Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 25 September 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

A 16-Week Aerobic Exercise Intervention Does Not Affect Hippocampal Volume and Cortical Thickness in Mild to Moderate Alzheimer’s Disease

\r\nKristian Steen Frederiksen*&#x;Kristian Steen Frederiksen1*†Christian Thode Larsen,&#x;Christian Thode Larsen2,3†Steen Gregers HasselbalchSteen Gregers Hasselbalch1Anders Nymark ChristensenAnders Nymark Christensen3Peter Hgh,Peter Høgh4,5Lene WermuthLene Wermuth6Birgitte Bo AndersenBirgitte Bo Andersen1Hartwig Roman Siebner,Hartwig Roman Siebner2,7Ellen GardeEllen Garde2
  • 1Danish Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurology, Rigshospitalet, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 2Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance, Copenhagen University Hospital Hvidovre, Hvidovre, Denmark
  • 3Department of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science, Technical University of Denmark, Kongens Lyngby, Denmark
  • 4Regional Dementia Research Center, Department of Neurology, Zealand University Hospital, Roskilde, Denmark
  • 5Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 6Dementia Clinic, Odense University Hospital, Odense, Denmark
  • 7Department of Neurology, Bispebjerg Hospital, Copenhagen University Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark

Introduction: Brain imaging studies in healthy elderly subjects suggest a positive effect of aerobic exercise on both brain structure and function, while the effects of aerobic exercise in Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) has been scarcely investigated.

Methods: In a single-blinded randomized MRI study, we assessed the effects of an aerobic exercise intervention on brain volume as measured by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and its correlation to cognitive functioning in patients with AD. The study was a sub-study of a larger randomized controlled trial (ADEX study). Forty-one patients were assigned to a control or exercise group. The exercise group performed 60-min of aerobic exercise three times per week for 16 weeks. All participants underwent whole-brain MRI at 3 Tesla and cognitive assessment at baseline and after 16 weeks. Attendance and intensity were monitored providing a total exercise load. Changes in regional brain volumes and cortical thickness were analyzed using Freesurfer software.

Results: There was no effect of the type of intervention on MRI-derived brain volumes. In the entire group with and without training, Exercise load showed a positive correlation with changes in volume in the hippocampus, as well as frontal cortical thickness. Volume changes in frontal cortical thickness correlated with changes in measures of mental speed and attention and exercise load in the exercise group.

Conclusion: We did not find evidence to support an effect of 16 weeks of aerobic exercise on brain volume changes in patients with AD. Longer intervention periods may be needed to affect brain structure as measured with volumetric MRI.

Clinical Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01681602, registered September 10th, 2012 (Retrospectively registered).

Introduction

Observational studies have found a physically active lifestyle to be associated with a reduced risk for later cognitive decline including progression to dementia (Heyn et al., 2004; Aarsland et al., 2010), and accumulation of cerebral beta-amyloid and tau (Liang et al., 2010; Okonkwo et al., 2014; Merrill et al., 2016). Several randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of exercise interventions have demonstrated a positive effect on cognition in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Baker et al., 2010a; Suzuki et al., 2013) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Venturelli et al., 2011; Vreugdenhil et al., 2012; Hoffmann et al., 2015). In a recent RCT in patients with mild-to-moderate AD, we found a positive effect of a 16-week aerobic exercise intervention on physical fitness, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and, in a subgroup, an effect on mental speed and attention (Hoffmann et al., 2015; Sobol et al., 2016). These findings highlight the fact that a relatively short exercise intervention is able to impact factors important for patient wellbeing.

Evidence for the biological mechanisms underlying a relationship between brain and exercise remains sparse. Animal studies, which have aimed to elucidate such mechanisms indicate several different pathways to be involved (Kramer et al., 2006; van Praag, 2008, 2009). This includes induction of neurogenesis in the hippocampus (van Praag et al., 1999; Intlekofer and Cotman, 2013), and anti-inflammatory changes (Ryan and Kelly, 2016). Similarly, pathology such as beta-amyloid and tau accumulation have been shown to be attenuated by exercise in animal models (Adlard et al., 2005; Yuede et al., 2009; Kang and Cho, 2015; Moore et al., 2016). In humans, exercise may have an effect on the hippocampus in healthy elderly subjects (Erickson et al., 2009, 2011) and MCI (ten Brinke et al., 2015), although results have been conflicting (Colcombe et al., 2006; Niemann et al., 2014; Tamura et al., 2015; Wagner et al., 2015). Other brain regions, most consistently frontal areas, have also been reported to be affected by exercise in healthy older subjects (Taubert et al., 2011; Scheewe et al., 2013; Reiter et al., 2015; Tamura et al., 2015; Köbe et al., 2016).

Hippocampal atrophy is a pathological hallmark of AD, but is also found in other neurodegenerative diseases (Toyoshima et al., 2003; Whitwell et al., 2011; Abdulla et al., 2014), healthy aging (Chowdhury et al., 2011), and depression (Frodl et al., 2002). Moreover, in patients with MCI where biomarkers confirming the presence of AD pathology are not available the underlying cause may include many different etiologies. Hence, hippocampal atrophy in healthy aging and MCI may be driven by a plethora of pathological processes. An effect of exercise on the hippocampus in healthy subjects and MCI may therefore not directly translate to patients in the dementia stage of AD pathology. As follows, studies of exercise interventions in AD are needed to test whether hippocampal atrophy in AD may be modifiable by exercise. In a single study, a mixed patient population consisting of both MCI and AD patients underwent an exercise intervention of 26 weeks, in which hippocampal atrophy rates did not differ between groups (Morris et al., 2017).

