Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 06 May 2021
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Cognitive Function Changes in Older People. Results of Second Wave of Cognition of Older People, Education, Recreational Activities, NutritIon, Comorbidities, fUnctional Capacity Studies (COPERNICUS)

\r\nS&#x;awomir Kujawski*Sławomir Kujawski1*Agnieszka Kujawska,Agnieszka Kujawska2,3Rados&#x;aw PerkowskiRadosław Perkowski2Joanna Androsiuk-PerkowskaJoanna Androsiuk-Perkowska2Weronika HajecWeronika Hajec2Ma&#x;gorzata KwiatkowskaMałgorzata Kwiatkowska2Natalia SkierkowskaNatalia Skierkowska2Jakub HusejkoJakub Husejko2Daria BieniekDaria Bieniek4Julia L. NewtonJulia L. Newton5Karl J. MortenKarl J. Morten6Pawe&#x; ZalewskiPaweł Zalewski1Kornelia K&#x;dziora-KornatowskaKornelia Kędziora-Kornatowska2
  • 1Department of Hygiene, Epidemiology, Ergonomics and Postgraduate Training, Division of Ergonomics and Exercise Physiology, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Torun, Bydgoszcz, Poland
  • 2Department of Geriatrics, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Toruń, Poland
  • 3Department of Physiology, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Toruń, Poland
  • 4Department of Gastroenterology and Nutrition Disorders, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Toruń, Poland
  • 5Population Health Sciences Institute, The Medical School, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
  • 6Nuffield Department of Women’s and Reproductive Health, The Women Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom

Background: Cognitive reserve explains why subjects with more years of education, professional achievement, or participation in recreational activities show less cognitive decline with aging. We hypothesize that levels of recreational travel, education, occupation, systemic health, physical performance, and current cognitive activity levels affect the trajectory of cognitive function in older, healthy people in Poland.

Materials and Methods: Healthy, older people (N = 205) were examined and followed-up at 2 years. Participants completed physical and cognitive function assessments: including the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and its two subtests Delayed Recall (DR) and Verbal Fluency (VF), and Trail Making Test Part B (TMT B). Factors associated with cognitive functioning were also examined.

Results: The MMSE result significantly decreased over 2 years. No significant decrease in other cognitive tests was noted. However, the trajectory of cognitive tests results varied between individual participants. Percentage of variance of change was explained by the following predictors: 21 in MMSE, 24 in MoCA, 8 in DR, 25 in VF, and 24 in TMT B. Age and the presence of varicose veins were significantly linked to negative changes in MMSE and MoCA scores, while working in a professional occupational status associated with a higher score. The subgroup with varicose veins did worse on the Delayed Recall subtest of MoCA.

Conclusion: Cognitive reserve could be extended by proxies of reserve that are related to systemic health and travel activity. The latter is a combination of social, physical, and cognitive activity and potentially might serve as an intervention to improve cognitive function in older people. However, due to the limitations of this study, results should be interpreted with caution and needs to be replicated in the further studies.

Introduction

Aging is an unavoidable process in all living organisms. Discrepancies between biological and recorded age are often observed (Mianowany et al., 2004). Biological age, linked to body functionality is associated with how subjects adapt to the aging process and is a better indication of age than chronological age. In relation to the desire to successfully age, there is a growing interest in the promotion of health and prevention of diseases of the elderly (Schulz and Heckhausen, 1996). The concept of “cognitive reserve” described by Yaakov Stern in 2002 relates to the possibility of increasing the resistance of the brain to cognitive decline. The cognitive reserve hypothesis assumes that individual differences in cognitive performance are associated with biological differences in the brain and how neurons are coping with the aging process and is linked to the depletion of cognitive resources (Stern, 2002; Jodzio, 2012). Cognitive reserve explains why subjects with more years of education, professional achievement, or participation in recreational activities show less cognitive function decline during aging (Tucker and Stern, 2011).

Low gait velocity is related to poor cognitive function in older people (Peel et al., 2019). Moreover, physical activity might improve brain functioning (Pedersen, 2019). In addition, cognitive activity level and social activity correlated positively with cognitive function in healthy older adults (Clare et al., 2017; Kelly et al., 2017). Traveling abroad is a combination of social, physical, and cognitive activity. Bauer distinguishes activities related to trips such as sightseeing, physical activity, and interaction with nature, memory training, training of the senses of hearing and touching, attention and senses training, and taking part in additional language courses (Bauer, 2012).

Factors related to systemic health, in addition to lifestyle factor, also could be related to cognitive function level in older people. Visceral fat level was associated with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in community-dwelling older Japanese women (Chiba et al., 2020). Hormones produced by visceral fat might play a role in cognitive function decline (Nota et al., 2020). Varicose veins presence is a risk factor for cardiovascular events and mortality (Wu et al., 2020). Varicose veins might induce inflammation (Markovic and Shortell, 2013) and endothelial dysfunction (Komarów et al., 2014). As a consequence, systemic inflammation and endothelial dysfunction could be related to cognitive function disturbance (Buie et al., 2019).

In this study, we hypothesize that levels of recreational travel, educational achievements, occupation, presence of varicose veins, visceral fat level, physical performance, and current cognitive activity levels could affect the trajectory of cognitive function in older, healthy people in Poland.

Materials and Methods

Study Group

Sample Size Calculation

Sample size calculation was done using the General Linear Mixed Model Power and Sample Size 3.0 calculator (GLIMMPSE 3.0) (Kreidler et al., 2013). Primary outcomes were MMSE and MoCA score changes in 2 years. Decrease in mean values of MMSE from 28 to 27.65 (SD = 3.5) and MoCA from 24 to 23.6 points (SD = 4.5) for outcomes were assumed. Using Hotelling Lawley Trace with power 0.8 and type I error rate 0.05, total sample size was calculated as 364. Assuming a 12% dropout rate, an additional 43 subjects were included, giving 407 participants in total.

