Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 12 June 2020
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Anatomical and Functional Neural Networks Changes in Cognitive Impairment and Healthy Aging View all 18 articles

The Role of Resting-State Network Functional Connectivity in Cognitive Aging

  • 1Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory, College of Public Health and Health Professions, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 4Department of Biostatistics, College of Public Health and Health Professions, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 5Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, Neuroscience and Physiological Sciences Graduate Interdisciplinary Programs, and BIO5 Institute, University of Arizona and Arizona Alzheimer’s Disease Consortium, Tucson, AZ, United States

Aging is associated with disruptions in the resting-state functional architecture of the brain. Previous studies have primarily focused on age-related declines in the default mode network (DMN) and its implications in Alzheimer’s disease. However, due to mixed findings, it is unclear if changes in resting-state network functional connectivity are linked to cognitive decline in healthy older adults. In the present study, we evaluated the influence of intra-network coherence for four higher-order cognitive resting-state networks on a sensitive measure of cognitive aging (i.e., NIH Toolbox Fluid Cognition Battery) in 154 healthy older adults with a mean age of 71 and education ranging between 12 years and 21 years (mean = 16). Only coherence within the cingulo-opercular network (CON) was significantly related to Fluid Cognition Composite scores, explaining more variance in scores than age and education. Furthermore, we mapped CON connectivity onto fluid cognitive subdomains that typically decline in advanced age. Greater CON connectivity was associated with better performance on episodic memory, attention, and executive function tasks. Overall, the present study provides evidence to propose CON coherence as a potential novel neural marker for nonpathological cognitive aging.

Introduction

By the year 2050, the number of adults over the age of 65 in the United States population is expected to double, signifying one of the fastest-growing age cohorts. With advanced age, crystallized cognitive abilities, such as accumulated knowledge and vocabulary, are maintained and can even improve over time. Conversely, even in the absence of pathology, older adults experience declines in fluid cognitive abilities such as thinking abstractly, reasoning, and decision-making (Salthouse, 2010; Murman, 2015). Declines in these capacities are linked to underlying age-related deficits in processing speed, attention, memory, and executive function (i.e., cognitive aging; Salthouse, 1994, 2000, 2010; Glisky, 2007). Cognitive aging in later life is associated with functional impairments in managing new information, manipulating the environment, and solving problems (Salthouse, 1994, 2000, 2010; Buckner, 2004; Wecker et al., 2005; Harada et al., 2013). These impairments can affect older adults’ ability to function independently within society and the home. Also, extensive evidence reveals that cognitive decline and associated functional decline in older adults has been related to changes in the structure and function of the brain (Salat et al., 1999; Raz, 2000; Reuter-Lorenz et al., 2000; Cabeza, 2002; Raz et al., 2005; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Kennedy and Raz, 2009). Therefore, it is imperative to identify what brain-based factors significantly contribute to cognitive aging. A more comprehensive understanding of age-related neural alterations within cognitive aging may facilitate better differentiation of nonpathological aging from disease states and the identification of target brain areas for intervention.

One of the increasingly popular methods used to study age and pathology-related changes in the brain is resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI). rs-fMRI is used to identify coherent fluctuations in brain activity when a person is not actively engaging in a cognitive task (i.e., at rest; Biswal et al., 2010; Biswal, 2012). rs-fMRI is particularly advantageous when studying aging populations because it allows for the examination of functional connectivity while removing the demand of a task that may be confounded by potential cognitive or motor impairments. Additionally, scan times are relatively short (5–15 min), benefitting older adults that may have difficulties lying on one’s back for long periods or those with psychological concerns regarding a novel scanning environment.

Across the lifespan, rs-fMRI has identified low-frequency resting-state networks that depict the functional architecture of the brain. Resting-state networks have been characterized for aspects of attention (Fox et al., 2006), memory (Vincent et al., 2006), cognitive control (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Vincent et al., 2008; Cole et al., 2010), default mode (Raichle et al., 2001; Buckner et al., 2008), motor (Biswal et al., 1995), and sensory systems (De Luca et al., 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006). Yeo et al. (2011) published a parcellation of the brain into seven major resting-state networks: the default mode network (DMN), the dorsal attention network (DAN), the frontoparietal control network (FPCN), the cingulo-opercular network (CON) [commonly referred to as the salience (Seeley et al., 2007) or ventral attention network (Fox et al., 2006)], the limbic network, the visual network, and the somatomotor network.