The paucity of data prompted the present study with the primary objective to test whether an aerobic exercise intervention attenuates hippocampal atrophy in patients with mild dementia due to AD. Furthermore, we investigated whether brain atrophy in other brain regions may be slowed down by exercise, in AD. Lastly, we examined correlations between changes in brain volume and exercise attendance, physical fitness and cognitive measures.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Study Design

Preserving Cognition, Quality of Life, Physical Health and Functional Ability in AD: The Effect of Physical Exercise (ADEX) study is a multicenter single-blinded randomized study of moderate-to-high intensity exercise in patients with mild to moderate AD. The primary objective was to assess the effects of moderate-to-high intensity exercise on cognitive and physical functioning, neuropsychiatric symptoms, quality of life and Activities of daily living (ADL). A total of 200 participants were included. Participants were randomly assigned to either the intervention arm, consisting of three weekly sessions of 60 min of moderate-to-high-intensity aerobic exercise for 16 weeks, or control arm, consisting of usual care, in a blinded manner. Assessment of cognitive function, ADL, neuropsychiatric symptoms, blood sampling and physical function was carried out before and after intervention for all participants. Assessment was carried out by blinded assessors. Inclusion criteria included: (1) AD according to NINCDS-ADRDA and DSM-IV criteria, (2) age between 50 and 90 years, (3) Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of more than 19, (4) at least 3 months of stable doses if the patient was receiving anti-dementia medication or mood stabilizing medication, (5) informed consent. Exclusion criteria included (1) severe psychiatric illness, (2) alcohol or drug abuse within the last 2 years, (3) participation in aerobic exercise (moderate-to-hard intensity) more than twice weekly on a regular basis, and (4) any medical condition which precluded engagement in the exercise program (e.g., severe neurological or medical illness, presence of several cardiovascular risk factors). Detailed description of rationale and design may be found elsewhere (Hoffmann et al., 2013) as well as the principle findings (Hoffmann et al., 2015; Sobol et al., 2016). The present study reports results from a MRI substudy within the ADEX study. Seventy-one participants from three centers underwent brain MRI at baseline. Thirteen participants dropped out of the study, and did not undergo MRI at 16-week follow-up, and 17 patients were excluded from further analysis of MRI due to poor MRI data quality which did not enable imaging processing, leaving 41 participants for the present study. For correlations between brain volume and estimated VO2 max, an additional six subjects were excluded due to missing data on estimated VO2 max. See Figure 1 for flow diagram.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Flow diagram of participant flow through the study.

The ADEX trial was approved by the The Committees of Biomedical Research Ethics for the Capital Region (Protocol no.: H-3-2011-128) and by the Danish Data Protection Agency (j.no.: 30-0718). Both approvals were given, in accord with Danish national legislation, as umbrella approvals which covered any and all centers involved.

MRI Acquisition

Both baseline and follow-up MRI was performed using a 3.0-T Siemens Trio scanner and included T1-weighed magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) (TE 3.04 ms, TR 1550 ms, FoV read 256 mm, FoV phase 100%, 192 slices), T2-weighed fast spin echo (TE 354 ms, TR 3000 ms, FoV read 282 mm, FoV phase 76.6%, 192 slices) and fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) (TE 353 ms, TR 6000 ms, FoV read 282 mm, FoV phase 85.9%, 192 slices) sequences. Furthermore, diffusion-weighted imaging, resting state fMRI and arterial spin labeling sequences were acquired and will be reported elsewhere.

Data Processing

Analysis of MRI data was carried out in a blinded manner, which was unblinded for the statistical analysis.

Cortical Thickness and Hippocampal Volume

The T1-weighed data was gradient unwarped to correct for spatial distortions (Jovicich et al., 2006) and processed with version 5.3 of the cross-sectional (Fischl et al., 2002) and longitudinal (Reuter et al., 2012) Freesurfer stream, in order to obtain segmentations of cortical regions defined according to the Destrieux atlas (Destrieux et al., 2010) as well as the hippocampal subfields (Van Leemput et al., 2009), caudate and putamen. The pipeline was specifically tuned to correct for intensity inhomogeneity that can be observed at 3T (Boyes et al., 2008; Zheng et al., 2009).

In cases where Freesurfer failed to properly delineate the white matter and pial surface, the pipeline were manually guided following the steps outline in the Freesurfer documentation.1 This specifically involved correcting the skull stripping to better delineate the pial surface, insertion of control points to guide white matter normalization for the purpose of improving white matter segmentation, and finally editing the white matter segmentation itself. Two trained readers edited the pipeline; to avoid segmentation bias, one was responsible for skull stripping and white matter editing, while the other was responsible for control point insertion.

Finally, overall quality of the longitudinal segmentation output was assessed by experienced raters blinded to group allocation (CTL, KSF, and EG). Specifically, the pial and white matter surface outlines, as well as the hippocampal subcortical segmentation were visually inspected and consensus reached for all. One volume was excluded due to significant segmentation error in the hippocampus.

To explore regional, cortical effects, cortical thickness measures obtained from Freesurfer were divided into four categories (early, middle, late, and very late) each including areas reported to be progressively affected by atrophy from MCI to full AD diagnosis (Whitwell et al., 2007): “early” (temporal, precuneus, and cingulate), “middle” (parietal, temporal-occipital, occipital, fusiform, and parahippocampus), and “late” (frontal). A “very late” region composed by the pre and postcentral cortex were also defined (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Anatomical labels for statistical analysis.

