Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 01 November 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Effects of Video Game Training on Measures of Selective Attention and Working Memory in Older Adults: Results from a Randomized Controlled Trial

  • 1Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain
  • 2Department of Basic Psychology II, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain
  • 3Department of Methodology of the Behavioral Sciences, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain

Video game training with older adults potentially enhances aspects of cognition that decline with aging and could therefore offer a promising training approach. Although, previous published studies suggest that training can produce transfer, many of them have certain shortcomings. This randomized controlled trial (RCT; Clinicaltrials.gov ID: NCT02796508) tried to overcome some of these limitations by incorporating an active control group and the assessment of motivation and expectations. Seventy-five older volunteers were randomly assigned to the experimental group trained for 16 sessions with non-action video games from Lumosity, a commercial platform (http://www.lumosity.com/) or to an active control group trained for the same number of sessions with simulation strategy games. The final sample included 55 older adults (30 in the experimental group and 25 in the active control group). Participants were tested individually before and after training to assess working memory (WM) and selective attention and also reported their perceived improvement, motivation and engagement. The results showed improved performance across the training sessions. The main results were: (1) the experimental group did not show greater improvements in measures of selective attention and working memory than the active control group (the opposite occurred in the oddball task); (2) a marginal training effect was observed for the N-back task, but not for the Stroop task while both groups improved in the Corsi Blocks task. Based on these results, one can conclude that training with non-action games provide modest benefits for untrained tasks. The effect is not specific for that kind of training as a similar effect was observed for strategy video games. Groups did not differ in motivation, engagement or expectations.

Introduction

Aging produces declines in several cognitive processes, especially in executive function and attentional control, mediated by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. These brain areas as well as the hippocampus suffer the highest degree of age-related atrophy (Raz et al., 2005). Moreover, the prefrontal cortex facilitates the organization and contextualization of incoming information and interacts with the hippocampus when carrying out working memory (WM) tasks (Baddeley, 2003; Dennis et al., 2008; Spaniol et al., 2009). The failure of these basic cognitive abilities is a significant predictor of older adults' difficulties with the instrumental activities of daily living, leading to loss of independence (Owsley et al., 2002). Therefore, it is vital to investigate whether cognitive decline can be reversed or delayed through cognitive training interventions (Ball et al., 2007).

The efficacy of computer-based cognitive training to improve executive functions, including selective attention and working memory in older adults has been extensively investigated (Lussier et al., 2015; see Ballesteros et al., 2015a, for a review). Executive functions are central to most cognitive processes (Barkley, 2001). Selective attention refers to the ability to focus on the task at hand while simultaneously suppressing (inhibiting) irrelevant or distracting information. This ability is closely related to the quantity of the information stored in working memory. Selective attention filters out irrelevant information, enhancing encoding, and maintenance of information in working memory (Blacker et al., 2014). This is important, as WM is a capacity-limited cognitive system responsible for temporarily storing and actively processing information needed for ongoing cognition. This cognitive system is vital to keep information in mind while performing complex tasks such as, comprehension and reasoning (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974). This key component of cognition declines in healthy aging (Park et al., 2002; Bopp and Verhaeghen, 2005) and more profoundly in patients with Alzheimer's disease (e.g., Baddeley et al., 1991; Belleville et al., 2007; Huntley and Howard, 2010) and type 2 diabetes mellitus (e.g., Redondo et al., 2016; see Monette et al., 2014; Mayeda et al., 2015 for meta-analyses), amongst others. A question of great practical relevance is whether WM training methods are effective in older adults, their effect size, their cost effectiveness, and how they affect untrained tasks, in near (between very similar but not identical contexts) and far transfer (between contexts that appear on the surface to be remote and unrelated to each other).

Results of training studies are mixed. Some studies have shown positive transfer effects in young adults (e.g., Brehmer et al., 2012; Blacker et al., 2014; Maraver et al., 2016) and in older adults (e.g., Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Borella et al., 2010, 2014; Heinzel et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2016) while others have reported negative results (e.g., Dahlin et al., 2008; Zinke et al., 2012; von Bastian et al., 2013; Ballesteros et al., 2014; Bürki et al., 2014; Kable et al., 2017). A meta-analysis of training studies conducted with older adults reported improvements in tasks similar to the trained tasks (near transfer) as well as small far-transfer effects (Karback and Verhaeghen, 2014). However, despite the positive results of some training studies, far transfer effects have been questioned. A recent meta-analysis of working-memory training studies with pre-post design and control groups (87 publications) reported reliable improvements immediately after training on measures of verbal and visuospatial WM, but these specific training effects did not generalize to other cognitive skills (Melby-Lervåg et al., 2016).

