older adults.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Goethe University Frankfurt. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AI conducted the data analysis, interpretation of results, and write up of the manuscript. FF was in charge of participant recruitment, data collection, and write up of the manuscript. TMK contributed to the DNA genotyping of participants and calculation of the final individual PRS. TK was involved in the project development, data acquisition and analysis. DP, AR, and JP contributed to designing the study, data collection phase, and revision of the manuscript. EA contributed to the revision of the manuscript. SM was responsible for the conception of the study, study design, supervision of the data analysis and interpretation, and write up and revision of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Neurodegeneration and Alzheimer’s Disease Research grant of the LOEWE program “Neuronal Coordination Research Focus Frankfurt” (NeFF). Open access funding was provided by the Università Della Svizzera Italiana.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all participants for contributing their time to take part of the study. We would also like to thank all authors for their contribution to the data collection, data analysis, writing and revision of the final manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2022.837284/full#supplementary-material

References

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L., Hutton, C., Ashburner, J., Turner, R., and Friston, K. (2001). Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series. Neuroimage 13, 903–919. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews, S. J., Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Anstey, K. J., and Easteal, S. (2016). Association of genetic risk factors with cognitive decline: the PATH through life project. Neurobiol. Aging 41, 150–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26, 839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2000). “Short-term and working memory,” in The Oxford Handbook of Memory, eds E. Tulving and F. I. M. Craik (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 77–92.

Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (2005). Human Memory: theory and Practice, Revised Edn. Hove: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Baker, E., and Escott-Price, V. (2020). Polygenic risk scores in Alzheimer’s disease: current applications and future directions. Front. Digit. Health 2:14. doi: 10.3389/fdgth.2020.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beason-Held, L. L. (2011). Dementia and the default mode. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 8, 361–365.

Google Scholar

Becker, J. T. (1988). Working memory and secondary memory deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 10, 739–753. doi: 10.1080/01688638808402811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belloy, M. E., Napolioni, V., and Greicius, M. D. (2019). A quarter century of APOE and Alzheimer’s disease: progress to date and the path forward. Neuron 101, 820–838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berron, D., van Westen, D., Ossenkoppele, R., Strandberg, O., and Hansson, O. (2020). Medial temporal lobe connectivity and its associations with cognition in early Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 143, 1233–1248. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilgel, M., An, Y., Zhou, Y., Wong, D. F., Prince, J. L., Ferrucci, L., et al. (2016). Individual estimates of age at detectable amyloid onset for risk factor assessment. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 12, 373–379. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.08.166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binnewijzend, M. A. A., Schoonheim, M. M., Sanz-Arigita, E., Wink, A. M., van der Flier, W. M., Tolboom, N., et al. (2012). Resting-state fMRI changes in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2018–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookheimer, S. Y., Strojwas, M. H., Cohen, M. S., Saunders, A. M., Pericak-Vance, M. A., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2000). Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 343, 450–456. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200008173430701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressler, J., Mosley, T. H., Penman, A., Gottesman, R. F., Windham, B. G., Knopman, D. S., et al. (2017). Genetic variants associated with risk of Alzheimer’s disease contribute to cognitive change in midlife: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 174, 269–282. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Shannon, B. J., LaRossa, G., Sachs, R., Fotenos, A. F., et al. (2005). Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J. Neurosci. 25, 7709–7717. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2177-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., and Vincent, J. L. (2007). Unrest at rest: default activity and spontaneous network correlations. Neuroimage 37, 1091–1096; discussion 1097–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. C., Chow, C. C., Tellier, L. C., Vattikuti, S., Purcell, S. M., and Lee, J. J. (2015). Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. Gigascience 4:7. doi: 10.1186/s13742-015-0047-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudhury, S., Brookes, K. J., Patel, T., Fallows, A., Guetta-Baranes, T., Turton, J. C., et al. (2019). Alzheimer’s disease polygenic risk score as a predictor of conversion from mild-cognitive impairment. Transl. Psychiatry 9:167. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0485-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, S. W., and O’Reilly, P. F. (2019). PRSice-2: polygenic risk score software for biobank-scale data. Gigascience 8:giz082. doi: 10.1093/gigascience/giz082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CONN toolbox CONN Toolbox Rrid:Scr_009550. Available online at: www.nitrc.org/projects/conn (accessed March 27, 2022).

