Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 24 December 2021
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Behavioral Models of Human Cognitive Diseases with Focus on Validity, Reproducibility, and Statistics View all 7 articles

Dual-Task Gait as a Predictive Tool for Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults: A Systematic Review

  • 1Programa Magíster en Kinesiología Gerontológica, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Mayor, Santiago, Chile
  • 2Escuela de Kinesiología, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de Las Américas, Santiago, Chile
  • 3Centro de Estudio del Movimiento Humano, Escuela de Kinesiología, Facultad de Odontología y Salud, Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile
  • 4Unidad de Cerebro Saludable, Hospital Clínico Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile

The use of the dual-task model as a cognitive-motor interface has been extensively investigated in cross-sectional studies as a training task in cognitive impairment. However, few existing longitudinal studies prove the usefulness of this tool as a clinical marker of cognitive impairment in older people. What is the evidence in prospective studies about dual-task gait as a predictor of cognitive impairment in older adults? This study aims to review and discuss the current state of knowledge in prospective studies on the use of dual-task gait as a predictive tool for cognitive impairment in older adults. The methodology used was a systematic review, according to the PRISMA criteria for the search, summarize and report. A search in 3 databases (Pubmed, Web of Science, and Scopus) was carried out until April 2021. The search terms used were: “(gait OR walking) AND (cognitive decline) AND (dual-task) AND (follow-up OR longitudinal OR long-term OR prospective OR cohort OR predict).” We included prospective research articles with older people with cognitive evaluation at the beginning and the end of the follow-up and dual-task gait paradigm as initial evaluation associated with the presentation of cognitive impairment prediction using any dual-task gait parameters. After exclusion criteria, 12 studies were reviewed. The results indicate that eight studies consider dual-task gait parameters a useful cognitive-motor tool, finding that some of the evaluated parameters of dual-task gait significantly correlate with cognitive impairment over time. The most promising DT parameters associated with cognitive impairment prediction seem to be gait speed, speed cost, DT time, numbers of words during DT, among others. In sum, this study reviews the variety of dual-task gait parameters and their relevance as a simple tool for early cognitive impairment screening, opening a diagnostic window for the screening of cognitive impairment in older people.

Introduction

World population aging brings up several challenges, including an increase in cognitive impairment and dementia cases. There are 10 million new cases of dementia every year worldwide (WHO, 2020), and this number will double in 20 years (Ferri et al., 2005). The prevalence of dementia is rapidly increasing in middle and low-income countries (Prince et al., 2013). In Chile, 1% of the population has dementia, and it is projected to triple by 2050 (Ministerio de Salud, 2010, 2017). Cognitive impairment and dementia are a priority in public health planning programs considering their high economic and social cost (Alzheimer's Research UK, 2017). It is associated with a high burden on formal and informal caregivers (Brodaty et al., 2013). In Chile, this economic and social cost is carried mainly by female relatives, diminishing their job opportunities (Hojman et al., 2017). Considering the progression from cognitive impairment to dementia, it is crucial to identify risk factors for cognitive impairment for early prevention, proper management, and optimizing the quality of life of the patient and family (Organización Panamericana de la Salud, 2020). Early stages include mild cognitive impairment (MCI), with a five-fold higher risk of developing dementia (Petersen, 2011). MCI can be diagnosed by neuropsychological tests such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975) or the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005). However, its evolution is difficult to predict because of the variety in the clinical manifestations and the speed of the cognitive functions change and decline progression (Storandt et al., 2002). For this reason, screening tools for early stages of cognitive impairment assessment are critical.

Cognitive impairment diagnosis is fundamentally clinical. Complementary tests to predict the risk among life courses are also relevant (Livingston et al., 2020). They include medical history evaluation, blood tests, neuropsychological tests, brain imaging, cerebrospinal fluid analysis, positron emission tomography (PET), and biomarkers as indexes for health (US National Library of Medicine, 2016). For diagnosis and progression, neuropathological biomarkers include amyloid β1-42 (inversely related to amyloid burden in the brain), total tau (for neuronal degeneration), and phospho-tau (for the density of neurofibrillary tangles) (Bayer, 2018). Nevertheless, these evaluations are expensive, time-consuming, invasive, and not always available, making it even more complex to establish a diagnosis and prognosis of cognitive impairment (Tolonen et al., 2018). Therefore, alternative clinical tools are required to help to identify early stages of cognitive impairment (Laske et al., 2014).

In the search for new screening tools for cognitive impairment, non-cognitive markers have gained relevance (Montero-Odasso et al., 2020). One is the simultaneously cognitive and motor performance, named dual-task (DT). DT is the simultaneous performance of these two tasks. The most common DT paradigm is the use of a cognitive task that involves executive functions, plus a motor task that challenges the performance of the first task (Petrigna et al., 2019), such as gait. This assessment considers shared neural networks between movement and cognition (Waite et al., 2005; Aggarwal et al., 2006; Liu-Ambrose et al., 2008; Verghese et al., 2008; Buchman and Bennett, 2011; Mielke et al., 2013; Beauchet et al., 2016). Indeed, cerebral image-based evidence shows functional and structural correlates between motor control and cognition, such as prefrontal and temporal brain regions activated during motion (Scherder et al., 2007; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012b; Rosano et al., 2012; Rosso et al., 2013). Also, cognitive functions such as attention, memory, and executive function are necessary during walking (Camicioli et al., 1997; Kluger et al., 1997; Sheridan et al., 2003; Al-Yahya et al., 2011). Furthermore, hippocampal activity is critical during spatial memory, and it is necessary for visual, somatosensory, and vestibular integration during spatial orientation in gait (Scherder et al., 2007). Moreover, it has been observed that MCI and decreased gait speed share a decrease in hippocampal volume (Rosso et al., 2017).