White Matter Hyperintensities

For delineation of white matter hyperintensities (WMH), MPRAGE and T2-weighted images were co-registered and re-sliced to the corresponding FLAIR image using a six parameter rigid transformation. WMH were defined as clearly hyperintense areas relative to surrounding white matter on both FLAIR and T2-weighted images and identified by simultaneous inspection of both aligned images. For WMH volume local thresholding was applied and WMH volumes for the whole brain quantified automatically using the Jim image analysis package, Version 6.0, (Xinapse Systems Ltd., Northants, United Kingdom)2. Visual identification and delineation was carried out by a single trained rater blinded to clinical information. For nine subjects (five control, four intervention) WMH could not delineated due to movement artifacts. WMH volume was used solely as covariate in the statistical analysis.

Longitudinal and Normalized Measures

Longitudinal measures of brain volume, cortical thickness and cognitive scores for each subject were computed as the relative change between baseline and follow-up by subtracting baseline from follow-up, and dividing the difference with the baseline measure, thereby canceling out within-subject correlations, as well as accounting for between-subject differences in brain size. Decreases in volume are indicated by negative (-) sign. Throughout the paper, we will refer to the relative change simply as change. A normalized WMH measure was obtained by dividing WMH volume with white matter volume.

Cognitive Outcome Measures

Cognitive assessment included the MMSE for global cognitive impairment (Folstein and Folstein, 1975), the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) after 120s for mental speed and attention (Smith, 1973), and the Stroop Color and Word Test (Stroop) (Golden, 1978) incongruent score for reaction time.

Assessment of Activities of Daily Living

Activities of daily living (ADL) functioning was assessed using the AD Cooperative Study-ADL scale, and is reported for baseline.

Assessment of Physical Activity Level

The Physical activity scale in the elderly (PASE) (Washburn and Smith, 1993) was used to assess baseline physical activity level. The PASE is a 5-min questionnaire, which asks the participant to recollect activities in the 7 days up until questioning in the domains of work-related, household-related and leisuretime-related physical activity. For the present study, we used proxy-rated PASE scores (only reported for baseline).

Test of Physical Performance

The 6-min Astrand Cycle Ergometer test (Monark Ergomedic 839E; Monark Exercise AB, Sweden) was used to estimate the maximal oxygen uptake based on workload and average heart rate (HR) during the last minute of the 6-min cycle test, corrected for age and body weight. Maximal oxygen uptake (estimated VO2 max) was used as a measure of aerobic, physical fitness (Cink and Thomas, 1981).

Exercise Load (Attendance and Intensity)

To assess attendance and intensity of training the physiotherapist in each group kept a training log. Attendance ratio was defined as number of attended exercise sessions over total number of offered sessions. Exercise intensity was based on the per-session average HR recorded using continued monitoring during exercise (including rest). Average HR for all sessions was calculated, and intensity defined as average HR over maximum expected HR (220 minus subject age). To obtain total exercise load, measures for attendance ratio and intensity was multiplied.

Statistical Analysis

Student’s unpaired t-test and the chi squared-test were used to compare baseline demographic and clinical characteristics. To test whether participants who were randomized but were not included in the final analysis differed from those who were included in the analysis, we carried out Student’s unpaired t-test to test for differences in age and baseline MMSE and chi squared-test for gender.

Brain Volume Measures

Separate multivariable linear regression models were used to compare changes in volume between groups for the hippocampal subfields (model 1), para-hippocampus (model 2), caudate and putamen (model 3) with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Similarly, separate models were used to compare changes in cortical thickness for each of the “early,” “middle,” “late,” and “very late” categories previously described. For all group tests, Hoteling’s T2 multivariate test (Hotelling, 1931) was applied. Since outliers were detected in scatter plots of the variables, a further non-parametric Oja rank sum test (Oja and Randles, 2004) were performed, using 10.000 permutations, to confirm validity of p-values from Hoteling’s T2 test. Gender, age, and baseline WMH were used as covariates.

Correlation Tests

The relationship between changes in frontal and cingulate cortical thickness, and mental speed and attention (SDMT, Stroop) were assessed with pre- and postcentral cortex as a control region, since these regions show no or only very late atrophy in AD patients. The SDMT and Stroop scales were chosen based on findings from previous studies indicating a specific effect of exercise on measures of mental speed and attention (Baker et al., 2010b; Frederiksen et al., 2014), which was also confirmed in the ADEX study with findings of a significant effect of the intervention on SDMT in a subgroup of participants (Hoffmann et al., 2015). Finally, relationship between exercise load, change in estimated VO2 max and changes in hippocampal subfield volume as well as frontal cortical thickness were also investigated. The hippocampus was chosen because hippocampal atrophy is a hallmark pathological finding in AD, and since episodic memory deficits is the main clinical feature of AD. The frontal lobe was chosen due to the aforementioned effect of exercise on mental speed and attention, cognitive functions, which to a high degree are reliant on frontal lobe circuitry. Lastly, to assess whether participants who at baseline had more advanced brain atrophy were less able to participate in the intervention, we examined correlations between exercise load and hippocampal volume and frontal cortical thickness.

