Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 29 November 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Gait Speed and Gait Variability Are Associated with Different Functional Brain Networks

  • 1Institute for Aging Research, Hebrew SeniorLife, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 3Division of Gerontology, Department of Medicine, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 4Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 5Department of Neurology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, MA, United States

Gait speed and gait variability are clinically meaningful markers of locomotor control that are suspected to be regulated by multiple supraspinal control mechanisms. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the relationships between these gait parameters and the functional connectivity of brain networks in functionally limited older adults. Twelve older adults with mild-to-moderate cognition “executive” dysfunction and relatively slow gait, yet free from neurological diseases, completed a gait assessment and a resting-state fMRI. Gait speed and variability were associated with the strength of functional connectivity of different brain networks. Those with faster gait speed had stronger functional connectivity within the frontoparietal control network (R = 0.61, p = 0.04). Those with less gait variability (i.e., steadier walking patterns) exhibited stronger negative functional connectivity between the dorsal attention network and the default network (R = 0.78, p < 0.01). No other significant relationships between gait metrics and the strength of within- or between- network functional connectivity was observed. Results of this pilot study warrant further investigation to confirm that gait speed and variability are linked to different brain networks in vulnerable older adults.

Introduction

Age-related decline in locomotor control often leads to falls and adversely affects one’s quality of life and independence. Locomotor control is most commonly assessed by measuring average preferred gait speed and/or gait variability (i.e., the degree of steadiness about the average of a given stride parameter over consecutive strides). Intriguingly, these two metrics are often uncorrelated (Hollman et al., 2011; Lord et al., 2013) and may be independently influenced by experimental stressors (Hausdorff, 2005, 2007). It seems reasonable to hypothesize, therefore, that gait speed and gait variability may be regulated by fundamentally different functional networks within the brain.

The relationships between metrics of gait and brain function during walking have been challenging to establish primarily because current neuroimaging tools are sensitive to head and body movements (Hamacher et al., 2015; Wittenberg et al., 2017). Alternatively, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) is a powerful tool that enables estimation of functional organization within the brain (Biswal et al., 1995; van den Heuvel and Pol, 2010) and subsequently, determination of how this organization is linked with function and behavior (Lee et al., 2013; Cruz-Gómez et al., 2014; Connolly et al., 2016). Rs-fMRI can be used to identify highly replicable functional networks (Fox and Raichle, 2007; Yeo et al., 2011) and to quantify the patterns of functional connectivity within and between networks (Yeo et al., 2011), providing a reliable and measureable tool to assess cortico-cortical connectivity and its link with complicated human behaviors such as gait (Yuan et al., 2015). Moreover, as rs-fMRI is a “task-free” tool, it minimizes physical movements and avoids confounding from unrelated cortical processes present during the execution of a given task (Yeo et al., 2011).

Recent studies have demonstrated that slow gait speed associates with alterations in the function of the frontoparietal control network (Yuan et al., 2015; Jor’dan et al., 2017) – a network closely linked to executive function. Yuan et al. (2015) reported that functional connectivity within a cluster of frontal and parietal regions was related to gait speed in healthy adults; however, they did not report on the strength or direction of this relationship. Moreover, no studies to date have used rs-fMRI to establish links between fundamentally different properties of gait (i.e., speed and variability) and the functional connectivity of established brain networks. The objective of this study was thus to establish the relationship between clinically important measures of locomotor control and the strength of resting-state functional connectivity within and between functional brain networks in older adults. To accomplish this objective, we performed an analysis of an existing dataset collected from a small sample of ambulatory, non-demented older adults with mild-to-moderate cognitive-motor deficits. We hypothesized that gait speed and variability would be dependent upon distinct functional networks within the brain.

Materials and Methods

We conducted a secondary analysis of baseline data from of a double-blinded, pilot randomized controlled trial on the effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on older adults. Inclusion criteria for that study included men and women who (1) were aged 65 years or older, (2) walked relatively slowly as indicated by a 4 m over-ground preferred walking speed of less than 1.0 m/s (Guralnik et al., 1995), and (3) exhibited mild-to-moderate cognitive “executive” dysfunction as indicated by a Trail Making Test (TMT) B time below the 25th percentile of age- and education-based norms (Tombaugh, 2004). The TMT test is considered as an index of executive function (Arbuthnott and Frank, 2000). In Part A, participants were asked to connect a series of numbers in sequential order on a sheet of paper as quickly and accurately as possible. In Part B, participants were asked to connect numbers or letters in alternating sequence (e.g., 1, A, 2, B, etc.). The time taken to complete each part was recorded. Participants were given up to 300 s to complete each part of the TMT test.

