- 1Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
- 2Departments of Psychiatry and Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- 3University of Florida Norman Fixel Institute for Neurological Diseases, Gainesville, FL, United States
Almost 25% of all older adults experience difficulty walking. Mobility difficulties for older adults are more pronounced when they perform a simultaneous cognitive task while walking (i.e., dual task walking). Although it is known that aging results in widespread brain atrophy, few studies have integrated across more than one neuroimaging modality to comprehensively examine the structural neural correlates that may underlie dual task walking in older age. We collected spatiotemporal gait data during single and dual task walking for 37 young (18–34 years) and 23 older adults (66–86 years). We also collected T1-weighted and diffusion-weighted MRI scans to determine how brain structure differs in older age and relates to dual task walking. We addressed two aims: (1) to characterize age differences in brain structure across a range of metrics including volumetric, surface, and white matter microstructure; and (2) to test for age group differences in the relationship between brain structure and the dual task cost (DTcost) of gait speed and variability. Key findings included widespread brain atrophy for the older adults, with the most pronounced age differences in brain regions related to sensorimotor processing. We also found multiple associations between regional brain atrophy and greater DTcost of gait speed and variability for the older adults. The older adults showed a relationship of both thinner temporal cortex and shallower sulcal depth in the frontal, sensorimotor, and parietal cortices with greater DTcost of gait. Additionally, the older adults showed a relationship of ventricular volume and superior longitudinal fasciculus free-water corrected axial and radial diffusivity with greater DTcost of gait. These relationships were not present for the young adults. Stepwise multiple regression found sulcal depth in the left precentral gyrus, axial diffusivity in the superior longitudinal fasciculus, and sex to best predict DTcost of gait speed, and cortical thickness in the superior temporal gyrus to best predict DTcost of gait variability for older adults. These results contribute to scientific understanding of how individual variations in brain structure are associated with mobility function in aging. This has implications for uncovering mechanisms of brain aging and for identifying target regions for mobility interventions for aging populations.
1. Introduction
Nearly 25 percent of older adults report serious mobility problems such as difficulty walking or climbing stairs (Kraus, 2016). Older adults tend to encounter even greater difficulty with performing a secondary cognitive task while walking, i.e., dual task walking (e.g., Springer et al., 2006; Hollman et al., 2007; Malcolm et al., 2015; Smith et al., 2016). A common measure of dual task walking performance is dual task cost (DTcost), or the magnitude of performance decline when conducting two tasks at once as opposed to individually (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008; Bayot et al., 2020). Older adults typically exhibit greater DTcosts compared with young adults, such as greater slowing of gait speed from dual to single task conditions (for review, see Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Beurskens and Bock, 2012). Examining DTcost is considered more useful than assessing single or dual condition performance in isolation, as cost metrics incorporate individual differences in baseline performance (Verhaeghen et al., 2003).
Poorer dual task walking abilities have been related to increased fall risk (e.g., Lundin-Olsson et al., 1997; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012; Bridenbaugh and Kressig, 2015), cognitive decline (Montero-Odasso et al., 2017), frailty, disability, and mortality (Verghese et al., 2012). Importantly, dual task walking performance is more predictive of falls in aging than single task walking performance (Ayers et al., 2014; Johansson et al., 2016; Verghese et al., 2017; Halliday et al., 2018; Gillain et al., 2019). This could be because dual task walking provides a better analog for real-world scenarios. Indeed, a recent study reported that in-lab dual task walking attributes were more similar to real-world gait, as compared with normal walking in the lab with no dual tasking requirements (Hillel et al., 2019). Thus, given the link between dual task walking performance and falls, and its greater ecological validity, we selected to analyze dual instead of single task walking.
There are clear cortical contributions to the control of walking (Miyai et al., 2001; Petersen et al., 2012; Allali et al., 2014; Koenraadt et al., 2014; Takakusaki, 2017). Thus, poorer dual task walking performance in older age has been attributed, at least in part, to age-related brain atrophy (Allali et al., 2019; Lucas et al., 2019; Ross et al., 2021). A large body of literature suggests that age-related structural brain atrophy occurs in an anterior-to-posterior pattern, with the frontal cortices atrophying earlier and faster than other regions of the brain (e.g., Salat et al., 2004; Fjell et al., 2009a; Thambisetty et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012). Given this, it is not surprising that previous work has linked lower prefrontal cortex gray matter volume with poorer dual task walking abilities in older adults (Tripathi et al., 2019; Wagshul et al., 2019). Aging is hypothesized to increase reliance on alternative (i.e., non-motor) neural resources, such as the frontal cortex (Mirelman et al., 2017), to compensate for brain atrophy in sensorimotor regions and maintain performance (Cabeza et al., 2002; Steffener and Stern, 2012; Fettrow et al., 2021b). Interestingly, recent work in a large sample of middle- to older-aged adults (n = 966) has reported disproportionately steep age differences (i.e., atrophy, demyelination, and iron reduction) in the sensorimotor cortices rather than in more anterior prefrontal regions (Taubert et al., 2020). Thus, structural changes in the sensorimotor cortices with aging may also contribute to age-related mobility declines.
Many previous studies have reported relationships between age differences in regional brain structure and worse gait for older adults during single task walking (for review, see Tian et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2019). However, compared to the extensive literature examining single task walking, only limited work examining brain structure has focused on dual task walking in aging. A majority of the studies examining correlates of dual task walking in aging have instead focused on brain function, using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). These studies have largely found increases in prefrontal cortex oxygenation levels from single to dual task walking for older adults, suggesting that dual compared with single task walking demands more prefrontal neural resources (e.g., Doi et al., 2013; Beurskens et al., 2014; Holtzer et al., 2015). As dual task walking is more cognitively demanding than normal walking, it is logical that functional contributions from the prefrontal cortex increase during dual task walking (Holtzer et al., 2015); thus, markers of prefrontal cortex structure might also relate to dual task walking performance in older age. Overall, while these functional studies provide important insight into the vasodynamic response to dual task walking, further work is needed to understand how markers of brain structure relate to dual task walking in aging.
The small body of work that has investigated relationships between brain structure and dual task walking in older adults suggests an important link between “maintenance” of brain structure and maintenance of dual task walking abilities. Two previous studies found associations between greater gait slowing during dual task walking in older adults and lower gray matter volume in the middle frontal gyrus (Allali et al., 2019), medial prefrontal and cingulate cortices, and thalamus (Tripathi et al., 2019). Further, several studies found that older adults who showed a greater increase in prefrontal cortex oxygenation from single to dual task walking also had lower white matter fractional anisotropy (averaged across the whole white matter mask; Lucas et al., 2019), lower gray matter volume within the frontal lobe (Wagshul et al., 2019), and reduced thickness across the cortex (Ross et al., 2021). These imaging metrics were not related to faster dual task walking, though, suggesting that the observed increases in prefrontal cortex activity represented compensation to maintain walking performance, despite atrophying brain structure.
The prior work described above examining the brain structural correlates of dual task walking tested only one imaging modality in isolation. Here we combined across multiple structural imaging modalities to provide more comprehensive information about age differences in brain structure and how these relate to dual task walking. We assessed volumetric measures of atrophy, i.e., gray matter, cerebellum, hippocampus, and ventricular volume. In addition to widespread declines in gray matter volume (paired with ventricular enlargement) with aging (Raz et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012), prior work has also reported widespread cerebellar atrophy with aging, particularly in the anterior and superior-posterior lobes of the cerebellum (Koppelmans et al., 2017). We also examined surface metrics, including cortical thickness (Dahnke et al., 2013), sulcal depth (Yun et al., 2013), cortical complexity (i.e., folding complexity of the cortex; Yotter et al., 2011b), and gyrification index (i.e., mean curvature of the cortex; Luders et al., 2006). Surface-based morphometry metrics have several advantages over volume-based metrics (Hutton et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012), including more accurate spatial registration (Desai et al., 2005), sensitivity to surface folding, and independence from head size (Gaser and Kurth, 2017). Despite these potential benefits, compared to volumetric measures, less work has examined how surface measures relate to dual task walking in aging.
We also examined white matter microstructure metrics derived from diffusion MRI, including free-water (FW) corrected fractional anisotropy (FAt, “t” refers to the tissue compartment remaining after FW correction), axial diffusivity (ADt), and radial diffusivity (RDt), and the fractional volume of FW (Pasternak et al., 2009). FW correction is particularly important for analyses of older adult brains because age-related white matter degeneration can lead to enlarged interstitial spaces (Meier-Ruge et al., 1992) and thereby increased partial volume effects between white matter fibers and extracellular water (Chad et al., 2018). Recent work found that FW correction results in less pronounced age differences in white matter microstructure than previously reported (Chad et al., 2018), suggesting that prior age difference results are at least partially driven by fluid effects. Thus, to increase interpretability of white matter microstructural effects, it is important to correct for FW when examining white matter in aging. Moreover, higher FW has been related to poorer cognition in aging (Maillard et al., 2019; Gullett et al., 2020) and poorer function (e.g., bradykinesia) in Parkinson's disease (Ofori et al., 2015).
In the present work, we addressed several aims: (1) To characterize age differences in brain structure; we predicted the most pronounced age differences in the prefrontal cortex. (2) To identify regions of age differences in the relationship between brain structure and DTcost of gait speed and variability; given the fNIRS literature reporting increased prefrontal cortex activation during dual task walking (Doi et al., 2013; Beurskens et al., 2014; Holtzer et al., 2015), we predicted that greater prefrontal atrophy would correlate with greater DTcost of gait speed and variability for older but not younger adults. (3) To determine the strongest predictors(s) of DTcost of gait in older adults using a stepwise regression approach. This was an exploratory aim, and thus we did not define an a priori hypothesis.
2. Materials and Methods
The University of Florida's Institutional Review Board provided ethical approval for the study. All individuals provided their written informed consent.
2.1. Participants
37 young and 25 older adults from the Gainesville, FL community participated in this study. Participants were in generally good health, with no reported neurologic or psychiatric problems. Two older adults were excluded from analyses of the T1-weighted images. Thus, n = 23 older adults for all analyses involving the T1-weighted images. A diffusion MRI was not collected for one young and two older adults; thus, n = 36 young and n = 21 older adults for all diffusion MRI analyses. See Supplementary Section 1 for further details regarding participant selection and exclusion criteria. Of note, we reported on a different subset of behavioral and brain metrics from this same cohort in two recent publications (Fettrow et al., 2021a; Hupfeld et al., 2021a).
2.2. Testing Sessions
Before the first session, we collected self-reported participant information on: demographics (e.g., age, sex, and years of education), medical history, handedness, footedness, exercise, and sleep. We also collected anthropometric information (e.g., height and weight). Participants then completed mobility testing, followed by an MRI scan approximately 5 days later (Figure 1). For 24 h prior to each session, participants were requested to not consume alcohol, nicotine, or any drugs other than the medications they disclosed to us.
Figure 1. Methods overview. Left: During Session 1, participants first completed a normal (single task) overground walk (NW) at a comfortable self-selected speed. Next, participants completed three trials in a counterbalanced order: two walking while talking trials (WWT-1 and WWT-2) in which participants counted backwards by 7s while walking, and one talking only trial in which participants stayed seated while counting backwards by 7s. Right: Approximately 5 days later, during Session 2, participants completed an MRI protocol, which included a T1-weighted anatomical scan and a diffusion-weighted scan.
2.3. Session 1: Mobility Testing
Participants completed three walking tasks while instrumented with six Opal inertial measurement units (IMUs; v2; APDM Wearable Technologies Inc., Portland, OR, USA). IMUs were placed on the feet, wrists, around the waist at the level of the lumbar spine, and across the torso at the level of the sternal angle (Figure 1). First, participants walked back and forth across a 9.75 m room for 4 min at whichever pace they considered to be their “normal” walking speed (NW). Participants were instructed to refrain from talking, to keep their arms swinging freely at their sides, and to keep their head up and gaze straight ahead. Each time they reached the end of the room, they completed a 180-degree turn and walked the length of the room again.