Study Recruitment

Study recruitment is summarized in Figure 1. In total, 407 participants (95 males) took part in the first series of assessments with 205 returning 2 years later for a second assessment (40 males).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of the study.

Participants were recruited into studies based on a regional TV and radio advertising campaign, during health-promoting lectures at Collegium Medicum University and many older people’s organizations in Bydgoszcz, in Day Care Centers for the Elderly, and at various meeting groups for older people. Study marketing material included literature and information describing an opportunity to participate free-of-charge in a study measuring physical, physiotherapeutic, dietary, social, and cognitive measures for people 60 years old and over. Age under 60 years old was the only excluding factor from participation in the study. Examination was conducted in the Department of Geriatrics, Collegium Medicum University Hospital in Bydgoszcz, Poland. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee, Ludwik Rydygier Memorial Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University, Torun (KB 340/2015). Written, informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Assessment Methods

Cognitive Tests

Neuropsychological tests were conducted by two trained staff. The majority of the neuropsychological tests (97.1%) were conducted by the same person (SK). Cognitive function was examined using the Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and Trail Making Test Part B (TMT B). MMSE is a screening 30-point questionnaire, consisting of questions associated with current time and place, immediate recall and short-term verbal memory, calculation, language, and ability to construct a 3D figure (Folstein et al., 1983). A high score indicates better cognitive performance.

MoCA measures all main cognitive domains: visuospatial skills, short-term memory recall, executive functioning, phonemic fluency, and a two-item verbal abstraction (Nasreddine et al., 2005). During the MoCA test, the results of two subtests [Verbal Fluency (VF) subtest and Delayed Recall (DR)] of five nouns were considered separately during the analysis. The VF subtest result is the number of words in Polish starting with letter “S,” which are not own nouns (conjugation was prohibited).

The Trail Making Test part B is a rapidly assessed neuropsychological tool measuring various skills including executive functioning domain: visuospatial skills, task switching, and working memory (Reitan, 1958).

Functional Performance Assessment

An 8-ft up-and-go, where the subject is asked to get up from chair, walk an 8-ft distance to and around a marker placed on the floor, get back, and sit on the chair again, as fast as possible, is an indicator of functional performance, gait speed, and balance in a dynamic manner (Rikli and Jones, 1999).

Body Composition Analysis

Weight and body fat was measured using a Tanita BC-545 body-fat analyzer. Body fat was measured using bioelectric impedance analysis (BIA) made while subjects were standing on electrodes on the sole of the foot and holding electrodes in the hands. Weighing accuracy is 0.1%. Participants were weighed in light clothing. Respondents themselves gave information about height to reduce the amount of time spent on examination, and BMI was calculated in accordance with the WHO recommendations.

Occupation Status, Activity Level Questionnaire, and Physical Examination

Subjects were asked about time spent in education. Occupational status was initially assigned as: white collar worker, white collar worker in a managerial position, owner of the craft/entrepreneur, military/policeman/other uniformed services, seller/employee of trade, farmer in an individual farm, physical worker—qualified, unskilled worker; the last three were then recoded as “low occupational status” and the rest as “high occupational status.” Eventually, a binary variable for the highest occupational status during lifetime was generated (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Occupational status categories.

Assessment of recreational and mental activities completed over the last year were assessed: reading newspapers and books, watching TV, listening to the radio, going to the café or restaurant, going to the cinema or theater, going to the concert, going to church, visiting friends or family, taking part in social group meetings, computer use, card game, chess/checkers, and solving crosswords. Answers on questions about frequency of current mental activities were coded in the following way: “never” was coded as 0, “once a year” as 1, “several times a year” as 2, “1–2 times a month” as 3, “once a week” as 4, “few times a week” as 5, “daily” as 6. The maximum score to obtain with this scale was 78 points (13 questions with 6 as the highest score in each item).

Traveling abroad in the last 3 years was coded in the following binary way: “no” as “0” and “yes” as “1.”

Subjects were asked about the presence of varicose veins on the lower limbs. Moreover, every subject underwent physical examination by trained staff, where presence of varicose veins was examined. Based on the results of physical examination, a binary variable was created with codes “0” (no) or “1” (yes).

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical package R (R Core Team, 2013). If assumption was met, dependent t-test was used for analysis, otherwise the Wilcoxon test was used to compare results of tests before vs after 2 years. To compare participants who were lost to follow-up vs those who were re-examined, independent t-test was used if assumptions on normality and homogeneity of variance were met, otherwise the Mann–Whitney U test was used. Mean and standard deviation (SD) values are presented.

The effect size for the Wilcoxon test was calculated using the following formula (Field, 2009):

r Z N

in which Z is the result of the Wilcoxon test, while N is the number of all observations. To calculate the effect size for the dependent t-test, the following r was used (Field, 2009):

r t 2 t 2 + d f

in which t is the result from the t-test, and df is the degrees of freedom.

To assess the impact of selected factors on the dynamics of cognitive functioning (before vs after 2 years), a linear mixed model with restricted maximum likelihood approach and t tests using the Satterthwaite method using R statistical packages (Lme4 and LmerTest) (Bates et al., 2015) were used. The subject and time factor were determined as random effects, and the factors explaining the change in cognitive functioning were determined as fixed effects. To calculate the confidence interval (95%) for the coefficient parameters, the confint command was used to calculate 5,000 simulations using the bootstrap method. The R2 value and confidence interval (–95%; 95%) were calculated using the r2beta command. The Dwplot command was used to visualize the results of the linear mixed models.