Participants were excluded if they (1) could not stand or ambulate unassisted, (2) had a clinical history of stroke, Parkinson’s disease, or other physician-diagnosed neurological disorders, (3) had a score of 18 or lower on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975; Tombaugh and McIntyre, 1992) (to ensure that enrolled participants were able to understand and complete the study protocol), (4) had self-report of physician-diagnosed schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or other psychiatric illness, (5) had severe depressive symptoms as indicated by a Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) score > 12 (van Marwijk et al., 1995), (6) had severe arthritis or lower-extremity pain, or (7) had physician-diagnosed peripheral neuropathy affecting the lower extremities.

Seventeen of 201 screened individuals were included in the parent study. Of these, 12 participants were eligible for and completed a baseline brain MRI scan and included in this analysis (Mean ±SDage = 76.2 ± 9.5 years; 4 males and 8 females). The five participants who did not complete the MRI were ineligible due to the presence of potentially unsafe ocular implants.

All participants signed an informed consent form and the study was approved by the Hebrew SeniorLife Institutional Review Board.

Data Acquisition and Analysis

Data analyzed in the current study were acquired during a screening visit, a baseline assessment and a functional MRI scan of the brain. Screening tests included MMSE, the TMT Parts A and B, and the Four Meter Walk Test (see inclusion and exclusion criteria above). Eligible participants then completed a gait assessment and resting-state fMRI measurement on two separate days separated by less than a week. Prior to obtaining a gait assessment, we also measured resting blood pressure and heart rate.

Gait Assessment

Participants completed an established protocol (Jor’dan et al., 2017), in which they performed one practice and five official trials of over-ground walking at preferred speed on a 60-foot oval indoor track with a 16-foot GAITRite mat placed along one side (CIR systems, Inc., Franklin, NJ, United States, 100 Hz sampling frequency). Participants walked approximately 1.25 times around the track such that they passed over the mat twice per trial. Across all participants, the fewest number of GAITRite-identified strides was 15. Previous reports have indicated that as few as 10 strides is sufficient for accurate estimation of both gait speed (Hollman et al., 2010) and stride time variability (Perera et al., 2016; Kroneberg et al., 2017). Participant instructions were as follows:

When I say go, walk across the mat and then continue walking until I tell you to stop. Walk at your normal speed, as if you were walking down the street to go to the store”.

Average gait speed (m/s) and stride-to-stride time variability (%) were derived from each trial based upon concatenated footfalls from both passes over the mat. Gait speed was obtained by dividing the distance traveled (over the mat) by time. Gait variability (%) was defined as the coefficient of variation (CoV) about the mean right stride time. We chose to focus on stride time because stride time variability is reliable over time (Hausdorff, 2005; Brach et al., 2008) and sensitive to important health outcomes including falls in older adults (Brach et al., 2001; Hausdorff et al., 2001) and those with neurological disorders (Hausdorff et al., 1998; Sheridan et al., 2003; Balasubramanian et al., 2009). Each gait metric was averaged across the five trials for each participant. Participants were encouraged to rest between each walking trial to avoid potential fatigue. The GAITRite system has demonstrated high concurrent validity and test-retest reliability (McDonough et al., 2001; Bilney et al., 2003).

Resting-State MRI Acquisition and Analysis

Participants completed the MRI within a GE Signa HDxt 3 Tesla system with an 8-channel head coil within the Center for Advanced MR Imaging at the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center. Standard structural imaging was first acquired [MDEFT (Modified Driven Equilibrium Fourier Transform) sequence acquired axially with: 1.000 mm × 0.9375 mm × 0.9375 mm resolution; 6.616 ms TR, 2.84 ms TE; 15° flip angle; 1100 ms inversion time] followed by three 6-min runs of rs-fMRI BOLD sequences (3 mm × 3.75 mm × 3.75 mm, 3.2 s TR, 30 ms TE, 90° flip angle, 52 axial slices). Only two runs were available for three participants and in these cases, outcomes were derived from the two available runs. During the resting-state runs, participants were asked to fixate a cross within the MR bore for the entire duration of the resting run.