An omnibus test was performed on the correlations. The null-hypothesis was that all correlations between the measure of interest and the cerebral volume/thickness measures were zero. In this way the multiple comparison problem was minimized. If the null-hypothesis were rejected, a post hoc analysis of the individual correlations was performed.

For all tests, the significance level was 0.05. Statistics were obtained with SAS Statistical Software version 9.4.

Results

A total of 41 subjects (intervention: 21; control: 20) were included. There were no significant differences between the two groups with regards to age, gender, baseline MMSE, baseline ADL score, or baseline PASE (Table 2). There was no significant difference at baseline for participants who dropped out and those who did not regarding baseline MMSE, age and gender. Participants in the intervention group showed an improvement in estimated VO2 max (mean 2.5 mL/kg/min; (SD: ± 3.4); p = 0.005) following the intervention, whereas the control group did not (mean 0.2 mL/kg/min; (SD: ± 7.4); p = 0.93). Baseline MRI measures are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Baseline demographic and clinical data.

Brain Volumes

In Model 1 (hippocampal subregions), significant difference were found for change between the two groups in the left fimbria (p = 0.012) and CA2 + 3 (p = 0.016) which did not survive correction for multiple comparison. No difference between groups was observed for Model 2 (parahippocampal) or Model 3 (caudate and putamen). No significant between-group difference in changes in regional cortical thickness was found. See Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Baseline and change from baseline total hippocampal and subregional volumes.

Cognitive Performance Correlations

Relative change in frontal and cingulate cortical thickness correlated significantly with change in SDMT (p = 0.0078) in the intervention group. Specifically, for frontal area in the intervention group, a post hoc analysis revealed change in cortical thickness in the right frontal inferior-orbital gyri (r = 0.48, p = 0.038) and right frontal inferior-triangular gyri (r = 0.64, p = 0.003) (Figure 2) to be significantly correlated with change in SDMT. Change in cortical thickness and SDMT did not correlate in the usual care group. When combining the two groups, there was a significant correlation [right frontal inferior-orbital gyri: r = 0.46, p = 0.004; right frontal inferior-triangular gyri (r = 0.38, p = 0.02)]. There were no significant correlations between change in cortical thickness and Stroop scores (p = 0.08).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Correlation between change in cortical thickness and change in SDMT. The figure shows correlations between relative change in inferior-triangular gyri (A) and right frontal inferior-orbital (B) thickness and relative change in SDMT (Unitless) for each region in the exercise group. Female participants are indicated by solid circles, males by crosses.

Exercise Load and Physical Performance Correlations

In the intervention group, exercise load was correlated with change in frontal cortical thickness (p = 0.011), with a post hoc analysis revealing right inferior frontal area to be significantly correlated with exercise load (r = 0.51, p = 0.035). Similarly, a significant correlation with changes in hippocampal volume (p = 0.009) was found, with post hoc analysis revealing no individually significant areas, but with the largest correlation in the right subiculum (r = 0.44, p = 0.086). There was no significant correlation between hippocampal subfield volume and exercise load or between hippocampal volume and frontal cortical thickness and estimated VO2 max. There was no correlation between baseline hippocampal volume or frontal cortical thickness and exercise load.

Discussion

The effect of physical exercise on hippocampal volume has only been reported in a small number of studies, but nevertheless it remains the most consistent finding. Other brain regions have been reported to be influenced by exercise, including whole brain volume (Mortimer et al., 2013), anterior and posterior cingulate gyrus (Scheewe et al., 2013; Reiter et al., 2015; Köbe et al., 2016), parietal cortical area (Taubert et al., 2011; Köbe et al., 2016), insula (Reiter et al., 2015), and precentral gyri (Reiter et al., 2015). Another region, which has been reported to be positively affected by exercise interventions in several studies, is the frontal cortical region (Taubert et al., 2011; Scheewe et al., 2013; Reiter et al., 2015; Tamura et al., 2015; Köbe et al., 2016). Although we were not able to show a similar effect, we found change in SDMT and exercise load to be correlated with relative change in frontal cortical thickness. In the main ADEX study population totaling 200 patients with AD, we have previously reported a positive effect on SDMT in a subpopulation of participants who adhered to the exercise program (Hoffmann et al., 2015). Our findings in the present study could be interpreted to indicate that changes in frontal cortical thickness may be the underlying biology mediating the effect on SDMT. However, such speculation must be made with caution since the present correlation was in a relatively small group and since correlations can only imply, but not prove a causative relationship. It is also not surprising that changes in frontal regions may be associated with change in SDMT, primarily a measure of mental speed and attention, which rely on frontal circuitry. Furthermore, the fact that several studies report effects of exercise in frontal regions may also simply be an effect of the relative size of the frontal lobe. Nevertheless, other studies have found cognitive functions subserved by frontal areas (e.g., attention, executive function) to be more amenable to effects of exercise, relative to e.g., memory function (Baker et al., 2010b; Frederiksen et al., 2014). The correlation between exercise load and change in frontal cortical thickness may be interpreted as an indication of a dose-response relationship or possibly a responder effect where the effect of the intervention is confined to those individuals who were the most active. From a biological standpoint, it would be a reasonable assumption, and is seemingly supported by the previously mentioned findings regarding SDMT in the subpopulation in the ADEX study (Hoffmann et al., 2015). However, the lack of a correlation between change in frontal cortical thickness and physical fitness (measured by estimated VO2 max), which may be a more accurate measure of the effects of the intervention, contradicts such an interpretation. Rather, it could be speculated that the significant correlation indicates a reverse relationship, i.e., that participants with less pronounced brain atrophy, were able to participate more in the exercise intervention. We however, also tested this hypothesis by examining correlations between hippocampal volume and frontal cortex and exercise load, and found no evidence for this reverse relationship.