In older people, neuroimaging studies have shown greater use of frontal-subcortical circuits during gait (Malouin et al., 2003; Al-Yahya et al., 2011). When using a dual-task gait paradigm, older people show higher activation of this area than younger people (Ohsugi et al., 2013). Evidence of the use of motor parameters for the MCI screening tool has positioned gait speed as a good marker (Quan et al., 2017). Particularly, MCI accompanied by slow gait speed is a more significant risk factor for dementia than slow gait speed or cognitive impairment alone (Doi et al., 2018). This evidence supports the increased diagnostic value of dual-task assessment over the evaluation of motor and cognitive capacities alone.

There are several models of dual-task for assessment and treatment. Gait as a motor task using usual pace gait (UPG) in straight-line or the Timed Up and Go test (TUG), plus naming animals or counting backward as the cognitive task, are examples of dual-task gait (Leone et al., 2020). This ability to stress the cognitive and motor system makes the dual-task an ideal tool to reflect the motor-cognitive interface (Camicioli et al., 1997; Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Albers et al., 2016; Macaulay et al., 2017; Smith et al., 2017).

Furthermore, considering early changes in functional performance activities of daily living (ADL) in MCI (Burton et al., 2009), it has even been observed that gait parameters can also be observed before cognitive changes (Mielke et al., 2013; Best et al., 2016). Conventional functional assessment tests would not be sensitive to detect these changes (Gillain et al., 2009). On the other hand, assessing gait alterations can predict cognitive functions (Scherder et al., 2007). This idea is supported by the presence of gait disorders associated with hippocampal degeneration and the nigrostriatal system (Scherder et al., 2007) and due to the motor impairment that accompanies cognitive impairment before the development of dementia (Montero-odasso et al., 2018). Indeed, a brain with less functional reserve is more exposed to overload due to the activation of additional areas during gait (Scherder et al., 2007; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012b; Montero-odasso et al., 2014), especially by making this task more complex with an additional task (Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2017).

Previous studies have used gait speed (GS) as the main motor parameter of dual-task gait. These studies consider the significant relationship between increased stride variability and cognitive impairment (Laske et al., 2014). Other DT parameters associated with cognitive impairment prediction include instrumental biomechanical variables such as step symmetry, speed cost, swing time, stride time, among others.

Increasing evidence supports the use of dual-task gait as a tool to discriminate the progression between different levels of cognitive impairment (Laske et al., 2014; Montero-odasso et al., 2014; Macaulay et al., 2017; Åhman et al., 2020b; Latorre et al., 2020). It has been proposed as a predictive assessment of the progression from MCI to dementia (Laske et al., 2014; Bahureksa et al., 2017; Chiaramonte and Cioni, 2021). The decrease in gait speed while dual-task gait is associated with deficits in executive, attention, and memory processes, as well as the progression from a healthy state or MCI to dementia (Camicioli et al., 1997; Bootsma-van der Wiel et al., 2003; Hausdorff et al., 2005; Montero-odasso et al., 2009; Chiaramonte and Cioni, 2021).

It has also been reported that gait speed in the dual-task gait paradigm could be more valuable than gait speed in a simple task (only gait) to discriminate between healthy people and those with MCI (Macaulay et al., 2017), explained by an activation overlap of brain areas that interfere with the execution of the double task (Bürki et al., 2017). Furthermore, dual-task is most useful in older people since walking alone can be complex (Hausdorff et al., 2005).

All these antecedents taken together make dual-task gait a promising tool for diagnosing cognitive impairment. However, few longitudinal studies test a dual-task gait paradigm to predict cognitive impairment and dementia in older people. What is the evidence in prospective studies about dual-task gait as a predictor of cognitive impairment in older adults? This study aims to review and discuss the current stage of knowledge on prospective studies of the dual-task gait model as a predictor of cognitive impairment in older people.

Methods

The strategy for developing this systematic review followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) protocol (Moher et al., 2009) for the search, summarize, and report (Page et al., 2021). We defined the research question regarding populations, interventions, comparators, outcomes, and study designs (PICOS) as follows: What is the evidence in prospective studies about dual-task gait as a predictor of cognitive impairment in older adults? Is dual-task gait able to predict cognitive impairment in older adults? For addressing this question, during March 14, 2021, a pilot search strategy was carried out in the Scopus, Web of Science, and PubMed databases using the following search terms: (gait) AND (cognitive dysfunction) AND (dual-task) AND (follow-up OR longitudinal OR long-term OR prospective OR cohort OR predict) AND (2012–2021). It showed 291 possible candidate studies.

For a more comprehensive review, we eliminated time restrictions. We performed a final search during April 20, 2021, considering medical topic headings (MeSH) and search words detailed in Table 1: “(gait OR walking) AND (cognitive decline) AND (dual-task) AND (follow-up OR longitudinal OR long-term OR prospective OR cohort OR predict).” It showed 1,607 records screened. We included research articles written in English and Spanish with a prospective design which sample were older people (60 years and over as stated by World Health Organization) able to perform the required test independently. For a comprehensive analysis of outcomes, we considered the use of cognitive evaluation at the beginning and the end of the follow-up (dependent variable) and dual-task gait paradigm as initial evaluation (independent variable) associated with the presentation of cognitive impairment prediction using any dual-task gait parameters. Cognitive decline was defined as worsening performance in the neuropsychological assessment over time. Cognitive impairment was considered a stage (from mild to moderate) between normal aging cognitive decline and a pathologic cognitive decline. It excluded severe cognitive impairment status or major neurocognitive disorder (dementia) at baseline since this study aims to predict cognitive impairment progression. We excluded studies with subjects with chronic pathologies that markedly affect mobility because it would limit them from performing the required motor test based on literature (e.g., Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease, multiple sclerosis, cerebrovascular accident). We also excluded case studies and research in an animal model. This research included only original articles with a prospective design for older people to address the research question. After applying inclusion and exclusion criteria and manually removing duplicated articles, 12 articles were selected for this review (Figure 1). To improve the completeness of reporting, organization of results, synthesis of the findings, and review's utility, we consulted the PRISMA reporting checklist and abstract checklist (Page et al., 2021).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Search terms according to PICOS.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Article selection process. This figure compiles the selected studies according to PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for systematic reviews. The boxes provide the selection process according to search methods (see text). There were 1,635 initial records identified from databases with 27 duplicates. After title screening based on inclusion/exclusion criteria and duplicates, 114 reports were sought for retrieval. After assessing eligibility, 12 studies were included in this review (Page et al., 2021).