Violin plots were made using the ggstatsplot package (Patil, 2018). Red dots connected by a red line indicate the arithmetic mean value; a horizontal black line inside the frame means the median value. The green dots before and the orange dots after 2 years connected by dashed lines indicate the results of individual participants. The shape of the graph indicates the distribution of values. In circular histograms, the height of the column indicates the number of values, and the percentage of the value appears next to the value label.

Results

The mean age of the participants coming back for the 2-year follow-up assessment was 69.67 (–95% CI = 68.85; 95% CI = 70.5, range 60–88) with a breakdown of the different age groups shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Histogram of age.

Table 2 presents the distribution of education level of the re-examined group. Most of the participants (84.39%) obtained full secondary education or higher.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Distribution of education categories in the re-examined group.

Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics of quantitative predictors included into the model used in explaining the variance of changes in cognitive test scores after 2 years. Data on years of education, 8-ft test, and visceral fat level from the initial time point were included into the linear mixed models. On the contrary, data on cognitive activity levels from both time points were presented further into models.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Quantitative variables included in the linear mixed model.

Table 4 shows the prevalence of varicose veins on the lower limbs. Data on sex, occupational status, and traveling abroad during the last 3 years at initial assessment were included into the linear mixed models.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Qualitative variables included in the linear mixed model.

The MMSE score decreased significantly (27.8 ± 2.1 before vs 27.45 ± 2.3 after, Z = 2.56, p = 0.01, r = 0.15) (Supplementary Figure 1). No significant changes in MoCA score were noted (23.63 ± 3.5 before vs 23.21 ± 4.1 after, Z = 1.55, p = 0.12, r = 0.08) (Supplementary Figure 2). No significant changes in delayed recall subtest of MoCA were noted (2.25 ± 1.6 words recalled before vs 2.25 ± 1.7 after, Z = 0.07, p = 0.94, r = 0) (Supplementary Figure 3). No significant changes in verbal fluency subtest of MoCA was noted (12.73 ± 4.5 words spoken before vs 13.31 ± 4.8 after, t = –1.93, p = 0.06, r = 0.02) (Supplementary Figure 4). No significant changes in TMT B result were noted (138.36 ± 88.5 s before vs 132.88 ± 89.1 after, Z = 1.48, p = 0.14, r = 0.07) (Supplementary Figure 5).

Figure 3A represents the results of the mixed linear model with MMSE as a predicted variable. The model explained 22% (17%; 31%) of the variance of the change in the MMSE score before vs after 2 years. For every additional year of age, there was0.05 (–0.1; 0.01) less point in the MMSE score (t = –1.99, p = 0.048). The subgroup with varicose veins on the lower limbs had a mean of 0.67 (–1.12; –0.21) points less in MMSE after 2 years (t = –2.99, p = 0.003). The subgroup with high occupational status was characterized by 1.37 (0.47; 2.3) more points in MMSE after 2 years (t = 3.09, p = 0.002). For every additional point in the cognitive activity level, there was 0.03 (0.0004; 0.06) more point in the MMSE score (t = 2.08, p = 0.04).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mixed linear model predicting Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (A) and Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (B) score changes.

Figure 3B represents a linear mixed model on the results of MoCA. The applied model explained 24% (19%; 33%) of the variance in MoCA score changes. For every additional year of age, there was 0.1 (–0.19; –0.01) less point in the MoCA score (t = –2.45, p = 0.02). For every additional unit of visceral fat, there was 0.18 (–0.35; –0.01) less point in the MoCA score (t = –2.21, p = 0.03). The subgroup with varicose veins on the lower limbs had a mean of 0.74 (–1.46; –0.01) less point in MoCA after 2 years (t = –2.02, p = 0.04). For every second longer to finish the 8-ft test, in the first time point, there was 0.25 (–0.48; –0.002) point less in MoCA after 2 years (t = –2.1, p = 0.04). Participants with high occupational status and who traveled abroad in the 3-year period before the first time point had on a mean of 2.07 (0.6; 3.5) and 1.09 (0.11; 2.1), respectively, more points in MoCA in the second time point (t = 2.85, p = 0.005 and t = 2.31, p = 0.02.

The horizontal axis denotes the difference in the cognitive score test. The vertical dashed line indicates no change in cognitive test score (0). On the left side of the dashed line, there are predictors with a negative estimate, which are related to worse cognitive scores after 2 years, while on the right side of the dashed line, predictors related to better scores after 2 years are placed. Red lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values are negative, blue lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values cross zero, green lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values are positive.

Figure 4A represents the linear mixed model on the results of the delayed recall subtests of the MoCA, which explained 9% (6%; 17%) of the variance in the score changes before vs after 2 years. Subjects with varicose veins on lower limb recalled a mean of 0.47 (–0.84; –0.11) less words in the MoCA delayed recall after 2 years (t = –2.56, p = 0.01). For every second longer to finish the 8-ft test, in the first time point, there was 0.11 (–0.22; 0.0005) point less words recalled after 2 years (t = –1.99, p = 0.048).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mixed linear model predicting MoCA Delayed Recall (A) and Verbal Fluency (B) subtests score changes.

Figure 4B represents the linear mixed model on the results of verbal fluency subtests of MoCA, which explained 16% (12%; 25%) of the variance in the score changes. For every increase in the level of visceral fat, there was 0.3 (–0.51; –0.09) less words recalled after 2 years (t = –2.88, p = 0.004). The subgroup with a high occupational status had 3.05 (1.19; 4.88) more words pronounced in the verbal fluency subtest after 2 years (t = 3.26, p = 0.001). For every additional point in the cognitive activity level, there was 0.07 (0.0004; 0.14) more words pronounced in the verbal fluency subtest after 2 years (t = 2.11, p = 0.04).