The study has several limitations, which must be taken into account when interpreting the presented findings. Our study population was relatively small, with a total of 41 participants. Moreover, the duration of the intervention was short compared to some other studies, in which interventions up to 2 years have been applied (Tamura et al., 2015), the most common being 6–12 months (Colcombe et al., 2006; Erickson et al., 2011; Niemann et al., 2014; Best et al., 2015). There is a tendency for studies, which reported significant effects on brain volumes to apply interventions between 6 and12 months of moderate intensity (target of 60–70% of maximal HR) (Erickson et al., 2011; Mortimer et al., 2013; Scheewe et al., 2013; Niemann et al., 2014; Best et al., 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015), indicating a threshold of around 6 months as a minimum to elicit detectable changes. In this context it is noteworthy that the exercise intervention in the present subgroup as well as the ADEX cohort as a whole, was able to improve aerobic fitness in participants (Sobol et al., 2016). Further, in the main ADEX study, an improvement in SDMT in a subgroup, and fewer neuropsychiatric symptoms in the intervention group, was found, demonstrating target engagement regarding these measures. The present finding do not clearly support detectable structural brain changes to underlie these improvements in clinical measures, although they cannot be interpreted as negating such a relationship.

One study examined an intervention of 19 weeks in patients with stroke. 18F-FDG-PET scans were used to assess regional brain glucose metabolism, a marker of synaptic function. The authors reported a significant increase in glucose metabolism in the medial temporal lobe, but not a concomitant effect on MRI derived measure of cortical atrophy (Moore et al., 2015). These findings highlight the issue of whether the applied method of MRI may be less sensitive compared to other methods to detect changes induced by exercise in brain pathophysiology and normal physiology, especially with regards to shorter interventions. Other potentially more sensitive imaging modalities include resting state fMRI, MR spectroscopy, perfusion MRI and ultra-high field MRI, but studies are lacking and as such remain speculative. These imaging modalities should be explored in future studies. Moreover, results from animal models of aging and AD have shown that many biological mechanisms may be responsible for the brain-exercise relationship, such as neurogenesis (van Praag et al., 1999; Intlekofer and Cotman, 2013), angiogenesis (Paillard et al., 2015) and reduction of beta-amyloid (Yuede et al., 2009) which are not readily quantified by MRI.

The methodology of our study also has several strengths. As described elsewhere (Reuter et al., 2012), the longitudinal Freesurfer pipeline utilizes cross-sectionally processed time points to generate a common template, which is then used as a point of initialization for an unbiased analysis of each individual timepoint. This procedure helps to avoid potential bias in the outcome measures due to e.g., registration to the baseline time point, as pointed out in (Fox et al., 2011). Furthermore, it increases statistical power because inter-subject variation is reduced. The ADEX study was conducted following a rigorous methodology regarding planning of the study, randomization procedures and assessment of attendance and intensity of exercise. Study participants were well characterized with regards to diagnosis, clinical and demographic data, cognitive performance and aerobic fitness. In this subgroup, this included biomarkers, which confirmed the presence of AD pathology such as beta-amyloid of phosphorylated-tau (i.e., amyloid PET and CSF sampling). Furthermore, we conducted a pilot study prior to the main study to be able to design an exercise program specifically designed to patients with dementia (64). In the present study, a drop-out rate of 18 % was found (compared to 10 percent in the whole ADEX cohort (Hoffmann et al., 2015). For the main ADEX study, a drop-out rate of 20 % was expected (Hoffmann et al., 2013), and as such, the drop-out rate is less than expected. In a systematic review of intervention studies in AD, drop-out rates of 22–51% (for placebo 22–41%) was found (Grill et al., 2017), and for exercise interventions, drop-out rates between 10–20% have previously been reported (Rolland et al., 2007; Santana-Sosa et al., 2008; Eggermont et al., 2009; Baker et al., 2010b; Yu et al., 2011).

Conclusion

In conclusion, a 16-week intervention of moderate-to-high intensity aerobic exercise was unable to attenuate atrophy rates in patients with mild to moderate AD. The intervention period may not have been long enough for target engagement with regards to MRI measures of gray matter atrophy, although patients in the intervention group improved with regards to aerobic fitness. In this context, it is pertinent to highlight the fact that even though the present study failed to find significant effects on cortical volume, we have shown that the 16-week exercise intervention did improve symptoms in the participants (Hoffmann et al., 2015). This has practical implications in that it underscores the need to offer exercise regimes even of shorter duration (e.g., due to restricted resources or inability of patients to participate for longer periods) to patients with dementia. Correlations between frontal cortical thickness and exercise load and a measure of mental speed and attention, are in line with previous findings of an effect on the frontal lobe, and functions subserved by this brain region. Further studies in AD populations applying rigorous methodology and longer interventions of more than 6 months are needed to elucidate effects on brain volume.