Investigators have used rs-fMRI techniques to examine how patterns of resting-state network functional connectivity change with age (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Chan et al., 2014; Damoiseaux, 2017; Siman-Tov et al., 2017; Spreng and Turner, 2019). While previous studies have primarily focused on age-related declines in the connectivity of the DMN (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Damoiseaux et al., 2007; Ferreira and Busatto, 2013), recent research has uncovered several additional “higher-order cognitive” networks vulnerable to the aging process (e.g., FPCN, CON, DAN; Geerligs et al., 2015a; Siman-Tov et al., 2017). While it is clear that resting-state network connectivity is disrupted in healthy older adults, it is unclear whether these age-related changes in resting-state functional connectivity contribute to the cognitive aging process. Studies examining the relationship between network connectivity and cognition in older adults have used a variety of cognitive tasks and resting-state methods, producing inconsistent results (see Ferreira and Busatto, 2013 for review). For example, some studies have shown DMN, FPCN, and CON connectivity in older adults to be associated with performance on specific executive functioning, memory, and processing speed tasks (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Damoiseaux et al., 2007; Shaw et al., 2015). However, Onoda et al. (2012) only found an association between CON connectivity and executive functioning (i.e., the Frontal Assessment Battery and Kohs’ Block Design) and did not replicate a relationship between the other higher-order cognitive networks and cognitive performance in older adults. Lastly, Geerligs et al. (2015a) did not find a significant relationship between any of the seven major resting-state networks and cognitive performance in older adults. These inconsistent findings are surprising given the emphasis in the literature on DMN connectivity in aging (Ferreira and Busatto, 2013), suggesting future research should include other networks in analyses and incorporate sensitive cognitive measures that model the cognitive aging process. Importantly, a majority of previous studies compared resting-state connectivity patterns and cognitive performance between younger and older adults. This type of experimental design can be problematic for characterizing connectivity, as it is confounded by cohort effects and age differences in head motion, heart rate variability, and cerebrovascular function (D’Esposito et al., 1999; Geerligs et al., 2015b, 2017; Prins and Scheltens, 2015). Additionally, a majority of these studies had small sample sizes, potentially contributing to null findings. Consequently, there is a need for a study with a large sample of older adults to evaluate the association between sensitive measures of cognitive aging and inter-individual differences in resting-state network functional connectivity.

To comprehensively assess cognitive aging, we used the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (Weintraub et al., 2013)1. The NIH Toolbox yields a Fluid Cognition Composite, which consists of measures categorized as skills that decline as a function of advanced age: the Dimensional Change Card Sort (executive function), Flanker (executive function and attention), Picture Sequence Memory (episodic memory), List Sorting (working memory), and Pattern Comparison tasks (processing speed). The primary aim was to identify a relationship between resting-state network connectivity and the overall cognitive aging process. As such, this study analyzed the contribution of the four higher-order cognitive networks (DMN, DAN, FPCN, CON) on Fluid Cognition Composite scores in a large sample of healthy older adults. Although the prior literature has been highly variable regarding which networks relate to cognition in older adults, the DMN, FPCN, and CON networks are the most common networks implicated (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Onoda et al., 2012; Shaw et al., 2015) with little evidence for the DAN, which may be less sensitive to the effects of age compared to the other higher-order cognitive networks (Chan et al., 2014; Grady et al., 2016). Therefore, we predicted that older adults with greater within-network resting-state functional connectivity of the DMN, FPCN, and CON networks would have higher Fluid Cognition Composite scores. In secondary analyses, we mapped networks significantly related to the Fluid Cognition Composite onto the subtests to identify which specific domains characterized the relationship between network connectivity and cognitive aging. Our results provide important new insights into age-related cognitive declines and inter-individual variability in resting-state network connectivity, identifying a potential neural marker for nonpathological cognitive aging.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data were collected at baseline from participants recruited for the Stimulated Brain (K01AG050707) and the Augmenting Cognitive Training in Older Adults (ACT, R01AG054077) studies (Woods et al., 2018). Our sample included 159 healthy older adults ranging from 65 to 87 years old (mean age = 71.4 ± 5.1; 92 females; mean education = 16.2 ± 2.4, education range = 12–21 years; Table 1) recruited at the University of Florida (n = 110) and the University of Arizona (n = 49). Woods et al. (2018) detail the inclusion and exclusion criteria. In brief, participants were between the ages of 65–89, had no history of major psychiatric illness, no history of brain or head injury resulting in loss of consciousness greater than 20 min, and no formal diagnosis or evidence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), dementia, or neurological brain disease. The Unified Data Set (UDS) of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) was used to screen for individuals with possible MCI or dementia (Weintraub et al., 2009). Possible MCI was defined by 1.5 standard deviations below the mean in any of the following domains: general cognition, memory, visuospatial, executive functioning/working memory, or language. All participants were right-handed and had no contraindications for MRI scanning. Before beginning all study procedures, participants signed a consent form approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the University of Florida and the University of Arizona. At the baseline visit, participants completed a variety of cognitive assessments, medical history and mood questionnaires, and an MRI scan. In this study, we used the NIH Toolbox Cognition battery and the rs-fMRI data for our analyses. Three participants were excluded due to incomplete or extreme scores (greater than three standard deviations from the mean) on the NIH toolbox. Additionally, two participants were excluded as outliers due to extreme network connectivity values resulting in a total sample size of 154 older adults.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics and NIH toolbox scores.