Google Scholar

Crook, T. H., Feher, E. P., and Larrabee, G. J. (1992). Assessment of memory complaint in age-associated memory impairment: the MAC-Q. Int. Psychogeriatr. 4, 165–176. doi: 10.1017/S1041610292000991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutsuridis, V., and Yoshida, M. (2017). Editorial: memory processes in medial temporal lobe: experimental, theoretical and computational approaches. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 11:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Prater, K. E., Miller, B. L., and Greicius, M. D. (2012). Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 828.e19–828.e30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deichmann, R., Schwarzbauer, C., and Turner, R. (2004). Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T. Neuroimage 21, 757–767. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delbeuck, X., Van der Linden, M., and Collette, F. (2003). Alzheimer’s disease as a disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychol. Rev. 13, 79–92. doi: 10.1023/a:1023832305702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Dobbs, A. R., and Rule, B. G. (1989). Adult age differences in working memory. Psychol. Aging 4, 500–503. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.4.4.500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fessel, J. (2021). Does synaptic hypometabolism or synaptic dysfunction, originate cognitive loss? Analysis of the evidence. Alzheimers Dement. Transl. Res. Clin. Interv. 7:e12177. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, T., Sabuncu, M. R., Smoller, J. W., Sperling, R. A., and Mormino, E. C., and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2018). Dissociable influences of APOE ε4 and polygenic risk of AD dementia on amyloid and cognition. Neurology 90, e1605–e1612. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, J. M. (1997). Auditory working memory and Wisconsin card sorting test performance in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54, 159–165. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140071013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Supekar, K., Menon, V., and Dougherty, R. F. (2009). Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb. Cortex 19, 72–78. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gui, H., Jiang, C. Q., Cherny, S. S., Sham, P. C., Xu, L., Liu, B., et al. (2014). Influence of Alzheimer’s disease genes on cognitive decline: the guangzhou biobank cohort study. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2422.e3–2422.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustavson, D. E., Reynolds, C. A., Hohman, T. J., Jefferson, A. L., Elman, J. A., Panizzon, M. S., et al. (2022). Alzheimer’s disease polygenic scores predict changes in episodic memory and executive function across 12 years in late middle age. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. doi: 10.1017/S1355617722000108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C. J., Wedeen, V. J., et al. (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, T. M., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2016). Neuroimaging genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease in preclinical individuals: from candidate genes to polygenic approaches. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 1, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, T. M., Mahmood, Z., Lau, E. P., Karacozoff, A. M., Burggren, A. C., Small, G. W., et al. (2016). An Alzheimer’s disease genetic risk score predicts longitudinal thinning of hippocampal complex subregions in healthy older adults. eNeuro 3:ENEURO.0098-16.2016. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0098-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henson, R., Büchel, C., Josephs, O., and Friston, K. (1999). The Slice-Timing Problem in Event-Related fMRI. 1. Available Online at: https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/doc/papers/rnah_slicetime.pdf