Next, participants completed two trials of walking while talking (WWT-1 and WWT-2) and one trial of talking only. The WWT and talking only trials lasted for 2 min each. During the WWT trials, participants walked at their normal speed while counting backwards by 7s (Li et al., 2014), starting at number 299, 298, or 296. The WWT instructions were identical to those provided for the 4-min walk, except that participants were additionally instructed to “try and pay equal attention to walking and talking” (Verghese et al., 2007). For the talking only trial, participants sat in a chair and counted backwards by 7s for 2 min. We counterbalanced the order of the WWT-1, WWT-2, and talking only trials and the starting number across all participants.
2.4. Spatiotemporal Variable Calculation
During the walking tasks, we recorded inertial data using MobilityLab software (v2; APDM Wearable Technologies Inc., Portland, OR, USA). After each trial, MobilityLab calculated 14 spatiotemporal gait variables based on the straight-ahead (non-turning) portions of each walking trial. The algorithm for calculating these metrics has been validated through comparison to force plate and motion capture data (see internal validation by MobilityLab: https://support.apdm.com/hc/en-us/articles/360000177066-How-are-Mobility-Lab-s-algorithms-validated- and Washabaugh et al., 2017). To condense the gait variables into several summary metrics, for each trial, we extracted one variable from each of the four gait domains described by Hollman et al. (2011a): gait rhythm [cadence (steps/min)], gait phase [stance (% gait cycle)], gait pace [speed (m/s)], and gait variability [step time variability (standard deviation)]. Each of these variables was reported to have high validity when compared to the same metrics calculated using force plate data (Washabaugh et al., 2017). We then calculated the average of each of these four variables for the NW and WWT-1 and WWT-2 trials to produce one variable for each of the four gait domains for NW and WWT.
2.5. Cognitive Outcome Variable Calculation
We also measured cognitive performance during the seated compared to WWT conditions. We examined both speed (i.e., total number of subtraction problems attempted) and accuracy (i.e., % correct) during both the seated and WWT conditions.
2.6. DTcost Calculation
To characterize differences in these gait and cognitive performance summary metrics between single and dual task conditions, similar to a large body of previous work (e.g., Kelly et al., 2010; Van Impe et al., 2011; Patel et al., 2014), we calculated the DTcost of each variable as follows:
We then calculated a correlation matrix for the four resulting DTcost of gait measures across the whole sample. This revealed that DTcost of gait speed was highly correlated with the DTcost of cadence (r = 0.90, p < 0.001) and DTcost of stance time (r = –0.85, p < 0.001). Thus, we opted to analyze only two variables as primary outcome metrics in our final statistical analyses: (1) DTcost of gait speed; and (2) DTcost of step time variability. Both slower gait speed and increased step time variability have been related to higher fall risk for older adults (Espy et al., 2010; Callisaya et al., 2011; Quach et al., 2011).
2.7. Session 2: MRI Scan
We acquired an MRI scan for each participant using a Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma 3 T scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) with a 64-channel head coil. We collected a 3D T1-weighted anatomical image using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence. The parameters for this anatomical image were as follows: repetition time (TR) = 2,000 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.06 ms, flip angle = 8°, field of view = 256 × 256 mm2, slice thickness = 0.8 mm, 208 slices, voxel size = 0.8 mm3. We also collected a diffusion-weighted spin-echo prepared echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: 5 b0 scans (without diffusion weighting), 64 gradient directions with diffusion weighting 1,000 s/mm2, TR = 6,400 ms, TE = 58 ms, isotropic resolution = 2 x 2 x 2 mm, FOV = 256 x 256 mm2, 69 slices, phase encoding direction = Anterior to Posterior. Immediately prior to this acquisition, we collected 5 b0 scans (without diffusion weighting) in the opposite phase encoding direction (Posterior to Anterior) for later use in distortion correction.
2.8. T1-Weighted Image Processing for Voxelwise Analyses
For further details regarding T1-weighted image preprocessing, see Supplementary Section 2.
2.8.1. Gray Matter Volume
We processed the T1-weighted scans using the Computational Anatomy Toolbox toolbox (version r1725; Gaser and Dahnke, 2016; Gaser and Kurth, 2017) in MATLAB (R2019b). We implemented default CAT12 preprocessing steps, which ultimately produces whole-brain modulated, normalized gray matter maps for each participant. To increase signal-to-noise ratio, we smoothed these modulated, normalized gray matter segments using Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM12, v7771; Ashburner et al., 2014) with an 8 mm full width at half maximum kernel. We entered these preprocessed gray matter volume maps into the group-level voxelwise statistical models described in Section 2.11.2. We used CAT12 to calculate total intracranial volume for each participant for later use as a covariate in these group-level statistical analyses.
2.8.2. Cortical Surface Metrics
The CAT12 pipeline also extracts surface-based morphometry metrics (Yotter et al., 2011a; Dahnke et al., 2013). We used CAT12 to extract four surface metrics: (1) cortical thickness: the thickness of the cortical gray matter between the outer surface (i.e., the gray matter-cerebrospinal fluid boundary) and the inner surface (i.e., the gray matter-white matter boundary) (Dahnke et al., 2013); (2) cortical complexity: fractal dimension, a metric of folding complexity of the cortex (Yotter et al., 2011b); (3) sulcal depth: the Euclidean distance between the central surface and its convex hull (Yun et al., 2013); and (4) gyrification index: a metric based on the absolute mean curvature, which quantifies the amount of cortex buried within the sulcal folds as opposed to the amount of cortex on the “outer” visible surface (Luders et al., 2006). We resampled and smoothed the surfaces at 15 mm for cortical thickness and 20 mm for the three other metrics (Gaser and Kurth, 2017). We entered these resampled and smoothed surface files into the group-level voxelwise statistical models described in Section 2.11.2.
2.8.3. Cerebellar Volume
To improve the normalization of the cerebellum (Diedrichsen, 2006; Diedrichsen et al., 2009), similar to our past work (Salazar et al., 2020, 2021; Hupfeld et al., 2021b), we applied specialized preprocessing steps to the cerebellum to produce cerebellar volume maps. First, we entered each participant's whole-brain T1-weighted image into the CEREbellum Segmentation (CERES) pipeline (Romero et al., 2017). We then used the Advanced Normalization Tools package (ANTs; v1.9.17; Avants et al., 2010, 2011) to warp (in a single step) each participant's extracted subject space cerebellum to the Spatially Unbiased Infratentorial Template (SUIT) template (Diedrichsen, 2006; Diedrichsen et al., 2009). The flowfields that were applied to warp these cerebellar segments to SUIT space were additionally used to calculate the Jacobian determinant image, using ANTs' CreateJacobianDeterminantImage.sh function. We multiplied each normalized cerebellar segment by its corresponding Jacobian determinant to produce modulated cerebellar images in standard space for each participant. Lastly, to increase signal-to-noise ratio, we smoothed the modulated, normalized cerebellar images using a kernel of 2 mm full width at half maximum and entered the resulting cerebellar volume maps into the group-level voxelwise statistical models described in Section 2.11.2. Of note, we examined cerebellar total volumes in our statistical analyses instead of segmenting the cerebellum by tissue type, in order to avoid any inaccuracy due to low contrast differences between cerebellar gray and white matter.
2.9. Diffusion-Weighted Image Processing for Voxelwise Analyses
See Supplementary Section 3 for further details regarding preprocessing of the diffusion-weighted data.
2.9.1. Diffusion Preprocessing
We then corrected images for signal drift (Vos et al., 2017) using the ExploreDTI graphical toolbox (v4.8.6; www.exploredti.com; Leemans et al., 2009) in MATLAB (R2019b). Next, we used the FMRIB Software Library (FSL; v6.0.1; Smith et al., 2004; Jenkinson et al., 2012) processing tool topup to estimate the susceptibility-induced off-resonance field (Andersson et al., 2003). This procedure yielded a single corrected field map for use in eddy current correction. We used FSL's eddy_cuda to simultaneously correct the data for eddy current-induced distortions and both inter- and intra-volume head movement (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016).
2.9.2. FW Correction and Tensor Fitting
We implemented a custom FW imaging algorithm (Pasternak et al., 2009) in MATLAB. This algorithm estimates FW fractional volume and FW-corrected diffusivities by fitting a two-compartment model at each voxel (Pasternak et al., 2009). The two-compartment model consists of: (1) a tissue compartment modeling water molecules within or in the vicinity of white matter tissue, quantified by diffusivity (FAt, RDt, and ADt); and (2) a FW compartment, reflecting the proportion of water molecules with unrestricted diffusion, and quantified by the fractional volume of this compartment. FW ranges from 0 to 1; FW = 1 indicates that a voxel is filled with freely diffusing water molecules (e.g., as in the ventricles). These metrics (FAt, RDt, ADt, FW) are provided as maps for each voxel in the brain.
2.9.3. Tract-Based Spatial Statistics
We applied FSL's tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) processing steps to prepare the data for voxelwise analyses across participants (Smith et al., 2006). We used the TBSS pipeline as provided in FSL, which first includes eroding the FA images slightly and zeroing the end slices. Next, each participant's FA data is brought into a common space (i.e., the FMRIB58_FA 1 mm isotropic template) using the nonlinear registration tool FNIRT (Andersson et al., 2007a,b). A mean FA image is then calculated and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton. Then, each participant's aligned FA data is projected onto the group mean skeleton. Lastly, we applied the same nonlinear registration to the FW, FAt, RDt, and ADt maps to project these data onto the original mean FA skeleton. Ultimately, these TBSS procedures resulted in skeletonized FW, FAt, ADt, and RDt maps in standard space for each participant. These were the maps that we entered in the group-level voxelwise statistical models described in Section 2.11.2.
2.10. Image Processing for Region of Interest Analyses
See Supplementary Section 4 for further details regarding extraction of the regions of interest (ROIs).
2.10.1. Ventricle and Gray Matter Volume ROIs
CAT12 automatically calculates the inverse warp, from standard space to subject space, for several volume-based atlases. We isolated multiple ROIs from these atlases in subject space: the lateral ventricles and pre- and postcentral gyri from the Neuromorphometrics (http://Neuromorphometrics.com) volume-based atlas, and the thalamus, striatum, and globus pallidus from the CoBra Subcortical atlas (Tullo et al., 2018; Supplementary Figure S1). We then calculated ROI volume in mL as: (number of voxels in the ROI mask)*(mean intensity of the tissue segment within the ROI mask)*(volume/voxel). In subsequent statistical analyses, we used the average of the left and right side structures for each ROI, and we entered these ROI volumes as a percentage of total intracranial volume (to account for differences in head size).
2.10.2. FW ROIs
We also extracted FW values from the diffusion MRI maps for the same ROIs for which we calculated gray matter volume. We rigidly registered the subject space T1-weighted image to the subject space FW image. We then used ANTs to apply the inverse of that transformation to the subject T1-space atlases described in Section 2.10.1. This resulted in volumetric atlases for each participant in their native diffusion space. We then isolated masks for the same ROIs described in Section 2.10.1. Finally, we used fslstats to extract mean image intensity in the FW map within each ROI mask. Here we used mean intensity as our outcome metric (rather than volume in mL as above) to estimate the fractional volume of FW within the ROI and obtain a metric more representative of microstructural FW, rather than the size of the ROI which represents macrostructural atrophy. We calculated the average mean intensity for the left and right side for each structure and used this average value in subsequent statistical analyses.
2.10.3. Hippocampal ROIs
We implemented the Automatic Segmentation of Hippocampal Subfields (ASHS)-T1 (Yushkevich et al., 2015) pipeline within ITK-SNAP (Yushkevich et al., 2015) to segment and extract the volume in mL of three hippocampal structures: anterior hippocampus, posterior hippocampus, and parahippocampal cortex. Though this pipeline is currently validated for use on only older adults (defined as those 55+ years old; Yushkevich et al., 2015), for completeness, here we also implemented the pipeline on my younger adult participants. For statistical analyses, we used the average of the left and right side structures, and we entered these volumes as a percentage of total intracranial volume (to account for differences in head size).