NIH Toolbox

The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery is a brief set of sensitive measures used to assess a range of cognitive domains (Weintraub et al., 2013). In the present study, we used the unadjusted standard scores for the Fluid Cognition Composite and its five subtests that measure cognitive abilities shown to decline with advanced age. These subtests measure components of executive function, attention, episodic memory, working memory, and processing speed. For instance, the Dimensional Change Card Sort task assesses the set-shifting component of executive function (i.e., the ability to switch among multiple task strategies and rules). Here, a participant must match a target stimulus to a choice stimulus according to the shifting criterion of either shape or color. The Flanker task is a visuospatial attention task that also requires inhibitory control over automatic responses. The goal of this task is to determine the direction of a central target arrow that is flanked by similar stimuli on the left and right. The Picture Sequence Memory task targets episodic memory, a cognitive process involved in the retrieval of learned information. In this task, thematically related pictures are displayed in a sequence, and participants must remember and move the pictures into the sequence demonstrated. The List Sorting task is a measure of working memory, the ability to temporarily hold and manipulate a limited capacity of information. This requires participants to sequence and sort a list of visual and auditory stimuli from smallest to largest increasing in the number of categories and items. Lastly, the Pattern Comparison task is a measure of processing speed, where participants quickly identify whether or not two visual patterns are the same.

Imaging Acquisition

rs-fMRI data were collected using a 3-Tesla Siemens Magnetom Prisma scanner with a 64-channel head coil at the Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory at the University of Florida and using a 3-Tesla Siemens Magnetom Skyra scanner with a 32-channel head coil at the University of Arizona. Scanner type was included as a covariate in our statistical analyses to control for potential differences in the quality and acquisition of MRI data. Both study sites followed the same scanning procedures and used identical sequences. Participant head motion was constrained by foam padding, and participants were provided with earplugs to reduce the adverse effects of scanner noise. For acquiring resting-state data, participants were asked to rest for about 6 min while keeping their eyes open, as a blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) scan was acquired with an echo-planar functional protocol [number of volumes = 120, repetition time (TR) = 3,000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms; flip angle = 70°, 3.0 × 3.0 × 3.0 mm3 voxels; 44 slices, field of view (FOV) = 240 × 240 mm]. To assist the normalization of the resting-state functional images in the preprocessing stage, high-resolution T1-weighted 3D magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) images were collected (TR = 1,800 ms; TE = 2.26 ms; 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3 voxels; 176 slices; FOV = 256 × 256 mm; FA = 8°; time = 3 min and 3 s).

rs-fMRI Preprocessing

Structural and functional images were preprocessed and analyzed using the MATLAB R2016b based functional connectivity toolbox “Conn toolbox” version 18b and SPM 12 (Penny et al., 2007; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). We followed a preprocessing pipeline which included the functional realignment and unwarping, functional centering of the image to (0, 0, 0) coordinates, slice-timing correction, structural centering to (0, 0, 0) coordinates, structural segmentation and normalization to MNI space, functional normalization to MNI space, and spatial smoothing with a smoothing kernel of 8 mm FWHM. During preprocessing, the Conn toolbox implements an anatomical, component-based, noise correction strategy (aCompCor) for spatial and temporal processing to remove physiological noise factors from the data (Behzadi et al., 2007). The implementation of aCompCor combined with the quantification of participant motion and the identification of outlier scans through the Artifact Rejection Toolbox (ART)2 allows for better interpretation of functional connectivity results (Behzadi et al., 2007; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012; Shirer et al., 2015). The ART was set to the 97th percentile setting with the mean global-signal deviation threshold set at z = ±5 and the participant-motion threshold set at 0.9 mm. Due to potential confounding effects, the resulting motion information and frame-wise outliers were included as covariates in our first-level analyses (Behzadi et al., 2007; Power et al., 2012; van Dijk et al., 2012). Applying linear regression and using a band-pass filter of 0.008–0.09 Hz, data were denoised to exclude signal frequencies outside of the range of expected BOLD signals (such as low-frequency scanner drift), minimize participant motion, extract white matter and cerebral spinal fluid noise components, and control for within-participant realignment and scrubbing covariates.

Within-Network Connectivity and Cognitive Performance Analyses

For the rs-fMRI analyses, we used a publicly available network parcellation of the brain defined by Yeo et al. (2011) that has been commonly used in the resting-state literature (Betzel et al., 2014; Fjell et al., 2017; Khasawinah et al., 2017; Dixon et al., 2018; Dubois et al., 2018; Ruiz-Rizzo et al., 2019). The resting-state networks were projected into MNI152 space, and we specifically defined four of the networks (DMN, DAN, FPCN, and CON) as regions of interests (ROIs) for ROI-ROI functional connectivity analyses. ROI-ROI analyses are Fisher z-transformed bivariate correlations between brain regions’ BOLD time-series that quantify associations in the activation at rest and serve as a proxy for connectivity. Using the CONN toolbox, every participant’s average within-network connectivity was calculated by computing the mean of the pairwise correlations between the specified (Yeo et al., 2011) ROIs that comprised each of the four higher-order cognitive networks (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Visualization of the regions of interest (ROI)-ROI connections in each of four higher-order cognitive networks (Yeo et al., 2011) in our sample of healthy older adults from (A) anterior (B) superior and (C) right hemisphere views. Each network is color-coded; however, the colors do not depict different levels of correlation strength.