Google Scholar

Ishii, K., Mori, T., Hirono, N., and Mori, E. (2003). Glucose metabolic dysfunction in subjects with a clinical dementia rating of 0.5. J. Neurol. Sci. 215, 71–74. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(03)00206-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., et al. (2018). NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 14, 535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Knopman, D. S., Jagust, W. J., Shaw, L. M., Aisen, P. S., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2010). Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 9, 119–128. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Spottke, A., Boecker, H., Brosseron, F., Buerger, K., Catak, C., et al. (2018). Design and first baseline data of the DZNE multicenter observational study on predementia Alzheimer’s disease (DELCODE). Alzheimers Res. Ther. 10:15. doi: 10.1186/s13195-017-0314-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Wolfsgruber, S., Wiese, B., Bickel, H., Mösch, E., Kaduszkiewicz, H., et al. (2014). AD dementia risk in late MCI, in early MCI, and in subjective memory impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessels, R. P. C., Molleman, P. W., and Oosterman, J. M. (2011). Assessment of working-memory deficits in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s dementia using Wechsler’s working memory index. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 23, 487–490. doi: 10.1007/BF03325245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krajcovicova, L., Marecek, R., Mikl, M., and Rektorova, I. (2014). Disruption of resting functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s patients and at-risk subjects. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 14:491. doi: 10.1007/s11910-014-0491-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunkle, B. W., Grenier-Boley, B., Sims, R., Bis, J. C., Damotte, V., Naj, A. C., et al. (2019). Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity and lipid processing. Nat. Genet. 51, 414–430. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0358-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, J. C., Ibrahim-Verbaas, C. A., Harold, D., Naj, A. C., Sims, R., Bellenguez, C., et al. (2013). Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Genet. 45, 1452–1458. doi: 10.1038/ng.2802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonenko, G., Baker, E., Stevenson-Hoare, J., Sierksma, A., Fiers, M., Williams, J., et al. (2021). Identifying individuals with high risk of Alzheimer’s disease using polygenic risk scores. Nat. Commun. 12:4506. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-24082-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logue, M. W., Panizzon, M. S., Elman, J. A., Gillespie, N. A., Hatton, S. N., Gustavson, D. E., et al. (2019). Use of an Alzheimer’s disease polygenic risk score to identify mild cognitive impairment in adults in their 50s. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 421–430. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0030-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matura, S., Köhler, J., Reif, A., Fusser, F., Karakaya, T., Scheibe, M., et al. (2020). Intrinsic functional connectivity, CSF biomarker profiles and their relation to cognitive function in mild cognitive impairment. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 32, 206–213. doi: 10.1017/neu.2019.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matura, S., Prvulovic, D., Butz, M., Hartmann, D., Sepanski, B., Linnemann, K., et al. (2014a). Recognition memory is associated with altered resting-state functional connectivity in people at genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 3128–3135. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matura, S., Prvulovic, D., Jurcoane, A., Hartmann, D., Miller, J., Scheibe, M., et al. (2014b). Differential effects of the ApoE4 genotype on brain structure and function. Neuroimage 89, 81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matura, S., Prvulovic, D., Hartmann, D., Scheibe, M., Sepanski, B., Butz, M., et al. (2016). Age-related effects of the apolipoprotein E gene on brain function. J. Alzheimers Dis. 52, 317–331. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matura, S., Prvulovic, D., Mohadjer, N., Fusser, F., Oertel, V., Reif, A., et al. (2021). Association of dietary fat composition with cognitive performance and brain morphology in cognitively healthy individuals. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 33, 134–140. doi: 10.1017/neu.2021.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mormino, E. C., Smiljic, A., Hayenga, A. O., Onami, S. H., Greicius, M. D., Rabinovici, G. D., et al. (2011). Relationships between beta-amyloid and functional connectivity in different components of the default mode network in aging. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2399–2407. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mormino, E. C., Sperling, R. A., Holmes, A. J., Buckner, R. L., De Jager, P. L., Smoller, J. W., et al. (2016). Polygenic risk of Alzheimer disease is associated with early- and late-life processes. Neurology 87, 481–488. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C., Mohs, R. C., Rogers, H., Fillenbaum, G., and Heyman, A. (1988). Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 24, 641–652.

Google Scholar

Niemann, H., Sturm, W., Thöne-Otto, A., and Willmes, K. (2008). California Verbal Learning Test. Deutsche Adaptation. Frankfurt am Main: Pearson Assessment & Information GmbH.

Google Scholar

Office of the Commissioner (2021). FDA Grants Accelerated Approval for Alzheimer’s Drug. Silver Spring, MD: FDA.

