Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 08 November 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Multimodal and Longitudinal Bioimaging Methods for Characterizing the Progressive Course of Dementia View all 15 articles

Disrupted Thalamus White Matter Anatomy and Posterior Default Mode Network Effective Connectivity in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment

  • 1Intelligent Systems Research Centre, University Ulster, Derry, United Kingdom
  • 2Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience and Cognitive Systems Group, Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 3Mercer’s Institute for Research on Ageing, St. James’s Hospital, Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 4St. Patrick’s Hospital, Dublin, Ireland

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and its prodromal state amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) are characterized by widespread abnormalities in inter-areal white matter fiber pathways and parallel disruption of default mode network (DMN) resting state functional and effective connectivity. In healthy subjects, DMN and task positive network interaction are modulated by the thalamus suggesting that abnormal task-based DMN deactivation in aMCI may be a consequence of impaired thalamo-cortical white matter circuitry. Thus, this article uses a multimodal approach to assess white matter integrity between thalamus and DMN components and associated effective connectivity in healthy controls (HCs) relative to aMCI patients. Twenty-six HC and 20 older adults with aMCI underwent structural, functional and diffusion MRI scanning using the high angular resolution diffusion-weighted acquisition protocol. The DMN of each subject was identified using independent component analysis (ICA) and resting state effective connectivity was calculated between thalamus and DMN nodes. White matter integrity changes between thalamus and DMN were investigated with constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD) tractography. Significant structural deficits in thalamic white matter projection fibers to posterior DMN components posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and lateral inferior parietal lobe (IPL) were identified together with significantly reduced effective connectivity from left thalamus to left IPL. Crucially, impaired thalamo-cortical white matter circuitry correlated with memory performance. Disrupted thalamo-cortical structure was accompanied by significant reductions in IPL and PCC cortico-cortical effective connectivity. No structural deficits were found between DMN nodes. Abnormal posterior DMN activity may be driven by changes in thalamic white matter connectivity; a view supported by the close anatomical and functional association of thalamic nuclei effected by AD pathology and the posterior DMN nodes. We conclude that dysfunctional posterior DMN activity in aMCI is consistent with disrupted cortico-thalamo-cortical processing and thalamic-based dissemination of hippocampal disease agents to cortical hubs.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a chronic neurodegenerative disorder affecting approximately 6% of people over the age of 65 and accounting for 60%–70% of dementia cases (Burns and Iliffe, 2009). Typically, the AD-prodromal stage presents as mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Stephan et al., 2012) clinically defined as cognitive difficulties beyond those expected based on age and education, but insufficient to interfere with daily activities (Petersen et al., 1999; Petersen, 2004). MCI can present with a variety of symptoms but is termed amnestic MCI (aMCI) in cases where memory loss is the predominant symptom.

In AD, the first neurofibrillary tangles appear in the parahippocampal regions (Stage I) followed later, and accompanied by cognitive symptoms, in the hippocampus formation (stage III; Braak and Braak, 1991a,b, 1995). Understandably, this knowledge has reinforced focus on the hippocampus in the context of memory loss in AD but much less well-known and less well-understood are the appearance of tangles and plaques in the thalamic nuclei in parallel with those in the hippocampus. Their appearance is often characterized as an event downstream of the hippocampus pathology transmitted by the projections of the mammillary bodies, but this view is challenged by metabolic studies indicating that the earliest consistent declines occur not in hippocampus but in posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; Minoshima et al., 1994, 1997) where amyloid deposition is highest (Buckner et al., 2005; Mintun et al., 2006). The thalamus, with its dense network of reciprocal interconnections with both hippocampus and PCC, is therefore implicated by association (Vann et al., 2009; Aggleton et al., 2010).

Such a view is supported by detection of thalamic atrophy in pre-symptomatic familial AD on average 5.6 years prior to expected symptom onset (Ryan et al., 2013) together with increased amyloid burden (Knight et al., 2011a,b) and substantial evidence suggesting that thalamic atrophy is present in MCI prior to AD (Chételat et al., 2005; Shiino et al., 2006; de Jong et al., 2008; Ferrarini et al., 2008; Cherubini et al., 2010; Roh et al., 2011; Pedro et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2013). Structural irregularities have a sufficient impact on thalamo-cortical circuits to allow healthy subjects to be differentiated from those with MCI through impaired functional integrity (Cantero et al., 2009). Conversely, carriers of the apolipoprotein ε2 allele i.e., those showing a genetic predisposition against developing AD, demonstrate significantly enhanced functional (Patel et al., 2013) and structural (Chiang et al., 2012) integrity of the thalamus.

Analysis of low frequency BOLD signal oscillations have revealed several resting state networks. Of these, the default mode network (DMN; Raichle et al., 2001; Greicius et al., 2003; Damoiseaux et al., 2006) has consistently been identified as dysfunctional in both MCI and AD in the context of amyloid burden (Hedden et al., 2009; Drzezga et al., 2011; Mormino et al., 2011; Sheline et al., 2011) and genetic risk (Roses, 1996; Sheline et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2012; Chhatwal et al., 2013).

The DMN comprises medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), lateral inferior parietal lobes (IPL), PCC and hippocampus regions. These nodes have been identified as important hubs within the cortex (Buckner et al., 2009) whose persistent background activity and dense, long range interconnectivity may facilitate the early deposition and prion-like transmission of amyloid plaques (Wermke et al., 2008; Raj et al., 2012). DMN topography is therefore recapitulated in the pattern of atrophy, hypometabolism and amyloid deposition within the cortex (Buckner et al., 2005, 2008).

Thalamus appears to play a role modulating distributed cortical networks (Di and Biswal, 2014). It is therefore of note, that direct structural connections between the thalamus and DMN (or thalamo-DMN pathway) components have been described in vivo using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI; Fernández-Espejo et al., 2012) and that these are sites of atrophy (Zarei et al., 2010). Crucially, lesions to the thalamus are known to cause DMN dysfunction (Jones et al., 2011). One suggestion is that abnormal task-induced deactivation of DMN response patterns in aMCI are a consequence of impaired thalamo-cortical signaling (Pihlajamäki and Sperling, 2009).

The thalamus sends widespread connections to its ipsilateral cortical hemisphere which are returned via cortico-thalamic feedback connections. Together these form a thalamo-cortico-thalamic feedback loop (Sherman and Guillery, 2006; Sherman, 2007; Zhang et al., 2008, 2010). Such an arrangement is critical for generating the ubiquitous oscillations of the cortex recorded by EEG and fMRI but its contribution (and other subcortical components) to regulating the DMN in health and disease is largely unexplored. On this basis, we chose to investigate the impact of impaired thalamo-cortical microscopic white matter anatomy on interactions in the DMN in aMCI patients.

We performed constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD)-based probabilistic fiber tractography of the thalamo-DMN white matter pathways in a cohort of older adults with aMCI and healthy age-matched controls. We also examined the effective connectivity of the resting state thalamo-DMN interactions using a spatio-temporal formulation of Granger Causality (GC). In contrast to simple statistical correlation (i.e., functional connectivity), effective connectivity is more ambitious and attempts to quantify the causal influence one region exerts over another. Given that thalamo-cortical neural signals appear to coordinate distributed networks (Di and Biswal, 2014), such an approach provides greater scope for clarifying the interactions between thalamus and cortex during the transition between health and disease. We predicted that abnormal DMN causal activity would be linked to structural deficits in the thalamo-DMN pathway.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty six HC participants and 20 older participants with aMCI took part in the study. The HCs were community-dwelling older adults recruited from the greater Dublin area (Ireland) via newspaper advertisements. They underwent a health screening questionnaire and a neuropsychological assessment, the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD; Morris et al., 1989), in order to rule out possible cognitive impairment before inclusion in the study. The CERAD battery has been shown to be sensitive to the presence of age related cognitive decline (Welsh et al., 1991, 1992). All of the older participants included in the study scored no more than 1.5 SD below the standardized mean scores for subjects of a similar age and education level on any of the sub-tests. The aMCI participants were recruited from memory clinics in St. James Hospital and St. Patrick’s Hospital in Dublin, Ireland, and were diagnosed by a clinician according to the Peterson criteria (Petersen et al., 1999)—i.e., abnormal memory scores for age and education level with no dementia. Four were single amnestic aMCI, and 16 were multi-domain aMCI (Petersen, 2004). Neuropsychological measures were administered or supervised by an experienced neuropsychologist and included the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) and Cambridge cognitive examination (Huppert et al., 1995).