First, to assess how the four networks contribute to the general domain of cognitive aging, we ran a multiple linear regression evaluating the unique effect of within-network connectivity on Fluid Cognition Composite scores. For secondary analyses, linear regressions were conducted to characterize the resting-state networks’ domain-specific influence in cognitive aging by regressing within-network connectivity onto the cognitive subtests: Dimensional Change Card Sort, Flanker, Picture Sequence Memory, List Sorting, and Pattern Comparison. In the secondary analyses, we only evaluated networks that significantly contributed to Fluid Cognition Composite scores, since our primary question concerns identifying important resting-state networks in the cognitive aging process overall. We controlled for age, education, sex, and scanner type in all of our models. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 25.

Results

Primary Analyses: Within-Network Connectivity and Composite Scores

First, we regressed within-network connectivity values for DMN, DAN, FPCN, and CON simultaneously on Fluid Cognition Composite scores to evaluate which network has the greatest influence in the general cognitive aging process. In these primary analyses, age and education were significantly associated with Fluid Cognition Composite scores (p-values < 0.01), such that older age was associated with lower composite scores and more years of education was associated with higher composite scores. Conversely, sex and scanner type were not significantly associated with Fluid Cognition Composite scores. Out of the four high-order cognitive networks, only CON within-network connectivity had a significant relationship with Fluid Cognition Composite scores, such that greater CON connectivity was associated with better performance (R2 = 0.20, β = 0.33, p < 0.001). The overall model explained 20% of the variance in Fluid Cognition Composite scores, and notably, CON connectivity explained 7.4% of the variance, while age and education explained 3.8% and 5.7%, respectively (Table 2; Figures 2, 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Within-network connectivity and fluid composite score results.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A scatterplot depicting the primary regression analysis with the standardized predicted values (X-axis) resulting from regressing age, sex, education, scanner, and within-network connectivity values of the cingulo-opercular network (CON), frontoparietal control network (FPCN), default mode network (DMN), and dorsal attention network (DAN) on the Fluid Cognition Composite unadjusted standard scores (Y-axis).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatterplots depicting the unique relationships between each network and the Fluid Cognition Composite controlling for the rest of the predictors in the regression. The X and Y axes represent the standardized residuals for the independent and dependent variables, partialling out the effects of the remaining predictors. As such, the slopes reflect partial correlations.

Within-Network Connectivity and NIH Toolbox Subtests

To better characterize the relationship between CON connectivity and cognitive aging, we ran secondary analyses to identify which specific fluid cognitive subtests were associated with the CON network. The distribution of scores on the Flanker subtest was positively skewed; therefore, we performed a square root transformation to meet normality assumptions before analyses. Notably, CON within-network connectivity was related to better performance across three of the five fluid cognition subtests, suggesting the network has a relatively broad relationship with cognitive aging rather than a relationship-driven by a specific domain. Greater CON within-network connectivity was associated with higher scores on Dimensional Change Card Sort (R2 = 0.15, β = 0.26, p = 0.001), Flanker (R2 = 0.17, β = 0.29, p < 0.001), and Picture Sequence Memory tasks (R2 = 0.17, β = 0.25, p = 0.001). CON connectivity was not significantly related to performance on List Sorting (R2 = 0.07, β = 0.10, p = 0.20) or Pattern Comparison (R2 = 0.09, β = 0.12, p = 0.13; Table 3; Figure 4).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. CON within-network connectivity and NIH toolbox subtest results.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Scatterplots showing the significant relationships between (A) Picture Sequence Memory (B) Flanker and (C) Dimensional Change Card Sort subtest scores and CON within-network resting-state connectivity controlling for age, education, sex, and scanner covariates. The Y-axes reflect the unadjusted standard scores for Picture Sequence Memory and Dimensional Change Card Sort and the square root transformed scores for the Flanker task.

In these subtest analyses, age was significantly related to performance on the Pattern Comparison task, such that older age was associated with worse performance (β = −0.22, p = 0.008). Sex was significantly related to performance on Picture Sequence Memory, such that females performed better than males (β = 0.28, p < 0.001). Lastly, those with higher education performed significantly better on the List Sorting (β = 0.21, p = 0.01), Pattern Comparison (β = 0.15, p = 0.05), Flanker (β = 0.23, p = 0.004), and Dimensional Change Card Sort (β = 0.26, p = 0.001) tasks.