Resting-state fMRI were analyzed using a custom combination of software packages as previously described (Eldaief et al., 2011; Yeo et al., 2011; Halko et al., 2014). Acquired data were preprocessed with the following steps: spatial normalization to the MNI template, slice-time correction, motion-correction, and bandpass filtered for low frequency data (<0.1 Hz) spatial smoothing (7 mm FWHM). Ventricles, white matter and the global signal nuisance signals were regressed from the time-series.

After preprocessing, seven networks were identified based on a previously defined parcellation from 1,000 brains (Yeo et al., 2011) and observed spontaneous activity within and between these seven networks. These seven highly replicated networks included: visual, somatomotor, limbic, dorsal attention, ventral attention, frontoparietal control, and default networks. These entire networks were selected as regions of interest to extract time-series. All functional connectivity measures were expressed as z-transformed Pearson correlation coefficients between time-series. For between-network connectivity (e.g., between dorsal attention network and default network), z-transformed Pearson correlation coefficients were computed between time-series from each of the network masks. For within-network connectivity, the mean z-transformed Pearson correlation coefficient was taken of the average time-series with each voxel’s time-series within the network mask. The strength of functional connectivity refers to the magnitude of Pearson correlation coefficients between the fMRI time-series among each spatial location. To create voxelwise maps of network connectivity, z-transformed Pearson correlation coefficients were computed for each voxel against the mean time-course from the network of interest. These maps were inspected to confirm that the spatial organization of these networks were similar to those observed in healthy controls, as can be observed in Figures 1 and 2. For display, these voxelwise maps were projected onto an average cortical surface within the Human Connectome Viewer.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Resting-state functional connectivity of the frontoparietal control network. The standard map (A) of the frontoparietal control network derived from a large sample of healthy adults (Yeo et al., 2011) was used as a functional seed to determine the strength of functional connectivity within this network of the older adults with slow gait and executive dysfunction (B). Warmer colors indicate stronger connectivity. The black outlined region represents the region selected for visualization of the voxel-wise analysis depicted in Figures 4A,B.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Resting-state functional connectivity between dorsal attention network and default network. The standard maps (A) of the dorsal attention network (green) and the default network (red) derived from a large sample of healthy young adults (Yeo et al., 2011) were used as functional seeds to determine the strength of functional connectivity between these networks of the older adults with slow gait and executive dysfunction (B). Warmer colors represent regions with stronger in-phase functional connectivity to the default network; cooler colors represent regions with stronger anti-phase functional connectivity to the default network. The black outlined region represents the region selected for visualization of the voxel-wise analysis depicted in Figures 4C,D.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to summarize participant characteristics and study outcomes including gait speed, gait variability and the strength of both within- and between-network functional connectivity. Bivariate analyses were used to test our primary hypothesis by determining the correlations between gait metrics and functional connectivity outcomes. Those functional connectivity variables that were significantly associated with gait outcomes were then entered into a regression model in order to adjust for participant age. The level of statistical significance for this proof-of-principle analysis was set at 0.05 after adjusting for age. Finally, secondary voxel-wise analyses were performed to localize individual collections of voxels that were correlated with gait metrics. The significant threshold was set at 0.001 to account for multiple comparisons among brain voxels. As this was an exploratory aim, no cluster-wise correction was applied. These secondary analyses enabled us to visually compare the location of voxel clusters and validated network-level relationships between functional connectivity and locomotor control relationships.