The horizontal axis denotes difference in cognitive score test. The vertical dashed line indicates no change in cognitive test score (0). On the left side of the dashed line, there are predictors with negative estimate, which are related to worse cognitive scores after 2 years, while on the right side of the dashed line, predictors related to better scores after 2 years are placed. Red lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values are negative, blue lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values cross zero, green lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values are positive.

Figure 5 represents the linear mixed model on the results of TMT B, which explained 25% (19%; 33%) of the score changes. With every additional year of age, there was an additional 3.74 (1.61; 6.01) s to complete TMT B after 2 years (t = 3.55, p = 0.0005). The subgroup with a high occupational status had, on average, 68.14 (–104.1; –31.77) less seconds to complete the test after 2 years (t = –3.74, p = 0.0002).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Mixed linear model predicting TMT B score changes.

The horizontal axis denotes a difference in cognitive score test. The vertical dashed line indicates no change in cognitive test score (0). On the left side of the dashed line, there are predictors with a negative estimate, which are related to worse cognitive scores after 2 years, while on the right side of the dashed line, predictors related to better scores after 2 years are placed. Red lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values are negative, blue lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values cross zero, green lines indicate predictors, in which confidence interval values are positive.

Table 5 summarizes the comparison of cognitive function test subtest results before vs after 2 years.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Comparison cognitive function before vs after 2 years.

Discussion

In this study, a significant deterioration in MMSE results were observed over the 2-year study period. This was unexpected as previous studies have shown that MMSE is not an effective tool for monitoring cognitive decline in aging (Gluhm et al., 2013). Other cognitive function tests as MoCA and verbal fluency and delayed recall subtests and TMT B failed to detect a decline in cognitive function. As the vocabulary is part of the crystallized intelligence, this could potentially explain lack of decrease in the verbal fluency MoCA subtest (Craik and Bialystok, 2006). Moreover, lack of decrease in the rest test could be potentially explained by practice effect, as no alternative version of MMSE, MoCA, or TMT B were used in the second time point. Consistent with the results presented, in the 3-year Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam, deterioration of cognitive functioning was observed in all subjects (Comijs et al., 2004). In a review of the 2004 study by Bruscoli and Lovestone, they observed that the quantitative assessment of the conversion of people from MCI to dementia can vary between studies (Bruscoli and Lovestone, 2004). Potentially due to the practice effect, improvement in cognitive function was noted in some participants. In this study, we showed that the presence of varicose veins links to a greater decline in MMSE and MoCA scores over 2 years, while high occupational status was related to better results. The subgroup who actively traveled abroad for leisure had better functional fitness correlating to an improved MoCA score after 2 years. The subgroup with varicose veins did worse on the Delayed Recall subtest of MoCA. Visceral fat level was related to a decrease in verbal fluency result, while high occupational status was related to better results. Over the 2 years, age was related to worse results in the TMT B test, while high occupational status associated with better results.

Sex, Education, Occupational Status, Cognitive Activity Level

In the above study, sex was not a significant predictor for change in any cognitive test, while high occupational status was a positive predictor of good scores in MMSE, MoCA tests, verbal fluency subtests, and TMT B scores were also higher. A higher level of cognitive activity level was related to better results in verbal fluency and global cognitive function measured by MMSE after 2 years. However, years spent in education was not a significant predictor of cognitive function scores after 2 years. Presumably, the lack of correlation between these factors could be explained by specific distribution of years of education in the examined group in the above study. Most of the participants (84.39%) obtained full secondary education or higher. Therefore, the above sample is underrepresented in subjects with a lower level of education than secondary. In the above study, an underrepresentation of males was observed (19.5%). Therefore, it might explain why sex was not a significant predictor in the above study, contrary to previous longitudinal studies, where higher resilience to cognitive decline was noted in older women compared with men (McCarrey et al., 2016). A meta-analysis study shows that occupational status and participation in cognitively stimulating activities is related to cognitive function in older people (Opdebeeck et al., 2016). Future studies should focus on determining why such interventions promote successful cognitive aging rather than focus on the time spent in education. What is interesting, in the present study, years of education were not related to change in cognitive function decline. In previous studies, it was suggested that IQ is a more powerful predictor of cognitive reserve than years spent in education (Alexander et al., 1997). Armstrong et al. (2012) has suggested that IQ may have more influence than education level in Parkinson’s Disease-Mild Cognitive Impairment. IQ level is strongly related to education attainment (Butler et al., 1985). In the above study, the IQ of the participants was not determined. It is interesting to consider why years of education were not related to cognitive function changes in our Polish cohort. One possible explanation is that many factors influence the ability of subjects to progress through the education system. Participants may not have the opportunity to attend higher education even though they have a high IQ. The highest occupational status and subject obtained throughout their life might be a more reliable indicator of IQ and, hence, explain the link with this group having a slower rate of cognitive function decline compared with the subjects with higher levels of education where a correlation was not observed. Authors of the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS) underlie the role of intellectual abilities in comparison with developmental and socioeconomic characteristics of early life in functional decline and successful aging (Bosma et al., 2007). A low socioeconomic status (measured based on wealth) was related to greater decline in cognitive function during 6 to 8 years (Steptoe and Zaninotto, 2020). Moreover, alternative ways of explanation of relationship between IQ and job performance were described (Byington and Felps, 2010). The authors suggest that subjects with a higher IQ have an opportunity to gain access to developmental resources allowing them to undergo the process of on-the-job learning (Byington and Felps, 2010). In addition, subjects with a high occupational status will also have the added long-term cognitive stimulation of their work, which not everybody of a degree status will not necessary have. An active and challenged mind into old age is potentially the key to aging well. Further studies should examine the relationship between education level, occupational status, socioeconomic status, and IQ level with each other and with cognitive function changes during lifetime. Identification of modifiable factors that influence cognitive function dynamics would be of high importance for public health recommendations.