All of the participants were right-handed with no history of head trauma, neurological disease, stroke, transient ischemic attack, heart attack, or psychiatric illness. They completed the Geriatric Depression Scale (GSD; Yesavage et al., 1983), the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised Edition Short Scale (EPQ-R; Eysenck and Eysenck, 1994), and a Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire (Rami et al., 2011) before the MRI scan (Table 1). The groups did not differ in terms of age, gender, education level, or levels of cognitive reserve as assessed by the self-report Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire. The aMCI group had lower MMSE scores, higher GDS scores, and scored lower on the EPQ measure of extraversion than the HC group. The study had full ethical approval from the St. James Hospital and the Adelaide and Meath Hospital, incorporating the National Children’s Hospital Research Ethics Committee and St. Patrick’s University Hospital Research Ethics Committee. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of independent samples t-tests, except for gender which was compared with a Fischer’s exact test.

MRI Data Acquisition

Whole-brain high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI) data were acquired on a 3.0 Tesla Philips Intera MR system (Best, Netherlands) equipped with an eight channel head coil. A parallel sensitivity encoding (SENSE) approach (Pruessmann et al., 1999) with a reduction factor of two was used during the diffusion weighted image (DWI) acquisition. Single-shot spin echo-planar imaging was used to acquire the DWI data with the following parameters: echo time (TE) = 79 ms, repetition time (TR) = = 20,000 ms, field of view (FOV) = 248 mm, matrix = 112 × 112, isotropic voxel of 2.3 mm × 2.3 mm × 2.3 mm, and 65 slices with 2.3 mm thickness with no gap between the slices. Diffusion gradients were applied in 61 isotropically distributed orientations with b = 3000 s/mm2, and four images with b = 0 s/mm2 were also acquired. A high-resolution 3D T1-weighted anatomical image was acquired for each participant with the following parameters: TE = 3.9 ms, TR = 8.5 ms, FOV = 230 mm, slice thickness = 0.9 mm, voxel size = 0.9 mm × 0.9 mm × 0.9 mm. Resting-state fMRI data were also acquired during the scanning session. The scan lasted for 7 min during which time the participants were asked to keep their eyes open and fixate on a cross hairs in the center of a screen behind the MR scanner, visible via a mirror. The BOLD signal changes were measured using a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence with TE = 30 ms and TR = 2000 ms. Each volume of data covered the entire brain with 39 slices, and the slices were acquired in interleaved sequence from inferior to superior direction. Two-hundred and ten volumes of data were acquired, with voxel dimensions of 3.5 mm × 3.5 mm × 3.85 mm and a 0.35 mm gap between the slices.

Face-Name Encoding and Recognition Task Protocol

Relationships between the participants’ structural/effective connectivity measures and memory were subsequently examined using data obtained from a face–name recognition task following the resting-state scan. The participants viewed a series of 27 emotional faces (Erwin et al., 1992) with a name presented underneath each one. This task was an implicit memory task, in that the participants later completed a surprise memory tasks to test their retention of both the faces and the face–name pairs, however, at the time of encoding, they were not explicitly asked to remember the face–name pairs. Rather, the participants were instructed to judge whether the names matched or suited the faces. It was explained that this was a subjective decision, with no right or wrong answer. The participants responded yes or no by pressing a button on a MR-compatible response pad held in their right or left hand, respectively, using the index finger of either hand. Each face–name combination was presented for 4 s and was shown twice during the run. The faces were positive, negative, or neutral in valence and there were equal numbers of valence types as well as gender. The presentation of the face–name pairs was grouped according to the emotional valence of the faces. In each instance, a group of either two, three, or four faces of one valence type was presented randomly using an event-related paradigm, subsequently, there was a delay during which a white cross hair was presented (control condition). The duration of the white cross was varied according to the duration of the face stimulus. For instance, if a single face was presented for 4 s the subsequent white cross was also shown for 4 s and then the next block of faces began. The stimuli were delivered using Presentation v.16.1 (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA, USA).

Approximately 15 min following the encoding phase, the participants performed a short computer-based recognition task. The emotional faces were presented one at a time on a black background with three names underneath. One of the names was the correct name; one name was a name that had been paired with a different face (distractor; incorrect name), while the third name was a new name (foil; incorrect name). The participants responded by pressing a button on the left, middle, or right side of a keyboard to correspond with the relative position of the name on the screen. The stimuli were presented for 5 s and followed by an inter-trial interval of 5 s. This longer trial length was to facilitate performance of this task as it was quite challenging. Before the task began, the participants completed a short practice run of five trials.

Resting State Pre-Processing

FMRI data processing was carried out using FMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) Version 6.00, part of FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL)1. Registration to high resolution structural and/or standard space images was carried out using FNIRT (Andersson et al., 2007). The following pre-statistics processing was applied; motion correction using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002), slice-timing correction using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting, non-brain removal using BET (Smith, 2002), spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 3.0 mm, grand-mean intensity normalization of the entire 4D dataset by a single multiplicative factor, highpass temporal filtering (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, with sigma = 50.0 s).

Resting State Effective Connectivity

GC is a standard statistical tool for detecting the directional influence one system component exerts over another. The concept, originally introduced by Wiener (1956), and later incorporated into a data analysis framework by Granger (1969) is described as follows. If historical information from time series X significantly improves prediction accuracy of the future of time series Y in a multivariate autoregressive model (MVAR), GC is identified. This may be viewed as a measure of model prediction error where GC quantifies the reduction in prediction error when past values of X are included in the explanatory variables of Y (Schelter et al., 2006). By fitting a time invariant MVAR model to the experimental time series the classic GC formulation ignores crucial time-varying properties of the system. Such an approach makes the tacit assumption that the longer the time series, the more reliable the GC estimates. While this may be correct in static circuit representations (Smith et al., 2011), under time-varying conditions this principle is no longer valid. A more robust method is to divide the time series into equal windows and consider them separately. Here, an optimal trade-off between the length of the time windows and the accuracy of the estimated coefficients for each window must be determined. Time windows that are too short prevent the accurate estimation of parameters, while time windows that are too long increase the probability of incorrect inferences of GC. Accordingly, the current article utilizes a novel spatio-temporal GC formulation to quantify the effective connectivity changes between region of interest (ROI; Luo et al., 2013). In this framework, finding the optimal time window length reduces to the solution of a constrained optimization problem,

minl0(m)(GCerr(l0(m))+1GCavg(l0(m)))

where we seek to simultaneously minimize model prediction error GCerr (i.e., the weighted average of the variances of the residuals in each time window) and maximize detected causality information GCavg (i.e., the average GC over all time windows). This is performed for time windows of different length lo(m) = t1, …, tm. The time window producing the lowest Bayesian information criterion (BIC) is considered optimal. By considering optimal time windows, the spatio-temporal framework allows a more reliable and precise estimate of GC in experimental datasets with time varying properties. This approach has been shown to yield more accurate estimates of GC on resting state fMRI data than traditional GC metrics. In this case, the last 208 time points for each region under consideration were extracted from the functional image volume and divided into four windows with the first two time points removed to avoid start up transients. In terms of spatial resolution, GC is calculated between all pairs of voxels from the two ROI under consideration. The mean GC among all pairs of voxels was then used as the final estimate.

CSD White Matter Tractography Using MRtrix3

A method for controlling free water contamination of tissue and the resultant partial volume effects is especially important around the fornix where atrophy and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) is prevalent. The free water elimination technique (Pasternak et al., 2009) has been successfully applied in previous tractography studies of ageing and aMCI (Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2012a,b; Fletcher et al., 2014; Kehoe et al., 2015) however at higher b-values the Gaussian assumption underlying the bi-tensor model is no longer valid and a more simple heuristic is indicated. Accordingly, we use the standard free water elimination approach to identify and mask voxels with high free water content but fit the conventional DTI model to each voxel.