Results

The clinical characteristics of study participants were summarized in Table 1. The spatial topography of the network organization of the frontoparietal control network (Figure 1) and default network-dorsal attention network “anticorrelation” (Figure 2) showed similarity to previously described spatial organization of these networks when observed in healthy participants (Fox et al., 2005; Vincent et al., 2008; Yeo et al., 2011). Gait speed (Mean ± SD: 0.74 ± 0.17 m/s) and gait variability (Mean ± SD: 5.07 ± 2.72%) were not significantly correlated with one another (R = -0.24, p = 0.45). These metrics were correlated with the strength of functional connectivity within or between unique brain networks (Figure 3). Specifically, those with faster gait speed had stronger functional connectivity within the frontoparietal control network (R = 0.61, p = 0.04, Figure 3A). This relationship was independent of age (adjusted p = 0.05). Gait speed was not significantly correlated with functional connectivity either within or between any other brain networks. In contrast, those with steadier gait (i.e., less stride time variability) exhibited stronger negative functional connectivity between the dorsal attention network and the default network (R = 0.78, p < 0.01, Figure 3D). In other words, less variable gait was linked to a greater degree of anti-phase correlation in BOLD signals between these two networks. This relationship also remained significant after adjusting for age (adjusted p < 0.01). Gait variability was not correlated with the strength of functional connectivity within the frontoparietal control network (p = 0.97, Figure 3B) or any other analyzed within- or between- network connectivity measure.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Clinical characteristics of study participants.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Gait speed (A,C) and variability (B,D) correlations with resting-state functional connectivity within cognitive networks: frontoparietal control (A,B) and default-dorsal attention (C,D). Participants who walked with greater gait speed tended to have stronger functional connectivity within the frontoparietal control network (A). Those who exhibited less stride time variability tended to have stronger functional connectivity between the dorsal attention network and the default network (greater negative values reflect stronger anti-phase connectivity, D). No other correlations between walking metrics and resting-state network connectivity reached significance (B,C; all other correlations not pictured).

Secondary voxel-wise analyses identified the locations where the strength of functional connectivity correlated with gait speed or variability, confirming a spatial organization consistient with increases within network for gait speed and between-network for gait variability. Several regions within the frontoparietal control network correlated with gait speed were found primarily located within the bilateral middle frontal gyrus [MNI coordinates: +39, +44, +11; -41, +42, 0; -40, +21, +23] (Figures 4A,C). In contrast, a between-network voxelwise correlation was found in the dorsal attention network, when correlating the strength of voxels connectivity with the default network functional connectivity against gait variability (Figures 4B,D). This region was located within the right superior parietal sulcus [MNI coordinates: +30, -48, +53]. None of these clusters remained significant after adjusting for multiple comparisons. However, their locations help confirm that the aforementioned relationships between functional connectivity and gait metrics at the network-level stemmed from more focal, voxel-level relationships within the larger functional networks.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Voxel-wise analysis of within-network (A,B) and between-network (C,D) correlation of functional connectivity with gait speed (A,C) or gait variability (B,D). In each panel, warmer colors represent voxels whose strength of functional connectivity correlated with gait speed or variability, when seeding either the frontoparietal control network (A,B) or the default network (C,D). Black outlines indicate the within-network regions of the frontoparietal control network (A,B) or dorsal attention network (C,D). Within frontoparietal control network, voxelwise functional connectivity is more strongly associated with gait speed (A) than gait variability (B). In contrast, between-network functional connectivity from the default network to the dorsal attention network (black outline) is less strongly associated with gait speed (C) but more strongly associated with gait variability (D).

Discussion

This study examined the relationship among two common metrics of locomotor control and the functional connectivity of established large-scale brain networks in older adults with slow gait and executive dysfunction. The results suggest that gait speed and variability were associated with separate functional brain networks: gait speed was correlated with the strength of functional connectivity within the frontoparietal control network, whereas gait stride time variability was correlated with the strength of anti-phase functional connectivity between the dorsal attention network and the default network. Exploratory voxel-wise analyses further suggest that gait speed was specifically linked to the functional connectivity of the bilateral middle frontal gyri within the frontoparietal control network. Gait variability, on the other hand, was primarily linked to the right superior parietal sulcus within the dorsal attention network.