Discussion

Aging is associated with disruptions in the functional architecture of the brain. However, due to previously mixed findings, it is unclear if age-related changes in resting-state network functional connectivity are linked to the cognitive aging process. The present study offers important new insights by uncovering a specific relationship between resting-state network functional connectivity and cognitive performance in a large sample of healthy older adults. Here, we identified a resting-state network involved in general fluid cognition. Additionally, we outlined the cognitive scope of this network by mapping connectivity onto processing speed, episodic memory, working memory, attention, and executive function subdomains. By linking resting-state network connectivity to various aspects of the cognitive aging process, we hope to create a foundation for future targeted intervention strategies. While the literature has focused on the DMN and its implications in Alzheimer’s disease (Buckner et al., 2008; Ferreira and Busatto, 2013; Sullivan et al., 2019), our findings suggest further examination of the CON in the context of nonpathological cognitive aging (Onoda et al., 2012; Geerligs et al., 2015a; Siman-Tov et al., 2017).

Cognitive Control in Cognitive Aging: Cingulo-Opercular Network

The CON is commonly referred to as one of the cognitive control networks (Cole and Schneider, 2007; Dosenbach et al., 2008). Cognitive control is necessary for flexibly allocating mental resources to produce goal-directed behavior. Examples of control processes include attending to stimuli, preparing and initiating a response, and adapting to feedback (Cole and Schneider, 2007). These components of cognition are necessary for the successful completion of a variety of tasks in daily life. In the present study, CON was the only network that was significantly associated with the NIH Toolbox Fluid Cognition Composite. Beyond the matter of significance, CON intra-network coherence explained more of the variance in composite scores than both age and education. These results suggest that CON connectivity is an important factor that influences fluid cognition and may be a compelling target for novel interventions (e.g., transcranial direct current stimulation) to enhance overall cognitive function in older adults. Additionally, CON within-network connectivity was positively associated with performance on three out of the five subtest domains typically vulnerable to the aging process: episodic memory, attention, and executive function. This pattern of findings suggests the CON network has a relatively global relationship with the cognitive aging process rather than a relationship driven by a specific domain. These results support the notion of the general widespread involvement of the CON network in cognitive control (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Sestieri et al., 2014). Broadly, CON is involved in the implementation and maintenance of the perceptual and attentional information in a task (Dosenbach et al., 2006, 2007, 2008). This is evident through CON’s sustained activation during trial initiation, target detection, task maintenance, and response (Dosenbach et al., 2007, 2008; Sestieri et al., 2014; Han et al., 2018). CON’s expansive functional responsibility is necessary for the brain’s moment-to-moment information processing, a general proficiency greatly affected by age (Salthouse, 2000, 2010). Therefore, our findings suggest that greater functional connectivity within CON at rest may reflect a better ability to properly activate this important network during the execution of fluid cognitive tasks in older adults.

Additionally, CON consists of brain regions important for fluid cognitive abilities like decision-making, planning, target and error detection, updating, and switching (i.e., frontal operculum, medial superior frontal cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and anterior insula; Gehring and Knight, 2000; Jung and Haier, 2007; Han et al., 2018). These regions are susceptible to gray matter atrophy and white matter disruptions with age, which contribute to functional activation alterations and declines in cognitive performance (Salat et al., 1999; Raz, 2000; Reuter-Lorenz et al., 2000; Cabeza, 2002; Raz et al., 2005; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Kennedy and Raz, 2009). He et al. (2014) showed that gray matter volume of the insular cortices and the dorsal anterior cingulate, major hubs in the CON network, as well as the functional connectivity of the left insula cortex were associated with scores on the Mini-Mental Status Examination in their sample of older adults. The resting-state functional connectivity of the left insula specifically has also been shown to mediate the association between age and visual processing speed in healthy older adults (Ruiz-Rizzo et al., 2019). Greater CON resting-state connectivity may signify greater maintenance of structural integrity in these regions involved in fluid cognition. Future research should utilize multimodal imaging to further assess the relationship between age-related structural changes and resting-state network functional connectivity.

Higher-Order Cognitive Networks in Cognitive Aging: Default Mode, Dorsal Attention, Frontoparietal Control, Cingulo-Opercular

CON is a part of a larger group of networks referred to as the higher-order cognitive networks (i.e., CON, FPCN, DMN, DAN). Functional connectivity within these networks typically decreases with age along the same trajectory of age-related structural deterioration and cognitive decline (Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Giorgio et al., 2010; Geerligs et al., 2015a; Siman-Tov et al., 2017). Conversely, connectivity within sensory and motor resting-state networks remains relatively stable in advanced age. Together, these patterns of age-related alterations in connectivity support the “last in, first out” hypothesis which suggests that brain regions that are the last to develop are the first to be affected by the aging process (Raz, 2000). In addition to CON, we also expected to find a relationship with DMN and FPCN connectivity and cognitive performance in our sample of healthy older adults.