Data Availability

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Author Contributions

KF and CL contributed to the design and data collection of the ADEX study and the present study, gave input to the statistical analysis of data, and drafted the manuscript. SH contributed to the design and data collection of the ADEX study and the present study, and was a major contributor to the paper. AC planned and carried out the statistical analysis of data and was a major contributor to the paper. PH contributed to the design and data collection of the ADEX study and the present study, and was a major contributor to the paper. LW contributed to the design and data collection of the ADEX study and the present study, and was a major contributor to the paper. BA contributed to the design and data collection of the ADEX study and the present study, and was a major contributor to the paper. HS contributed to the design of the present study, and was a major contributor to the paper. EG contributed to the design of the present study, and was a major contributor to the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. The corresponding author takes primary responsibility for communication with the journal and editorial office during the submission process, throughout peer review, and during publication. The corresponding author takes responsibility for ensuring that the submission adheres to all journal requirements including, but not exclusive to, details of authorship, study ethics and ethics approval, clinical trial registration documents, and conflict of interest declaration. The corresponding author will be available post-publication to respond to any queries or critiques.

Funding

The ADEX study was supported by a grant from The Danish Council for Strategic Research (j. no.: 10-092814).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Investigator list by site:

Memory Clinic, Copenhagen University Hospital, Rigshospitalet

Birgitte Bo Andersen, DMSc, M.D.

Memory Clinic, Roskilde Hospital

Peter Høgh, Ph.D., M.D.

Dementia Clinic, Odense University Hospital

Anette Lolk, Assoc. Professor, Ph.D., M.D.

Lene Wermuth, M.D.

Department of Geriatrics, Svendborg Hospital

Søren Jakobsen, M.D.

Department of Geriatrics, Slagelse Hospital

Lars P. Laugesen, M.D.

Robert Graff Gergelyffy, M.D.

Memory Clinic, Aarhus University Hospital

Hans Brændgaard, M.D.

Hanne Gottrup, Ph.D., M.D.

Memory Clinic, Aalborg Hospital

Karsten Vestergaard, M.D.

Memory Clinic, Glostrup University Hospital

Eva Bjerregaard, M.D.

We are grateful to all physiotherapists, study nurses and clinical raters for their contributions to the study. We also thank Jonathan Polimeni from the Athinoula Martinos Center, Massachussetts General Hospital, Boston, United States for supplying gradient unwarping software.

Footnotes

  1. ^http://freesurfer.net/fswiki/FreeSurferWiki
  2. ^www.xinapse.com

References

Aarsland, D., Sardahaee, F. S., Anderssen, S., and Ballard, C. (2010). Is physical activity a potential preventive factor for vascular dementia? A systematic review. Aging Ment. Health. 14, 386–395. doi: 10.1080/13607860903586136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abdulla, S., Machts, J., Kaufmann, J., Patrick, K., Kollewe, K., Dengler, R., et al. (2014). Hippocampal degeneration in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2639–2645. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adlard, P., Perreau, V. M., Pop, V., and Cotman, C. W. (2005). Voluntary exercise decreases amyloid load in a transgenic model of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 25, 4217–4221. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0496-05.2005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, L. D., Frank, L. L., Foster-Schubert, K., Green, P. S., Wilkinson, C. W., McTiernan, A., et al. (2010a). Aerobic exercise improves cognition for older adults with glucose intolerance, a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 22, 569–579. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, L. D., Frank, L. L., Foster-Schubert, K., Green, P. S., Wilkinson, C. W., McTiernan, A., et al. (2010b). Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial. Arch. Neurol. 67, 71–79. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Chiu, B. K., Liang Hsu, C., Nagamatsu, L. S., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2015). Long-term effects of resistance exercise training on cognition and brain volume in older women: results from a randomized controlled trial. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21, 745–756. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyes, R. G., Gunter, J. L., Frost, C., Janke, A. L., Yeatman, T., Hill, D. L., et al. (2008). Intensity non-uniformity correction using N3 on 3-T scanners with multichannel phased array coils. Neuroimage 39, 1752–1762. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chowdhury, M. H., Nagai, A., Bokura, H., Nakamura, E., Kobayashi, S., and Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Age-related changes in white matter lesions, hippocampal atrophy, and cerebral microbleeds in healthy subjects without major cerebrovascular risk factors. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 20, 302–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2009.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cink, R., and Thomas, T. (1981). Validity of the astrand-ryhming nomogram for predicting maximal oxygen intake. Br. J. Sports Med. 15, 182–185. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.15.3.182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P. E., Kim, J. S., Prakash, R., McAuley, E., et al. (2006). Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 61, 1166–1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A., and Halgren, E. (2010). Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage 53, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggermont, L. H. P., Swaab, D. F., Hol, E. M., and Scherder, E. J. (2009). Walking the line: a randomised trial on the effects of a short term walking programme on cognition in dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 80, 802–804. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.158444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Hu, L., Morris, K. S., et al. (2009). Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 19, 1030–1039. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: neurotechnique automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M., and Folstein, S. (1975). “Mini-mental state” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, N., Ridgway, G., and Schott, J. (2011). Algorithms, atrophy and Alzheimer’s disease: cautionary tales for clinical trials. Neuroimage 51, 15–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederiksen, K. S., Verdelho, A., Madureira, S., Bäzner, H., O’Brien, J. T., Fazekas, F., et al. (2014). Physical activity in the elderly is associated with improved executive function and processing speed: the LADIS Study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 30, 744–750. doi: 10.1002/gps.4220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frodl, T., Meisenzahl, E. M., Zetzsche, T., Born, C., Groll, C., Jager, M., et al. (2002). Hippocampal changes in patients with a first episode of major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 1112–1118. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test Manual. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Co.