The frontoparietal control network is critically involved in executive function (Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Reineberg et al., 2015). Executive function is an umbrella term for a series of cognitive processes that give rise to goal-oriented behavior such as response inhibition, shifting of attention and working memory (Testa et al., 2012; Reineberg et al., 2015; Reineberg and Banich, 2016). Each of these subcomponents of executive function is governed by specific brain regions within and beyond the frontoparietal control network (Miyake et al., 2000; Miller and Cohen, 2001; Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Reineberg et al., 2015; Connolly et al., 2016; Reineberg and Banich, 2016). Our observation that those with faster gait speed have stronger functional connectivity within the frontoparietal control network—and particularly the middle frontal gyri—suggests that this widely assessed characteristic of locomotor control depends upon the integrity of communication within a collection of brain regions linked to executive function. This result is supported by previous studies (Yuan et al., 2015; Jor’dan et al., 2017) that gait speed was correlated with functional connectivity of the frontoparietal control network. However, their works either did not specify the strength or direction of this gait speed – brain network relationship (Yuan et al., 2015) or did not investigate the link between gait variability and functional brain networks (Jor’dan et al., 2017). Our results strengthen the notion that gait speed is dependent upon the integrity of the frontal control network. Furthermore, we found that gait variability is correlated with the between-network functional connectivity.

The degree of anti-phase functional connectivity between the dorsal attention and default networks has been linked to one’s ability to allocate attention to a given task and sustain it over time (Esterman et al., 2014; Dixon et al., 2017). Specifically, individuals with stronger anti-phase activity between these networks, as measured during rest, tend to exhibit less “intra-individual variability” in behavioral performance; that is, they have less variation in reaction time to a congruent or incongruent stimulus when presented visually at random time intervals. This outcome is considered an index for how efficiently one is able to allocate and sustain attentional resources (Bellgrove et al., 2004; Kelly et al., 2008) and exaggerated intra-individual variability is often viewed as a hallmark of attentional impairments (West et al., 2002; Kelly et al., 2008; Duchek et al., 2009). Our observations indicate that those with stronger anti-correlated resting brain activity between the dorsal attention network and the default network have less gait variability. Together, these results suggest that gait variability (or steadiness) is at least in part reliant upon one’s ability to sustain their attention over time, and at the physiologic level, dissociate the activity of these two networks. This notion is supported by previous studies demonstrating that as compared to walking under normal conditions, walking while simultaneously completing an attention-demanding task (e.g., mental arithmetic) increases gait variability, especially in those individuals with Parkinson’s disease (Hausdorff et al., 2003; Yogev et al., 2005) or Alzheimer’s disease (Sheridan et al., 2003).

While the current preliminary results provide proof-of-principle that gait speed and gait variability are linked to different brain networks, the small sample size limited our ability to identify more specific brain regions associated with gait metrics. Moreover, participants in this study presented with a common aging phenotype of relatively slow gait and mild-to-moderate cognitive impairment, yet did not suffer from dementia or other major neurological or musculoskeletal disorder. Future studies are warranted with larger sample sizes and a wider range of clinical populations to further identify and delineate relationships between functional connectivity and gait. As hypertension may affect resting-state functional connectivity in older adults (D’Esposito et al., 2003; Son et al., 2015), future studies are needed to examine the role of this and other cardiovascular and cerebrovascular outcomes on the current observed relationships. Moreover, in this study, we only examined gait during walking under normal, quiet conditions. Future studies should thus investigate the relationships between one’s ability to walk while performing a cognitive “dual” task, as related gait outcomes provide an accurate estimation of cognitive excitability (Hobert et al., 2017) and have been linked to future falls and dementia in older adults.

Author Contributions

O-YL: Analysis and interpretation of the data, wrote the manuscript. MH: Analysis of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. JZ and LL: Critical revisions of the manuscript for important intellectual content. RH: Acquisition of data. BM: Study concept and design, study supervision, critical revisions of the manuscript for important intellectual content.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from an NIA training grant (T32-AG023480), an NIA career development grant (K01-AG044543-01A1), an NIA research project grant (R01 AG041785), the Dr. Ralph and Marian Falk Medical Research Trust, and the Boston Claude D. Pepper Older Americans Independence Center (P30-AG013679). LL holds the Irving and Edyth S. Usen Chair in Geriatric Medicine at Hebrew SeniorLife.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alvarez, J. A., and Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 16, 17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbuthnott, K., and Frank, J. (2000). Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 518–528. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasubramanian, C. K., Neptune, R. R., and Kautz, S. A. (2009). Variability in spatiotemporal step characteristics and its relationship to walking performance post-stroke. Gait Posture 29, 408–414. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.10.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellgrove, M. A., Hester, R., and Garavan, H. (2004). The functional neuroanatomical correlates of response variability: evidence from a response inhibition task. Neuropsychologia 42, 1910–1916. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilney, B., Morris, M., and Webster, K. (2003). Concurrent related validity of the GAITRite walkway system for quantification of the spatial and temporal parameters of gait. Gait Posture 17, 68–74.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541.