Quality Assessment

Two researchers performed the discrimination and selection of studies. They independently reviewed abstracts and registered findings into a database. Then hand-searched and selected full relevant articles and documents for data extraction using the abovementioned preset criteria. There was agreement in the selected studies. Discrepancies were solved through discussion with a third reviewer. The contributions of both authors are described in the author's contributions section.

To assess the risk of bias assessment of the studies included, we included Figure 2 based on Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale for assessing the risk of bias of longitudinal studies (Wells et al., 2000).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Bias assessment. Diagram presenting the risk of bias assessment of the studies included according to the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale (NOS). The number of stars indicates the quality of the reviewed article: Good quality: 3 or 4 stars in selection domain AND 1 or 2 stars in comparability domain AND 2 or 3 stars in outcome/exposure domain; Fair quality: 2 stars in selection domain AND 1 or 2 stars in comparability domain AND 2 or 3 stars in outcome/exposure domain; Poor quality: 0 or 1 star in selection domain OR 0 stars in comparability domain OR 0 or 1 stars in outcome/exposure domain (Wells et al., 2000).

Results

This review includes 12 studies (Table 2). Characteristics include a follow-up time from a minimum of 1 year and a maximum of 9 years (average 3.54 years). The sample size included a minimum of 13 (pilot study) and a maximum of 2,250 subjects. The participants were older people aged 60 years and older (average age 71.4) who did not require assistance to perform motor or cognitive tests. The initial cognitive state of the participants included: 25.0% (3 studies) only “with MCI” (Gillain et al., 2016; Montero-Odasso et al., 2017; Åhman et al., 2020a); 25.0% (3 studies) “without dementia” (Beauchet et al., 2017; Ceïde et al., 2018; Donoghue et al., 2018); 16.7% (2 studies) “without cognitive impairment” (Rosso et al., 2019; Osuka et al., 2020a); and 33.3% (4 studies) “cognitively healthy” (CH) people and with “some degree of cognitive impairment” (Deshpande et al., 2009; Nielsen et al., 2018; De Cock et al., 2019; Muurling et al., 2020).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Main results of reviewed articles.

According to 66.7% of the reviewed articles (8 studies), the dual-task (DT) paradigm has predictive value for cognitive impairment (Gillain et al., 2016; Beauchet et al., 2017; Montero-Odasso et al., 2017; Ceïde et al., 2018; De Cock et al., 2019; Rosso et al., 2019; Åhman et al., 2020a; Osuka et al., 2020a). Contrariwise, 30.7% (4 studies) did not find a significant relationship between the DT variables evaluated and the appearance of cognitive impairment (Deshpande et al., 2009; Donoghue et al., 2018; Nielsen et al., 2018; Muurling et al., 2020).

The DT parameters measured in the studies that analyzed gait as a motor task were: gait speed, gait speed cost, cadence, cadence cost, steps/meter, steps/meter cost, step, variability of step length, step width, step width cost, variability of step width, variability of step width cost, swing percentage, stance percentage, stance time, stance time variability, swing time, swing time variability, swing time variability cost, cycle time variability, cycle time variability cost, step time, stride time, stride time variability, the mean value in usual gait stride time, coefficient of variability (CoV) in usual gait stride time, stride time mean value, delta stride time mean value, delta stride time CoV, regularity, and symmetry between steps and gait speed (GS). The main results in each analyzed study, including a significative association between DT gait parameters, are summarized in Table 2.

GS was the most used variable in DT models that used the usual gait as a motor task. Beauchet et al. (2017) used stride time only. However, GS was not always associated with the appearance of cognitive impairment (Deshpande et al., 2009; Donoghue et al., 2018; Muurling et al., 2020). Significant associations with cognitive impairment over time were found considering other DT motor variables, such as coefficient of variability (CoV) stride time, delta CoV stride time, variability, swing time, stride width, steps per meter, time variability of the gait cycle, step width cost, step symmetry, and speed cost as relevant gait variables.

Considering unconventional motor tasks, Muurling et al. (2020) used a gait test consisting of getting up from a chair, walking 5 meters as quickly and comfortably as possible, turning a cone, turning the chair, back to the cone, and sitting, forming an 8. This test is similar to the TUG and simple gait tests. In contrast, Osuka et al. (2020a) used a motor test created by their team, consisting of a 1 × 1 m2 with 16 squares inside, numbered from 1 to 16, where the evaluated subject is asked to step on the numbers consecutively (Osuka et al., 2020b). Comparing both tests, Murling measured gait parameters, while Osuka evaluated task execution time.

The cognitive tasks of the DT paradigm considered primarily three capabilities: working memory (reciting the months of the year backward, subtraction, reciting the alphabet by alternating letters, counting backward, and numbers counted forward during DT gait), verbal fluency function (naming animals, reciting names, and the number of words during DT gait), attention and visuospatial ability (Trail making test and visuospatial task). It is worth mentioning that Åhman et al. (2020b), who used a verbal fluency test as a cognitive task, measured the number of months named during performance time × 10 and the number of animals named during performance time × 10. This test achieved a strong association with cognitive impairment for the total population (OR = 4.06, 95% CI 2.28–7.23, p < 0.001) and for people under 72 years of age (OR = 20.9, 95% CI 3.29 −133.13, p = 0.001). This result was not obtained with other variables. Also, it is worth mentioning that the study by Nielsen et al. (2018) that only used time (OR = 0.222, 95% CI 0.045–1.094) and cost (OR = 0.682, 95% CI 0.122–3.825) found no significant relationship between its variables and cognitive impairment over time.

TUG was used as the motor task in 2 studies (Nielsen et al., 2018; Åhman et al., 2020a). The motor task parameters considered included: DT time (s), GS (cm / s), DT cost (%), and qualitative performance of DT. The cognitive parameters that were associated with cognitive impairment are: naming animals DT time (s), numbers of words recited in naming animals DT (n), words recited per 10 s during the naming animals DT (n), counting backward DT time (s), number of words recited in the reciting months backward DT (n), words recited per 10 s during the reciting months backward DT (n).