Google Scholar

Grill, J. D., Apostolova, L. G., Bullain, S., Burns, J. M., Cox, C. G., Dick, M., et al. (2017). Communicating mild cognitive impairment diagnoses with and without amyloid imaging. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 9, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s13195-017-0261-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyn, P., Abreu, B. C., and Ottenbacher, K. J. (2004). The effects of exercise training on elderly persons with cognitive impairment and dementia: a meta-analysis. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 1694–1704. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, K., Frederiksen, K. S., Sobol, N. A., Beyer, N., Vogel, A., Simonsen, A. H., et al. (2013). Preserving cognition, quality of life, physical health and functional ability in Alzheimer’s disease: the effect of physical exercise (ADEX Trial): rationale and design. Neuroepidemiology 41, 198–207. doi: 10.1159/000354632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, K., Sobol, N. A., Frederiksen, K. S., Beyer, N., Vogel, A., Vestergaard, K., et al. (2015). Moderate-to-high intensity physical exercise in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized controlled trial. J. Alzheimers Dis. 50, 443–453. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hotelling, H. (1931). The Generalization of student’s ratio. Ann. Math. Stat. 2, 360–378.

Google Scholar

Intlekofer, K. A., and Cotman, C. W. (2013). Exercise counteracts declining hippocampal function in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis. 57, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovicich, J., Czanner, S., Greve, D., Haley, E., Kouwe, A., Gollub, R., et al. (2006). Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. Neuroimage 30, 436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, E., and Cho, J. (2015). Effect of treadmill exercise on PI3K/AKT/mTOR, autophagy, and Tau hyperphosphorylation in the cerebral cortex of NSE/htau23 transgenic mice. J. Exerc. Nutr. Biochem. 19, 199–209. doi: 10.5717/jenb.2015.15090806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köbe, T., Witte, A. V., Schnelle, A., Lesemann, A., Fabian, S., Tesky, V. A., et al. (2016). Combined omega-3 fatty acids, aerobic exercise and cognitive stimulation prevents decline in gray matter volume of the frontal, parietal and cingulate cortex in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage 131, 226–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., and Colcombe, S. J. (2006). Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. J. Appl. Physiol. 101, 1237–1242. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00500.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, K. Y., Mintun, M. A., Fagan, A. M., Goate, A. M., Bugg, J. M., Holtzman, D. M., et al. (2010). Exercise and Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers in cognitively normal older adults. Ann. Neurol. 68, 311–318. doi: 10.1002/ana.22096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merrill, D. A., Siddarth, P., Raji, C. A., Emerson, N. D., Rueda, F., Ercoli, L. M., et al. (2016). Modifiable risk factors and brain PET measures of Amyloid and Tau in Non-demented adults with memory complaints. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, K. M., Girens, R. E., Larson, S. K., Jones, M. R., Restivo, J. L., Holtzman, D. M., et al. (2016). A spectrum of exercise training reduces soluble Aβ in a dose-dependent manner in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis. 85, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, S. A., Hallsworth, K., Jakovljevic, D. G., Blamire, A. M., He, J., Ford, G. A., et al. (2015). Effects of community exercise therapy on metabolic, brain, physical, and cognitive function following stroke: a randomized controlled pilot trial. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 29, 623–635. doi: 10.1177/1545968314562116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. K., Vidoni, E. D., Johnson, D. K., Van Sciver, A., Mahnken, J. D., Honea, R. A., et al. (2017). Aerobic exercise for Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized controlled pilot trial. PLoS One 12:e0170547. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mortimer, J., Ding, D., Borenstein, A., DeCarli, C., Guo, Q., Wu, Y., et al. (2013). Changes in brain volume and cognition in a randomized trial of exercise and social interaction in a community-based sample of non-demented chinese elders. J. Alzheimers Dis. 30, 757–766. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-120079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemann, C., Godde, B., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2014). Not only cardiovascular, but also coordinative exercise increases hippocampal volume in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:170. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oja, H., and Randles, R. (2004). Multivariate nonparametric tests. Stat. Sci. 4, 598–605. doi: 10.1214/088342304000000558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okonkwo, O. C., Schultz, S., Oh, J. M., Larson, J., Edwards, D., Cook, D., et al. (2014). Physical activity attenuates age-related biomarker alterations in preclinical AD. Neurology 83, 1753–1760. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paillard, T., Rolland, Y., and de Souto Barreto, P. (2015). Protective effects of physical exercise in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease: a narrative review. J. Clin. Neurol. 11, 212–219. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2015.11.3.212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter, K., Nielson, K. A., Smith, T. J., Weiss, L. R., Alfini, A. J., and Smith, J. C. (2015). Improved cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with increased cortical thickness in mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21, 757–767. doi: 10.1017/S135561771500079X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Schmansky, N. J., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2012). Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 61, 1402–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolland, Y., Pillard, F., Klapouszczak, A., Reynish, E., Thomas, D., Andrieu, S., et al. (2007). Exercise program for nursing home residents with Alzheimer’s disease: a 1-year randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 55, 158–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01035.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, S. M., and Kelly, ÁM. (2016). Exercise as a pro-cognitive, pro-neurogenic and anti-inflammatory intervention in transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Res. Rev. 27, 77–92. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santana-Sosa, E., Barriopedro, M. I., López-Mojares, L. M., Pérez, M., and Lucia, A. (2008). Exercise training is beneficial for Alzheimer’s patients. Int. J. Sports Med. 29, 845–850. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1038432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheewe, T. W., van Haren, N. E. M., Sarkisyan, G., Schnack, H. G., Brouwer, R. M., de Glint, M., et al. (2013). Exercise therapy, cardiorespiratory fitness and their effect on brain volumes: a randomised controlled trial in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 23, 675–685. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. (1973). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Sobol, N. A., Hoffmann, K., Frederiksen, K. S., Vogel, A., Vestergaard, K., Braendgaard, H., et al. (2016). Effect of aerobic exercise on physical performance in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 12, 1207–1215. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, T., Shimada, H., Makizako, H., Doi, T., Yoshida, D., Ito, K., et al. (2013). A randomized controlled trial of multicomponent exercise in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One 8:e61483. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamura, M., Nemoto, K., Kawaguchi, A., Kato, M., Arai, T., Kakuma, T., et al. (2015). Long-term mild-intensity exercise regimen preserves prefrontal cortical volume against aging. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 30, 686–694. doi: 10.1002/gps.4205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taubert, M., Lohmann, G., Margulies, D. S., Villringer, A., and Ragert, P. (2011). Long-term effects of motor training on resting-state networks and underlying brain structure. Neuroimage 57, 1492–1498. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ten Brinke, L. F., Bolandzadeh, N., Nagamatsu, L. S., Hsu, C. L., Davis, J. C., Miran-Khan, K., et al. (2015). Aerobic exercise increases hippocampal volume in older women with probable mild cognitive impairment: a 6-month randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 248–254. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-093184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toyoshima, Y., Piao, Y.-S., Tan, C.-F., Morita, M., Tanaka, M., Oyanagi, K., et al. (2003). Pathological involvement of the motor neuron system and hippocampal formation in motor neuron disease-inclusion dementia. Acta Neuropathol. 106, 50–56. doi: 10.1007/s00401-003-0696-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Leemput, K., Bakkour, A., Benner, T., Wiggins, G., Wald, L. L., Augustinack, J., et al. (2009). Automated segmentation of hippocampal subfields from ultra-high resolution in vivo MRI. Hippocampus 19, 549–557. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H. (2008). Neurogenesis and exercise: past and future directions. Neuromolecular Med. 10, 128–140. doi: 10.1007/s12017-008-8028-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H. (2009). Exercise and the brain: something to chew on. Trends Neurosci. 32, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H., Christie, B. R., Sejnowski, T. J., Gage, F. H., and van Praag, H. (1999). Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 13427–13431. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.23.13427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venturelli, M., Scarsini, R., and Schena, F. (2011). Six-month walking program changes cognitive and ADL performance in patients with Alzheimer. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 26, 381–388. doi: 10.1177/1533317511418956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vreugdenhil, A., Cannell, J., Davies, A., and Razay, G. (2012). A community-based exercise programme to improve functional ability in people with Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized controlled trial. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 26, 12–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2011.00895.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, G., Herbsleb, M., de la Cruz, F., Schumann, A., Brünner, F., Schachtzabel, C., et al. (2015). Hippocampal structure, metabolism, and inflammatory response after a 6-week intense aerobic exercise in healthy young adults: a controlled trial. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 35, 1570–1578. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Washburn, R., and Smith, K. (1993). The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): development and evaluation. J. Clin. 46, 153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitwell, J. L., Przybelski, S., Weigand, S. D., and David, S. (2007). 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from MCI to AD. Brain 130, 1777–1786. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm112.3D