Visceral Fat Level

In this study, levels of visceral fat level were a negative predictor of verbal fluency and global cognitive function measured by the MoCA test changes after 2 years. Previous studies have suggested that patterns of fat storage might play an important role in pathophysiological mechanisms of obesity and cognitive impairment (Papachristou et al., 2015). The link between obesity and reduced executive function might be related to fat stored in a specific region such as visceral fat (Schwartz et al., 2013). Significant, negative correlation was found between phonemic fluency and visceral fat in adolescents (12–18 years old) (Schwartz et al., 2013). Fat mass and obesity-associated gene (FTO) polymorphisms were associated with worse verbal fluency in overweight and obese older men (Benedict et al., 2011). Moreover, hormones produced by visceral fat might presumably cross the blood–brain barrier and play a role in cognitive function decline (Nota et al., 2020). Leptin and adiponectin can cross the blood–brain barrier (Lee et al., 2019). A negative correlation between the level of leptin in the peripheral blood with the volume of brain gray matter was observed (Pannacciulli et al., 2007). In contrast, a higher level of adiponectin seems to be associated with more beneficial cognitive function dynamics in older subjects (Rizzo et al., 2020). However, in the above study, levels of hormones produced by fat tissue were not determined. Therefore, the inability to explain the relationship between visceral fat level in cognitive function changes is a potential limitation of the above results.

The relationship between visceral fat and global cognition decline and brain changes have been observed. Debette et al. showed that middle-aged subjects had an inverse association between levels of abdominal, especially visceral, fat with total brain volume and low cognitive function scores (Debette et al., 2010; Isaac et al., 2011). In a study of older subjects, visceral fat was negatively correlated with the level of cognitive function in subjects under 70 years (Yoon et al., 2012).

Cortical thinning (gray matter volume) in young and middle-aged populations was linked to BMI and amounts of visceral adipose tissue in patients with diabetes (Veit et al., 2014). Various factors including aerobic fitness, blood pressure, and cognitive functioning strongly link to BMI (Waldstein and Katzel, 2006). Drawing conclusions from associations linked to BMI are challenging because BMI is not a reliable indicator of body composition, and more research is needed to untangle the multiple factors including lean body mass, aerobic fitness, physical activity, and blood pressure measurement associated with BMI.

Fitness Level and Cognitive Decline

In this study, the 8-ft up-and-go test results were a significant predictor of changes in the MMSE test and delayed recall subtest. Among older people, executive functions are positively related to a 4-m course at usual pace and walking speed on a 7-m obstacle course at fast pace. The authors suggest that execution of a physical performance task requires attention resources (Ble et al., 2005). A meta-analysis shows that poor gait results predict dementia with gait issues also observed in diabetes when management of the disease starts to decline (Beauchet et al., 2016; Henderson et al., 2019). In dementia, the type of dementia is important; other than Alzheimer disease, poor gait performance is a stronger predictor of dementia (Henderson et al., 2019). A slight slowdown in gait and impairment of cognitive functions co-occur in the elderly (Atkinson et al., 2007). Correlations between specific cognitive functions and gait speed in the elderly in longitudinal studies are less pronounced than in cross-sectional studies (Callisaya et al., 2015). Participants in “The Tasmanian Study of Cognition and Gait,” aged 60–85, randomly selected from the electoral list, were evaluated twice in 3 years. A decrease in executive functions, but not other cognitive domains, was associated with a decrease in gait speed (Callisaya et al., 2015). The results of the study indicate both cross-sectional and longitudinal links between cognitive functioning and gait speed among older people living in the community. Worse verbal fluency and lower psychomotor speed were associated with lower maximum gait speed and greater deterioration of maximum gait speed over time (Soumaré et al., 2009).

Varicose Veins

The presence of varicose veins was a strong negative predictor of MMSE, MoCA, and MoCA Delayed Recall subtest score changes over the 2-year study period. Varicose veins could be a sign of venous insufficiency. Several studies have proposed a correlation between venous insufficiency and cardiac disorders (Auzky et al., 2011). In addition, varicose vein presence might be related to peripheral artery disease, although some confounding factors may be involved (Chang et al., 2018). Other cross-sectional studies also indicate that peripheral arterial disease and cardiovascular disorders were associated with an increased risk of vascular dementia (Newman et al., 2005; Laurin et al., 2007). The presence of varicose veins is related to an increased risk of cardiovascular events and mortality (Wu et al., 2020). Varicose veins might induce inflammation (Markovic and Shortell, 2013) and endothelial dysfunction (Komarów et al., 2014). In turn, high body fat percentage, systemic inflammation, and endothelial dysfunction might be related to cognitive function impairment (Buie et al., 2019). Therefore, further studies should examine this indirect relationship between varicose veins and cognitive function in older people. If such a correlation would be confirmed, then varicose veins presence could serve as an easy-to-examine factor relevant to cognitive function in older people.

Traveling Abroad

Traveling abroad in the last 3 years before the first time point was a significant predictor of changes in MoCA score after 2 years. Moreover, traveling abroad was a significant predictor of cognitive function in the baseline results in this study (Kujawski et al., 2018). However, these results need to be treated with caution as many factors could link to enhanced and reduced travel activity. Self-selection might occur, where participants who did not travel abroad may lack motivation, were depressed or fearful of traveling, had poor economic and/or health status, and had difficulty accessing information (Crawford and Godbey, 1987). Moreover, marital status and small size of social networks might be other restriction factors (Nyaupane and Andereck, 2008). Moreover, traveling abroad might be related to some level of fluency in the second language, and bilingualism might contribute to cognitive reserve (Arce Rentería et al., 2019). Travel activity might work as an aging intervention by influencing a range of parameters feeding into cognitive function improvement. Bauer distinguishes activities related to trips such as sightseeing, physical activity, and interaction with nature, memory training, training of the senses of hearing and touching, attention and sense training, and taking part in additional language courses (Bauer, 2012). To our best knowledge, the above study is the first to examine the relationship between traveling abroad and cognitive function changes in older people. However, a binary variable considering traveling abroad was included only. Therefore, further studies should use continuous variables to examine various types of touristic activities and its relation to changes in cognitive function.