The DT parameters used (Table 2) as predictors for cognitive impairment, the protocols employed, the populations included, and how the outcome was defined are summarized in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of reviewed articles.

Discussion

The evidence collected in this review suggests that dual-task (DT) gait could be a promising predictor of cognitive impairment since 69.2% of the reviewed articles (8 studies) concluded that a DT paradigm could be helpful as a predictor of cognitive impairment. This finding is consistent with the vast majority of cross-sectional studies that have been done previously regarding the ability of DT to be associated with cognitive impairment (Laske et al., 2014; Montero-odasso et al., 2014; Bahureksa et al., 2017; Macaulay et al., 2017; Åhman et al., 2020b; Latorre et al., 2020). This review's results can help strengthen existing recommendations on the use of DT as an early clinical marker of dementia.

This review highlights gait speed (GS) parameters because it is the most frequently used motor task in the DT paradigm. Most studies consider that GS allows the discrimination between the different states of cognitive impairment (Montero-odasso et al., 2009; Macaulay et al., 2017). Indeed, most studies found a significant association between simple task GS and DT GS with cognitive impairment over time (Gillain et al., 2016; De Cock et al., 2019). This is similar to findings in other studies that associate GS with cognitive performance (Perrochon et al., 2013; Doi et al., 2014; Suk et al., 2020), while other studies found such an association only DT GS (Montero-Odasso et al., 2017; Rosso et al., 2019; Osuka et al., 2020a). However, although a decrease in GS has suggested an elevated risk of cognitive impairment (Quan et al., 2017), the results of this review suggest that simple gait variables such as GS could not be enough to predict cognitive impairment. Three of the analyzed studies that used GS in a DT paradigm did not find a significant association with cognitive impairment progression over time (Deshpande et al., 2009; Ceïde et al., 2018; Muurling et al., 2020). The lack of association can be explained by the sensitivity of the cognitive tasks used in the protocols or simple gait as the motor task, as indicated in Table 2.

These results highlight the higher predictive value of DT gait over simple task gait to discriminate between different stages of cognitive impairment (Montero-odasso et al., 2014; Bahureksa et al., 2017; Macaulay et al., 2017). DT model is consistent with the multidimensional analysis of gait (Aoki et al., 2019; Ehsani et al., 2019) or combining gait variables with cognitive tests, which allows gait to improve the sensitivity of detection of cognitive impairment during DT gait (Speechley et al., 2020).

Additionally, previous research agrees that stride length and step length are good discriminators of the different stages of cognitive impairment (Bahureksa et al., 2017; Latorre et al., 2020). These results are similar to previous studies in stride length in DT in healthy people vs. MCI (Gillain et al., 2009; Maquet et al., 2010). However, three of the reviewed studies did not find a significant association with cognitive impairment over time (Ceïde et al., 2018; De Cock et al., 2019; Muurling et al., 2020).

On the other hand, cadence seems not to be a not good indicator in three studies (Al-Yahya et al., 2011). This can be explained because the association between gait and the subcortical structures affected in the studied populations of the three studies (Ceïde et al., 2018; De Cock et al., 2019; Muurling et al., 2020). The variability of the length of the steps and the stride has been considered a good marker of cognitive impairment (Montero-Odasso et al., 2012a; Tarnanas et al., 2015). It is consistent with Beauchet et al. (2017), who used a DT model of walking and naming animals. There is still no clarity about the length of stride since it has also been seen that it has poor discriminating power for different groups of MCI (Montero-odasso et al., 2014). This DT paradigm is similar to the walking and counting backward tasks used by Muurling et al. (2020) for different states of cognitive impairment (Montero-Odasso et al., 2012a; Tarnanas et al., 2015). According to other authors, using a test that involves walking but also considers getting up, turning, and sitting down could decrease the sensitivity of the assessment (Muurling et al., 2020).

Conversely, DT GS could not be a significant parameter associated with cognitive impairment according to 30.7% of the reviewed studies. Indeed, they did not find a statistically significant association between the initial performance of DT and the change in the cognitive performance of the population evaluated after a specific time (Nascimbeni et al., 2015). In half of these studies (Nielsen et al., 2018; Muurling et al., 2020), a DT paradigm consisted of walking and counting backward one at a time. These tasks combined could be considered a low-cost DT paradigm (Montero-odasso et al., 2014), which implicates a poor, challenging cognitive task, that can be not sensitive enough.

Previous studies have shown the sensitivity of DT to discriminate between different stages of cognitive impairment, such as backward spelling when walking (Macaulay et al., 2017) or holding a glass of water when walking (Suk et al., 2020). But these models have not been included in this review since they used a cross-sectional design. It would be interesting to carry out prospective studies with these DT models to evaluate their predictive value as a marker of cognitive impairment. Indeed, DT has been used to discriminate cognitive decline in patients with dementia (Chiaramonte and Cioni, 2021).

Diverse cognitive components should accompany the use of diverse gait parameters since it has been observed that each gait variable can be a marker of specific cognitive variables (Jayakody et al., 2019). The cognitive task “count backward” has shown a significant relationship with cognitive performance when performed while walking (Suk et al., 2020). Using this cognitive task in an upper limb flexion-extension motor activity shows a high sensitivity to differentiate between the different states of cognitive impairment with the number of repetitions (speed) and the variability of the push-ups (Toosizadeh et al., 2019). This cognitive task shows significant results in 4 reviewed studies (Gillain et al., 2016; Montero-Odasso et al., 2017; De Cock et al., 2019; Åhman et al., 2020a). Also, cross-sectional studies have previously been studied using the variable “words per unit of time” (TUG + count back and TUG + name animals). It seems to be an excellent discriminator between the different levels of cognition: healthy, MCI, and dementia (Åhman et al., 2020b) and a possible good cognitive impairment predictor (Åhman et al., 2020a).