Methodological Issues

Researchers are increasingly using new technology, including cognitive training platforms and video games, to investigate its impact on cognition, brain plasticity and aging (e.g., Basak et al., 2008; Mozolic et al., 2011; Buitenweg et al., 2012; Boot et al., 2013; Ballesteros et al., 2014; Anguera and Gazzaley, 2015; Boot, 2015; Binder et al., 2016; Toril et al., 2016). The idea that video games could enhance aspects of older adults' cognition has attracted the interest of researchers and led to a great explosion of software devoted to brain training (Anguera et al., 2013; Baniquet et al., 2013; Ballesteros et al., 2015a). However, a number of methodological concerns have been raised related to the efficacy and validity of video-game training studies (Boot et al., 2011; but see Green et al., 2014). A recent extensive review (Simons et al., 2016) concluded: “practicing a cognitive task consistently improves performance on that task and closely related tasks, but the available evidence that such training generalizes to other tasks, or to real-world performance, is not compelling” (p. 173). Non-specific factors like expectancy, motivation, and engagement, as well as the quality of the active control group, are important aspects of the intervention design that should be taken into account to be certain that computerized cognitive training is a good method for enhancing cognition (Motter et al., 2016).

An effective cognitive intervention in older adults should show a transfer of training gains to untrained tasks. It is also of paramount importance that the intervention encourages compliance. Older adults prefer mentally challenging games (Nap et al., 2009), while studies with young adults have shown that fast-paced action games result in broader transfer effects (Green and Bavelier, 2003; Baniqued et al., 2014; for a review see Bavelier et al., 2012).

A systematic review (Kueider et al., 2012) and several meta-analyses suggest that playing video games improves information processing, with interesting larger effects in old-older adults than in young-older adults (Powers et al., 2013; Lampit et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2014). A recent meta-analysis of action video game training studies found that healthy young and older adults benefited from training in overall and specific cognitive domains, but that young adults benefited more than older adults (Wang et al., 2016). These findings suggest the potential of video-game training as an intervention tool for cognitive improvement.

In a previous RCT study (Ballesteros et al., 2014), two groups of older adults participated in 20 1-h training sessions with non-action games or were assigned to a passive control group. Groups were similar at baseline on demographics, vocabulary, global cognition, and depression status. The results showed improvements in the video-game group and no change in the control group in processing speed, attention, immediate, and delayed visual recognition memory, and a trend to improve in Affection and Assertivity, two dimensions of the Wellbeing Scale (Nieboer et al., 2005). However, visuospatial WM and executive control (shifting strategy) functions did not improve. These enhancements in processing speed, selective attention, and spatial memory disappeared after a 3-month non-contact period (Ballesteros et al., 2015b), suggesting that cognitive plasticity can be induced in healthy older adults by training, but that periodic boosting sessions are needed to maintain the benefits.

In a more recent intervention study, we investigated specifically the effects of video game training on visuospatial WM and episodic memory in healthy older adults after 15 1-h sessions playing six non-action video games. Training produced significant improvements compared to a passive control group in two visuospatial WM tasks (Corsi blocks and Jigsaw puzzle task) and other episodic and short-term memory tasks. Gains in the Jigsaw puzzle task, short-term memory, and episodic memory were maintained over a 3-month follow-up period (Toril et al., 2016). In both studies, we compared the performance of experimental groups trained with non-action video games with that of passive control groups who participated in discussion groups on themes related to aging (Ballesteros et al., 2014, 2015b) or who attended courses in the community center for older adults (Toril et al., 2016). It could be argued that participants in the control group who simply met the trainer several times would not expect to improve as much on the transfer tasks as those who received video-game training (Boot and Kramer, 2014; Melby-Lervåg et al., 2016; Simons et al., 2016). Expectancy can influence training results through the placebo effect. In the present RCT, we address several of these significant issues in cognitive training research (Boot et al., 2011, 2013; see Baniqued et al., 2014; Blacker et al., 2014).