The dwipreproc preprocessing script was to perform eddy current-induced distortion and motion correction using the FSL tool eddy (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016). The standard MRtrix3 processing script dwibiascorrect was used to eliminate low frequency intensity inhomogeneities across the DWI series. The script uses bias field correction algorithms available in the FSL software package (Zhang et al., 2001).

Probabilistic white matter tractography was performed on the DWIs using the MRtrix3 software package2. Crossing fibers were resolved using the CSD algorithm (Tournier et al., 2004, 2012). MRtrix3 pre-processing included computing the diffusion tensors images (or diffusion ellipsoids) for each voxel from which the fractional anisotropy (FA), axial (DA), radial (RD) and mean (MD) diffusivity.

Whole-brain tractography was performed using every voxel as a seed point. The principle diffusion orientation at each point was estimated by the CSD tractography algorithm, which propagated in 0.1 mm steps along this direction. At each new location the fiber orientation(s) was estimated before the tracking moved a further 0.1 mm along the direction that subtended the smallest angle to the current trajectory. A trajectory was followed through the data until the scaled height of the fiber orientation density function peak dropped below the default threshold, or the direction of the pathway changed through an angle of more than 90°.

Anatomical masks were used to divide the results into circumscribed regions. The DMN was defined by probabilistic template (Wang et al., 2014) and the hippocampus and thalamus using the Harvard-Oxford subcortical structural atlas (Figure 1). Streamlines beginning in one mask and terminating in another were considered in a pairwise fashion for all ROI. In addition, the FSL tool FMRIB’s Automated Segmentation Tool (FAST) was used to derive a white matter brain mask to constrain tractography. Any tracks tracts exiting the white matter were considered spurious and discarded. Tracts were prevented from propagating between hemispheres by a stop region placed down the midline corresponding to the corpus callosum.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Regions of interest moving from inferior (top left) to superior (bottom right) defining thalamo-DMN white matter tractography masks. DMN components mPFC, MTG, IPL, and PCC were defined using probabilistic template (Wang et al., 2014) while thalamus and hippocampus were defined using the Harvard-Oxford subcortical structural atlas.

Statistically significant differences in the mean FA, DA, RD and MD of tracks in HC vs. aMCI were tested by way of a two tailed two sample t-test at p < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons.

Independent Component Analysis (ICA)

The DMN was identified for each subject using ICA. Analysis was carried out using Probabilistic ICA (Beckmann and Smith, 2004) as implemented in Multivariate Exploratory Linear Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC) Version 3.14, part of FSL. The following data pre-processing was applied to the input data: masking of non-brain voxels, voxel-wise de-meaning of the data, normalization of the voxel-wise variance. Pre-processed data were whitened and projected into a 62-dimensional subspace using probabilistic Principal Component Analysis where the number of dimensions was estimated using the Laplace approximation to the Bayesian evidence of the model order (Minka, 2001; Beckmann and Smith, 2004). The whitened observations were decomposed into sets of vectors which describe signal variation across the temporal domain (time-courses) and across the spatial domain (maps) by optimizing for non-Gaussian spatial source distributions using a fixed-point iteration technique (Hyvärinen, 1999). Estimated component maps were divided by the standard deviation of the residual noise and thresholded by fitting a mixture model to the histogram of intensity values (Beckmann and Smith, 2004). The number of components was automatically estimated. The component corresponding to the DMN was selected by cross correlating all the components with a probabilistic DMN template (Wang et al., 2014). The fMRI BOLD signal was extracted from DMN components mPFC, MTG, IPL and PCC, combined with those extracted from hippocampus and thalamus masks, and analyzed using the spatio-temporal GC method to determine the effective connectivity. A standard two tailed t-test was used to determine significant differences between the HC and aMCI patients at p < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons.

Results

Comparison of Resting State Thalamo-DMN Effective Connectivity in HC vs. aMCI Subjects

The spatio-temporal GC effective connectivity analysis revealed significant differences in a circumscribed set of regions at the Bonferroni corrected threshold of p < 0.0014.

In aMCI, several incoming connections to PCC and left IPL showed reduced casual connectivity. An especially pronounced decrease in causal interaction to left IPL from other DMN components, hippocampus, and thalamus was observed (Figure 2A). Reduced connectivity to left IPL included incoming connections from left thalamus (t(44) = 3.77, p < 0.001), left (t(44) = 4.3, p < 0.0001) and right (t(44) = 3.80, p < 0.001) MTG and from right IPL (t(44) = 3.83 p < 0.001). These changes correspond to a highly significant (t(44) = 5.10, p < 0.00001) decrease in average FA in the white matter between left thalamus and left IPL (Figure 2B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Significantly reduced incoming effective connectivity to left IPL from thalamus and posterior DMN nodes. (B) Significantly reduced FA in thalamo-IPL tracts where the magnitude of reduction corresponded to the degree of effective connectivity disruption in (A). (C) Significantly reduced DA in the left Papez circuit including hippocampo-thalamus, thalamo-PCC and PCC-hippocampal tracts. (D) As in (C), significantly reduced MD in the left Papez circuit. (E) Significantly reduced RD in left hippocampo-PCC tracts.

Also in aMCI, significant reductions in connectivity to PCC from left MTG (t(44) = 3.93, p < 0.001) were found, together with significant reductions in connectivity to right IPL from PCC (t(44) = 3.73, p < 0.001) (Figure 2A).

Comparison of Thalamo-DMN Microstructural Integrity in HC vs. aMCI Subjects

In aMCI, CSD white matter tractography identified statistically significant increases at the Bonferroni corrected threshold in average DA, RD and MD in the white matter fiber pathways connecting thalamus to hippocampus, PCC and IPL (Figure 3). Significant decreases in average FA were also detected in the white matter between thalamus and IPL. These included:

Significant decreases in FA (Figure 2B) between right thalamus and right IPL (t(44) = 3.81, p < 0.001) and between left thalamus and left IPL (t(44) = 5.24, p < 0.00001).

Significant increases in DA (Figure 2C) between right thalamus and right hippocampus (t(44) = −4.68, p < 0.0001), right hippocampus and PCC (t(44) = −4.02, p < 0.001), left thalamus and left hippocampus (t(44) = −5.33, p < 0.00001), left thalamus and PCC (t(44) = −3.91, p < 0.001), left hippocampus and left IPL (t(44) = −4.13 p < 0.001), and left hippocampus and PCC (t(44) = −3.91, p < 0.001).

These changes were recapitulated in the MD metric (Figure 2D) with significant increases between right hippocampus and PCC (t(44) = −3.69, p < 0.001), left thalamus and left hippocampus (t(44) = −4.34, p < 0.0001), left thalamus and PCC (t(44) < −3.63, p = 0.001) and left hippocampus and PCC (t(44) = −4.17, p < 0.001).

Finally, a significant increase in RD (Figure 2E) between left hippocampus and PCC (t(44) = −3.66, p < 0.001) was also found.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Example thalamo-DMN white matter tracts from a single representative healthy subject.

Empirical Measures of Effective and Structural Connectivity Predict Memory Performance

To investigate whether empirical measures of effective and structural connectivity relate to memory, we regressed the diffusivity and GC metrics against the results from a face-name encoding and recognition task using gender, age and motion parameter estimates as covariates of no interest.

The aMCI cohort displayed a significant negative correlation between the integrity of the left thalamo-cortical white matter connectivity and memory in three DMN regions (Figure 4A) including IPL (t(24) = −2.43, p < 0.05), hippocampus (t(24) = −2.31, p < 0.05), and PCC (t(24) = −2.21, p < 0.05). The healthy subjects displayed no such relationship.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Significant association between structural integrity of thalamo-cortical white matter pathways and memory performance in aMCI subjects. (B) Significant association between inferior parietal lobe effective connectivity and memory performance in HCs.

Conversely, the healthy cohort demonstrated a significant negative correlation between the effective connectivity of IPL and memory and three other DMN regions (Figure 4B) including left MTG (t(24) = −2.47, p < 0.05), right IPL (t(24) = −2.54, p < 0.05), and PCC (t(24) = −2.21, p < 0.05). The same relationship was absent in the aMCI cohort. All results survived multiple-comparison correction with FDR (q < 0.1).