2.11. Statistical Analyses
2.11.1. Participant Characteristics and Behavioral Data
We conducted all statistical analyses on the demographic and behavioral data using using R (v4.0.0; R Core Team, 2013). For each set of analyses, we applied the Benjamini-Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR) correction to the p-values for the age group predictor (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).
First, we compared demographic, physical characteristics, and testing timeline variables between the age groups. We tested the parametric t-test assumptions: normality within each group (Shapiro test, p > 0.05) and homogeneity of variances between groups (Levene's test, p > 0.05). The majority of variables did not meet parametric assumptions, so we conducted nonparametric two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum tests for age group differences. We report the group medians and interquartile ranges for each of these variables. We also report nonparametric effect sizes (Rosenthal et al., 1994; Field et al., 2012). To test for differences in the sex distribution within each age group, we conducted a Pearson chi-square test.
To examine whether gait and subtraction performance differed between the single and dual task conditions and/or between the age groups, we used a linear mixed model approach (lme; Pinheiro et al., 2007). We entered age group, condition (i.e., single or dual task), and the age group*condition interaction as predictors, and included a random intercept for each subject. In the case of outliers (i.e., ±3 SD from the whole-group mean), we reran the linear mixed model excluding outlier data points. In all of these instances, the statistical significance of each predictor did not change with the exclusion of outliers.
2.11.2. Voxelwise Statistical Models
We tested the same voxelwise models for each of the imaging modalities. In each case, we defined the model using SPM12 and then re-estimated each model using the Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement toolbox (TFCE; http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/tfce) with 5,000 permutations. This toolbox provides non-parametric estimation using TFCE for models previously estimated using SPM parametric designs. Non-parametric estimation avoids parametric (e.g., random field theory) distribution assumptions. TFCE produces results in which voxelwise values represent the amount of cluster-like local spatial support. TFCE is favorable as it does not require an arbitrary cluster-forming threshold, and it is more sensitive compared with other thresholding methods (Smith and Nichols, 2009). Statistical significance was determined at p < 0.05, family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons.
2.11.2.1. Age Differences in Brain Structure
First, we conducted two-sample t-tests to test for age differences in brain structure. In each of these models, we set the imaging modality (e.g., normalized, modulated gray matter volume segments) as the outcome variable and controlled for sex (as there are reported sex differences in brain structure across the lifespan Ruigrok et al., 2014, including greater between-subject variability in brain structure for males compared to females Wierenga et al., 2020). In the gray matter and cerebellar volume models, we also controlled for head size (i.e., total intracranial volume). Also in the gray matter volume models only, we set the absolute masking threshold to 0.1 (Gaser and Kurth, 2017) and used an explicit gray matter mask that excluded the cerebellum (because we analyzed cerebellar volume separately from "whole brain" gray matter volume; Section 2.8.3).
2.11.2.2. Age Differences in Brain Structure - DTcost of Gait Relationships
Our primary analysis of interest then tested for regions in which the relationship between brain structure and the DTcost of gait differed between young and older adults. We ran two-group t-test models and included the DTcost of gait speed or step time variability for young and older adults as covariates of interest. We tested for regions in which the correlation between brain structure and DTcost was greater for the young compared with the older adults, and where the correlation between brain structure and DTcost was lower for the young compared with the older adults. As above, we controlled for sex in all models, and we controlled for head size in the gray matter and cerebellar volume models.
2.11.3. ROI Statistical Models
We conducted ROI analyses in R. For each set of analyses, we applied the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR correction to the p-values for the predictor(s) of interest (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).
2.11.3.1. Age Differences in Brain Structure
Similar to the above voxelwise models, we first ran linear models to test for age group differences in ROI volume or mean intensity, controlling for sex. We applied the FDR correction to the p-values for the age group predictor (i.e., the primary analysis of interest). Post hoc, we also FDR-corrected the p-values for the sex predictor, to better interpret several statistically significant sex difference results.
2.11.3.2. Age Differences in Brain Structure - DTcost of Gait Relationships
Also similar to above, we ran linear models testing for an interaction of age group with the DTcost of gait speed or step time variability, controlling for sex. We FDR-corrected the p-values for the interaction term.
2.11.4. Multiple Regression to Identify the Best Predictors of DTcost of Gait in Older Adults
We used two stepwise multivariate linear regressions to directly compare the neural correlates of the DTcost of gait identified by the voxelwise and ROI analyses described above. We ran one model for the DTcost of gait speed, and one model for the DTcost of step time variability. We included only the older adults in these models because the older adults showed stronger relationships between brain structure and the DTcost of gait (whereas the young adults tended to show either a weak relationship or no clear relationship between brain structure and the DTcost of gait).
In each of the two full models, we included sex and values from the peak result coordinate for each voxelwise model that indicated a statistically significant age difference in the relationship between brain structure and the DTcost of gait as predictors. We also included ROI values as predictors in any cases where the linear model yielded a significant age group by DTcost interaction term. We used stepAIC (Venables and Ripley, 1999) to produce a final model that retained only the best predictor variables; stepAIC selects a maximal model based on the combination of predictors that produces the smallest Akaike information criterion (AIC). Overall, this stepwise regression approach allowed us to fit the best models using brain structure to predict the DTcost of gait for the older adults.
3. Results
3.1. Comparison of Participant Characteristics and Testing Timeline
There were no statistically significant differences between the age groups in sex, handedness, footedness, alcohol use, or hours of sleep prior to each testing session. Both groups reported a strong preference for using their right hand and right foot for motor tasks (Oldfield, 1971; Elias et al., 1998), and both groups reported "low-risk" consumption of alcohol (Saunders et al., 1993). In addition, there were no age group differences in the number of days elapsed between the testing sessions or in the difference in start time for the sessions. Older adults did report higher body mass indices (BMIs); the group median young adult BMI (22.71 kg/m2) fell into the "normal" range, while the group median older adult BMI (25.86 kg/m2) fell into the "overweight" range. Older adults also self-reported less physical activity than the young adults, though both groups reported sufficient physical activity to be classified as "active" (Godin and Shephard, 1985). Compared to the young adults, the older adults reported lower balance confidence and greater fear of falling, though the older adults did not report a clinically significant fear of falling (i.e., scores >70; Tinetti et al., 1990). See Table 1 for complete demographic information.
3.2. Age and Condition Differences in Performance
Across both age groups, gait speed slowed and gait variability increased during WWT compared to NW (Table 2 and Supplementary Figure S2). There was not a statistically significant difference in serial subtraction accuracy between the seated and WWT conditions (Table 2), though both young and older adults attempted fewer subtraction problems during the WWT conditions compared to the seated condition (Table 2 and Supplementary Figure S2). Thus, across both age groups, subtraction speed decreased from single to dual task, but accuracy did not change.
Across both conditions, the young adults performed with higher accuracy compared with the older adults (Table 2). However, there were no statistically significant age group differences in the DTcost of walking or subtraction performance (i.e., there were no significant age group by condition interactions; Figure 2 and Supplementary Figure S2). That is, the magnitude of single to dual task decrements in gait speed and number of subtraction problems attempted, as well as the magnitude of the increase in gait variability, was similar for young and older adults.
Figure 2. Differences in walking and subtraction performance during single vs. dual task conditions. Gait and serial subtraction performance are depicted for each young (orange) and older (blue) adult. Each line represents one participant. Group means are shown in red. Across both age groups, gait speed slowed, gait variability increased, and number of subtraction problems attempted decreased from single to dual task conditions. *pFDR−corr < 0.05, **pFDR−corr < 0.01, ***pFDR−corr < 0.001.
3.3. Comparison of Brain Structure Between Age Groups
3.3.1. T1-Weighted MRI Metrics
Across the whole brain, older adults had significantly lower gray matter volume compared with young adults (Figure 3). The greatest differences between young and older adults occurred in the bilateral pre- and postcentral gyri, temporal lobe, insula, and inferior portion of the frontal cortex. Cerebellar volume was lower for older compared with younger adults across most of the cerebellum, though there were no age differences in some regions, including the vermis and bilateral crus I (Figure 3). Across the entire cortical surface, older adults had lower cortical thickness compared with young adults (Figure 4). The largest age differences in cortical thickness occurred in the bilateral pre- and postcentral gyri and portions of the superior frontal cortex. Gyrification index was lower for older adults in the bilateral insula only. Cortical complexity was lower for older adults across portions of the bilateral insula, left middle frontal cortex, and posterior cingulate gyrus. Sulcal depth was reduced for older adults across the bilateral temporal lobes and insula, within the lateral fissure of the brain. Sulcal depth was higher for older compared with young adults across the superior frontal cortex, along the midline (Figure 4).
Figure 3. Age differences in gray matter and cerebellar volume. Increasingly warm colors indicate regions where young adult volumes were greater than older adult volumes. Results are overlaid onto a whole brain MNI-space template (left) and onto the SUIT cerebellar template (right). pFWE−corr < 0.05.
Figure 4. Age differences in surface measures. Warm colors indicate regions where young adult values were greater than older adult values. Cool colors indicate regions where young adult values were lower than older adult values. Results are overlaid onto CAT12 standard space templates. L, left; R, right. pFWE−corr < 0.05.
3.3.2. Diffusion MRI Metrics
Compared with young adults, older adults showed lower FAt, lower ADt, higher RDt, and higher FW across almost the entire white matter skeleton (Figure 5). There were some exceptions to this pattern, however, in portions of the superior corona radiata, corpus callosum (e.g., splenium), internal capsule, and thalamic radiations in which older adults showed higher FAt, higher ADt, and lower RDt compared with young adults.
Figure 5. Age differences in FW-corrected white matter microstructure. Warm colors indicate regions where young adult values were greater than older adult values. Cool colors indicate regions where young adult values were lower than older adult values. Results are shown on the FMRIB58 FA template with the group mean white matter skeleton (green) overlaid. Age differences at pFWE−corr < 0.05 covered almost the entire white matter skeleton; these results are depicted in the rightmost column of each panel. The left portion of each panel depicts more conservative statistical thresholding (noted under each colorbar) to better illustrate which regions showed the most pronounced age differences.
3.3.3. ROIs
Lateral ventricular volume was higher for older compared with younger adults (Supplementary Figure S3 and Supplementary Table S1). Older adults exhibited lower gray matter volume in all ROIs except for the globus pallidus and higher FW in all ROIs except for postcentral gyrus (Supplementary Figure S4 and Supplementary Table S1). Older adults had lower hippocampal volume across each of the three hippocampal ROIs (Supplementary Figure S5 and Supplementary Table S1). In several regions, pooling across both age groups, females had higher gray matter volume (thalamus) and FW (pre- and postcentral gyri and thalamus) compared with males.
3.4. Age Differences in the Relationship of Brain Structure With the DTcost of Gait Speed
There were no statistically significant age group by DTcost of gait speed interactions for gray matter or cerebellar volume. However, for the older adults, shallower sulcal depth across the sensorimotor, supramarginal, and superior frontal and parietal cortices was associated with greater DTcost of gait speed (Figure 6 and Table 3). That is, those older adults who showed the largest decreases in gait speed from single to dual task also had the shallowest sulcal depth across these regions. Young adults did not exhibit a clear relationship between sulcal depth in these regions and the DTcost of gait speed. There were no statistically significant age group differences in the correlation of cortical thickness, cortical complexity, or gyrification index with the DTcost of gait speed.
Figure 6. Age differences in the relationship of surface metrics with the DTcost of gait. Top. Regions showing statistically significant (pFWE−corr < 0.05) age group differences in the relationship of cortical thickness (left) and sulcal depth (middle, right) with the DTcost of gait speed and step time variability. Warmer colors indicate regions of greater age differences in brain-behavior correlations. Results are overlaid onto CAT12 standard space templates. L = left; R = right. Bottom. Surface values for the peak result coordinate for each model are plotted against DTcost of gait to illustrate examples of the relationships identified by the voxelwise statistical tests. The fit line and confidence interval shading are included only to aid visualization of these relationships. We plotted the residuals instead of the raw values here to adjust for the effects of the sex covariate included in each model.
Table 3. Regions of age difference in the relationship of sulcal depth with the DTcost of gait speed and step time variability.