The Current Randomized Controlled Trial

To attribute possible training-related improvements to the intervention and avoid placebo effects (Boot et al., 2011; Foroughi et al., 2016), the current RCT (Ballesteros et al., 2017) compared performance on a series of transfer tasks of an experimental group playing selected adaptive non-action video games from Lumosity (http://www.lumosity.com) with that of an active control group. The active control group had the same number of sessions playing The Sims (Electronic Arts Inc.), a simulation strategy game in which the player takes control of the life of a character in everyday activities, and SimCity, a life simulation game in which the player is the Mayor of a city that he or she must expand. Unlike the non-action games, control games were not adaptive (the difficulty was not adjusted over the training to the actual level of performance of the trainee). Some results suggest that adaptive computerized training regimes may improve executive functioning (e.g., Ball et al., 2002; Dahlin et al., 2008; Morrison and Chain, 2011). Both groups used a mobile tablet device to play. At the end of the assessment session, participants in the current study reported their expectations (increase or decrease) regarding their performance on the assessment tasks, using a 5-point Likert scale. Moreover, at the 1st, 8th, and 16th training sessions, participants responded to questions about motivation and engagement for each of the video games.

In sum, we investigated possible cognitive and neural changes in attention and working memory functions in healthy older adults trained in small groups with adaptive non-action video games selected from Lumosity for 16 sessions in the presence of the trainer. Their performance on two attentional and two working-memory tasks was compared pre- and post-training with that of an active control group who played a simulation strategy games for the same number of sessions. The electrophysiological data recorded to assess possible neural changes will not be presented in this paper. The objectives of this study were as follows. First, to examine possible effects of playing adaptive non-action video games on older adults' performance on a series of cognitive tasks designed to assess selective attentional functions, mainly distraction and alertness (Oddball Task), effortful (Stroop) and automatic inhibition (Negative Priming), and working memory, mainly maintenance and updating (N-back task and Corsi blocks) in verbal and visuospatial working memory. Second, to explore whether motivation, engagement, and expectations account for possible training-related improvements. We hypothesized that the non-action, adaptive video game group would show greater improvements in selective attention (exhibiting less distraction, more alertness, and better effortful inhibition after training), and enhanced working memory (maintenance and updating) than the active control group.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were volunteers recruited from several older adult groups attending lectures and courses for senior citizens in UNED Associated Centers in Madrid. Eligible participants were randomized into the cognitive non-action video-game training group and the active control group in which participants played a simulation game. Exclusion criteria were self-reported neurological, psychiatric, or addictive disorders. All the participants lived independently, with normal or corrected-to-normal hearing and vision and were free of neurological and psychiatric disorders, or traumatic brain injury. To determine their eligibility, each participant completed a screening battery consisting of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) to rule out possible cognitive impairment (cut-off score of 27 out of a maximum of 30 points), the Yesavage Depression Scale (Yesavage et al., 1983) to screen for depression (more than six points), and the Information subtest of the WAIS-III scale (Wechsler, 1999). Exclusion criteria were a diagnosis of dementia, cognitive impairment (score of <27 on the Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE), <20/60 vision with or without correction, inability to complete the study activities, or communication problems. Demographic data and screening test scores corresponding to each group are summarized in Table 1. T-tests showed that groups did not differ on these measures (all ps > 0.05) at pre-test.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information for participants in each group.

Twenty participants (26.6%) were lost at post-test. The study was completed by 30 of the 38 participants in the non-action video game training group and by 25 of the 37 in the active control group. Analyses of background characteristics showed no differences between dropouts and participants remaining in the respective group. Figure 1 shows the CONSORT Flow diagram of the present study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Consort flowchart.

Transfer of training was measured as performance improvement at post-test relative to pre-test (baseline) on untrained tasks, measuring selective attention, and working memory. To explore successful transfer of training gains to attentional mechanisms and working memory, data were recorded and analyzed at pre-training (T1) and post-training (T2). Participants in both groups (experimental and active control) completed pre-test and post-test assessments individually in the laboratory. The assessment lasted ~3 1/2 h (including rest periods). It included a Cross-modal oddball task and a Stroop-Negative Priming task to assess the effects of video-game training on attention and top-down control mechanisms, and a n-back task and the Corsi Blocks task to assess working memory.

All the methodological designs of the outcome measures were constructed using the rules of counterbalancing and stimulus rotation. Response keys were counterbalanced across conditions. All computerized cognitive tasks were programmed with E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburg, PA, USA). The statistical analyses of the behavioral results were conducted with SPSS (version 22). Results were considered significant at p < 0.05, with Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests performed as appropriate. The present study was conducted in accordance with the recommendations of the Research Ethics Committee of the UNED (Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid). The UNED Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol. All participants provided written informed consent and were informed of their right to cease participation in the study at any time. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013).

Cognitive Evaluation: Assessment Tasks and Procedures

The experimental tasks performed before and after training are described below. At the end of the last assessment session, participants answered questions regarding their study expectations and perceived improvement in the different tasks.