Discussion

The appearance of atrophy, tangles and plaques in thalamus is often characterized as a secondary process resulting from atrophy in the hippocampus and the prion-like transmission of pathology along the white matter topography (Raj et al., 2012). But such a view is inconsistent with evidence suggesting that the earliest metabolic changes occur not in hippocampus but in posterior DMN node PCC. Thus, structural deficits in thalamus may be driving early PCC hypometabolism and initiating the cascade of DMN functional anomalies typically associated with early AD. Accordingly, we used a multimodal approach to assess the impact of thalamo-cortico-thalamic feedback loop integrity on DMN functionality in aMCI.

We found significant structural abnormalities in the thalamo-PCC and thalamo-IPL white matter fiber pathways in the aMCI cohort (Figures 2B–E). A pronounced reduction in left thalamo-IPL effective connectivity (Figure 2A) corresponded with significant thalamo-IPL structural impairment (Figure 2B). Critically, the integrity of thalamic white matter and memory was correlated in the aMCI cohort but not in the HCs (Figure 4A). In general, the gradient of structural impairment followed a hippocampo-thalamo-PCC axis consistent with a prion-like dissemination of pathology (Raj et al., 2012) along the major white matter fiber pathways of the Papez circuit (Papez, 1937). No structural abnormalities were identified between cortical DMN components mPFC, MTG, IPL and PCC, however significant disruption to incoming IPL effective connectivity was observed (Figure 2A), and this distinguished HC and aMCI memory performance (Figure 4B).

Overall, our findings are broadly suggestive. One interpretation is that disrupted effective connectivity in posterior DMN nodes PCC and IPL is, to some extent, inspired by incipient thalamo-cortical deafferentation. If true, this finding may help explain abnormal task-induced DMN response patterns typically found in aMCI and AD subjects (Pihlajamäki and Sperling, 2009).

Impaired Hippocampo-Thalamo-PCC White Matter Anatomy and Abnormal PCC Effective Connectivity

The current article identified significant structural impairment between fiber pathways connecting hippocampus and thalamus (Figures 2C,D), thalamus and PCC (Figures 2C,D), and PCC and hippocampus (Figures 2C–E). Measures of left thalamo-cortical structural integrity (including tracts to hippocampus and PCC) correlated with memory performance in the aMCI cohort but not in the HCs (Figure 4A).

Impaired structural relations within the hippocampo-thalamo-PCC complex are likely mediated by their close anatomical association. Together these structures comprise a limbic-diencephalic memory network (Nestor et al., 2003) connected through the circuit of Papez (1937). This structure runs from hippocampus through fornix to anterior thalamus via mammillary bodies and onto PCC before returning to hippocampus to complete the circuit. Interestingly, the current study identified a decreasing gradient of structural impairment between hippocampus, thalamus, and PCC, suggesting that structural deafferentation of PCC through impaired hippocampus and thalamus fiber pathways, likely stems from pathology and atrophy originating in the hippocampal complex. Such a view is consistent with postmortem studies indicating that thalamic nuclei connected to hippocampus are a site of primary degeneration in AD (Xuereb et al., 1991).

Previous work has highlighted a staged disconnection process occurring both along the cingulum bundle between hippocampus and PCC (i.e., the direct route) and within the memory circuit of Papez encompassing thalamic intermediaries (i.e., the indirect route; Villain et al., 2008). Such findings are consistent with early PCC hypometabolism (Matsuda, 2001; Valla et al., 2001; Mosconi et al., 2008; Zhu et al., 2013; Mutlu et al., 2016) where it frequently presents before clinical diagnosis (Minoshima et al., 1997; Johnson et al., 1998) as part of a constellation of metabolic effects focused around medial temporal lobe and thalamus, when memory loss is still a relatively isolated feature (Nestor et al., 2003). Interestingly, PCC hypometabolism appears to correlate with remote hippocampus atrophy early in MCI but transition to both remote and local effects over the course of progression to AD (Teipel and Grothe, 2016).

The current study identified a significant correlation between the structural integrity of hippocampo-thalamus and thalamo-PCC fiber pathways (i.e., the indirect route) and memory in the aMCI cohort which was absent in the HCs (Figure 4A). A similar pattern was identified in the hippocampo-PCC fiber pathway (i.e., the direct route) however this did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Dysfunction of structures along the hippocampal output pathways to PCC have been linked to episodic memory impairment (Yakushev et al., 2011).

The PCC’s hub status (Hagmann et al., 2008) may predispose to amyloid deposition, atrophy, and hypometabolism (Buckner et al., 2005, 2009) where remote often diffuse damage accumulates as altered PCC connectivity through a form of diaschisis (Meguro et al., 1999; Leech and Sharp, 2014). One suggestion is that direct thalamo-PCC (Figures 2C,D) and distal thalamo-IPL (Figure 2B) white matter structural deficits operate in tandem to initiate a cascade of aberrant effective connectivity in PCC (Figure 2A). Taken together, these findings are consistent with a progressive disconnection of PCC from downstream cortical and subcortical sources with differential effects operating on the direct vs. indirect hippocampo-PCC pathways.

Impaired Thalamo-IPL White Matter Anatomy and Abnormal IPL Effective Connectivity

The current article identified significant impairments in thalamic white matter circuitry serving bilateral IPL where the magnitude of diffusivity change (Figure 2B) correlated with the intensity and extent of effective connectivity disruption in each hemisphere (Figure 2A).

Marked structural deficits were observed in left thalamo-IPL white matter connectivity together with significantly reduced effective connectivity from left thalamus. In the aMCI cohort, measures of reduced thalamo-IPL structural integrity correlated with memory performance (Figure 4A). Left thalamo-IPL structural abnormalities were accompanied by widespread decreases in effective connectivity from other DMN regions. Similarly, in right hemisphere, thalamo-IPL structural deficits coocurred with disrupted incoming and outgoing IPL effective connectivity. Crucially, the relationship between IPL effective connectivity and memory was disrupted in the aMCI subjects but not in the HCs (Figure 4B).

Several converging findings implicate the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus in this dysfunction. The pulvinar nuclei appear to play a role in cortico-cortical communication where they receive driving input from IPL and relay signals back to cortex via ascending thalamo-cortical projections (Saalmann et al., 2012). Since direct cortico-cortical projections far outnumber projections to pulvinar nucleus from cortex, the pulvinar is unlikely to be the primary route for the transfer of cortico-cortical sensory signals, rather, it may act to coordinate interactions between distributed cortical networks as a function of attention (Basso et al., 2005). Interestingly, entorhinal cortex connects directly to pulvinar nucleus via a non-fornical temporopulvinar tract (Saunders et al., 2005; Zarei et al., 2013) which may provide a conduit for the prion-like transsynaptic spread of disease agents originating in hippocampus (Raj et al., 2012). Consistent with this hypothesis, the present study identified significant structural impairment between thalamus and hippocampus (Figures 2C,D).

Taken together, these findings are consistent with the idea that disrupted posterior DMN node effective connectivity is, to some extent, mediated by impaired thalamo-cortical white matter circuitry.

Methodological Considerations

Some limitations should be noted. The major weakness of the article is that each thalamic nucleus has specific cortical connections and functions, yet the present analysis uses a holo-thalamic approach. It would be more informative to determine whether sub-nuclei show differential causal interactions between specific regions of thalamus and crucial DMN nodes and likewise, whether these connections show varying degrees of structural impairment. Such an approach would reveal the specificity of AD pathology for individual thalamic nuclei. Analyzing the entire thalamus may dilute these results. Our findings should therefore be considered as preliminary evidence warranting further investigation.