There were age differences in the relationship between DTcost of gait speed and both ADt and RDt in portions of the left superior corona radiata involving the superior longitudinal fasciculus and corticospinal tract (Figure 7 and Table 4). For the older adults only, higher ADt and lower RDt in these regions was associated with greater slowing of gait speed from single to dual task conditions. Young adults showed no relationship between ADt or RDt in these regions and DTcost of gait speed. There were no statistically significant age group differences in the correlation of FAt or FW with the DTcost of gait speed.
Figure 7. Age differences in the relationship of FW-corrected white matter microstructure with the DTcost of gait speed. Left. Regions showing statistically significant (pFWE−corr < 0.05) age group differences in the relationship of ADt (top) and RDt (bottom) with the DTcost of gait speed. Warmer colors indicate regions of greater age differences. Results are shown on the FMRIB58 FA template with the group mean white matter skeleton (green) overlaid. Right. ADt and RDt values for the peak result coordinate for each model are plotted against the DTcost of gait speed to illustrate examples of the relationships identified by the voxelwise statistical tests. The fit line and confidence interval shading are included only to aid visualization of these relationships. We plotted the residuals instead of the raw values here to adjust for the effects of the sex covariate included in each model.
Table 4. Regions of age difference in the relationship of FW-corrected white matter microstructure with the DTcost of gait speed.
For older adults only, larger lateral ventricular volume was associated with greater decreases in gait speed from single to dual task walking (Figure 8 and Table 5). There was no relationship between lateral ventricular volume and DTcost of gait speed for young adults. Older adult relationships between DTcost of gait speed with several other ROIs [i.e., thalamus gray matter volume (p = 0.025; pFDR−corr = 0.172) and parahippocampal cortex volume (p = 0.045; pFDR−corr = 0.208)] did not survive FDR correction. There were no other statistically significant interactions between age group and DTcost of gait speed for the remaining ROIs.
Figure 8. Age differences in the relationship of lateral ventricular volume with the DTcost of gait speed. Left. Here we depict the lateral ventricular volume mask for a single exemplar participant overlaid onto that participant's native space cerebrospinal fluid segment. Right. Lateral ventricular volume residuals (expressed as a percentage of total intracranial volume) are plotted against the DTcost of gait speed. We plotted the residuals instead of the raw values here to adjust for the effects of the sex covariate included in the model. **pFDR−corr < 0.01.
Table 5. Regions of age difference in the relationship of structural ROIs with the DTcost of gait speed.
3.5. Age Differences in the Relationship of Brain Structure With the DTcost of Step Time Variability
There were no statistically significant age group by DTcost of step time variability interactions for gray matter or cerebellar volume. For older adults, thinner temporal lobe cortex was associated with greater DTcost of step time variability (Figure 6 and Table 6). That is, those older adults with the thinnest temporal cortex also showed the greatest increase in step time variability from single to dual task. Young adults showed a weak opposite relationship between temporal cortex thickness and the DTcost of step time variability. In addition, those older adults with shallower sulcal depth across the sensorimotor, supramarginal, insular, and superior frontal and parietal cortices also showed a greater DTcost of step time variability (Figure 6 and Table 3). Young adults showed a weak opposite relationship between sulcal depth in these regions and the DTcost of step time variability. There were no statistically significant age differences in the relationship of cortical complexity or gyrification index with the DTcost of step time variability.
Table 6. Regions of age difference in the correlation of cortical thickness with the DTcost of step time variability.
There were no statistically significant age differences in the relationship between the DTcost of step time variability and FW-corrected white matter microstructure. Greater DTcost of step time variability was associated with lower parahippocampal cortex volume for the older adults, though this relationship did not survive FDR correction (p = 0.039; pFDR−corr = 0.433). There were no statistically significant interactions between age group and the DTcost of step time variability for the remaining ROIs (Supplementary Table S2).
3.6. Multiple Regression to Identify the Best Predictors of DTcost of Gait in Older Adults
For the DTcost of gait speed full model, we entered each participant's left precentral gyrus sulcal depth and superior longitudinal fasciculus ADt and RDt (extracted from the peak region resulting from each voxelwise model). We also entered lateral ventricular volume (expressed as a percentage of total intracranial volume) and sex. The stepwise regression returned a model containing only sulcal depth, ADt, and sex, indicating that the combination of these three variables best predicts the DTcost of gait speed for older adults (Table 7).
Table 7. Stepwise multiple regression results for the best models of DTcost of gait in older adults.
For the DTcost of step time variability full model, we entered each participant's right superior temporal gyrus cortical thickness and left precentral gyrus sulcal depth, as well as sex. The stepwise regression returned a model containing only cortical thickness, indicating that this surface metric best predicts the DTcost of step time variability for older adults (Table 7).
4. Discussion
We examined a comprehensive set of structural MRI metrics in relation to dual task walking in older adults. We identified widespread brain atrophy for older adults; across imaging modalities, we found the most prominent age-related atrophy in brain regions related to sensorimotor processing. Moreover, though the DTcost of gait speed and variability did not differ by age group, we identified multiple age differences in the relationship between brain structure and DTcost of gait. These age differences occurred both in regional metrics such as the temporal cortices and white matter tracts involved in motor control, and also for more general markers of brain atrophy, such as the lateral ventricles. We selected dual task walking performance as our outcome metric, as it is more predictive of falls in aging than single task walking (Ayers et al., 2014; Johansson et al., 2016; Verghese et al., 2017; Halliday et al., 2018; Gillain et al., 2019) and more related to real-world mobility (Hillel et al., 2019). Together, these results provide greater scientific understanding of the structural correlates of dual task walking in aging and highlight potential targets for future mobility interventions.
4.1. No Age Differences in the DTcost of Gait
Gait speed slowed, gait variability increased, and total number of subtraction problems attempted decreased between the single and dual task conditions. However, there were no age differences in the DTcost of gait speed, step time variability, or serial subtraction performance. That is, older adults did not exhibit a disproportionately larger decrease in gait speed or increase in gait variability between the NW and WWT conditions. Older adults also did not exhibit a disproportionately larger decrease in the total number of subtraction problems attempted between the seated and WWT conditions. While previous literature has mostly reported larger DTcosts to gait in older adults (e.g., for review see Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Beurskens and Bock, 2012), other previous work (which used a similar linear mixed model approach to our study) has found no age differences in the DTcost of gait speed (Holtzer et al., 2011). Moreover, much of this prior work has focused on comparisons of aging with pathologies such as cognitive impairment (Pettersson et al., 2007; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012), rather than comparisons of young and older adults. In our sample of relatively high-functioning older adults, the lack of group differences in the DTcost of gait and subtraction performance is perhaps unsurprising. Of note, we do believe that our cognitive task (serial 7s) was sufficiently difficult to divide attention between walking and talking for both age groups, as our task was more difficult than other common paradigms, such as reciting alternate letters of the alphabet (Verghese et al., 2007; Ayers et al., 2014; Tripathi et al., 2019). This lack of group differences in behavioral performance then frames our brain structure analyses to probe the neural correlates of preservation of function in aging. Thus, we can explore the neural correlates that might underlie compensation for normal brain aging and permit successful maintenance of dual task walking abilities into older age.
4.2. Age Differences in Brain Structure
4.2.1. Gray Matter Volume, Cerebellar Volume, and Cortical Thickness
Overall, we found evidence of widespread brain atrophy for older compared with young adults. This observation is well in line with previous literature, which has similarly identified widespread age differences in brain gray matter volume (e.g., Raz et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012; Storsve et al., 2014), cerebellar volume (e.g., Raz et al., 2010; Bernard et al., 2015; Koppelmans et al., 2017; Han et al., 2020), and cortical thickness (e.g., Salat et al., 2004; Fjell et al., 2009b; Thambisetty et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012; van Velsen et al., 2013; Storsve et al., 2014). Many reports suggest that age-related atrophy occurs disproportionately in the frontal cortices (e.g., Salat et al., 2004; Fjell et al., 2009a; Thambisetty et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012). However, our finding of the most prominent age differences in gray matter volume and thickness of the sensorimotor cortices (and comparatively less age difference in the frontal cortices) fits with recent work which identified the greatest age differences (gray and white matter atrophy, demyelination, FW, and iron reduction) within the sensorimotor cortices in a large (n = 966) sample of middle- to older-aged adults (Taubert et al., 2020). Taubert et al. suggested that the particular age differences in sensorimotor cortex structure could be either a cause or an effect of age-related impairments to motor control (Papegaaij et al., 2014; Taubert et al., 2020).
4.2.2. Additional Surface Metrics
While previous reports indicate that patterns of cortical thinning with aging largely mirror age-related changes in gray matter volume, the effects of aging on the other surface metrics studied here (i.e., sulcal depth, cortical complexity, and gyrification index) are not as well characterized. A couple of prior reports have indicated that, with aging, sulci become wider and shallower (Rettmann et al., 2006; Jin et al., 2018), and the cortex becomes less complex (Madan and Kensinger, 2016), with lower gyrification indices (Hogstrom et al., 2013; Cao et al., 2017; Madan and Kensinger, 2018; Lamballais et al., 2020; Madan, 2021). Our findings fit with these patterns, although across each of these metrics, we found the most prominent age differences within the lateral sulcus, whereas some previous work identified the largest age differences in other regions such as the central sulcus (cortical thickness; Rettmann et al., 2006), parietal lobe (sulcal depth; Jin et al., 2018), and frontal lobe (cortical complexity; Madan and Kensinger, 2016; and gyrification index; Lamballais et al., 2020). Differences in subject characteristics across studies might explain these differences; for instance, Jin et al. (2018) reported sulcal depth differences in middle vs. older aged adults, rather than young compared with older adults.
4.2.3. FW-Corrected White Matter Microstructure
Only one previous study has directly compared FW-corrected white matter microstructure between healthy young and older adults (Chad et al., 2018), despite that FW-corrected diffusion metrics have significantly higher test-retest reliability than conventional diffusion-weighted metrics (Albi et al., 2017), and that FW correction allows for separation of atrophy effects (i.e., increased extracellular fluid) from changes to the structure of the remaining white matter. Our findings here of age differences in FW-corrected white matter microstructure largely mirror those of Chad et al. (2018). As anticipated, we found lower FAt and ADt, paired with higher RDt and FW across almost the entire white matter skeleton. This pattern fits with previous literature examining FW-uncorrected white matter as well: prominent declines in FA, typically interpreted as decreased white matter microstructural organization and integrity (Bennett et al., 2010; Sexton et al., 2014) although also reflective of crossing fiber integrity (Chad et al., 2018), decreases in AD, interpreted as accumulation of debris or metabolic damage with age (Pierpaoli et al., 2001; Song et al., 2003; Madden et al., 2012), and increases in RD, interpreted as decreased myelin integrity or demyelination (Song et al., 2002, 2005; Madden et al., 2012).
After applying the FW correction to our data, we found several areas of opposite age differences, quite similar to the results described by Chad et al. (2018). Specifically, we observed a seemingly paradoxical finding in portions of the superior corona radiata, corpus callosum (e.g., splenium), internal capsule, and thalamic radiations, in which FAt and ADt were higher and RDt was lower for the older compared with the young adults. In addition to the report by Chad et al. (2018), several large datasets of normal aging (examining FW-uncorrected white matter) also corroborate this finding (Sexton et al., 2014; de Groot et al., 2016; Miller et al., 2016). Previous interpretations of this increased FA include selective degeneration of non-dominant tracts paired with a relative sparing of the primary bundle at fiber crossings (Chad et al., 2018). In particular, in this region, the corona radiata, internal capsule, and corpus callosum all cross the corticospinal tract (Tuch et al., 2003). The diffusion tensors in these regions indicate that the corticospinal tract is the principal fiber (Chad et al., 2018); bedpostx tractography analyses by Chad et al. (2018) suggest that the superior longitudinal fasciculus crosses the corona radiata in this region, and that the thalamic radiations also cross the corticospinal tract in this region of the internal capsule. Thus, as the superior longitudinal fasciculus and thalamic radiations are thought to degenerate substantially with age (Cox et al., 2016), while the corticospinal tract is thought to be relatively spared in aging (Jang and Seo, 2015), it is likely that the selective degeneration of non-dominant fibers in these locations is driving this seemingly paradoxical finding in the older adults.