Attentional Tasks

Distraction and alertness, two important functions of selective attention were assessed with a cross-modal oddball task. Effortful inhibitory control and automatic passive inhibition were measured with the Stroop task and the Negative Priming task, respectively, both included in a computerized task.

Cross-modal oddball attention task

As in our previous RCT (Ballesteros et al., 2014; Mayas et al., 2014), we assessed selective attention with a cross-modal oddball task. This would allow us to compare performance after non-action video game training when the control group was a passive control group (as in our previous study) with that of an active group (the present study). The task comprised three blocks of 384 trials each (24 practice trials and 360 test trials, as described below). In each trial, participants categorized a visual digit from 1 to 8 as odd or even by pressing one of two response keys (counterbalanced across participants). Each trial began with the presentation of a white fixation cross in the center of a black screen together with a 200 ms sound. The digit appeared in white in the center of the screen 100 ms after the sound's offset, and remained on the screen for 200 ms. A response window was displayed for 1,200 ms from the digit's onset. There were three sound conditions: A silent block and two blocks of trials containing two different sounds, the standard sound presented in 80% of the trials that was a 600 Hz sine wave tone of 200 ms, and a novel sound used in 20% of the trials taken from a list of 72 environmental sounds (e.g., hammer, drill, door, rain, etc.) used in Andrés et al. (2006). All sounds were normalized and presented binaurally through headphones at a constant volume. Participants were instructed to focus on the digit categorization task and ignore any sound, and to respond as fast and accurately as possible.

Stroop-negative priming (NP) task

Cognitive processes that involve top-down control mechanisms decline with aging, but more automatic processes do not (Ballesteros and Reales, 2004). The Stroop interference effect reflects the extra time needed to resolve the conflict generated by an automatically processed irrelevant dimension. NP and the standard Stroop effect were evaluated within the same task. In the standard NP procedure, participants are presented with pairs of prime and probe displays containing two stimuli, the to-be-responded target and the to-be-ignored distractor. In the critical trials, participants have to respond to a target that served as a distractor in the previous prime display (the ignored repetition condition). Reaction times (RTs) to targets in the ignored repetition condition are slower than in the control condition, in which the distractor in the prime display is not repeated as the target in the probe display (Tipper and Cranston, 1985; Tipper, 2001; Andrés et al., 2008). The aim was to investigate whether training older adults improved controlled, effortful inhibition (measured by Stroop interference) to a greater extent than automatic passive inhibition (NP).

The stimuli were three basic color words (“red,” “green,” and “blue”) written in red, green, or blue, presented in the center of the computer screen. Participants responded by pressing the appropriate key of the computer keyboard. Each trial started with a black fixation cross, presented in the center of the screen on a white background. Stimuli were presented randomly and remained on the screen for 200 ms. Participants responded as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing a key according to the color of the stimulus word while ignoring its semantic meaning. Participants performed a block of 18 practice trials (with feedback) and four experimental blocks of 144 trials each. Responses for the Stroop analysis were coded as a function of the congruency between the color and the meaning of the stimulus.

Working Memory (WM)

Visuospatial WM was assessed with a computerized version of the Corsi Blocks task. Maintaining and updating in verbal working memory was evaluated with the n-back task.

Corsi blocks task

Visuospatial working memory (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974) was assessed with a computerized version of the Corsi task (Milner, 1971), similar to the task used in our previous intervention studies (Ballesteros et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2016), with six levels of increasing difficulty (2, 3, 4, 5, 6, and 7 cube positions) and 12 trials per level. The first two trials in each difficulty level were used as practice trials and were not analyzed. The stimuli consisted of black squares that appeared one by one in the center of the computer screen inside a 3 × 3 matrix for 1,000 ms each, with a 500 ms inter-stimulus interval (ISI). The positions in each sequence were randomly selected for each participant, the only restriction being that two cubes did not appear in the same position within the same trial. In each trial, participants were asked to reproduce the sequence of cubes in the same order as in the presentation. Participants responded by marking the presentation order of the cubes on a separate response sheet. They started the next trial by pressing the space bar. The final score was the proportion of correct sequences reproduced at each difficulty level.