Study Limitations

Two screening tests were used in this study. Future longitudinal studies carried out in Poland should include in their methodology tests that assess all cognitive domains and are not based almost exclusively on the visual and basic motor functions. It is important that the next studies include a representative sample of the whole country if the results are to be relevant to the whole Polish population; only people from Bydgoszcz and the surrounding area were included in this study. Subject drop out in studies like this one can be higher in subjects with greater cognitive impairment or who are sick introducing potential bias. The authors of a systematic review of 12 articles that analyze the number of dropouts in longitudinal studies, other than death, showed that people who did not participate in the study for reasons other than death, were older and had greater cognitive impairment. Sick or weak people are more likely to give up, and people who cannot be contacted may have worse health than other respondents (Chatfield et al., 2005).

A potential limitation of the above results is the low number of males (19.5%) included. Moreover, the IQ of the participants were not examined. In addition, recruitment based mainly on advertisement in mass media is not free from limitations. In further studies on aging in Poland, recruitment methods should be carefully chosen to limit potential bias, representational fraction of males should be included, and IQ level of participants should be examined.

The limitation of these presented research results is the method of recruitment into the study via mass media, which could lead to bias. Moreover, the number of people who resigned from participating in the second wave could be considered as high (n = 202), leading the result to be potentially underpowered. In the MAAS, people who refused to follow the follow-up had a lower level of education and worse baseline results in cognitive tests. Those who did not survive by the time of re-examination were more often men, were older, and had worse results in cognitive tests than the other participants. The authors concluded that despite the fact that participants continuing the study and people who were dropouts differed in sociodemographic and cognitive characteristics, dropouts seemed to have little impact on the analysis of cognitive dynamics (Van Beijsterveldt et al., 2002).

Conclusion

The MMSE results significantly decreased after 2 years. No significant decrease in other cognitive tests was noted. However, the trajectory of the cognitive tests results varied between individual participants.

Age and the presence of varicose veins associated with declining MMSE and MoCA scores over 2 years, while high occupational status was linked positively to a slower decline in test scores. Age was a negative predictor of TMT B, while a high occupational status was a positive one. Moreover, there was a relationship between travel activity and functional fitness and MoCA score changes and between functional fitness and MoCA Delayed Recall subtest score changes. Varicose vein presence was related to decline in the results of Delayed Recall subtest of MoCA within 2 years. Visceral fat level was a negative predictor of verbal fluency result changes, while high occupational status was a positive predictor. Moreover, visceral fat level was a negative predictor of MoCA score changes. Cognitive activity level was a positive predictor of changes in MMSE and verbal fluency.

Based on those results, we suggest that the original cognitive reserve hypothesis should be extended by proxies of reserve that are related to systemic health and travel activity. The latter is a combination of social, physical, and cognitive activity and potentially might serve as an intervention to improve cognitive function in older people. However, due to the limitations of this study, results should be interpreted with caution. Moreover, further studies on the relationship between the abovementioned factors with cognitive function in older people changes are needed.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee, Ludwik Rydygier Memorial Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University, Torun. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.653570/full#supplementary-material

References

Alexander, G. E., Furey, M. L., Grady, C. L., Pietrini, P., Brady, D. R., Mentis, M. J., et al. (1997). Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease, implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 165–172. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.2.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arce Rentería, M., Casalletto, K., Tom, S., Pa, J., Harrati, A., Armstrong, N., et al. (2019). The contributions of active Spanish-english bilingualism to cognitive reserve among older hispanic adults living in California. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34:1235.

Google Scholar

Armstrong, M. J., Naglie, G., Duff-Canning, S., Meaney, C., Gill, D., Eslinger, P. J., et al. (2012). Roles of education and IQ in cognitive reserve in Parkinson’s disease-mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra 2, 343–352. doi: 10.1159/000341782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, H. H., Rosano, C., Simonsick, E. M., Williamson, J. D., Davis, C., Ambrosius, W. T., et al. (2007). Cognitive function, gait speed decline, and comorbidities, the health, aging and body composition study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 62, 844–850. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.8.844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auzky, O., Lanska, V., Pitha, J., and Roztocil, K. (2011). Association between symptoms of chronic venous disease in the lower extremities and cardiovascular risk factors in middle-aged women. Int. Angiol. 30, 335–341.

Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48.

Google Scholar

Bauer, I. (2012). Australian senior adventure travelers to Peru, Maximising older tourists’ travel health experience. Travel Med. Infect. Dis. 110, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.tmaid.2012.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O., Annweiler, C., Callisaya, M. L., De Cock, A. M., Helbostad, J. L., Kressig, R. W., et al. (2016). Poor gait performance and prediction of dementia, results from a meta-analysis. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 1, 482–490. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2015.12.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedict, C., Jacobsson, J. A., Rönnemaa, E., Sällman–Almén, M., Brooks, S., Schultes, B., et al. (2011). The fat mass and obesity gene is linked to reduced verbal fluency in overweight and obese elderly men. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 1159.e1–e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ble, A., Volpato, S., Zuliani, G., Guralnik, J. M., Bandinelli, S., Lauretani, F., et al. (2005). Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons, the InCHIANTI study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 410–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53157.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosma, H., van Boxtel, M. P., Kempen, G. I., van Eijk, J. T., and Jolles, J. (2007). To what extent does IQ’explain’socio-economic variations in function? BMC Public Health 7:179. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruscoli, M., and Lovestone, S. (2004). Is MCI really just early dementia? A systematic review of conversion studies. Int. Psychogeriatr. 16, 129–140. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204000092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buie, J. J., Watson, L. S., Smith, C. J., and Sims-Robinson, C. (2019). Obesity-related cognitive impairment: the role of endothelial dysfunction. Neurobiol. Dis. 132:104580. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.104580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, S. R., Marsh, H. W., Sheppard, M. J., and Sheppard, J. L. (1985). Seven-year longitudinal study of the early prediction of reading achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 77:349.