4.2.4. Structural ROIs
We selected the ROIs used in this study because of their purported roles in mobility function (i.e., the sensorimotor cortices, basal ganglia, and hippocampus; Callisaya et al., 2013; Beauchet et al., 2015, 2019). We also examined the lateral ventricles as a more general metric of subcortical atrophy. As anticipated, almost all of these ROIs showed significant age differences (i.e., reduced gray matter volume, increased FW, and increased ventricular volume). This fits with the existing literature reporting ventricular expansion in older age (Carmichael et al., 2009; Fjell et al., 2009a). However, it is interesting to note that FW fractional volumes showed less pronounced age differences compared to gray matter volumes. This could indicate that microstructural FW does not change as markedly with normal aging, in comparison to macrostructural gray matter tissue. Comparison of FW fractional volumes to prior aging work is difficult, as most previous papers report increased subcortical (e.g., substantia nigra) FW in pathological aging (e.g., Parkinson's disease) compared with controls (Guttuso et al., 2018; Yang et al., 2019), as opposed to reporting comparisons of healthy young and older adults.
4.3. Interaction of Age Group With the DTcost of Gait
4.3.1. Gray Matter and Cerebellar Volumes
We did not identify any statistically significant age group differences in the relationship between the DTcost of gait speed or variability and regional gray matter volume. While extensive previous literature has examined relationships of single task overground walking with gray matter and cerebellar volume (e.g., Rosano et al., 2007; Dumurgier et al., 2012; Callisaya et al., 2013; Beauchet et al., 2015; Demnitz et al., 2017), comparatively less work has examined such relationships with dual task walking (Allali et al., 2019; Lucas et al., 2019; Tripathi et al., 2019; Wagshul et al., 2019; Ross et al., 2021). Further, these studies had methodological differences from our work (e.g., they used an alphabet task instead of serial 7s as the cognitive task). Moreover, it could be that we did not identify gray matter volume associations with the DTcost of gait because other measures (e.g., surface-based morphometry metrics) may provide a more sensitive correlate of behavior as compared with volume metrics. Surface-based metrics have been found to have several advantages over volume-based metrics (Hutton et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012), including more accurate spatial registration (Desai et al., 2005), sensitivity to surface folding, and independence from head size (Gaser and Kurth, 2017).
4.3.2. Surface Metrics
We identified several age differences in brain-behavior relationships for two surface metrics: cortical thickness and sulcal depth. Only a few previous studies have examined relationships between cortical thickness and dual task walking in aging (Maidan et al., 2021; Ross et al., 2021), and, to our knowledge, no prior literature has examined sulcal depth in relation to dual task walking in aging. In the present work, we identified a relationship between thinner temporal cortex and greater increases in step time variability from single to dual task walking for older adults. Interestingly, the superior, middle, and transverse temporal gyri where we identified this result have functions in visual perception (Miyashita, 1993; Ishai et al., 1999), multimodal sensory integration (Mesulam, 1998; Downar et al., 2000), and spatial navigation (Howard et al., 2005). Given these functional roles, it is plausible that these regions of the temporal cortex would play a role in gait control.
Moreover, this region of temporal cortex is not one in which we found prominent age-related cortical thinning. Thus, it is possible that this temporal region plays a compensatory role in aging, to compensate for the substantial cortical thinning with aging that we identified in classical sensorimotor brain regions, such as the pre- and postcentral gyri. This notion fits with the hypothesis of neural inefficiency in aging (Zahodne and Reuter-Lorenz, 2019; Fettrow et al., 2021b), which suggests that, when neural resources become limited (as with age-related atrophy of the sensorimotor cortices), different neural resources (e.g., in this case, the temporal cortices) are used to compensate and maintain performance (e.g., as seen in the lack of age differences in the DTcost of gait). This also results in a stronger relationship between temporal lobe structure and dual task walking, which only emerges in older age when these neural resources start to become limited. This interpretation fits with a recent report of an association between lower cortical thickness and greater increases in prefrontal oxygenation from single to dual task walking, with no effect on performance (Ross et al., 2021). The study authors suggested that older adults with the poorest neural resources (i.e., the thinnest cortex) also required the most compensation from alternative brain regions (i.e., the greatest increases in prefrontal oxygenation) to maintain performance. One caveat to this interpretation, however, is that hypotheses of neural compensation with aging were largely developed in relation to functional, not structural, MRI data—though our data appear to follow a similar pattern.
We also identified two relationships between sulcal depth in aging and greater DTcost of gait speed and variability for older adults. Similar to cortical thickness, these brain-behavior relationships did not fall within the prominent regions of age difference in sulcal depth (i.e., the bilateral temporal lobes and insula), and instead spanned the sensorimotor, supramarginal, superior frontal and parietal cortices. Thus, these sulcal depth findings could similarly represent an age-related compensation. That is, in compensation for shallowing of other cortical regions in aging, those who retained deeper sulci into older age were also able to maintain the best functional walking performance.
Of note, while young adults did not show a clear relationship between cortical thickness or sulcal depth and DTcost of gait speed, young adults did exhibit a relationship between greater sulcal depth and lower DTcost of step time variability (which is in the opposite direction of what we might expect). Greater step time variability is clearly related to negative outcomes for older adults, such as higher fall risk (Callisaya et al., 2011). However, the case is less clear for young adults (Beauchet et al., 2009; Moe-Nilssen et al., 2010). For instance, higher gait variability for younger adults can indicate more stable gait (Beauchet et al., 2009). Additionally, it could be that young adults were using a different strategy to complete the task.
4.3.3. FW-Corrected White Matter Microstructure
Several prior studies have linked lower white matter diffusivity metrics to poorer overground walking (e.g., Bruijn et al., 2014; Tian et al., 2016; Verlinden et al., 2016) and dual task walking in older adults (e.g., Ghanavati et al., 2018). However, though one prior study identified relationships between FW-corrected white matter microstructure and cognition in normal aging (Gullett et al., 2020), to our knowledge, no previous work has examined how FW-corrected white matter microstructure relates to mobility in older adults.
We identified two relationships in which higher ADt and lower RDt were associated with worse dual task performance, i.e., greater slowing of gait speed from single to dual task conditions. This is perhaps the opposite pattern from what one might expect, as lower ADt is often associated with accumulation of debris or metabolic damage (Pierpaoli et al., 2001; Song et al., 2003; Madden et al., 2012), and higher RDt is interpreted as decreased myelin integrity or demyelination (Song et al., 2002, 2005; Madden et al., 2012). However, this result occurred in the superior corona radiata, where older adults had higher ADt and lower RDt than young adults (see Section 4.2.3). It could be that, in these white matter regions, the poorest performing older adults also have the greatest degeneration of crossing fibers, such as the superior longitudinal fasciculus crossing the corticospinal tract. As the superior longitudinal fasciculus is implicated in functions such as motor control, proprioception, and visuospatial attention and awareness (Spena et al., 2006; Shinoura et al., 2009; Rodŕıguez-Herreros et al., 2015; Amemiya and Naito, 2016), it is logical that deterioration of this pathway could negatively impact dual task walking in aging.
4.3.4. Structural ROIs
We identified a relationship between larger lateral ventricular volume and greater DTcost of gait speed for older but not younger adults. This fits with some previous work that has linked larger ventricular volume with higher gait variability (Annweiler et al., 2014) and slower gait speed (Camicioli et al., 1999) in older adults. However, it is surprising that we did not identify relationships between DTcost of gait and the remaining structural ROIs, as previous work has linked sensorimotor (Rosano et al., 2007), basal ganglia (Dumurgier et al., 2012), and hippocampal (Beauchet et al., 2015) volumes to gait in aging. Our results thus suggest that generalized atrophy of subcortical structures, as opposed to atrophy of a single subcortical structure, is a better correlate of dual task locomotor function in aging.
4.4. Best Models of DTcost of Gait in Aging
Across the multimodal neuroimaging markers examined, left precentral gyrus sulcal depth, left superior longitudinal fasciculus ADt, and sex were the best predictors of DTcost of gait speed for older adults, and right superior temporal gyrus cortical thickness represented the best predictor of DTcost of step time variability. Given the purported benefits of surface metrics over volumetric measures (Desai et al., 2005; Hutton et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2010; Lemaitre et al., 2012), the inclusion of sulcal depth and cortical thickness in these final models is perhaps unsurprising. Further, by minimizing partial volume effects resulting from white matter atrophy with aging, FW-corrected measures should provide greater sensitivity than traditional diffusion metrics for detecting true microstructural effects in aging cohorts. Thus, it is also perhaps unsurprising that ADt in a region (superior longitudinal fasciculus) particularly affected by aging (Cox et al., 2016) was also a good predictor of DTcost of gait in aging. Females showed larger DTcosts of gait speed; previous literature has only infrequently reported sex differences in dual task walking in older adults (e.g., Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2010; Hollman et al., 2011b; MacAulay et al., 2014), and findings have been conflicting.
We would like to note that these surface and white matter metrics are complicated measures and that, although these produced the best models of DTcost of gait, it is worth mentioning that lateral ventricular volume also represented a good predictor of DTcost of gait speed in aging. Ventricular volume can be extracted easily by applying automated algorithms to common T1-weighted MRI sequences, and provides a useful general metric of subcortical atrophy, which our data suggest contributes functionally to gait speed slowing in aging.
4.5. Limitations
Our cross-sectional approach precluded us from tracking concurrent changes in brain structure and mobility over time. Additionally, our statistical models focused on the interaction of age group with the DTcost of gait, in order to identify regions where the relationship between brain structure and DTcost of gait differed for young vs. older adults. We did not test for regions where brain structure related to DTcost of gait in the same manner for each age group. Such models may have uncovered more brain-behavior relationships in classical motor control regions, such as pre- and postcentral gyrus and the cerebellum. However, this was not a focus of the present work. Instead, our primary goal was to understand what brain regions contributed differently to maintenance of dual task walking in older age, to probe age-related shifts in the cortical control of gait and potential compensatory processes. In addition, we did not test for relationships between brain structure and subtraction performance. Subtraction accuracy did not differ between single and dual task conditions (i.e., most DTcost scores were close to 0) and thus it would not have made sense to assess brain-behavior relationships in this case. The total number of subtraction problems attempted was lower for both age groups during single compared to dual task, though this difference was less pronounced compared to the gait metrics. Future work could test whether there are different brain structure-behavior relationships for the DTcost of serial subtraction speed compared to the DTcost of gait metrics. Finally, though instructions affect self-selected gait speed (Brinkerhoff et al., 2019) and we provided identical instructions to all participants, we cannot be sure that participants all similarly interpreted and followed our instructions to “try and pay equal attention to walking and talking.”
4.6. Conclusions
In this multimodal neuroimaging study, we found widespread age-related atrophy across cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar regions, but particularly in regions related to sensorimotor processing (e.g., the pre- and postcentral gyri). We then identified potential compensatory relationships between better maintenance of brain structure in regions not classically associated with motor control (e.g., the temporal cortices) and preserved dual task walking abilities in older adults. This suggests a role for the temporal cortices in maintaining behavioral function in aging, particularly when other brain regions responsible for locomotor control (e.g., the sensorimotor cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum) may be largely atrophied. Additionally, we identified one relationship between less specific subcortical atrophy (i.e., larger lateral ventricles) and greater slowing during dual task walking in aging. As the global population quickly ages, and emerging evidence continues to relate mobility problems with pathologies such as cognitive decline (Dodge et al., 2012; Knapstad et al., 2019), it is becoming increasingly critical to understand the structural neural correlates of locomotor function in aging. Identifying such brain markers could help identify those at the greatest risk of mobility declines, as well as identify targets for future interventions to preserve mobility and prevent disability among older adults.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Florida Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
KH led the initial study design, collected and preprocessed all of the neuroimaging and gait data, conducted all statistical analyses, created the figures and tables, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JG assisted with data collection, data processing, and manuscript preparation. OP and HM consulted on DWI preprocessing and contributed to manuscript preparation. CH consulted on the design and analysis of the gait assessments. RS oversaw project design and led the interpretation and discussion of the results. All authors participated in revision of the manuscript.