It should also be noted that the indirect relationship between fMRI BOLD signal and the underlying neural mechanism is especially problematic when applying GC and should be noted as a weakness in the present study. First, the study’s sampling rate (repetition time or TR) of 2 s is considerably slower than the millisecond temporal resolution of the neuronal activity we seek to qualify. Second, the temporal precedence assumptions of GC can be violated by regional differences in the latency of the hemodynamic response (Handwerker et al., 2004; Friston, 2011). Since neurovascular coupling can be altered in complex ways by disease, the likelihood of such an event is magnified in the aMCI patient cohort (Handwerker et al., 2012). One typical scenario, is that region X causally influences Y at the neuronal level but has a longer time-to-peak in its HRF due to pathology of the neurovasculature. Thus, GC analysis of BOLD signal may incorrectly suggest that Y is causally implicated in causing X. Simulations show that GC performs well when the HRF delay between regions is short (Deshpande et al., 2010; Schippers et al., 2011) however sufficiently fast sampling, on par with the neuronal delays themselves, is required to ensure GC is fully invariant to HRF latency (Seth et al., 2013). Other simulations suggest that the relationship between GC at the neuronal level and GC at the fMRI level is reasonably preserved over a range of sampling rates and convolution parameters (Wen et al., 2013). Whatever the case, sub-second temporal resolutions have been made available (Feinberg et al., 2010; Feinberg and Yacoub, 2012) and are standard as part of the Human Connectome Project (Van Essen et al., 2013). The most recent advances enable a temporal resolution as fast as 50 ms (Boyacioğlu and Barth, 2013).

Critically, GC makes no claims regarding the underlying physical mechanisms responsible for the observed differences in causal relationships between regions. In contrast, the dynamic causal modeling approach (DCM; Di and Biswal, 2014) explicitly specifies dynamic effective relationships at the neuronal level, allowing the most likely structural model for generating the observed data to be identified. Applying DCM in future studies will help clarify thalamic involvement in posterior DMN dysfunction.

The choice of CSD-based tractography reflects the growing recognition that assumptions underlying the DTI model may not always be met in practice (Wheeler-Kingshott and Cercignani, 2009; Jones, 2010; Jones and Cercignani, 2010). The DTI model can only capture a unitary fiber direction within a single voxel despite observations that 90% of the brain is composed of multiple crossing fibers (Jeurissen et al., 2013). For this reason CSD attempts to map several fiber directions per voxel by taking advantage of the high number of diffusion encoding directions and large b-values acquired using the HARDI acquisition protocol (Tournier et al., 2007, 2008; Mielke et al., 2012; Farquharson et al., 2013). Using large b-values has an additional advantage. By allowing a sufficiently long diffusion path to be measured water molecules are more likely to collide with their container. This may be relevant in patients with neurodegenerative disorders who have increased permeability of membranes, greater extracellular space due to axonal atrophy, demyelination and glial pathology (Acosta-Cabronero and Nestor, 2014). To date, only a handful of tractography studies have utilized HARDI data and large b-values (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Meng et al., 2013; Yeatman et al., 2014; Xie et al., 2015) and only one specifically in clinical aMCI and AD (Kehoe et al., 2015).

The absence of indirect biomarkers of AD pathology (CSF biomarkers and/or amyloid PET imaging) should also be acknowledged as a weakness in the present article.

Conclusion

The dynamic nature of thalamo-cortical dialog suggests that abnormalities in DMN operation may best be understood from the perspective of thalamic dysfunction. The present study employed diffusion imaging and effective connectivity to clarify the relationship between the physical integrity of thalamic white matter projections and the activity of the DMN. Significant changes in the diffusivity metrics of thalamic white matter projection tracts to hippocampus, PCC and IPL (Figures 2B–D) were identified. Effective connectivity changes corresponding to the same regions were also observed (Figure 2A). Interestingly, no structural deficits were found between DMN nodes suggesting that early changes in DMN activity could be a result of impaired thalamo-cortical structural integrity.

Such a conclusion is supported by previous resting state MEG (Garcés et al., 2014), EEG (Schreckenberger et al., 2004; Garcés et al., 2013; Moretti, 2015) and fMRI (Greicius et al., 2004; Sorg et al., 2007; Damoiseaux et al., 2012) studies citing disruption in posterior thalamo-cortical alpha sources. Significant evidence suggests that thalamo-cortical circuitry underlie the generation and modulation of alpha and theta rhythms and that average power is attenuated in these frequency bands for MCI and AD subjects (Jeong, 2004; Koenig et al., 2005; Jelles et al., 2008; Park et al., 2008). Several recent modeling studies have proposed a candidate mechanism citing impairment to thalamic reticular fibers in MCI and AD as the source of the dysfunction (Bhattacharya et al., 2011, 2013; Li et al., 2011; Abuhassan et al., 2014).

A corollary of this discussion is the extent to which cortical activity is dependent on cortico-cortical verses thalamo-cortical connections. It has been suggested that thalamic nuclei coordinate distributed cortical regions through cortico-thalamo-cortical pathways. Abnormalities originating in thalamic to PCC and IPL white matter may therefore be sufficient to engender posterior DMN dysfunction without appealing to comparable deficits in cortico-cortical tracts between DMN nodes. Such a view is consistent with the anatomy and timeline of pathogenesis with thalamic nuclei demonstrating pathology an earlier stage of the disease than cortex. Cortical atrophy may therefore be in response to thalamic white matter disruption with commensurate causal abnormalities occurring in response to changes in thalamo-cortical signaling rather than being instigated by structural changes within the cortex. Importantly, the present study is unable to confirm this hypothesis. Other scenarios, in which cortical pathology is causing a degeneration of thalamo-cortical tracts is also possible or likewise, a parallel disruption in both thalamus and cortex.

Overall, these results provide a compelling and previously unexplored physical basis for posterior DMN dysfunction and abnormal fMRI task-induced deactivation response patterns in aMCI and AD patients and underscore the need to consider neurodegenerative changes within a wider system context including contributions of both cortical and subcortical thalamic components. This work complements a growing body of evidence that suggests effective connectivity is disrupted in neurodegenerative disorders such as aMCI and AD and that these changes are underpinned by structural deficits. For these reasons, joint effective and structural studies will play an increasingly important role in the future as we seek to understand how pathological changes in structural connectivity are reflected in altered network effective connectivity.

Author Contributions

TA: analysis, manuscript preparation. DC, ALWB and LM: supervisory support. EK, DF, BL, RAK and DL: data collection.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The research was funded by a Department for Employment and Learning Northern Ireland PhD studentship and the data collection was supported by the Northern Ireland Department for Education and Learning under the Strengthening the All Island Research Base programme. ALWB was funded in part by the Science Foundation Ireland Stokes Programme (07/SK/B1214a). RAK and BL funded in part by the Health Research Board (Ireland).

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl
  2. ^ http://www.mrtrix.org/

References

Abuhassan, K., Coyle, D., and Maguire, L. (2014). Compensating for thalamocortical synaptic loss in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 8:65. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acosta-Cabronero, J., and Nestor, P. J. (2014). Diffusion tensor imaging in Alzheimer’s disease: insights into the limbic-diencephalic network and methodological considerations. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:266. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aggleton, J. P., O’Mara, S. M., Vann, S. D., Wright, N. F., Tsanov, M., Erichsen, J. T., et al. (2010). Hippocampal-anterior thalamic pathways for memory: uncovering a network of direct and indirect actions. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 2292–2307. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07251.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L. R., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2007). Non-linear registration aka Spatial normalisation FMRIB Technial Report TR07JA2. In practice. Available online at: https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/datasets/techrep/tr07ja2/tr07ja2.pdf

Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L. R., and Sotiropoulos, S. N. (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage 125, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basso, M. A., Uhlrich, D., and Bickford, M. E. (2005). Cortical function: a view from the thalamus. Neuron 45, 485–488. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–152. doi: 10.1109/tmi.2003.822821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhattacharya, B. S., Cakir, Y., Serap-Sengor, N., Maguire, L., and Coyle, D. (2013). Model-based bifurcation and power spectral analyses of thalamocortical alpha rhythm slowing in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurocomputing 115, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2012.10.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhattacharya, B. S., Coyle, D., and Maguire, L. P. (2011). A thalamo-cortico-thalamic neural mass model to study alpha rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease. Neural Netw. 24, 631–645. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2011.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyacioğlu, R., and Barth, M. (2013). Generalized inverse imaging (GIN): ultrafast fMRI with physiological noise correction. Magn. Reson. Med. 70, 962–971. doi: 10.1002/mrm.24528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991a). Alzheimer’s disease affects limbic nuclei of the thalamus. Acta Neuropathol. 81, 261–268. doi: 10.1007/BF00305867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991b). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/bf00308809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1995). Staging of Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol. Aging 16, 271–278. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00021-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Sepulcre, J., Talukdar, T., Krienen, F. M., Liu, H., Hedden, T., et al. (2009). Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 29, 1860–1873. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Shannon, B. J., LaRossa, G., Sachs, R., Fotenos, A. F., et al. (2005). Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J. Neurosci. 25, 7709–7717. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2177-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, A., and Iliffe, S. (2009). Alzheimer’s disease. BMJ 338:b158. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantero, J. L., Atienza, M., Gomez-Herrero, G., Cruz-Vadell, A., Gil-Neciga, E., Rodriguez-Romero, R., et al. (2009). Functional integrity of thalamocortical circuits differentiates normal aging from mild cognitive impairment. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3944–3957. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cherubini, A., Péran, P., Spoletini, I., Di Paola, M., Di Iulio, F., Hagberg, G. E., et al. (2010). Combined volumetry and DTI in subcortical structures of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease patients. J. Alzheimers Dis. 19, 1273–1282. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chételat, G., Landeau, B., Eustache, F., Mézenge, F., Viader, F., de la Sayette, V., et al. (2005). Using voxel-based morphometry to map the structural changes associated with rapid conversion in MCI: a longitudinal MRI study. Neuroimage 27, 934–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chhatwal, J. P., Schultz, A. P., Johnson, K., Benzinger, T. L. S., Jack, C. Jr., Ances, B. M., et al. (2013). Impaired default network functional connectivity in autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease. Neurology 81, 736–744. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a1aafe

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, G. C., Zhan, W., Schuff, N., and Weiner, M. W. (2012). White matter alterations in cognitively normal apoEε2 carriers: insight into Alzheimer resistance? Am. J. Neuroradiol. 33, 1392–1397. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.a2984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Prater, K. E., Miller, B. L., and Greicius, M. D. (2012). Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 828.e19–828.30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong, L. W., van der Hiele, K., Veer, I. M., Houwing, J. J., Westendorp, R. G. J., Bollen, E. L. E. M., et al. (2008). Strongly reduced volumes of putamen and thalamus in Alzheimer’s disease: an MRI study. Brain 131, 3277–3285. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deshpande, G., Sathian, K., and Hu, X. (2010). Effect of hemodynamic variability on Granger causality analysis of fMRI. Neuroimage 52, 884–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di, X., and Biswal, B. B. (2014). Modulatory interactions between the default mode network and task positive networks in resting-state. PeerJ 2:e367. doi: 10.7717/peerj.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drzezga, A., Becker, J. A., Van Dijk, K. R., Sreenivasan, A., Talukdar, T., Sullivan, C., et al. (2011). Neuronal dysfunction and disconnection of cortical hubs in non-demented subjects with elevated amyloid burden. Brain 134, 1635–1646. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erwin, R. J., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Skolnick, B., Mawhinney-Hee, M., and Smailis, J. (1992). Facial emotion discrimination: I. Task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects. Psychiatry Res. 42, 231–240. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(92)90115-j

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G. (1994). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-R Adult). San Diego, CA: EdITS/Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

Farquharson, S., Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., Fabinyi, G., Schneider-Kolsky, M., Jackson, G. D., et al. (2013). White matter fiber tractography: why we need to move beyond DTI. J. Neurosurg. 118, 1367–1377. doi: 10.3171/2013.2.JNS121294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, D. A., Moeller, S., Smith, S. M., Auerbach, E., Ramanna, S., Glasser, M. F., et al. (2010). Multiplexed echo planar imaging for sub-second whole brain fmri and fast diffusion imaging. PLoS One 5:e15710. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, D. A., and Yacoub, E. (2012). The rapid development of high speed, resolution and precision in fMRI. Neuroimage 62, 720–725. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Espejo, D., Soddu, A., Cruse, D., Palacios, E. M., Junque, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., et al. (2012). A role for the default mode network in the bases of disorders of consciousness. Ann. Neurol. 72, 335–343. doi: 10.1002/ana.23635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrarini, L., Palm, W. M., Olofsen, H., van der Landen, R., Jan Blauw, G., Westendorp, R. G. J., et al. (2008). MMSE scores correlate with local ventricular enlargement in the spectrum from cognitively normal to Alzheimer disease. Neuroimage 39, 1832–1838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher, E., Carmichael, O., Pasternak, O., Maier-Hein, K. H., and DeCarli, C. (2014). Early brain loss in circuits affected by Alzheimer’s disease is predicted by fornix microstructure but may be independent of gray matter. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:106. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 1, 13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcés, P., Angel Pineda-Pardo, J., Canuet, L., Aurtenetxe, S., López, M. E., Marcos, A., et al. (2014). The Default Mode Network is functionally and structurally disrupted in amnestic mild cognitive impairment—a bimodal MEG-DTI study. Neuroimage Clin. 6, 214–221. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcés, P., Vicente, R., Wibral, M., Pineda-Pardo, J. Á., López, M. E., Aurtenetxe, S., et al. (2013). Brain-wide slowing of spontaneous α rhythms in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:100. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granger, C. W. (1969). Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross- spectral methods. Econometrica 37, 424–438. doi: 10.2307/1912791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 100, 253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C. J., Wedeen, V. J., et al. (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handwerker, D. A., Gonzalez-Castillo, J., D’Esposito, M., and Bandettini, P. A. (2012). The continuing challenge of understanding and modeling hemodynamic variation in fMRI. Neuroimage 62, 1017–1023. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handwerker, D. A., Ollinger, J. M., and D’Esposito, M. (2004). Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses. Neuroimage 21, 1639–1651. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedden, T., Van Dijk, K. R. A., Becker, J. A., Mehta, A., Sperling, R. A., Johnson, K. A., et al. (2009). Disruption of functional connectivity in clinically normal older adults harboring amyloid burden. J. Neurosci. 29, 12686–12694. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3189-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, F. A., Brayne, C., Gill, C., Paykel, E. S., and Beardsall, L. (1995). CAMCOG—a concise neuropsychological test to assist dementia diagnosis: socio-demographic determinants in an elderly population sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 34, 529–541. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01487.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyvärinen, A. (1999). Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 10, 626–634. doi: 10.1109/72.761722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelles, B., Scheltens, P., van der Flier, W. M., Jonkman, E. J., da Silva, F. H. L., and Stam, C. J. (2008). Global dynamical analysis of the EEG in Alzheimer’s disease: frequency-specific changes of functional interactions. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 837–841. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, J. (2004). EEG dynamics in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1490–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeurissen, B., Leemans, A., Tournier, J.-D., Jones, D. K., and Sijbers, J. (2013). Investigating the prevalence of complex fiber configurations in white matter tissue with diffusion magnetic resonance imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2747–2766. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. A., Jones, K., Holman, B. L., Becker, J. A., Spiers, P. A., Satlin, A., et al. (1998). Preclinical prediction of Alzheimer’s disease using SPECT. Neurology 50, 1563–1571. doi: 10.1212/WNL.50.6.1563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. K. (2010). Challenges and limitations of quantifying brain connectivity in vivo with diffusion MRI. Imaging Med. 2, 341–355. doi: 10.2217/iim.10.21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. K., and Cercignani, M. (2010). Twenty-five pitfalls in the analysis of diffusion MRI data. NMR Biomed. 23, 803–820. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. T., Mateen, F. J., Lucchinetti, C. F., Jack, C. R. Jr., and Welker, K. M. (2011). Default mode network disruption secondary to a lesion in the anterior thalamus. Arch. Neurol. 68, 242–247. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kehoe, E. G., Farrell, D., Metzler-Baddeley, C., Lawlor, B. A., Kenny, R. A., Lyons, D., et al. (2015). Fornix white matter is correlated with resting-state functional connectivity of the thalamus and hippocampus in healthy aging but not in mild cognitive impairment—a preliminary study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:10. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, W. D., Kim, L. G., Douiri, A., Frost, C., Rossor, M. N., and Fox, N. C. (2011a). Acceleration of cortical thinning in familial Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 1765–1773. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, W. D., Okello, A. A., Ryan, N. S., Turkheimer, F. E., Rodríguez Martinez De Llano, S., Edison, P., et al. (2011b). Carbon-11-Pittsburgh compound B positron emission tomography imaging of amyloid deposition in presenilin 1 mutation carriers. Brain 134, 293–300. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, T., Prichep, L., Dierks, T., Hubl, D., Wahlund, L. O., John, E. R., et al. (2005). Decreased EEG synchronization in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 165–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leech, R., and Sharp, D. J. (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 137, 12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Coyle, D., Maguire, L., Watson, D. R., and McGinnity, T. M. (2011). Gray matter concentration and effective connectivity changes in Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal structural MRI study. Neuroradiology 53, 733–748. doi: 10.1007/s00234-010-0795-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Q., Lu, W., Cheng, W., Valdes-Sosa, P. A., Wen, X., Ding, M., et al. (2013). Spatio-temporal Granger causality: a new framework. Neuroimage 79, 241–263. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, H. (2001). Cerebral blood flow and metabolic abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Nucl. Med. 15, 85–92. doi: 10.1007/BF02988596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meguro, K., Blaizot, X., Kondoh, Y., Le Mestric, C., Baron, J. C., and Chavoix, C. (1999). Neocortical and hippocampal glucose hypometabolism following neurotoxic lesions of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortices in the non-human primate as shown by PET. Implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 122, 1519–1531. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.8.1519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, X., Jun, C., Wang, Q., Zhang, X., Li, Z., Li, Q., et al. (2013). High b-value diffusion tensor imaging of the remote white matter and white matter of obstructive unilateral cerebral arterial regions. Clin. Radiol. 68, 815–822. doi: 10.1016/j.crad.2013.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzler-Baddeley, C., Hunt, S., Jones, D. K., Leemans, A., Aggleton, J. P., and O’Sullivan, M. J. (2012a). Temporal association tracts and the breakdown of episodic memory in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 79, 2233–2240. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827689e8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzler-Baddeley, C., Jones, D. K., Steventon, J., Westacott, L., Aggleton, J. P., and O’Sullivan, M. (2012b). Cingulum microstructure predicts cognitive control in older age and mild cognitive impairment. J. Neurosci. 32, 17612–17619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3299-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mielke, M. M., Okonkwo, O. C., Oishi, K., Mori, S., Tighe, S., Miller, M. I., et al. (2012). Fornix integrity and hippocampal volume predict memory decline and progression to Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 8, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.05.2416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minka, T. P. (2001). “Automatic choice of dimensionality for PCA,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, eds S. Becker, S. Thrun and K. Obermayer (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 598–604.