On the other hand, although the use of motion analysis platforms and body sensors allow an instrumentalized analysis of gait and a large number of parameters (Beauchet et al., 2017; Montero-Odasso et al., 2017; Ceïde et al., 2018), less expensive and infrastructure-free parameters are preferred for clinical use. One example is accelerometers, a device included in smartphones and smart bands (Gillain et al., 2016; Kikkert et al., 2017; Muurling et al., 2020). A recent study sheds some light on the gait parameters and variables that can be used to differentiate between the different stages of cognitive impairment, finding specific variables to differentiate between CS and MCI, CS and dementia, and MCI and dementia that could also be useful in the prediction of cognitive impairment: stride time, swing time, time of double support, stance time, step length, and stride time (Ghoraani et al., 2021).

This review focused on gait as a motor task in the DT paradigm. A proposed advantage of using a DT model is bringing the cognitive and motor systems close to the limit, eliminating the effect of gait variation during the previously observed day (Bessot et al., 2020). Even so, future research should consider using the upper extremities, as there is evidence to suggest that it may be a proper method for detecting cognitive impairment in a DT paradigm (Ehsani et al., 2019). Likewise, cognitive impairment and motor impairment trajectories could provide relevant and more significant information regarding its correlation with the progression to dementia (Montero-odasso et al., 2018). Besides, the age and cognitive level of subjects are factors to consider when planning a new study. Some studies suggest that the DT paradigm could have greater predictive and discriminatory power in younger people due to fewer comorbidities that can interfere with motor performance (Kikkert et al., 2017) and less cognitive impairment reflecting the ability to execute the double task (Nielsen et al., 2018).

Strengths

This is the first systematic review that considers exclusively prospective design studies to evaluate the predictive capacity of cognitive impairment of DT in pre-dementia stages. It contributes to collecting, synthesizing, classifying, and summarizing the main studies oriented to DT gait as a predictive marker of cognitive impairment. This study's findings are replicable and relevant for the early clinical screening of cognitive impairment and can support future clinical and biomedical studies.

Limitations and Projections

Because of the heterogeneity of the results, it is challenging to develop a meta-analysis using this data. This review could guide future studies to compare the use of DT more specifically as a predictive marker of cognitive impairment. Furthermore, it could consider more databases and software for database registration, such as Covidence. The keywords were oriented to the use of gait, but other motor tasks, cognitive tasks, and methodologies to study them could be included in new searches. For that purpose, it may be helpful to expand the research question beyond the utility of DT, determining if GS alone is enough for cognitive impairment screening or requires more complex instrumented gait variables. It would reduce the heterogeneity of the results and consider more comparable parameters. Considering the abovementioned, gait speed alone should not be sufficient. Also, it would be significant for clinical setting applications to determine the most promising cognitive performance task and the protocol with less variability and better reproducibility. Other studies could obtain the abovementioned considering the results of this review.

Conclusion

The evidence collected in this review suggests that DT gait is a useful predictor of cognitive impairment better than single-task gait speed measurement alone. The DT paradigm is a simple and inexpensive evaluation that helps predict and discriminate between different degrees of cognitive impairment applicable in the clinic at the outpatient level. Future research could focus on the use of coefficient of variability (CoV), stride time, delta CoV stride time, variability, swing time, stride width, steps per meter, cycle time variability gait, stride width cost, step symmetry, speed cost, DT time, numbers of words during DT, words recited per 10 s during DT, counting backward DT time, and delta DT time as parameters to predict cognitive impairment. Likewise, they could compare the older and younger population with different levels of cognitive impairment since these characteristics seem to enhance the predictive power of DT. It is relevant to note that walking as a motor task is not the only model since other motor tasks have also shown promising results.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

FR and MG conceived the idea, developed procedures for paper selection, data extraction, and drafted the manuscript. MG reviewed these procedures, developed the statistical analysis, and answered the reviewer's comments. FR coordinated, performed, reviewed paper selection, and data extraction. All authors critically reviewed, made important intellectual contributions to this manuscript, and read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

FR was funded by the Unidad de Postgrado, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Mayor APC grant for MSc studies research publication. MG was funded by Beca Especial de Apoyo DCBM tuition fee/stipend grant for Doctoral Studies 2021 from Escuela de Postgrado, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Manuela Garrido, Alejandra López, Lorena Aguilar, and Sandra Mahecha for their revisions, suggestions, and corrections. Also, thank Javier Silva for his enlightening initial discussions on the subject.