Google Scholar

Brach, J. S., Berthold, R., Craik, R., VanSwearingen, J. M., and Newman, A. B. (2001). Gait variability in community-dwelling older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 49, 1646–1650.

Google Scholar

Brach, J. S., Perera, S., Studenski, S., and Newman, A. B. (2008). The reliability and validity of measures of gait variability in community-dwelling older adults. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 2293–2296. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2008.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, J., McNulty, J. P., Boran, L., Roche, R. A. P., Delany, D., and Bokde, A. L. W. (2016). Identification of resting state networks involved in executive function. Brain Connect. 6, 365–374. doi: 10.1089/brain.2015.0399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cruz-Gómez,Á. J., Ventura-Campos, N., Belenguer, A.,Ávila, C., and Forn, C. (2014). The link between resting-state functional connectivity and cognition in MS patients. Mult. Scler. 20, 338–348. doi: 10.1177/1352458513495584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Esposito, M., Deouell, L. Y., and Gazzaley, A. (2003). Alterations in the BOLD fMRI signal with ageing and disease: a challenge for neuroimaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 863–872. doi: 10.1038/nrn1246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Spreng, R. N., Irving, Z. C., Mills, C., Girn, M., et al. (2017). Interactions between the default network and dorsal attention network vary across default subsystems, time, and cognitive states. Neuroimage 147, 632–649. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchek, J. M., Balota, D. A., Tse, C.-S., Holtzman, D. M., Fagan, A. M., and Goate, A. M. (2009). The utility of intraindividual variability in selective attention tasks as an early marker for Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 23, 746–758. doi: 10.1037/a0016583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eldaief, M. C., Halko, M. A., Buckner, R. L., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2011). Transcranial magnetic stimulation modulates the brain’s intrinsic activity in a frequency-dependent manner. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 21229–21234. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113103109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esterman, M., Rosenberg, M. D., and Noonan, S. K. (2014). Intrinsic fluctuations in sustained attention and distractor processing. J. Neurosci. 34, 1724–1730. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2658-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.

Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guralnik, J. M., Ferrucci, L., Simonsick, E. M., Salive, M. E., and Wallace, R. B. (1995). Lower-extremity function in persons over the age of 70 years as a predictor of subsequent disability. N. Engl. J. Med. 332, 556–561. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199503023320902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halko, M. A., Farzan, F., Eldaief, M. C., Schmahmann, J. D., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2014). Intermittent theta-burst stimulation of the lateral cerebellum increases functional connectivity of the default network. J. Neurosci. 34, 12049–12056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1776-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamacher, D., Herold, F., Wiegel, P., Hamacher, D., and Schega, L. (2015). Brain activity during walking: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 310–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M. (2005). Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2:19. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-2-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). Gait dynamics, fractals and falls: finding meaning in the stride-to-stride fluctuations of human walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 555–589. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Balash, J., and Giladi, N. (2003). Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 16, 53–58. doi: 10.1177/0891988702250580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Cudkowicz, M. E., Firtion, R., Wei, J. Y., and Goldberger, A. L. (1998). Gait variability and basal ganglia disorders: stride-to-stride variations of gait cycle timing in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Mov. Disord. 13, 428–437. doi: 10.1002/mds.870130310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Rios, D. A., and Edelberg, H. K. (2001). Gait variability and fall risk in community-living older adults: a 1-year prospective study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 82, 1050–1056. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.24893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobert, M. A., Meyer, S. I., Hasmann, S. E., Metzger, F. G., Suenkel, U., Eschweiler, G. W., et al. (2017). Gait is associated with cognitive flexibility: a dual-tasking study in healthy older people. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:154. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollman, J. H., Childs, K. B., McNeil, M. L., Mueller, A. C., Quilter, C. M., and Youdas, J. W. (2010). Number of strides required for reliable measurements of pace, rhythm and variability parameters of gait during normal and dual task walking in older individuals. Gait Posture 32, 23–28. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollman, J. H., McDade, E. M., and Petersen, R. C. (2011). Normative spatiotemporal gait parameters in older adults. Gait Posture 34, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jor’dan, A. J., Poole, V. N., Iloputaife, I., Milberg, W., Manor, B., Esterman, M., et al. (2017). Executive network activation is linked to walking speed in older adults: functional MRI and TCD ultrasound evidence from the MOBILIZE Boston study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 72, 1669–1675. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, A. M. C., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., Castellanos, F. X., and Milham, M. P. (2008). Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage 39, 527–537. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroneberg, D., Elshehabi, M., Meyer, A.-C., Doss, S., Kühn, A., Maetzler, W., et al. (2017). How many steps are enough? Assessment of gait variability in realistically confined clinical settings. Basal Ganglia 8, 3–4. doi: 10.1016/j.baga.2017.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. H., Smyser, C. D., and Shimony, J. S. (2013). Resting-state fMRI: a review of methods and clinical applications. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 34, 1866–1872. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S., Galna, B., Verghese, J., Coleman, S., Burn, D., and Rochester, L. (2013). Independent domains of gait in older adults and associated motor and nonmotor attributes: validation of a factor analysis approach. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 820–827. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonough, A. L., Batavia, M., Chen, F. C., Kwon, S., and Ziai, J. (2001). The validity and reliability of the GAITRite system’s measurements: a preliminary evaluation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 82, 419–425. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.19778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perera, S., Smith, C., Coffman, L., and Brach, J. (2016). Number of steps needed for reliable gait variability measurement. Gerontologist 56, 335–336. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw162.1366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reineberg, A. E., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Depue, B. E., Friedman, N. P., and Banich, M. T. (2015). Resting-state networks predict individual differences in common and specific aspects of executive function. Neuroimage 104, 69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reineberg, A. E., and Banich, M. T. (2016). Functional connectivity at rest is sensitive to individual differences in executive function: a network analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2959–2975. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheridan, P. L., Solomont, J., Kowall, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2003). Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 51, 1633–1637.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Son, S. J., Kim, J., Lee, E., Park, J. Y., Namkoong, K., Hong, C. H., et al. (2015). Effect of hypertension on the resting-state functional connectivity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 60, 210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2014.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Testa, R., Bennett, P., and Ponsford, J. (2012). Factor analysis of nineteen executive function tests in a healthy adult population. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 27, 213–224. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N., and McIntyre, N. J. (1992). The mini-mental state examination: a comprehensive review. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 40, 922–935.

Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., and Pol, H. E. H. (2010). Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 519–534. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Marwijk, H. W., Wallace, P., de Bock, G. H., Hermans, J., Kaptein, A. A., and Mulder, J. D. (1995). Evaluation of the feasibility, reliability and diagnostic value of shortened versions of the geriatric depression scale. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 45, 195–199.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I. M., and Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain Cogn. 49, 402–419.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wittenberg, E., Thompson, J., Nam, C. S., and Franz, J. R. (2017). Neuroimaging of human balance control: a systematic review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:170. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogev, G., Giladi, N., Peretz, C., Springer, S., Simon, E. S., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2005). Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 1248–1256. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, J., Blumen, H. M., Verghese, J., and Holtzer, R. (2015). Functional connectivity associated with gait velocity during walking and walking-while-talking in aging: a resting-state fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 1484–1493. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gait, resting-state fMRI, functional connectivity, gait speed, gait variability

Citation: Lo O-Y, Halko MA, Zhou J, Harrison R, Lipsitz LA and Manor B (2017) Gait Speed and Gait Variability Are Associated with Different Functional Brain Networks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:390. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00390

Received: 10 July 2017; Accepted: 13 November 2017;
Published: 29 November 2017.

Edited by:

Philip P. Foster, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States

Reviewed by:

Graham J. Galloway, Translational Research Institute, Australia
Richard B. Reilly, Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland

Copyright © 2017 Lo, Halko, Zhou, Harrison, Lipsitz and Manor. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: On-Yee Lo, YW15bG9AaHNsLmhhcnZhcmQuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.