In the present study, functional connectivity within DMN, FPCN, and DAN were not significantly related to the Fluid Cognition Composite. These results are surprising given the historic focus on the DMN as an individual network and its interactions with DAN and FPCN (Fox et al., 2005; Buckner et al., 2008; Ferreira and Busatto, 2013; Spreng et al., 2013). The most common finding in the resting-state and aging literature is that there are disruptions in resting-state functional connectivity within the DMN present in nonpathological aging and Alzheimer’s disease (Greicius et al., 2004; Buckner et al., 2008; Ferreira and Busatto, 2013; Ferreira et al., 2016). However, relating resting-state characteristics of the DMN and other networks to actual cognitive performance in older adults has resulted in mixed findings (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Damoiseaux et al., 2007; Geerligs et al., 2015a; Ferreira et al., 2016). While disruptions in DMN resting-state connectivity may be a marker for memory deficits and Alzheimer’s disease pathology (Buckner, 2004; Buckner et al., 2008), our findings suggest that CON resting-state connectivity is a more robust marker for declines in attention-demanding cognitive domains present in nonpathological cognitive aging. This distinction between the roles of DMN and CON in the cognitive aging process may help facilitate better differentiation of nonpathological aging from disease states.

Limitations and Future Directions

The present study is not without limitations regarding cohort characteristics and methodology. First, the age range of our older adult sample is relatively restricted (65–87). Therefore, we may only be representing connectivity and cognition relationships for a specific subset of older adults. Additionally, while our sample of older adults had an education range of 12–21 years, 67.5% of the sample obtained a bachelor’s degree or higher. Future work should examine this research question with a wider age range and a higher representation of older adults with education below 16 years to broaden the generalizability of these results. We conducted an ROI-ROI analysis to mitigate multiple comparisons and to limit our analyses to regions involved in established resting-state networks. Future studies should also examine voxel-wise approaches and whole-brain metrics to further explore the relationships found in the current study. Finally, we associated resting-state functional connectivity and cognitive performance in healthy older adults at one point in time. Therefore, we cannot make any conclusions about within-participant changes in connectivity and cognitive performance over time. In the course of neurodegenerative diseases, disrupted connectivity is apparent long before the presence of cognitive decline (Chen et al., 2016). As such, our findings may offer specific networks central to the early stages of cognitive aging. Future work should longitudinally assess changes in CON connectivity to see if disruptions in these networks are associated with the progression to MCI and Alzheimer’s disease.

Conclusion

This study provides important new insights on inter-individual differences in resting-state network connectivity and the cognitive aging process. Notably, we provide evidence to suggest CON coherence as a potential new marker for fluid cognitive performance capacity in nonpathological aging. Out of the four higher-order cognitive networks, connectivity within the CON network exhibited the strongest relationship with a sensitive measure of general fluid cognitive ability in our large sample of healthy older adults. Furthermore, CON connectivity explained a greater percentage of the variance in fluid cognitive performance than both age and education. CON connectivity mapped onto three out of the five fluid cognitive subtests, reflecting a global rather than domain-specific influence on the cognitive aging process. Collectively, these results suggest that CON connectivity may be a central facet of the cognitive aging process and deserves increased focus in future research investigating the neural substrates of age-related cognitive decline.

Data Availability Statement

Data are managed under the data-sharing agreement established with NIA and the parent R01 clinical trial Data Safety and Monitoring Board in the context of an ongoing Phase III clinical trial (ACT study, R01AG054077). All trial data will be made publicly available 2 years after completion of the parent clinical trial, per NIA and DSMB agreement. Requests for baseline data can be submitted to the ACT Publication and Presentation (P&P) Committee and will require submission of data use, authorship, and analytic plan for review by the P&P committee (YWp3b29kc0BwaGhwLnVmbC5lZHU=).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Boards at the University of Florida and the University of Arizona. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

HH and AW contributed to the conception and design of the study. HH extracted the data, performed the statistical analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AW, AO’S, JK, EB, and NE helped organize data and provide resources. EV, PB, and SS collected data. EP, GH, SW, SD, GA, MM, RC, and AW were involved in project administration. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

We would like to acknowledge support from the National Institute on Aging (NIA R01AG054077, NIA K01AG050707, NIA P30AG019610), the State of Arizona and Arizona Department of Health Services (ADHS), and the McKnight Brain Research Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.nihtoolbox.org
  2. ^ www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect

References

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D., Raichle, M. E., et al. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56, 924–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behzadi, Y., Restom, K., Liau, J., and Liu, T. T. (2007). A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage 37, 90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betzel, R. F., Byrge, L., He, Y., Goñi, J., Zuo, X.-N. N., Sporns, O., et al. (2014). Changes in structural and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan. NeuroImage 102, 345–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B. (2012). Resting state fMRI: a personal history. NeuroImage 62, 938–944. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B., Mennes, M., Zuo, X.-N., Gohel, S., Kelly, C., Smith, S. M., et al. (2010). Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 4734–4739. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911855107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and ad: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol. Aging 17, 85–100. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, M. Y., Park, D. C., Savalia, N. K., Petersen, S. E., and Wig, G. S. (2014). Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, E4997–E5006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1415122111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G. G., Shu, H., Chen, G. G., Ward, B. D., Antuono, P. G., Zhang, Z., et al. (2016). Staging Alzheimer’s disease risk by sequencing brain function and structure, cerebrospinal fluid, and cognition biomarkers. J. Alzheimers Dis. 54, 983–993. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., Pathak, S., and Schneider, W. (2010). Identifying the brain’s most globally connected regions. NeuroImage 49, 3132–3148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., and Schneider, W. (2007). The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. NeuroImage 37, 343–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Esposito, M., Zarahn, E., Aguirre, G. K., and Rypma, B. (1999). The effect of normal aging on the coupling of neural activity to the bold hemodynamic response. NeuroImage 10, 6–14. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S. (2017). Effects of aging on functional and structural brain connectivity. NeuroImage 160, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Beckmann, C. F., Arigita, E. J. S., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., et al. (2007). Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1856–1864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Luca, M., Smith, S., De Stefano, N., Federico, A., and Matthews, P. M. (2005). Blood oxygenation level dependent contrast resting state networks are relevant to functional activity in the neocortical sensorimotor system. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 587–594. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0059-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., La Vega, A. D., Mills, C., Andrews-Hanna, J., Spreng, R. N., Cole, M. W., et al. (2018). Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 115, E1598–E1607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715766115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A. T., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 11073–11078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Visscher, K. M., Palmer, E. D., Miezin, F. M., Wenger, K. K., Kang, H. C., et al. (2006). A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50, 799–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, J., Galdi, P., Paul, L. K., and Adolphs, R. (2018). A distributed brain network predicts general intelligence from resting-state human neuroimaging data. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 373:20170284. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, L. K., and Busatto, G. F. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity in normal brain aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 384–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, L. K., Regina, A. C. B., Kovacevic, N., da Martin, M. G. M., Santos, P. P., de Carneiro, C. G., et al. (2016). Aging effects on whole-brain functional connectivity in adults free of cognitive and psychiatric disorders. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3851–3865. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., Sneve, M. H., Grydeland, H., Storsve, A. B., and Walhovd, K. B. (2017). The disconnected brain and executive function decline in aging. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2303–2317. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A., Vincent, J. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2006). Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 10046–10051. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604187103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., and Van Essen, D. C. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Renken, R. J., Saliasi, E., Maurits, N. M., and Lorist, M. M. (2015a). A brain-wide study of age-related changes in functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1987–1999. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Rubinov, M., Cam-Can, and Henson, R. N. (2015b). State and trait components of functional connectivity: individual differences vary with mental state. J. Neurosci. 35, 13949–13961. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1324-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Tsvetanov, K. A., Cam-Can, and Henson, R. N. (2017). Challenges in measuring individual differences in functional connectivity using fMRI: the case of healthy aging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4125–4156. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehring, W. J., and Knight, R. T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 516–520. doi: 10.1038/74899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giorgio, A., Santelli, L., Tomassini, V., Bosnell, R., Smith, S., De Stefano, N., et al. (2010). Age-related changes in grey and white matter structure throughout adulthood. NeuroImage 51, 943–951. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glisky, E. L. (2007). “Changes in cognitive function in human aging,” in Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms., ed. D. R. Riddle (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis), 3–20.

Google Scholar

Grady, C., Sarraf, S., Saverino, C., and Campbell, K. (2016). Age differences in the functional interactions among the default, frontoparietal control and dorsal attention networks. Neurobiol. Aging 41, 159–172. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, S. W., Eaton, H. P., and Marois, R. (2018). Functional fractionation of the cingulo-opercular network: alerting insula and updating cingulate. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2624–2638. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harada, C. N., Natelson Love, M. C., and Triebel, K. L. (2013). Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 29, 737–752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, X., Qin, W., Liu, Y., Zhang, X., Duan, Y., Song, J., et al. (2014). Abnormal salience network in normal aging and in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3446–3464. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, R. E., and Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 30, 135–154. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, K. M., and Raz, N. (2009). Aging white matter and cognition: differential effects of regional variations in diffusion properties on memory, executive functions and speed. Neuropsychologia 47, 916–927. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khasawinah, S., Chuang, Y.-F., Caffo, B., Erickson, K. I., Kramer, A. F., and Carlson, C. M. (2017). The association between functional connectivity and cognition in older adults. J. Syst. Integr. Neurosci. 3:3. doi: 10.15761/jsin.1000164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murman, D. L. (2015). The impact of age on cognition. Semin. Hear. 36, 111–121. doi: 10.1055/s-0035-1555115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onoda, K., Ishihara, M., and Yamaguchi, S. (2012). Decreased functional connectivity by aging is associated with cognitive decline. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2186–2198. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penny, W., Friston, K., Ashburner, J., Kiebel, S., and Nichols, T. (2007). Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images. Amsterdam; Boston: Elsevier/Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage 59, 2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prins, N. D., and Scheltens, P. (2015). White matter hyperintensities, cognitive impairment and dementia: an update. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 11, 157–165. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N. (2000). “Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: integration of structural and functional findings,” in Handbook of Aging and Cognition, 2nd Edn, eds F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 1–90.