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitwell, J. L., Weigand, S. D., Gunter, J. L., Boeve, B. F., Rademakers, R., Baker, M., et al. (2011). Trajectories of brain and hippocampal atrophy in FTD with mutations in MAPT or GRN. Neurology 77, 393–398. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227047f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, F., Nelson, N. W., Savik, K., Wyman, J. F., Dysken, M., and Bronas, U. G. (2011). Affecting cognition and quality of life via aerobic exercise in Alzheimer’s disease. West J. Nurs. Res. 20, 1–15. doi: 10.1177/0193945911420174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuede, C. M., Zimmerman, S. D., Dong, H., Kling, M. J., Bero, A. W., Holtzman, D. M., et al. (2009). Effects of voluntary and forced exercise on plaque deposition, hippocampal volume, and behavior in the Tg2576 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis. 35, 426–432. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2009.06.002.Effects

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, W., Chee, M. W., and Zagorodnov, V. (2009). Improvement of brain segmentation accuracy by optimizing non-uniformity correction using N3. Neuroimage 48, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, physical exercise, aerobic exercise, MRI, dementia, hippocampus, cortical thickness, intervention

Citation: Frederiksen KS, Larsen CT, Hasselbalch SG, Christensen AN, Høgh P, Wermuth L, Andersen BB, Siebner HR and Garde E (2018) A 16-Week Aerobic Exercise Intervention Does Not Affect Hippocampal Volume and Cortical Thickness in Mild to Moderate Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:293. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00293

Received: 20 June 2018; Accepted: 06 September 2018;
Published: 25 September 2018.

Edited by:

Ashok Kumar, University of Florida, United States

Reviewed by:

Lydia Gimenez-Llort, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain
Belinda Brown, Murdoch University, Australia

Copyright © 2018 Frederiksen, Larsen, Hasselbalch, Christensen, Høgh, Wermuth, Andersen, Siebner and Garde. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kristian Steen Frederiksen, a3Jpc3RpYW4uc3RlZW4uZnJlZGVyaWtzZW5AcmVnaW9uaC5kaw==

These authors have contributed equally to this work and shared first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.