Google Scholar

Byington, E., and Felps, W. (2010). Why do IQ scores predict job performance? An alternative, sociological explanation. Res. Organ. Behav. 30, 175–202. doi: 10.1016/j.riob.2010.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callisaya, M. L., Blizzard, C. L., Wood, A. G., Thrift, A. G., Wardill, T., and Srikanth, V. K. (2015). Longitudinal relationships between cognitive decline and gait slowing, the Tasmanian Study of Cognition and Gait. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 70, 1226–1232. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, S. L., Huang, Y. L., Lee, M. C., Hu, S., Hsiao, Y. C., and Chang, S. W. (2018). Association of varicose veins with incident venous thromboembolism and peripheral artery disease. JAMA 319, 807–817. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.0246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatfield, M. D., Brayne, C. E., and Matthews, F. E. (2005). A systematic literature review of attrition between waves in longitudinal studies in the elderly shows a consistent pattern of dropout between differing studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 58, 13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiba, I., Lee, S., Bae, S., Makino, K., Shinkai, Y., and Shimada, H. (2020). Visceral fat accumulation is associated with mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older Japanese women. J. Nutr. Health Aging 24, 352–357. doi: 10.1007/s12603-020-1330-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clare, L., Wu, Y. T., Teale, J. C., MacLeod, C., Matthews, F., Brayne, C., et al. (2017). Potentially modifiable lifestyle factors, cognitive reserve, and cognitive function in later life: a cross-sectional study. PLoS Med. 14:e1002259. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comijs, H. C., Dik, M. G., Deeg, D. J., and Jonker, C. (2004). The course of cognitive decline in older persons, results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 17, 136–142. doi: 10.1159/000076346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I., and Bialystok, E. (2006). Cognition through the lifespan, mechanisms of change. Trends Cogn Sci. 10, 131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, D. W., and Godbey, G. (1987). Reconceptualizing barriers to family leisure. Leisure Sci. 19, 119–127.

Google Scholar

Debette, S., Beiser, A., Hoffmann, U., DeCarli, C., O’Donnell, C. J., Massaro, J. M., et al. (2010). Visceral fat is associated with lower brain volume in healthy middle−aged adults. Ann. Neurol. 68, 136–144. doi: 10.1002/ana.22062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. (2009). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. London: Sage publications, 550.

Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Robins, L. N., and Helzer, J. E. (1983). The mini-mental state examination. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 7:812.

Google Scholar

Gluhm, S., Goldstein, J., Loc, K., Colt, A., Van Liew, C., and Corey-Bloom, J. (2013). Cognitive performance on the mini-mental state examination and the montreal cognitive assessment across the healthy adult lifespan. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 26:1. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e31828b7d26

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, A. D., Johnson, A. W., Ridge, S. T., Egbert, J. S., Curtis, K. P., Berry, L. J., et al. (2019). Diabetic gait is not just slow gait, gait compensations in diabetic neuropathy. J. Diabetes Res. 2019:4512501. doi: 10.1155/2019/4512501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaac, V., Sim, S., Zheng, H., Zagorodnov, V., Tai, E., and Chee, M. (2011). Adverse associations between visceral adiposity, brain structure, and cognitive performance in healthy elderly. Front. Aging Neurosci. 3:12. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jodzio, K. (2012). Neuropsychologia jesieni życia. Psychol. Rozwojowa 1, 85–98.

Google Scholar

Kelly, M. E., Duff, H., Kelly, S., Power, J. E. M., Brennan, S., Lawlor, B. A., et al. (2017). The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review. Syst. Rev. 6, 1–18. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0632-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komarów, W., Hawro, P., Lekston, A., Urbanek, T., and Zagrodzki, P. (2014). Endothelial dysfunction in patients with chronic venous disease: an evaluation based on the flow-mediated dilatation test. Int. Angiol. 34, 36–42.

Google Scholar

Kreidler, S. M., Muller, K. E., Grunwald, G. K., Ringham, B. M., Coker-Dukowitz, Z. T., Sakhadeo, U. R., et al. (2013). GLIMMPSE: online power computation for linear models with and without a baseline covariate. J. Stat. Softw. 54:i10. doi: 10.18637/jss.v054.i10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kujawski, S., Kujawska, A., Gajos, M., Topka, W., Perkowski, R., Androsiuk-Perkowska, J., et al. (2018). Cognitive functioning in older people. results of the first wave of cognition of older people, education, recreational activities, nutrition, comorbidities, functional capacity studies (COPERNICUS). Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:421. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laurin, D., Masaki, K. H., White, L. R., and Launer, L. J. (2007). Ankle-to-brachial index and dementia, the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Circulation 13, 2269–2274. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.106.686477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. H. Y., Cheng, K. K. Y., Hoo, R. L. C., Siu, P. M. F., and Yau, S. Y. (2019). The novel perspectives of adipokines on brain health. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20:5638. doi: 10.3390/ijms20225638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markovic, J. N., and Shortell, C. K. (2013). Genomics of varicose veins and chronic venous insufficiency. Semin. Vasc. Surg. 1, 2–13. doi: 10.1053/j.semvascsurg.2013.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarrey, A. C., An, Y., Kitner-Triolo, M. H., Ferrucci, L., and Resnick, S. M. (2016). Sex differences in cognitive trajectories in clinically normal older adults. Psychol. Aging 31:166. doi: 10.1037/pag0000070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mianowany, M. E., Maniecka-Bryła, I., and Drygas, W. K. (2004). Starzenie się populacji jako ważny problem zdrowotny i społeczno-ekonomiczny. Gerontol. Pol. 4, 172–175.

Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., and Bédirian, V. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA, a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1037/t27279-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, A. B., Fitzpatrick, A. L., Lopez, O., Jackson, S., Lyketsos, C., Jagust, W., et al. (2005). Dementia and Alzheimer’s disease incidence in relationship to cardiovascular disease in the cardiovascular health study cohort. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 1101–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53360.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nota, M. H., Vreeken, D., Wiesmann, M., Aarts, E. O., Hazebroek, E. J., and Kiliaan, A. J. (2020). Obesity affects brain structure and function-rescue by bariatric surgery? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 108, 646–657. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyaupane, G. P., and Andereck, K. L. (2008). Understanding travel constraints, application and extension of a leisure constraints model. J. Travel Res. 46, 433–439.

Google Scholar

Opdebeeck, C., Martyr, A., and Clare, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people, a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 23, 40–60. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2015.1041450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pannacciulli, N., Le, D. S. N., Chen, K., Reiman, E. M., and Krakoff, J. (2007). Relationships between plasma leptin concentrations and human brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neurosci. Lett. 412, 248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papachristou, E., Ramsay, S. E., and Lennon, L. T. (2015). The relationships between body composition characteristics and cognitive functioning in a population-based sample of older British men. BMC Geriatr. 15:172. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0169-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patil, I. (2018). Ggstatsplot, ‘ggplot2’ Based Plots with Statistical Details. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.2074621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedersen, B. K. (2019). Physical activity and muscle–brain crosstalk. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 15, 383–392. doi: 10.1038/s41574-019-0174-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peel, N. M., Alapatt, L. J., Jones, L. V., and Hubbard, R. E. (2019). The association between gait speed and cognitive status in community-dwelling older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 74, 943–948. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2013). R, A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rikli, R. E., and Jones, C. J. (1999). Development and validation of a functional fitness test for community-residing older adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 7, 129–161. doi: 10.1123/japa.7.2.129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzo, M. R., Fasano, R., and Paolisso, G. (2020). Adiponectin and cognitive decline. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 21:2010. doi: 10.3390/ijms21062010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, R., and Heckhausen, J. (1996). A life span model of successful aging. Am. Psychol. 17:702.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, D. H., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Richer, L., Syme, C., Veillette, S., et al. (2013). Visceral fat is associated with lower executive functioning in adolescents. Int. J. Obes. (Lond). 37, 1336–1343. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soumaré, A., Tavernier, B., Alpérovitch, A., Tzourio, C., and Elbaz, A. (2009). A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of the relationship between walking speed and cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly people. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 1058–1065. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., and Zaninotto, P. (2020). Lower socioeconomic status and the acceleration of aging: an outcome-wide analysis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 14911–14917. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1915741117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 3, 448–460.

Google Scholar

Tucker, A. M., and Stern, Y. (2011). Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 8, 354–360. doi: 10.2174/156720511795745320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Van Boxtel, M. P., and Bosma, H. (2002). Predictors of attrition in a longitudinal cognitive aging study. The Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS). J. Clin. Epidemiol. 55, 216–223. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(01)00473-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veit, R., Kullmann, S., Heni, M., Machann, J., Häring, H. U., Fritsche, A., et al. (2014). Reduced cortical thickness associated with visceral fat and BMI. Neuroimage Clin. 6, 307–311. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldstein, S. R., and Katzel, L. I. (2006). Interactive relations of central versus total obesity and blood pressure to cognitive function. Int. J. Obes. (Lond). 30, 201–207. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, N. C., Chen, Z. C., Feng, I. J., Ho, C. H., Chiang, C. Y., Wang, J. J., et al. (2020). Severe varicose veins and the risk of mortality: a nationwide population-based cohort study. BMJ Open 10:e034245. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-034245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, D. H., Choi, S. H., Yu, J. H., Ha, J. H., Ryu, S. H., and Park, D. H. (2012). The relationship between visceral adiposity and cognitive performance in older adults. Age Ageing 41, 456–461. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: MMSE, MoCA, TMT B, occupational status, physical performance, cognitive reserve

Citation: Kujawski S, Kujawska A, Perkowski R, Androsiuk-Perkowska J, Hajec W, Kwiatkowska M, Skierkowska N, Husejko J, Bieniek D, Newton JL, Morten KJ, Zalewski P and Kędziora-Kornatowska K (2021) Cognitive Function Changes in Older People. Results of Second Wave of Cognition of Older People, Education, Recreational Activities, NutritIon, Comorbidities, fUnctional Capacity Studies (COPERNICUS). Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:653570. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.653570

Received: 14 January 2021; Accepted: 05 March 2021;
Published: 06 May 2021.

Edited by:

Boon-Seng Wong, Singapore Institute of Technology, Singapore

Reviewed by:

Ngiap Chuan Tan, SingHealth Polyclinics, Singapore
Shuen Yee Lee, Singapore Institute of Technology, Singapore

Copyright © 2021 Kujawski, Kujawska, Perkowski, Androsiuk-Perkowska, Hajec, Kwiatkowska, Skierkowska, Husejko, Bieniek, Newton, Morten, Zalewski and Kędziora-Kornatowska. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sławomir Kujawski, skujawski@cm.umk.pl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.