Google Scholar

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., et al. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 174–187. doi: 10.1162/089892900561814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz-Rizzo, A. L., Sorg, C., Napiórkowski, N., Neitzel, J., Menegaux, A., Müller, H. J., et al. (2019). Decreased cingulo-opercular network functional connectivity mediates the impact of aging on visual processing speed. Neurobiol. Aging 73, 50–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Kaye, J. A., and Janowsky, J. S. (1999). Prefrontal gray and white matter volumes in healthy aging and Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 56, 338–344. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1994). The aging of working memory. Neuropsychology 8, 535–543. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol. Psychol. 54, 35–54. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00052-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 754–760. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sestieri, C., Corbetta, M., Spadone, S., Romani, G. L., and Shulman, G. L. (2014). Domain-general signals in the cingulo-opercular network for visuospatial attention and episodic memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 551–568. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, E. E., Schultz, A. P., Sperling, R. A., and Hedden, T. (2015). Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance across cognitive domains in older adults. Brain Connect. 5, 505–516. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shirer, W. R., Jiang, H., Price, C. M., Ng, B., and Greicius, M. D. (2015). Optimization of rs-fMRI pre-processing for enhanced signal-noise separation, test-retest reliability and group discrimination. NeuroImage 117, 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siman-Tov, T., Bosak, N., Sprecher, E., Paz, R., Eran, A., Aharon-Peretz, J., et al. (2017). Early age-related functional connectivity decline in high-order cognitive networks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:330. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., Sepulcre, J., Turner, G. R., Stevens, W. D., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Intrinsic architecture underlying the relations among the default, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 74–86. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., and Turner, G. R. (2019). The shifting architecture of cognition and brain function in older adulthood. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 523–542. doi: 10.1177/1745691619827511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, M. D., Anderson, J. A. E., Turner, G. R., and Spreng, R. N. (2019). Intrinsic neurocognitive network connectivity differences between normal aging and mild cognitive impairment are associated with cognitive status and age. Neurobiol. Aging 73, 219–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dijk, K. R. A., Sabuncu, M. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2012). The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage 59, 431–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincent, J. L., Snyder, A. Z., Fox, M. D., Shannon, B. J., Andrews, J. R., Raichle, M. E., et al. (2006). Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory network. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 3517–3531. doi: 10.1152/jn.00048.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wecker, N. S., Kramer, J. H., Hallam, B. J., and Delis, D. C. (2005). Mental flexibility: age effects on switching. Neuropsychology 19, 345–352. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Tulsky, D. S., Zelazo, P. D., Bauer, P. J., et al. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH toolbox. Neurology 80, S54–S64. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weintraub, S., Salmon, D., Mercaldo, N., Ferris, S., Graff-Radford, N. R., Chui, H., et al. (2009). The Alzheimer’s disease centers’ uniform data set (UDS): the neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 23, 91–101. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318191c7dd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Nieto-Castanon, A. (2012). Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Cohen, R., Marsiske, M., Alexander, G. E., Czaja, S. J., and Wu, S. (2018). Augmenting cognitive training in older adults (The ACT Study): design and Methods of a Phase III tDCS and cognitive training trial. Contemp. Clin. Trials 65, 19–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2017.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cingulo-opercular, cognitive aging, functional connectivity, resting-state, networks

Citation: Hausman HK, O’Shea A, Kraft JN, Boutzoukas EM, Evangelista ND, Van Etten EJ, Bharadwaj PK, Smith SG, Porges E, Hishaw GA, Wu S, DeKosky S, Alexander GE, Marsiske M, Cohen R and Woods AJ (2020) The Role of Resting-State Network Functional Connectivity in Cognitive Aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:177. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00177

Received: 31 January 2020; Accepted: 22 May 2020;
Published: 12 June 2020.

Edited by:

P. Hemachandra Reddy, Texas Tech University Health Sciences Center, United States

Reviewed by:

Boon-Seng Wong, Singapore Institute of Technology, Singapore
Ben Nephew, Worcester Polytechnic Institute, United States

Copyright © 2020 Hausman, O’Shea, Kraft, Boutzoukas, Evangelista, Van Etten, Bharadwaj, Smith, Porges, Hishaw, Wu, DeKosky, Alexander, Marsiske, Cohen and Woods. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adam J. Woods, YWp3b29kc0BwaGhwLnVmbC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.