Google Scholar

Minoshima, S., Foster, N. L., and Kuhl, D. E. (1994). Posterior cingulate cortex in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 344:895. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-6704-6_17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minoshima, S., Giordani, B., Berent, S., Frey, K. A., Foster, N. L., and Kuhl, D. E. (1997). Metabolic reduction in the posterior cingulate cortex in very early Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 42, 85–94. doi: 10.1002/ana.410420114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mintun, M. A., Larossa, G. N., Sheline, Y. I., Dence, C. S., Lee, S. Y., Mach, R. H., et al. (2006). [11C]PIB in a nondemented population: potential antecedent marker of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 67, 446–452. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000228230.26044.a4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moretti, D. V. (2015). Theta and α EEG frequency interplay in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: evidence from EEG, MRI, and SPECT brain modifications. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:31. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mormino, E. C., Smiljic, A., Hayenga, A. O., Onami, S. H., Greicius, M. D., Rabinovici, G. D., et al. (2011). Relationships between β-amyloid and functional connectivity in different components of the default mode network in aging. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2399–2407. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., et al. (1989). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 39, 1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/WNL.39.9.1159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Mosconi, L., Tsui, W. H., Herholz, K., Pupi, A., Drzezga, A., Lucignani, G., et al. (2008). Multicenter standardized 18F-FDG PET diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and other dementias. J. Nucl. Med. 49, 390–398. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.107.045385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutlu, J., Landeau, B., Tomadesso, C., de Flores, R., Mézenge, F., de La Sayette, V., et al. (2016). Connectivity disruption, atrophy and hypometabolism within posterior cingulate networks in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neurosci. 10:582. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestor, P. J., Fryer, T. D., Smielewski, P., and Hodges, J. R. (2003). Limbic hypometabolism in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Ann. Neurol. 54, 343–351. doi: 10.1002/ana.10669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papez, J. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 38, 725–743. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, Y.-M., Che, H.-J., Im, C.-H., Jung, H.-T., Bae, S.-M., and Lee, S.-H. (2008). Decreased EEG synchronization and its correlation with symptom severity in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci. Res. 62, 112–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasternak, O., Sochen, N., Gur, Y., Intrator, N., and Assaf, Y. (2009). Free water elimination and mapping from diffusion MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 62, 717–730. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, K. T., Stevens, M. C., Pearlson, G. D., Winkler, A. M., Hawkins, K. A., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2013). Default mode network activity and white matter integrity in healthy middle-aged ApoE4 carriers. Brain Imaging Behav. 7, 60–67. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9187-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedro, T., Weiler, M., Yasuda, C. L., D’Abreu, A., Damasceno, B. P., Cendes, F., et al. (2012). Volumetric brain changes in thalamus, corpus callosum and medial temporal structures: mild alzheimer’s disease compared with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 34, 149–155. doi: 10.1159/000342118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56, 303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pihlajamäki, M., and Sperling, R. A. (2009). Functional MRI assessment of task-induced deactivation of the default mode network in Alzheimer’s disease and at-risk older individuals. Behav. Neurol. 21, 77–91. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2009-0231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruessmann, K. P., Weiger, M., Scheidegger, M. B., and Boesiger, P. (1999). SENSE: sensitivity encoding for fast MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 42, 952–962. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1522-2594(199911)42:5<952::aid-mrm16>3.3.co;2-j

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raj, A., Kuceyeski, A., and Weiner, M. (2012). A network diffusion model of disease progression in dementia. Neuron 73, 1204–1215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rami, L., Valls-Pedret, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Caprile, C., Solé-Padullés, C., Castellvi, M., et al. (2011). Cognitive reserve questionnaire. Scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer’s disease. Rev. Neurol. 52, 195–201.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Roh, J. H., Qiu, A., Seo, S. W., Soon, H. W., Kim, J. H., Kim, G. H., et al. (2011). Volume reduction in subcortical regions according to severity of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. 258, 1013–1020. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5872-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roses, A. D. (1996). Apolipoprotein E alleles as risk factors in Alzheimer’s disease. Annu. Rev. Med. 47, 387–400. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.47.1.387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, N. S., Keihaninejad, S., Shakespeare, T. J., Lehmann, M., Crutch, S. J., Malone, I. B., et al. (2013). Magnetic resonance imaging evidence for presymptomatic change in thalamus and caudate in familial Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 136, 1399–1414. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saalmann, Y. B., Pinsk, M. A., Wang, L., Li, X., and Kastner, S. (2012). The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337, 753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, R. C., Mishkin, M., and Aggleton, J. P. (2005). Projections from the entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, presubiculum and parasubiculum to the medial thalamus in macaque monkeys: identifying different pathways using disconnection techniques. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2361-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schelter, B., Winterhalder, M., and Timmer, J. (2006). Handbook of Time Series Analysis. Berlin: Wiley-VCH.