Google Scholar

Palmqvist, S., Schöll, M., Strandberg, O., Mattsson, N., Stomrud, E., Zetterberg, H., et al. (2017). Earliest accumulation of β-amyloid occurs within the default-mode network and concurrently affects brain connectivity. Nat. Commun. 8:1214. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01150-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rickham, P. (1964). Human experimentation. Code of ethics of the world medical association. Declaration of Helsinki. Br. Med. J. 2:177. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.5402.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio.

Google Scholar

Saunders, N. L. J., and Summers, M. J. (2011). Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive, and working memory in subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 25, 237–248. doi: 10.1037/a0021134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrano-Pozo, A., Das, S., and Hyman, B. T. (2021). APOE and Alzheimer’s disease: advances in genetics, pathophysiology, and therapeutic approaches. Lancet Neurol. 20, 68–80. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(20)30412-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sevigny, J., Chiao, P., Bussière, T., Weinreb, P. H., Williams, L., Maier, M., et al. (2016). The antibody aducanumab reduces Aβ plaques in Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 537, 50–56. doi: 10.1038/nature19323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorg, C., Riedl, V., Mühlau, M., Calhoun, V. D., Eichele, T., Läer, L., et al. (2007). Selective changes of resting-state networks in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 18760–18765. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708803104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., et al. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 7, 280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., LaViolette, P. S., O’Keefe, K., O’Brien, J., Rentz, D. M., Pihlajamaki, M., et al. (2009). Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron 63, 178–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Teipel, S. J., Metzger, C. D., Brosseron, F., Buerger, K., Brueggen, K., Catak, C., et al. (2018). Multicenter resting state functional connectivity in prodromal and dementia stages of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 64, 801–813. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, S., Merrick, R., Milne, R., and Brayne, C. (2021). Aducanumab for Alzheimer’s disease? BMJ 374:n1682. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n1682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Wang, J., He, Y., Li, H., Yuan, H., Evans, A., et al. (2015). Apolipoprotein E ε4 modulates cognitive profiles, hippocampal volume, and resting-state functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 45, 781–795. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Yiannopoulou, K. G., Anastasiou, A. I., Zachariou, V., and Pelidou, S.-H. (2019). Reasons for failed trials of disease-modifying treatments for Alzheimer disease and their contribution in recent research. Biomedicines 7:E97. doi: 10.3390/biomedicines7040097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarei, M., Beckmann, C. F., Binnewijzend, M. A. A., Schoonheim, M. M., Oghabian, M. A., Sanz-Arigita, E. J., et al. (2013). Functional segmentation of the hippocampus in the healthy human brain and in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage 66, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H.-Y., Wang, S.-J., Xing, J., Liu, B., Ma, Z.-L., Yang, M., et al. (2009). Detection of PCC functional connectivity characteristics in resting-state fMRI in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 197, 103–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, P., Qin, W., Wang, D., Liu, B., Zhang, Y., Jiang, T., et al. (2015). Impacts of PICALM and CLU variants associated with Alzheimer’s disease on the functional connectivity of the hippocampus in healthy young adults. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 1463–1475. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0738-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease (AD), genetic disposition, polygenic risk score (PRS), cognition, intrinsic functional connectivity, resting-state fMRI

Citation: Ibnidris A, Fußer F, Kranz TM, Prvulovic D, Reif A, Pantel J, Albanese E, Karakaya T and Matura S (2022) Investigating the Association Between Polygenic Risk Scores for Alzheimer’s Disease With Cognitive Performance and Intrinsic Functional Connectivity in Healthy Adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:837284. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.837284

Received: 16 December 2021; Accepted: 08 April 2022;
Published: 11 May 2022.

Edited by:

Annalena Venneri, Brunel University London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Shea Andrews, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States
Anbupalam Thalamuthu, University of New South Wales, Australia
Byron Creese, University of Exeter, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Ibnidris, Fußer, Kranz, Prvulovic, Reif, Pantel, Albanese, Karakaya and Matura. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aliaa Ibnidris, aliaa.ibnidris@usi.ch

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.