References

Åhman, H. B., Berglund, L., Cedervall, Y., Kilander, L., Giedraitis, V., McKee, K. J., et al. (2020a). Dual-task tests predict conversion to dementia—a prospective memory-clinic-based cohort study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–14. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17218129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Åhman, H. B., Cedervall, Y., Kilander, L., Giedraitis, V., Berglund, L., McKee, K. J., et al. (2020b). Dual-task tests discriminate between dementia, mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairment, and healthy controls – a cross-sectional cohort study. BMC Geriatrics 20:258. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01645-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aggarwal, N. T., Wilson, R. S., Beck, T. L., Bienias, J. L., and Bennett, D. A. (2006). Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 63, 1763–1769. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.12.1763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albers, M. W., Gilmore, G. C., Kaye, J., Murphy, C., Wingfield, A., Bennett, D. A., et al. (2016). At the interface of sensory and motor dysfunctions and Alzheimer's Disease Mark. Alzheimer's Dementia 11, 70–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Yahya, E., Dawes, H., Smith, L., Dennis, A., Howells, K., and Cockburn, J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzheimer's Research UK (2017). Cost and projections in the UK and globally | Dementia Statistics Hub. Available online at: https://www.dementiastatistics.org/statistics/cost-and-projections-in-the-uk-and-globally/ (accessed March 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Aoki, K., Ngo, T. T., Mitsugami, I., Okura, F., Niwa, M., Makihara, Y., et al. (2019). Early detection of lower MMSE scores in elderly based on dual-task gait. IEEE Access 7, 40085–40094. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2019.2906908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahureksa, L., Najafi, B., Saleh, A., Sabbagh, M., Coon, D., Mohler, J., et al. (2017). The impact of mild cognitive impairment on gait and balance. Gerontology 63, 67–83. doi: 10.1159/000445831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayer, A. J. (2018). The role of biomarkers and imaging in the clinical diagnosis of dementia. Age Ageing 47, 641–643. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afy004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O., Annweiler, C., Callisaya, M., De Cock, A.-M., Helbostad, J., Kressig, R. W., et al. (2016). Poor gait performance and prediction of dementia: results from a meta-analysis. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 17, 482–490. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2015.12.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O, livier, Launay, C. P., Sekhon, H., Barthelemy, J. C., Roche, F., Chabot, J., Levinoff, E. J., et al. (2017). Association of increased gait variability while dual tasking and cognitive decline: results from a prospective longitudinal cohort pilot study. GeroScience 39, 439–445. doi: 10.1007/s11357-017-9992-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bessot, N., Polyte, R., Quesney, M., Bulla, J., and Gauthier, A. (2020). Diurnal gait fluctuations in single- and dual- task conditions. Chronobiol. Int. 37, 836–844. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2020.1773493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Liu-ambrose, T., Boudreau, R. M., Ayonayon, H. N., Satterfield, S., Simonsick, E. M., et al. (2016). An evaluation of the longitudinal, bidirectional associations between gait speed and cognition in older women and men. J. Gerontol. 71, 1616–1623. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bootsma-van der Wiel, A., Gussekloo, J., de Craen, A. J. M., van Exel, E., Bloem, B. R., and Westendorp, R. G. J. (2003). Walking and talking as predictors of falls in the general population: the leiden 85-plus study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 51, 1466–1471. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51468.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodaty, H., Woodward, M., Boundy, K., Ames, D., and Balshaw, R. (2013). Prevalence and predictors of burden in caregivers of people with dementia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 22, 756–765. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchman, A. S., and Bennett, D. A. (2011). Loss of motor function in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Expert Rev. Neurotherap. 11, 665–676. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bürki, C. N., Bridenbaugh, S. A., Reinhardt, J., Stippich, C., Kressig, R. W., and Blatow, M. (2017). Imaging gait analysis: An fMRI dual task study. Brain Behav. 7:e00724. doi: 10.1002/brb3.724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, C. L., Strauss, E., Bunce, D., Hunter, M. A., and Hultsch, D. F. (2009). Functional abilities in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Gerontology 55, 570–581. doi: 10.1159/000228918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camicioli, R., Howieson, D., Lehman, S., and Kaye, J. (1997). Talking while walking: the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 48, 955–958. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.4.955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceïde, M. E., Ayers, E. I., Lipton, R., and Verghese, J. (2018). Walking while talking and risk of incident dementia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 580–588. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2017.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiaramonte, R., and Cioni, M. (2021). Critical spatiotemporal gait parameters for individuals with dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Hong Kong Physiother. J. 41, 1–14. doi: 10.1142/S101370252130001X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Cock, A. M., Fransen, E., Perkisas, S., Verhoeven, V., Beauchet, O., Vandewoude, M., et al. (2019). Comprehensive quantitative spatiotemporal gait analysis identifies gait characteristics for early dementia subtyping in community dwelling older adults. Front. Neurol. 10:313. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deshpande, N., Metter, E. J., Bandinelli, S., Guralnik, J., and Ferruci, L. (2009). Gait speed under varied challenges and cognitive decline in older persons: a prospective study. Age Ageing 38, 509–514. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afp093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doi, T., Makizako, H., Tsutsumimoto, K., Hotta, R., Nakakubo, S., Makino, K., et al. (2018). Combined effects of mild cognitive impairment and slow gait on the risk of dementia. Experiment. Gerontol. 110, 146–150. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doi, T., Shimada, H., Makizako, H., Tsutsumimoto, K., Uemura, K., Anan, Y., et al. (2014). Cognitive function and gait speed under normal and dual-task walking among older adults with mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurol. 14:67. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-14-67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donoghue, O., Feeney, J., O'Leary, N., and Kenny, R. A. (2018). Baseline mobility is not associated with decline in cognitive function in healthy community-dwelling older adults: findings from The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA). Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 438–448. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2017.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehsani, H., Mohler, M. J., O'connor, K., Zamrini, E., Tirambulo, C., and Toosizadeh, N. (2019). The association between cognition and dual-tasking among older adults: the effect of motor function type and cognition task difficulty. Clin. Interv. Aging 14, 659–669. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S198697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferri, C., Prince, M., Brayne, C., Brodaty, H., Fratiglioni, L., Ganguli, M., et al. (2005). Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study Cleusa. Lancet 366, 2112–2117. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67889-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-Mental State”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghoraani, B., Boettcher, L. N., Hssayeni, M. D., Rosenfeld, A., Tolea, M. I., and Galvin, J. E. (2021). Detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease using dual-task gait assessments and machine learning. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 64:102249. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2020.102249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillain, S., Dramé, M., Lekeu, F., Wojtasik, V., Ricour, C., Croisier, J. L., et al. (2016). Gait speed or gait variability, which one to use as a marker of risk to develop Alzheimer disease? A pilot study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 28, 249–255. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0392-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillain, S., Warzee, E., Lekeu, F., Wojtasik, V., Maquet, D., Salmon, E., et al. (2009). The value of instrumental gait analysis in elderly healthy, MCI or Alzheimer's disease subjects and a comparison with other clinical tests used in single and dual-task conditions. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 52, 453–474. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Yogev, G., Springer, S., Simon, E. S., and Giladi, N. (2005). Walking is more like catching than tapping: Gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp. Brain Res. 164, 541–548. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2280-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hojman, D. A., Duarte, F., Ruiz-Tagle, J., Budnich, M., Delgado, C., and Slachevsky, A. (2017). The cost of dementia in an unequal country: the case of Chile. PLoS ONE 12:e0172204. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jayakody, O., Breslin, M., Srikanth, V., and Callisaya, M. (2019). Gait characteristics and cognitive decline: a longitudinal population-based study. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 71, S5–S14. doi: 10.3233/JAD-181157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikkert, L. H. J. C., Vuillerme, N., Van Campen, J. P., Appels, B. A., Hortobágyi, T., and Lamoth, C. J. (2017). Gait characteristics and their discriminative power in geriatric patients with and without cognitive impairment. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 14, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0297-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluger, A., Gianutsos, J. G., Golomb, J., Ferris, S. H., George, A. E., Franssen, E., et al. (1997). Patterns of motor impairment in normal aging, mild cognitive decline, and early Alzheimer's disease. J. Gerontol. 52B, 28–39. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52B.1.P28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laske, C., Sohrabi, H. R., Frost, S. M., Karmele, L., Garrard, P., Buscema, M., et al. (2014). Innovative diagnostic tools for early detection of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement. 11, 561–578. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latorre, P., Muñoz, M., Salas, J., Consuegra, P., Moreno, R., Herrador, J., et al. (2020). Complex gait is related to cognitive functioning in older people : a cross-sectional study providing an innovative test. Gerontoly 66, 401–408. doi: 10.1159/000508245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leone, C., Moumdjian, L., Patti, F., Vanzeir, E., Baert, I., Veldkamp, R., et al. (2020). Comparing 16 different dual – tasking paradigms in individuals with multiple sclerosis and healthy controls: working memory tasks indicate cognitive – motor interference. Front. Neurol. 11:918. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu-Ambrose, T., Ashe, M. C., Graf, P., Beattie, B. L., and Khan, K. M. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment increases falls risk in older community-dwelling women. Phys. Ther. 88, 1482–1491. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20080117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macaulay, R. K., Wagner, M. T., Szeles, D., and Milano, N. J. (2017). Improving sensitivity to detect mild cognitive impairment : cognitive load dual-task gait speed assessment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 22, 1–9. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malouin, F., Richards, C. L., Jackson, P. L., Dumas, F., and Doyon, J. (2003). Brain activations during motor imagery of locomotor-related tasks: a PET study. Hum. Brain Map. 19, 47–62. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maquet, D., Lekeu, F., Warzee, E., Gillain, S., Wojtasik, V., Salmon, E., et al. (2010). Gait analysis in elderly adult patients with mild cognitive impairment and patients with mild Alzheimer's disease: simple versus dual task: a preliminary report. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 30, 51–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-097X.2009.00903.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mielke, M. M., Roberts, R. O., Savica, R., Cha, R., Drubach, D. I., Christianson, T., et al. (2013). Assessing the temporal relationship between cognition and gait: Slow gait predicts cognitive decline in the mayo clinic study of aging. J. Gerontol. 68, 929–937. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministerio de Salud (2017). Plan Nacional de Demencia. Available online at: https://www.minsal.cl/ministerio-de-salud-presento-plan-nacional-de-demencias/ (accessed January 4, 2021).