Funding
During completion of this work, KH was supported by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship under grant nos. DGE-1315138 and DGE-1842473, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke training grant no. T32-NS082128, and National Institute on Aging fellowship 1F99AG068440. HM was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada postdoctoral fellowship and a NASA Human Research Program augmentation grant. RS was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Aging U01AG061389. A portion of this work was performed in the McKnight Brain Institute at the National High Magnetic Field Laboratory's Advanced Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Spectroscopy (AMRIS) Facility, which is supported by National Science Foundation Cooperative Agreement No. DMR-1644779 and the State of Florida.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank Aakash Anandjiwala, Pilar Alvarez Jerez, and Alexis Jennings-Coulibaly for their assistance in subject recruitment and data collection, as well as Sutton Richmond for his help in applying the signal drift correction to the diffusion-weighted data. The authors also wish to thank all of the participants who volunteered their time, as well as the McKnight Brain Institute MRI technologists, without whom this project would not have been possible.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2022.809281/full#supplementary-material
References
Albi, A., Pasternak, O., Minati, L., Marizzoni, M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Bargallo, N., et al. (2017). Free water elimination improves test-retest reproducibility of diffusion tensor imaging indices in the brain: a longitudinal multisite study of healthy elderly subjects. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 12–26. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23350
Allali, G., Montembeault, M., Brambati, S. M., Bherer, L., Blumen, H. M., Launay, C. P., et al. (2019). Brain structure covariance associated with gait control in aging. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 74, 705–713. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly123
Allali, G., Van Der Meulen, M., Beauchet, O., Rieger, S. W., Vuilleumier, P., and Assal, F. (2014). The neural basis of age-related changes in motor imagery of gait: an fMRI study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 69, 1389–1398. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt207
Al-Yahya, E., Dawes, H., Smith, L., Dennis, A., Howells, K., and Cockburn, J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008
Amemiya, K., and Naito, E. (2016). Importance of human right inferior frontoparietal network connected by inferior branch of superior longitudinal fasciculus tract in corporeal awareness of kinesthetic illusory movement. Cortex 78, 15–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.01.017
Andersson, J. L., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., et al. (2007b). Non-linear Registration, Aka Spatial Normalisation. FMRIB technical report tr07ja2. FMRIB Analysis Group of the University of Oxford.
Andersson, J. L., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., and Andersson, J. (2007a). Non-linear optimisation. FMRIB technical report tr07ja1. FMRIB Analysis Group of the University of Oxford.
Andersson, J. L., Skare, S., and Ashburner, J. (2003). How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 20, 870–888. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7
Andersson, J. L., and Sotiropoulos, S. N. (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage 125, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019
Annweiler, C., Montero-Odasso, M., Bartha, R., Drozd, J., Hachinski, V., and Beauchet, O. (2014). Association between gait variability and brain ventricle attributes: a brain mapping study. Exp. Gerontol. 57, 256–263. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2014.06.015
Ashburner, J., Barnes, G., Chen, C.-C., Daunizeau, J., Flandin, G., Friston, K., et al. (2014). SPM12 Manual. London, UK: Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging.
Avants, B. B., Tustison, N. J., Song, G., Cook, P. A., Klein, A., and Gee, J. C. (2011). A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54, 2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025
Avants, B. B., Yushkevich, P., Pluta, J., Minkoff, D., Korczykowski, M., Detre, J., et al. (2010). The optimal template effect in hippocampus studies of diseased populations. Neuroimage 49, 2457–2466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.062
Ayers, E. I., Tow, A. C., Holtzer, R., and Verghese, J. (2014). Walking while talking and falls in aging. Gerontology 60, 108–113. doi: 10.1159/000355119
Bayot, M., Dujardin, K., Dissaux, L., Tard, C., Defebvre, L., Bonnet, C. T., et al. (2020). Can dual-task paradigms predict falls better than single task?-a systematic literature review. Neurophysiol. Clin. 50, 401–440. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2020.10.008
Beauchet, O., Allali, G., Annweiler, C., Bridenbaugh, S., Assal, F., Kressig, R. W., et al. (2009). Gait variability among healthy adults: low and high stride-to-stride variability are both a reflection of gait stability. Gerontology 55, 702–706. doi: 10.1159/000235905
Beauchet, O., Launay, C. P., Annweiler, C., and Allali, G. (2015). Hippocampal volume, early cognitive decline and gait variability: which association? Exp. Gerontol. 61, 98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2014.11.002
Beauchet, O., Launay, C. P., Sekhon, H., Montembeault, M., and Allali, G. (2019). Association of hippocampal volume with gait variability in pre-dementia and dementia stages of alzheimer disease: results from a cross-sectional study. Exp. Gerontol. 115, 55–61. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.11.010
Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B Stat. Methodol. 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
Bennett, I. J., Madden, D. J., Vaidya, C. J., Howard, D. V., and Howard, J. H. (2010). Age-related differences in multiple measures of white matter integrity: a diffusion tensor imaging study of healthy aging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 378–390. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20872
Bernard, J. A., Leopold, D. R., Calhoun, V. D., and Mittal, V. A. (2015). Regional cerebellar volume and cognitive function from adolescence to late middle age. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 1102–1120. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22690
Beurskens, R., and Bock, O. (2012). Age-related deficits of dual-task walking: a review. Neural Plast. 2012, 131608. doi: 10.1155/2012/131608
Beurskens, R., Helmich, I., Rein, R., and Bock, O. (2014). Age-related changes in prefrontal activity during walking in dual-task situations: a fNIRS study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 122–128. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.03.005
Bridenbaugh, S. A., and Kressig, R. W. (2015). Motor cognitive dual tasking: early detection of gait impairment, fall risk and cognitive decline. Zeitschrift für Gerontologie und Geriatrie 48, 15–21. doi: 10.1007/s00391-014-0845-0
Brinkerhoff, S. A., Murrah, W. M., Hutchison, Z., Miller, M., and Roper, J. A. (2019). Words matter: instructions dictate “self-selected” walking speed in young adults. Gait Posture. 93. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.07.379
Bruijn, S. M., Van Impe, A., Duysens, J., and Swinnen, S. P. (2014). White matter microstructural organization and gait stability in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6, 104. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00104
Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 17, 1394–1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280
Callisaya, M. L., Beare, R., Phan, T. G., Blizzard, L., Thrift, A. G., Chen, J., et al. (2013). Brain structural change and gait decline: a longitudinal population-based study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 61, 1074–1079. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12331
Callisaya, M. L., Blizzard, L., Schmidt, M. D., Martin, K. L., McGinley, J. L., Sanders, L. M., et al. (2011). Gait, gait variability and the risk of multiple incident falls in older people: a population-based study. Age Ageing 40, 481–487. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr055
Camicioli, R., Moore, M., Sexton, G., Howieson, D., and Kaye, J. A. (1999). Age-related brain changes associated with motor function in healthy older people. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 330–334. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb02997.x
Cao, B., Mwangi, B., Passos, I. C., Wu, M.-J., Keser, Z., Zunta-Soares, G. B., et al. (2017). Lifespan gyrification trajectories of human brain in healthy individuals and patients with major psychiatric disorders. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00582-1
Carmichael, O. T., Lopez, O., Becker, J. T., and Kuller, L. (2009). Trajectories of brain loss in aging and the development of cognitive impairment. Neurology 72, 771–772. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000339386.26096.93
Chad, J. A., Pasternak, O., Salat, D. H., and Chen, J. J. (2018). Re-examining age-related differences in white matter microstructure with free-water corrected diffusion tensor imaging. Neurobiol. Aging 71, 161–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.07.018
Cox, S. R., Ritchie, S. J., Tucker-Drob, E. M., Liewald, D. C., Hagenaars, S. P., Davies, G., et al. (2016). Ageing and brain white matter structure in 3,513 UK biobank participants. Nat. Commun. 7, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13629
Dahnke, R., Yotter, R. A., and Gaser, C. (2013). Cortical thickness and central surface estimation. Neuroimage 65, 336–348. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.050
de Groot, M., Cremers, L. G., Ikram, M. A., Hofman, A., Krestin, G. P., van der Lugt, A., et al. (2016). White matter degeneration with aging: longitudinal diffusion MR imaging analysis. Radiology 279, 532–541. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2015150103
Demnitz, N., Zsoldos, E., Mahmood, A., Mackay, C. E., Kivimäki, M., Singh-Manoux, A., et al. (2017). Associations between mobility, cognition, and brain structure in healthy older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9, 155. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00155
Desai, R., Liebenthal, E., Possing, E. T., Waldron, E., and Binder, J. R. (2005). Volumetric vs. surface-based alignment for localization of auditory cortex activation. NeuroImage 26, 1019–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.024
Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021
Diedrichsen, J.. (2006). A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 33, 127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056
Diedrichsen, J., Balsters, J. H., Flavell, J., Cussans, E., and Ramnani, N. (2009). A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 46, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045
Dodge, H., Mattek, N., Austin, D., Hayes, T., and Kaye, J. (2012). In-home walking speeds and variability trajectories associated with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 78, 1946–1952. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318259e1de
Doi, T., Makizako, H., Shimada, H., Park, H., Tsutsumimoto, K., Uemura, K., et al. (2013). Brain activation during dual-task walking and executive function among older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a fNIRS study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 25, 539–544. doi: 10.1007/s40520-013-0119-5
Downar, J., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J., and Davis, K. D. (2000). A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 277–283. doi: 10.1038/72991
Dumurgier, J., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., Ahmed, I., Tavernier, B., Grabli, D., et al. (2012). MRI atrophy of the caudate nucleus and slower walking speed in the elderly. Neuroimage 60, 871–878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.102
Elias, L. J., Bryden, M. P., and Bulman-Fleming, M. B. (1998). Footedness is a better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization. Neuropsychologia 36, 37–43. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00107-3
Espy, D. D., Yang, F., Bhatt, T., and Pai, Y.-C. (2010). Independent influence of gait speed and step length on stability and fall risk. Gait Posture 32, 378–382. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.06.013
Fettrow, T., Hupfeld, K., Reimann, H., Choi, J., Hass, C., and Seidler, R. (2021a). Age differences in adaptation of medial-lateral gait parameters during split-belt treadmill walking. Res. Square. 11, 1–17. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-777512/v1
Fettrow, T., Hupfeld, K., Tays, G., Clark, D. J., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Seidler, R. D. (2021b). Brain activity during walking in older adults: Implications for compensatory versus dysfunctional accounts. Neurobiol. Aging 105, 349–364. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.05.015
Field, A., Miles, J., and Field, Z. (2012). Discovering Statistics Using R. London: Sage Publications.
Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Fennema-Notestine, C., McEvoy, L. K., Hagler, D. J., Holland, D., et al. (2009a). One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. J. Neurosci. 29, 15223–15231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-09.2009
Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Espeseth, T., Reinvang, I., Raz, N., et al. (2009b). High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2001–2012. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn232
Gaser, C., and Dahnke, R. (2016). CAT-a computational anatomy toolbox for the analysis of structural MRI data. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016, 336–348. Available online at: https://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/hbm2016/GaserHBM2016.pdf
Gaser, C., and Kurth, F. (2017). Manual Computational Anatomy Toolbox-CAT12. Structural Brain Mapping Group at the Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, University of Jena.
Ghanavati, T., Smitt, M. S., Lord, S. R., Sachdev, P., Wen, W., Kochan, N. A., et al. (2018). Deep white matter hyperintensities, microstructural integrity and dual task walking in older people. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 1488–1496. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9787-7
Gillain, S., Boutaayamou, M., Schwartz, C., Brüls, O., Bruyère, O., Croisier, J.-L., et al. (2019). Using supervised learning machine algorithm to identify future fallers based on gait patterns: a two-year longitudinal study. Exp. Gerontol. 127, 110730. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2019.110730
Godin, G., and Shephard, R. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Can. J. Appl. Sport Sci. 10, 141–146.