Google Scholar

Schippers, M. B., Renken, R., and Keysers, C. (2011). The effect of intra- and inter-subject variability of hemodynamic responses on group level Granger causality analyses. Neuroimage 57, 22–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreckenberger, M., Lange-Asschenfeldt, C., Lochmann, M., Mann, K., Siessmeier, T., Buchholz, H. G., et al. (2004). The thalamus as the generator and modulator of EEG alpha rhythm: a combined PET/EEG study with lorazepam challenge in humans. Neuroimage 22, 637–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A. K., Chorley, P., and Barnett, L. C. (2013). Granger causality analysis of fMRI BOLD signals is invariant to hemodynamic convolution but not downsampling. Neuroimage 65, 540–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Morris, J. C., Snyder, A. Z., Price, J. L., Yan, Z., D’Angelo, G., et al. (2010). APOE4 allele disrupts resting state fMRI connectivity in the absence of amyloid plaques or decreased CSF Aβ42. J. Neurosci. 30, 17035–17040. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3987-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Raichle, M. E., Snyder, A. Z., Morris, J. C., Head, D., Wang, S., et al. (2011). Amyloid plaques disrupt resting state default mode network connectivity in cognitively normal elderly. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 584–587. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M. (2007). The thalamus is more than just a relay. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 417–422. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M., and Guillery, R. W. (2006). Exploring the Thalamus. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Shiino, A., Watanabe, T., Maeda, K., Kotani, E., Akiguchi, I., and Matsuda, M. (2006). Four subgroups of Alzheimer’s disease based on patterns of atrophy using VBM and a unique pattern for early onset disease. Neuroimage 33, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Miller, K. L., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Webster, M., Beckmann, C. F., Nichols, T. E., et al. (2011). Network modelling methods for FMRI. Neuroimage 54, 875–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorg, C., Riedl, V., Mühlau, M., Calhoun, V. D., Eichele, T., Läer, L., et al. (2007). Selective changes of resting-state networks in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 18760–18765. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708803104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, B. C. M., Hunter, S., Harris, D., Llewellyn, D. J., Siervo, M., Matthews, F. E., et al. (2012). The neuropathological profile of mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a systematic review. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 1056–1076. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teipel, S., and Grothe, M. J. (2016). Does posterior cingulate hypometabolism result from disconnection or local pathology across preclinical and clinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease? Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 43, 526–536. doi: 10.1007/s00259-015-3222-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Forkel, S. J., Simmons, A., Vergani, F., Murphy, D. G. M., et al. (2011). A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1245–1246. doi: 10.1038/nn.2905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J. D., Calamante, F., and Connelly, A. (2007). Robust determination of the fibre orientation distribution in diffusion MRI: non-negativity constrained super-resolved spherical deconvolution. Neuroimage 35, 1459–1472. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., and Connelly, A. (2012). MRtrix: diffusion tractography in crossing fiber regions. Int. J. Imaging Syst. Technol. 22, 53–66. doi: 10.1002/ima.22005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J.-D., Calamante, F., Gadian, D. G., and Connelly, A. (2004). Direct estimation of the fiber orientation density function from diffusion-weighted MRI data using spherical deconvolution. Neuroimage 23, 1176–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J.-D., Yeh, C.-H., Calamante, F., Cho, K.-H., Connelly, A., and Lin, C.-P. (2008). Resolving crossing fibres using constrained spherical deconvolution: validation using diffusion-weighted imaging phantom data. Neuroimage 42, 617–625. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valla, J., Berndt, J. D., and Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2001). Energy hypometabolism in posterior cingulate cortex of Alzheimer’s patients: superficial laminar cytochrome oxidase associated with disease duration. J. Neurosci. 21, 4923–4930.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Van Essen, D. C., Smith, S. M., Barch, D. M., Behrens, T. E. J., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K., et al. (2013). The WU-minn human connectome project: an overview. Neuroimage 80, 62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vann, S. D., Aggleton, J. P., and Maguire, E. A. (2009). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 792–802. doi: 10.1038/nrn2733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villain, N., Desgranges, B., Viader, F., de la Sayette, V., Mézenge, F., Landeau, B., et al. (2008). Relationships between hippocampal atrophy, white matter disruption and gray matter hypometabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 28, 6174–6181. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1392-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D., Kong, Y., Chu, W. C. W., Tam, C. W. C., Lam, L. C. W., Wang, Y., et al. (2014). Generation of the probabilistic template of default mode network derived from resting-state fMRI. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 61, 2550–2555. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2014.2323078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Roe, C. M., Snyder, A. Z., Brier, M. R., Thomas, J. B., Xiong, C., et al. (2012). Alzheimer disease family history impacts resting state functional connectivity. Ann. Neurol. 72, 571–577. doi: 10.1002/ana.23643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welsh, K., Butters, N., Hughes, J., Mohs, R., and Heyman, A. (1991). Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Arch. Neurol. 48, 278–281. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1991.00530150046016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welsh, K. A., Butters, N., Hughes, J. P., Mohs, R. C., and Heyman, A. (1992). Detection and staging of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. Use of the neuropsychological measures developed for the consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 49, 448–452. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530290030008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, X., Rangarajan, G., and Ding, M. (2013). Is granger causality a viable technique for analyzing fMRI data? PLoS One 8:e67428. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wermke, M., Sorg, C., Wohlschläger, A. M., and Drzezga, A. (2008). A new integrative model of cerebral activation, deactivation and default mode function in Alzheimer’s disease. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 35, S12–S24. doi: 10.1007/s00259-007-0698-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A. M., and Cercignani, M. (2009). About “axial” and “radial” diffusivities. Magn. Reson. Med. 61, 1255–1260. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiener, N. (1956). “The theory of prediction,” in Modern Mathematics for Engineers, (Vol. 58) ed. E. F. Beckenbach (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill), 323–329.

Xie, S., Zuo, N., Shang, L., Song, M., Fan, L., and Jiang, T. (2015). How does B-value affect HARDI reconstruction using clinical diffusion MRI data? PLoS One 10:e0120773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xuereb, J. H., Perry, R. H., Candy, J. M., Perry, E. K., Marshall, E., and Bonham, J. R. (1991). Nerve cell loss in the thalamus in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Brain 114, 1363–1379. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.3.1363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yakushev, I., Schreckenberger, M., Müller, M. J., Schermuly, I., Cumming, P., Stoeter, P., et al. (2011). Functional implications of hippocampal degeneration in early Alzheimer’s disease: a combined DTI and PET study. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 38, 2219–2227. doi: 10.1007/s00259-011-1882-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeatman, J. D., Weiner, K. S., Pestilli, F., Rokem, A., Mezer, A., and Wandell, B. A. (2014). The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, E5214–E5223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418503111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarei, M., Beckmann, C. F., Binnewijzend, M. A. A., Schoonheim, M. M., Oghabian, M. A., Sanz-Arigita, E. J., et al. (2013). Functional segmentation of the hippocampus in the healthy human brain and in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage 66, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarei, M., Patenaude, B., Damoiseaux, J., Morgese, C., Smith, S., Matthews, P. M., et al. (2010). Combining shape and connectivity analysis: an MRI study of thalamic degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage 49, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2001). Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Schuff, N., Camacho, M., Chao, L. L., Fletcher, T. P., Yaffe, K., et al. (2013). MRI markers for mild cognitive impairment: comparisons between white matter integrity and gray matter volume measurements. PLoS One 8:e66367. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Snyder, A. Z., Fox, M. D., Sansbury, M. W., Shimony, J. S., and Raichle, M. E. (2008). Intrinsic functional relations between human cerebral cortex and thalamus. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 1740–1748. doi: 10.1152/jn.90463.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Snyder, A. Z., Shimony, J. S., Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2010). Noninvasive functional and structural connectivity mapping of the human thalamocortical system. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1187–1194. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, D. C., Majumdar, S., Korolev, I. O., Berger, K. L., and Bozoki, A. C. (2013). Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment weaken connections within the default-mode network: a multi-modal imaging study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 34, 969–984. doi: 10.3233/JAD-121879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: diffusion MRI, tractography, effective connectivity, Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, default mode network, thalamus, resting state

Citation: Alderson T, Kehoe E, Maguire L, Farrell D, Lawlor B, Kenny RA, Lyons D, Bokde ALW and Coyle D (2017) Disrupted Thalamus White Matter Anatomy and Posterior Default Mode Network Effective Connectivity in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:370. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00370

Received: 17 July 2017; Accepted: 26 October 2017;
Published: 08 November 2017.

Edited by:

Stefan Teipel, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (HZ), Germany

Reviewed by:

Marco Duering, Klinikum der Universität München, Germany
Juan Zhou, Duke-NUS Medical School, Singapore

Copyright © 2017 Alderson, Kehoe, Maguire, Farrell, Lawlor, Kenny, Lyons, Bokde and Coyle. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Thomas Alderson, thomashenryalderson@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.