Google Scholar

Ministerio de Salud. (2010). Encuesta Nacional de Salud ENS Chile 2009-2010. Available online at: https://www.minsal.cl/portal/url/item/bcb03d7bc28b64dfe040010165012d23.pdf (accessed January 4, 2021).

Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., Altman, D., Antes, G., et al. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine. 6, e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-odasso, M., Bergman, H., Phillips, N. A., Wong, C. H., Sourial, N., and Chertkow, H. (2009). Dual-tasking and gait in people with Mild Cognitive Impairment. The effect of working memory. BMC Geriatr. 8, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-9-41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Muir, S. W., and Speechley, M. (2012a). Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking, and risk of falls. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-odasso, M., Oteng-amoako, A., Speechley, M., Gopaul, K., Beauchet, O., Annweiler, C., et al. (2014). The motor signature of mild cognitive impairment : results from the gait and brain study. J. Gerontol. 69, 1415–1421. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Pieruccini-Faria, F., Ismail, Z., Li, K., Lim, A., Phillips, N., et al. (2020). CCCDTD5 recommendations on early non cognitive markers of dementia: a Canadian consensus. Alzheimer's Dement. 6:e12068. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-odasso, M., Speechley, M., Muir-hunter, S. W., Sarquis-Adamson, Y., Sposato, L. A., Hachinski, V., et al. (2018). Motor and cognitive trajectories before dementia : results from gait and brain study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 66, 1676–1683. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Verghese, J., Beauchet, O., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2012b). Gait and cognition: A complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 2127–2136. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04209.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M. M., Sarquis-Adamson, Y., Speechley, M., Borrie, M. J., Hachinski, V. C., Wells, J., et al. (2017). Association of dual-task gait with incident dementia in mild cognitive impairment: results from the gait and brain study. JAMA Neurol. 74, 857–865. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.0643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muurling, M., Rhodius-Meester, H. F. M., Pärkk,ä, J., Van Gils, M., Frederiksen, K. S., Bruun, M., et al. (2020). Gait Disturbances are associated with increased cognitive impairment and cerebrospinal fluid tau levels in a memory clinic cohort. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 76, 1061–1070. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nascimbeni, A., Caruso, S., Salatino, A., Carenza, M., Rigano, M., Raviolo, A., et al. (2015). Dual task-related gait changes in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Funct. Neurol. 30, 59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA : a Brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, M. S., Simonsen, A. H., Siersma, V., Hasselbalch, S. G., and Hoegh, P. (2018). The diagnostic and prognostic value of a dual-tasking paradigm in a memory clinic. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 61, 1189–1199. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohsugi, H., Ohgi, S., Shigemori, K., and Schneider, E. B. (2013). Differences in dual-task performance and prefrontal cortex activation between younger and older adults. BMC Neurosci. 14:10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-14-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Organización Panamericana de la Salud. (2020). Reducción de los Riesgos de Deterioro Cognitivo Y Demencia. Directrices de La OMS Para La Reducción de Los Riesgos de Deterioro Cognitivo y Demencia. Available online at: https://iris.paho.org/bitstream/handle/10665.2/52426/9789275322574_spa.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y (accessed January 4, 2021).