Gullett, J. M., O'Shea, A., Lamb, D. G., Porges, E. C., O'Shea, D. M., Pasternak, O., et al. (2020). The association of white matter free water with cognition in older adults. Neuroimage 219, 117040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117040
Guttuso, T., Bergsland, N., Hagemeier, J., Lichter, D. G., Pasternak, O., and Zivadinov, R. (2018). Substantia nigra free water increases longitudinally in Parkinson disease. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 39, 479–484. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A5545
Halliday, D. W., Hundza, S. R., Garcia-Barrera, M. A., Klimstra, M., Commandeur, D., Lukyn, T. V., et al. (2018). Comparing executive function, evoked hemodynamic response, and gait as predictors of variations in mobility for older adults. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 151–160. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1325453
Han, S., An, Y., Carass, A., Prince, J. L., and Resnick, S. M. (2020). Longitudinal analysis of regional cerebellum volumes during normal aging. Neuroimage 220, 117062. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117062
Hillel, I., Gazit, E., Nieuwboer, A., Avanzino, L., Rochester, L., Cereatti, A., et al. (2019). Is every-day walking in older adults more analogous to dual-task walking or to usual walking? elucidating the gaps between gait performance in the lab and during 24/7 monitoring. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 16, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s11556-019-0214-5
Hogstrom, L. J., Westlye, L. T., Walhovd, K. B., and Fjell, A. M. (2013). The structure of the cerebral cortex across adult life: age-related patterns of surface area, thickness, and gyrification. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2521–2530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs231
Hollman, J. H., Kovash, F. M., Kubik, J. J., and Linbo, R. A. (2007). Age-related differences in spatiotemporal markers of gait stability during dual task walking. Gait Posture 26, 113–119. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.08.005
Hollman, J. H., McDade, E. M., and Petersen, R. C. (2011a). Normative spatiotemporal gait parameters in older adults. Gait Posture 34, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.024
Hollman, J. H., Youdas, J. W., and Lanzino, D. J. (2011b). Gender differences in dual task gait performance in older adults. Am. J. Men Health 5, 11–17. doi: 10.1177/1557988309357232
Holtzer, R., Mahoney, J. R., Izzetoglu, M., Izzetoglu, K., Onaral, B., and Verghese, J. (2011). fNIRS study of walking and walking while talking in young and old individuals. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 879–887. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr068
Holtzer, R., Mahoney, J. R., Izzetoglu, M., Wang, C., England, S., and Verghese, J. (2015). Online fronto-cortical control of simple and attention-demanding locomotion in humans. Neuroimage 112, 152–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.002
Howard, M. W., Fotedar, M. S., Datey, A. V., and Hasselmo, M. E. (2005). The temporal context model in spatial navigation and relational learning: toward a common explanation of medial temporal lobe function across domains. Psychol. Rev. 112, 75. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.75
Hua, K., Zhang, J., Wakana, S., Jiang, H., Li, X., Reich, D. S., et al. (2008). Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tract-specific quantification. Neuroimage 39, 336–347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.053
Hupfeld, K. E., Hyatt, H. W., Alvarez Jerez, P., Mikkelsen, M., Hass, C. J., Edden, R. A., et al. (2021a). In vivo brain glutathione is higher in older age and correlates with mobility. Cereb. Cortex 31, 4576–4594. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab107
Hupfeld, K. E., McGregor, H., Koppelmans, V., Beltran, N., Kofman, I., De Dios, Y., et al. (2021b). Brain and behavioral evidence for reweighting of vestibular inputs with long-duration spaceflight. Cereb. Cortex 239, bhab239. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab239
Hutton, C., Draganski, B., Ashburner, J., and Weiskopf, N. (2009). A comparison between voxel-based cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging. Neuroimage 48, 371–380. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.043
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., Martin, A., Schouten, J. L., and Haxby, J. V. (1999). Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 9379–9384. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.16.9379
Jang, S. H., and Seo, J. P. (2015). Aging of corticospinal tract fibers according to the cerebral origin in the human brain: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 585, 77–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.11.030
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Woolrich, M. W., and Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015
Jin, K., Zhang, T., Shaw, M., Sachdev, P., and Cherbuin, N. (2018). Relationship between sulcal characteristics and brain aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10, 339. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00339
Johansson, J., Nordström, A., and Nordström, P. (2016). Greater fall risk in elderly women than in men is associated with increased gait variability during multitasking. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 17, 535–540. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2016.02.009
Kelly, V. E., Janke, A. A., and Shumway-Cook, A. (2010). Effects of instructed focus and task difficulty on concurrent walking and cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. Exp. Brain Res. 207, 65–73. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2429-6
Knapstad, M. K., Steihaug, O. M., Aaslund, M. K., Nakling, A., Naterstad, I. F., Fladby, T., et al. (2019). Reduced walking speed in subjective and mild cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 42, E122–E128. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000157
Koenraadt, K. L., Roelofsen, E. G., Duysens, J., and Keijsers, N. L. (2014). Cortical control of normal gait and precision stepping: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage 85, 415–422. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.070
Koppelmans, V., Hoogendam, Y. Y., Hirsiger, S., Mérillat, S., Jäncke, L., and Seidler, R. D. (2017). Regional cerebellar volumetric correlates of manual motor and cognitive function. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 1929–1944. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1317-7
Kraus, L.. (2016). 2015 disability statistics annual report. a publication of the rehabilitation research and training center on disability statistics and demographics. Institute on Disability, University of New Hampshire.
Lamballais, S., Vinke, E. J., Vernooij, M. W., Ikram, M. A., and Muetzel, R. L. (2020). Cortical gyrification in relation to age and cognition in older adults. Neuroimage 212, 116637. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116637
Leemans, A., Jeurissen, B., Sijbers, J., and Jones, D. (2009). ExploreDTI: a graphical toolbox for processing, analyzing, and visualizing diffusion MR data. Proc. Int. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Sci. 17:3537.
Lemaitre, H., Goldman, A. L., Sambataro, F., Verchinski, B. A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D. R., et al. (2012). Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume? Neurobiol. Aging 33, 617–e1. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013
Li, C., Verghese, J., and Holtzer, R. (2014). A comparison of two walking while talking paradigms in aging. Gait Posture 40, 415–419. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.05.062
Lucas, M., Wagshul, M. E., Izzetoglu, M., and Holtzer, R. (2019). Moderating effect of white matter integrity on brain activation during dual-task walking in older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 74, 435–441. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly131
Luders, E., Thompson, P. M., Narr, K., Toga, A. W., Jancke, L., and Gaser, C. (2006). A curvature-based approach to estimate local gyrification on the cortical surface. Neuroimage 29, 1224–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.049
Lundin-Olsson, L., Nyberg, L., and Gustafson, Y. (1997). Stops walking when talking as a predictor of falls in elderly people. Lancet 349, 617. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)24009-2
MacAulay, R. K., Brouillette, R. M., Foil, H. C., Bruce-Keller, A. J., and Keller, J. N. (2014). A longitudinal study on dual-tasking effects on gait: cognitive change predicts gait variance in the elderly. PLoS ONE 9, e99436. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099436
Madan, C. R.. (2021). Age-related decrements in cortical gyrification: Evidence from an accelerated longitudinal dataset. European J. Neurosci. 53, 1661–1671. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15039
Madan, C. R., and Kensinger, E. A. (2016). Cortical complexity as a measure of age-related brain atrophy. Neuroimage 134, 617–629. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.029
Madan, C. R., and Kensinger, E. A. (2018). Predicting age from cortical structure across the lifespan. European J. Neurosci. 47, 399–416. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13835
Madden, D. J., Bennett, I. J., Burzynska, A., Potter, G. G., Chen, N.-K., and Song, A. W. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging of cerebral white matter integrity in cognitive aging. Biochim. Biophys. Acta Mol. Basis Dis. 1822, 386–400. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.08.003
Maidan, I., Mirelman, A., Hausdorff, J. M., Stern, Y., and Habeck, C. G. (2021). Distinct cortical thickness patterns link disparate cerebral cortex regions to select mobility domains. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-85058-z
Maillard, P., Fletcher, E., Singh, B., Martinez, O., Johnson, D. K., Olichney, J. M., et al. (2019). Cerebral white matter free water: a sensitive biomarker of cognition and function. Neurology 92, e2221–e2231. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007449
Malcolm, B. R., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., and De Sanctis, P. (2015). The aging brain shows less flexible reallocation of cognitive resources during dual-task walking: a mobile brain/body imaging (MoBI) study. Neuroimage 117, 230–242. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.028
Meier-Ruge, W., Ulrich, J., Brühlmann, M., and Meier, E. (1992). Age-related white matter atrophy in the human brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad Sci. 673, 260–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb27462.x
Mesulam, M.. (1998). From sensation to perception. Brain 121, 1013–1052. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013
Miller, K. L., Alfaro-Almagro, F., Bangerter, N. K., Thomas, D. L., Yacoub, E., Xu, J., et al. (2016). Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1523–1536. doi: 10.1038/nn.4393
Mirelman, A., Maidan, I., Bernad-Elazari, H., Shustack, S., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2017). Effects of aging on prefrontal brain activation during challenging walking conditions. Brain Cogn. 115, 41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.04.002
Miyai, I., Tanabe, H. C., Sase, I., Eda, H., Oda, I., Konishi, I., et al. (2001). Cortical mapping of gait in humans: a near-infrared spectroscopic topography study. Neuroimage 14, 1186–1192. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0905
Miyashita, Y.. (1993). Inferior temporal cortex: where visual perception meets memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 245–263. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.001333
Moe-Nilssen, R., Aaslund, M. K., Hodt-Billington, C., and Helbostad, J. L. (2010). Gait variability measures may represent different constructs. Gait Posture 32, 98–101. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.03.019
Montero-Odasso, M., Verghese, J., Beauchet, O., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2012). Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 2127–2136. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04209.x
Montero-Odasso, M. M., Sarquis-Adamson, Y., Speechley, M., Borrie, M. J., Hachinski, V. C., Wells, J., et al. (2017). Association of dual-task gait with incident dementia in mild cognitive impairment: results from the gait and brain study. JAMA Neurol. 74, 857–865. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.0643
Ofori, E., Pasternak, O., Planetta, P. J., Li, H., Burciu, R. G., Snyder, A. F., et al. (2015). Longitudinal changes in free-water within the substantia nigra of Parkinson's disease. Brain 138, 2322–2331. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv136
Oldfield, R. C.. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Papegaaij, S., Taube, W., Baudry, S., Otten, E., and Hortobágyi, T. (2014). Aging causes a reorganization of cortical and spinal control of posture. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6, 28. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00028
Pasternak, O., Sochen, N., Gur, Y., Intrator, N., and Assaf, Y. (2009). Free water elimination and mapping from diffusion MRI. Magn. Reson. Imaging 62, 717–730. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22055
Patel, P., Lamar, M., and Bhatt, T. (2014). Effect of type of cognitive task and walking speed on cognitive-motor interference during dual-task walking. Neuroscience 260, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.016
Petersen, T. H., Willerslev-Olsen, M., Conway, B. A., and Nielsen, J. B. (2012). The motor cortex drives the muscles during walking in human subjects. J. Physiol. 590, 2443–2452. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.227397
Pettersson, A. F., Olsson, E., and Wahlund, L.-O. (2007). Effect of divided attention on gait in subjects with and without cognitive impairment. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 20, 58–62. doi: 10.1177/0891988706293528
Piccinelli, M.. (1998). Alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT). Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 7, 70–73. doi: 10.1017/S1121189X00007144
Pierpaoli, C., Barnett, A., Pajevic, S., Chen, R., Penix, L., Virta, A., et al. (2001). Water diffusion changes in wallerian degeneration and their dependence on white matter architecture. Neuroimage 13, 1174–1185. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0765
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., and Team, R. C. (2007). Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R 3, 1–89.
Powell, L. E., and Myers, A. M. (1995). The activities-specific balance confidence (ABC) scale. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 50, M28–M34. doi: 10.1093/gerona/50A.1.M28
Quach, L., Galica, A. M., Jones, R. N., Procter-Gray, E., Manor, B., Hannan, M. T., et al. (2011). The nonlinear relationship between gait speed and falls: the maintenance of balance, independent living, intellect, and zest in the elderly of boston study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59, 1069–1073. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03408.x
R Core Team, X.. (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Core Team.
Raz, N., Ghisletta, P., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., and Lindenberger, U. (2010). Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: regional and individual differences. Neuroimage 51, 501–511. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.020
Rettmann, M. E., Kraut, M. A., Prince, J. L., and Resnick, S. M. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses of anatomical sulcal changes associated with aging. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1584–1594. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj095
Rodríguez-Herreros, B., Amengual, J. L., Gurtubay-Antolín, A., Richter, L., Jauer, P., Erdmann, C., et al. (2015). Microstructure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus predicts stimulation-induced interference with on-line motor control. Neuroimage 120, 254–265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.070
Romero, J. E., Coupé, P., Giraud, R., Ta, V.-T., Fonov, V., Park, M. T. M., et al. (2017). CERES: a new cerebellum lobule segmentation method. Neuroimage 147, 916–924. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.003
Rosano, C., Aizenstein, H. J., Studenski, S., and Newman, A. B. (2007). A regions-of-interest volumetric analysis of mobility limitations in community-dwelling older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 62, 1048–1055. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.9.1048
Rosenthal, R., Cooper, H., and Hedges, L. (1994). Parametric measures of effect size. Handbook Res. Synth. 621, 231–244.
Ross, D., Wagshul, M. E., Izzetoglu, M., and Holtzer, R. (2021). Prefrontal cortex activation during dual-task walking in older adults is moderated by thickness of several cortical regions. Geroscience 43, 1959–1974. doi: 10.1007/s11357-021-00379-1
Ruigrok, A. N., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Lai, M.-C., Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M. V., Tait, R. J., et al. (2014). A meta-analysis of sex differences in human brain structure. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 39, 34–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.004
Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S., Busa, E., et al. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb. Cortex 14, 721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032
Salazar, A. P., Hupfeld, K. E., Lee, J. K., Banker, L. A., Tays, G. D., Beltran, N. E., et al. (2021). Visuomotor adaptation brain changes during a spaceflight analog with elevated carbon dioxide (co2): a pilot study. Front. Neural Circ. 15, 51. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.659557
Salazar, A. P., Hupfeld, K. E., Lee, J. K., Beltran, N. E., Kofman, I. S., De Dios, Y. E., et al. (2020). Neural working memory changes during a spaceflight analog with elevated carbon dioxide: a pilot study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 14, 48. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.00048
Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., De la Fuente, J. R., and Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (audit): who collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-ii. Addiction 88, 791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x
Sexton, C. E., Walhovd, K. B., Storsve, A. B., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2014). Accelerated changes in white matter microstructure during aging: a longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging study. J. Neurosci. 34, 15425–15436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0203-14.2014
Shinoura, N., Suzuki, Y., Yamada, R., Tabei, Y., Saito, K., and Yagi, K. (2009). Damage to the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe plays a role in spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 47, 2600–2603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.010
Smith, E., Cusack, T., and Blake, C. (2016). The effect of a dual task on gait speed in community dwelling older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Gait Posture 44, 250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.017
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31, 1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051
Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061
Song, S.-K., Sun, S.-W., Ju, W.-K., Lin, S.-J., Cross, A. H., and Neufeld, A. H. (2003). Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia. Neuroimage 20, 1714–1722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.005
Song, S.-K., Sun, S.-W., Ramsbottom, M. J., Chang, C., Russell, J., and Cross, A. H. (2002). Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage 17, 1429–1436. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1267
Song, S.-K., Yoshino, J., Le, T. Q., Lin, S.-J., Sun, S.-W., Cross, A. H., et al. (2005). Demyelination increases radial diffusivity in corpus callosum of mouse brain. Neuroimage 26, 132–140. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.028
Spena, G., Gatignol, P., Capelle, L., and Duffau, H. (2006). Superior longitudinal fasciculus subserves vestibular network in humans. Neuroreport. 17, 1403–1406. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000223385.49919.61
Springer, S., Giladi, N., Peretz, C., Yogev, G., Simon, E. S., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2006). Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: The role of aging, falls, and executive function. Mov. Disord., 21, 950–957. doi: 10.1002/mds.20848
Steffener, J., and Stern, Y. (2012). Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve in aging. Biochim. Biophys. Acta Mol. Basis Dis. 1822, 467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.09.012
Storsve, A. B., Fjell, A. M., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., Overbye, K., Aasland, H. W., et al. (2014). Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. J. Neurosci. 34, 8488–8498. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0391-14.2014
Takakusaki, K.. (2017). Functional neuroanatomy for posture and gait control. J. Mov. Disord. 10, 1. doi: 10.14802/jmd.16062
Taubert, M., Roggenhofer, E., Melie-Garcia, L., Muller, S., Lehmann, N., Preisig, M., et al. (2020). Converging patterns of aging-associated brain volume loss and tissue microstructure differences. Neurobiol. Aging 88, 108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.01.006
Thambisetty, M., Wan, J., Carass, A., An, Y., Prince, J. L., and Resnick, S. M. (2010). Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness associated with normal aging. Neuroimage 52, 1215–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.258
Tian, Q., Chastan, N., Bair, W.-N., Resnick, S. M., Ferrucci, L., and Studenski, S. A. (2017). The brain map of gait variability in aging, cognitive impairment and dementia–a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 149–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.020
Tian, Q., Ferrucci, L., Resnick, S. M., Simonsick, E. M., Shardell, M. D., Landman, B. A., et al. (2016). The effect of age and microstructural white matter integrity on lap time variation and fast-paced walking speed. Brain Imaging Behav. 10, 697–706. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9449-6
Tinetti, M. E., Richman, D., and Powell, L. (1990). Falls efficacy as a measure of fear of falling. J. Gerontol. 45, P239–P243. doi: 10.1093/geronj/45.6.P239
Tripathi, S., Verghese, J., and Blumen, H. M. (2019). Gray matter volume covariance networks associated with dual-task cost during walking-while-talking. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 2229–2240. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24520
Tuch, D. S., Reese, T. G., Wiegell, M. R., and Wedeen, V. J. (2003). Diffusion MRI of complex neural architecture. Neuron 40, 885–895. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00758-X
Tullo, S., Devenyi, G. A., Patel, R., Park, M. T. M., Collins, D. L., and Chakravarty, M. M. (2018). Warping an atlas derived from serial histology to 5 high-resolution MRIs. Sci. Data 5, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2018.107
Van Impe, A., Coxon, J. P., Goble, D. J., Wenderoth, N., and Swinnen, S. P. (2011). Age-related changes in brain activation underlying single-and dual-task performance: visuomanual drawing and mental arithmetic. Neuropsychologia 49, 2400–2409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.016
van Velsen, E. F., Vernooij, M. W., Vrooman, H. A., van der Lugt, A., Breteler, M. M., Hofman, A., et al. (2013). Brain cortical thickness in the general elderly population: the Rotterdam Scan Study. Neurosci. Lett. 550, 189–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.06.063
Venables, W. N., and Ripley, B. D. (1999). Modern Applied Statistics With s-Plus. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Verghese, J., Holtzer, R., Lipton, R. B., and Wang, C. (2012). Mobility stress test approach to predicting frailty, disability, and mortality in high-functioning older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 1901–1905. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04145.x
Verghese, J., Kuslansky, G., Holtzer, R., Katz, M., Xue, X., Buschke, H., et al. (2007). Walking while talking: effect of task prioritization in the elderly. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.10.007
Verghese, J., Wang, C., Ayers, E., Izzetoglu, M., and Holtzer, R. (2017). Brain activation in high-functioning older adults and falls: prospective cohort study. Neurology 88, 191–197. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003421
Verhaeghen, P., Steitz, D. W., Sliwinski, M. J., and Cerella, J. (2003). Aging and dual-task performance: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 18, 443. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.443
Verlinden, V. J., De Groot, M., Cremers, L. G., van der Geest, J. N., Hofman, A., Niessen, W. J., et al. (2016). Tract-specific white matter microstructure and gait in humans. Neurobiol. Aging 43, 164–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.04.005
Vos, S. B., Tax, C. M., Luijten, P. R., Ourselin, S., Leemans, A., and Froeling, M. (2017). The importance of correcting for signal drift in diffusion MRI. Magn. Reson. Imaging 77, 285–299. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26124
Wagshul, M. E., Lucas, M., Ye, K., Izzetoglu, M., and Holtzer, R. (2019). Multi-modal neuroimaging of dual-task walking: Structural MRI and fNIRS analysis reveals prefrontal grey matter volume moderation of brain activation in older adults. Neuroimage 189, 745–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.045
Wakana, S., Caprihan, A., Panzenboeck, M. M., Fallon, J. H., Perry, M., Gollub, R. L., et al. (2007). Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter. Neuroimage 36, 630–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.049
Washabaugh, E. P., Kalyanaraman, T., Adamczyk, P. G., Claflin, E. S., and Krishnan, C. (2017). Validity and repeatability of inertial measurement units for measuring gait parameters. Gait Posture 55, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.04.013
Wierenga, L. M., Doucet, G. E., Dima, D., Agartz, I., Aghajani, M., Akudjedu, T. N., et al. (2020). Greater male than female variability in regional brain structure across the lifespan. Hum Brain Mapp. 43, 470–499. doi: 10.1101/2020.02.17.952010
Wilson, J., Allcock, L., Mc Ardle, R., Taylor, J.-P., and Rochester, L. (2019). The neural correlates of discrete gait characteristics in ageing: a structured review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 100, 344–369. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.017
Winkler, A. M., Kochunov, P., Blangero, J., Almasy, L., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., et al. (2010). Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? the importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage 53, 1135–1146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.028
Yang, J., Archer, D. B., Burciu, R. G., Müller, M. L., Roy, A., Ofori, E., et al. (2019). Multimodal dopaminergic and free-water imaging in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Related Dis. 62, 10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.01.007
Yogev-Seligmann, G., Hausdorff, J. M., and Giladi, N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Disord. 23, 329–342. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720
Yogev-Seligmann, G., Rotem-Galili, Y., Mirelman, A., Dickstein, R., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2010). How does explicit prioritization alter walking during dual-task performance? effects of age and sex on gait speed and variability. Phys. Ther. 90, 177–186. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090043
Yotter, R. A., Dahnke, R., Thompson, P. M., and Gaser, C. (2011a). Topological correction of brain surface meshes using spherical harmonics. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1109–1124. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21095
Yotter, R. A., Nenadic, I., Ziegler, G., Thompson, P. M., and Gaser, C. (2011b). Local cortical surface complexity maps from spherical harmonic reconstructions. Neuroimage 56, 961–973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.007
Yun, H. J., Im, K., Yang, J.-J., Yoon, U., and Lee, J.-M. (2013). Automated sulcal depth measurement on cortical surface reflecting geometrical properties of sulci. PLoS ONE 8, e55977. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055977
Yushkevich, P. A., Pluta, J. B., Wang, H., Xie, L., Ding, S.-L., Gertje, E. C., et al. (2015). Automated volumetry and regional thickness analysis of hippocampal subfields and medial temporal cortical structures in mild cognitive impairment. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 258–287. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22627
Keywords: aging, dual task walking, dual task cost (DTcost), gray matter volume, cortical thickness, sulcal depth, ventricular volume, free water
Citation: Hupfeld KE, Geraghty JM, McGregor HR, Hass CJ, Pasternak O and Seidler RD (2022) Differential Relationships Between Brain Structure and Dual Task Walking in Young and Older Adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:809281. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.809281
Received: 04 November 2021; Accepted: 31 January 2022;
Published: 11 March 2022.
Edited by:
Hakuei Fujiyama, Murdoch University, AustraliaReviewed by:
Emad Al-Yahya, University of Nottingham, United KingdomWoei-Nan Bair, University of the Sciences, United States
Copyright © 2022 Hupfeld, Geraghty, McGregor, Hass, Pasternak and Seidler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Rachael D. Seidler, rachaelseidler@ufl.edu
†Present Address: Justin M. Geraghty, School of Medicine, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, United States