Google Scholar

Osuka, Y., Kim, H., Watanabe, Y., Taniguchi, Y., Kojima, N., Seino, S., et al. (2020a). A combined stepping and visual tracking task predicts cognitive decline in older adults better than gait or visual tracking tasks alone: a prospective study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 1865–1873. doi: 10.1007/s40520-020-01714-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osuka, Y., Kojima, N., Sakurai, R., Watanabe, Y., and Kim, H. (2020b). Reliability and construct validity of a novel motor–cognitive dual-task test: a stepping trail making test. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 20, 291–296. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrochon, A., Kemoun, G., Watelain, E., and Berthoz, A. (2013). Walking Stroop carpet : an innovative dual-task concept for detecting cognitive impairment. Clin. Interv. Aging 8, 317–328. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S38667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2011). Mild cognitive impairment. New Engl. J. Med. 364, 2227–2234. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp0910237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrigna, L., Thomas, E., Gentile, A., Paoli, A., Pajaujiene, S., Palma, A., et al. (2019). The evaluation of dual-task conditions on static postural control in the older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. Syst. Rev. 8:188. doi: 10.1186/s13643-019-1107-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prince, M., Prina, M., Guerchet, M., and Alzheimer's Disease International (2013). World Alzheimer Report 2013 Journey of Caring an Alysis of long-term Care for dementia. Available online at: https://www.alzint.org/u/WorldAlzheimerReport2013.pdf (accessed April 17, 2021).

Google Scholar

Quan, M., Xun, P., Chen, C., Wen, J., Wang, Y., Wang, R., et al. (2017). Walking pace and the risk of cognitive decline and dementia in elderly populations: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. J. Gerontol. 72, 266–270. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosano, C., Studenski, S. A., Aizenstein, H. J., Boudreau, R. M., Longstreth, W. T., and Newman, A. B. (2012). Slower gait, slower information processing and smaller prefrontal area in older adults. Age Ageing 41, 58–64. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosso, A. L., Metti, A. L., Faulkner, K., Redfern, M., Yaffe, K., Launer, L., et al. (2019). Complex walking tasks and risk for cognitive decline in high functioning older adults. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 71, S65–S73. doi: 10.3233/JAD-181140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosso, A. L., Studenski, S. A., Chen, W. G., Aizenstein, H. J., Alexander, N. B., Bennett, D. A., et al. (2013). Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility. J. Gerontol. 68, 1379–1386. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosso, A. L., Verghese, J., Metti, A. L., Boudreau, R. M., Aizenstein, H. J., Kritchevsky, S., et al. (2017). Slowing gait and risk for cognitive impairment: the hippocampus as a shared neural substrate Andrea. Neurology 89, 336–342. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherder, E., Eggermont, L., Swaab, D., van Heuvelen, M., Kamsma, Y., de Greef, M., et al. (2007). Gait in ageing and associated dementias; its relationship with cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 485–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheridan, P. L., Solomont, J., Kowall, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2003). Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer's disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 51, 1633–1637. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51516.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E., Cusack, T., Cunningham, C., and Blake, C. (2017). The influence of a cognitive dual task on the gait parameters of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Aging Phys. Act. 25, 671–686. doi: 10.1123/japa.2016-0265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speechley, M., Kueper, J. K., Lizotte, D. J., and Montero-Odasso, M. (2020). Cognition and motor function: The gait and cognition pooled index. PLoS ONE 15:e0238690. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., Miller, J. P., and Morris, J. C. (2002). Rates of progression in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 59, 1034–1041. doi: 10.1212/WNL.59.7.1034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suk, H., Seok, K., Ryul, S., Chang, H., and Wook, S. (2020). Association between gait time and cognitive function in various walking conditions. Int. J. Gerontol. 14, 217–221. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-1433-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarnanas, I., Papagiannopoulos, S., Kazis, D., Wiederhold, M., Widerhold, B., Vuillermot, S., et al. (2015). Reliability of a novel serious game using dual-task gait profiles to early characterize aMCI. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:50. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolonen, A., Rhodius-Meester, H. F. M., Bruun, M., Koikkalainen, J., Barkhof, F., Lemstra, A. W., et al. (2018). Data-driven differential diagnosis of dementia using multiclass disease state index classifier. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:111. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toosizadeh, N., Ehsani, H., Wendel, C., Zamrini, E., O'Connor, K., and Mohler, J. (2019). Screening older adults for amnestic mild cognitive impairment and early-stage Alzheimer's disease using upper-extremity dual-tasking. Sci. Rep. 9:10911. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46925-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

US National Library of Medicine (2016). Biomarkers. Mesh Browser. Available online at: https://meshb.nlm.nih.gov/record/ui?ui=D015415 (accessed January 4, 2021).

Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Robbins, M., Holtzer, R., Zimmerman, M., Wang, C., Xue, X., et al. (2008). Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56, 1244–1251. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01758.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waite, L. M., Grayson, D. A., Piguet, O., Creasey, H., Bennett, H. P., and Broe, G. A. (2005). Gait slowing as a predictor of incident dementia: 6-Year longitudinal data from the Sydney Older Persons Study. J. Neurol. Sci. 229–230, 89–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2004.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, G., Shea, B., O'Connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., et al. (2000). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analyses. Ottawa Hospital Research Institute. Available online at: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed November 2, 2021).

Google Scholar

WHO (2020). Dementia. Key Facts. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/dementia#:~:text=Rates~of~dementia,is~between~5-8%25 (accessed March 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive-motor task, cognitive decline, screening tool, aging, dementia

Citation: Ramírez F and Gutiérrez M (2021) Dual-Task Gait as a Predictive Tool for Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults: A Systematic Review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:769462. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.769462

Received: 02 September 2021; Accepted: 06 December 2021;
Published: 24 December 2021.

Edited by:

Alena Savonenko, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Reviewed by:

Andrea L. Rosso, University of Pittsburgh, United States
Rita Chiaramonte, University of Catania, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Ramírez and Gutiérrez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Myriam Gutiérrez, myrgutierrez@uchile.cl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.