N-back memory task

As a measure of maintenance and updating of information in WM, participants performed a verbal n-back task (Wayne, 1958). This task has been widely used to assess WM in young (Baniqued et al., 2015; Kable et al., 2017) and older adult training studies (Basak et al., 2008; Dahlin et al., 2008; Redondo et al., 2016). We used an adapted version of the Robinson and Fuller (2004) computerized task. Participants viewed a sequence of centrally presented stimuli (letters) and indicated whether the last stimulus was identical to the one presented “n” trials back by pressing one key for “yes” and another for “no.” In this task we used three difficulty levels (1, 2, and 3 back). In the one-back level, participants had to remember the item presented just before the current item; in the two-back level, they had to remember the item presented two positions before; and in three-back level, three positions before. The stimuli in all blocks were 20 consonants (B, C, D, F, G, H, J, K, L, M, N, P, Q, R, S, T, V, W, Y, and Z). The letters appeared one by one in the center of the computer screen (font: Palatino Linotype, size: 30) for 500 ms, with an ISI of 3,000 ms for participants' responses. The task started with a practice session of 17 trials at each level, followed by the experimental session and feedback. Each level contained three blocks of 27 trials, giving a total of 81 trials per level. Each block of 27 trials consisted of 17 “non-targets” (“no” response) and 10 “targets” (“yes” response).

Motivation and engagement self-reports

Motivation and engagement across the training period were assessed at pretest, at training sessions 1, 8, and 16, and at post-test. Participants responded on a 10-point Likert scale to questions about their motivation (“How motivated were you to achieve the highest score on the game? 1 = not motivated at all; 10 = extremely motivated”) and engagement (“How engaging was each game? 1 = not engaging at all; 10 = extremely engaging”).

Post-experiment survey

At the very end of the post-assessment session, participants answered questions related to whether they felt that participation in the study changed how they performed daily life activities, memory, processing speed, emotions, attention, and visual acuity, using a 5-point Likert scale (1 very much; 5 not at all). They also provided information about their expectations of improvement after training on the four transfer tasks.

Overview of the Training Program

Training was conducted in small groups of 10–12 participants at the UNED Associated Center in Madrid. At the beginning of each training session, the experimenter handed out an iPad (Brigmton BTPC 1018OC) to each participant. Participants in the experimental and the active control groups completed 16 training sessions of ~40–50 min each over 10–12 weeks. According to the results of our meta-analytic study (Toril et al., 2014), short training regimes are better than long ones. We therefore used a training regime that was not too long to avoid loss of motivation.

Cognitive training intervention with non-action games

In each session, the trainees in the experimental group played 10 non-action video games selected from the commercial Lumosity (http://www.lumosity.com/) computerized training program. Some video games from this platform are based on traditional psychological tasks. The video games are quiet-pace, short (3–6 min), and were designed to be engaging. All participants played the same video games. The selected video games claim to train the following specific cognitive domains: working memory (20% of the games), attention (30%), response inhibition (10%), task switching (20%) and speed of processing (20%). Table 2 provides a short description of the games and their trained domains. These video games were: Playing Koi, Highway Hazards, Speed Match, Tidal Treasures, Star Search, Color Mach, Lost in Migration, Pinball Recall, Ebb and Flow, and Disillusion. A main feature of these video games is that they are adaptive meaning that as performance improved, difficulty increased progressively.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Short description of the 10 non-action video games played by the experimental group (selected from Lumosity).

Active control condition

Participants in the active control condition practiced video games not designed to train specific cognitive domains but allows to trigger expectancy, contact with the trainer, motivation, and novelty. Participants played the same number of sessions for the same time than the cognitive trained group. Game difficulty was not adapted each session to the user ability. Participants in this group started each training session from the beginning of the video game. It is important to mention that both groups had the same contact with the trainer as all the training sessions were conducted on the present of the trainer and in small groups. Moreover, participants in both conditions received the same completion incentives. The active control group played The Sims and SimCity Build (Electronic Arts Inc.), simulation strategy games. The Sims was used as control in previous intervention studies conducted with young adults (e.g., Oei and Patterson, 2013; Blacker et al., 2014). These simulation games did not appear to have the same cognitive demands as the behavioral tasks. They have some memory demands as the player has to keep track of the goals to achieve but it is not necessary to do so as the goals are available in the menu (see Oei and Patterson, 2013). For example, The Sims player created and controlled characters that accomplished and performed a number of tasks similar to real-life activities (making friends, sleeping, have a bath, find a job, and so on). SimsCity Build is also a simulation strategy game in which the player performed the tasks corresponding to the Mayor of the city. The task is to create and expand the city. Players are not required to accomplish objectives in a pre-determined order. Table 3 presents a short summary of these games.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Short description of the life simulation games played by the active control group (Electronic Arts, Inc.).

Results

We first examined whether there were practice-related improvements on the trained games across the 16 training sessions. Next, we analyzed whether video-game training gains transferred to untrained tasks by comparing baseline (pre-test) to post-test performance in each group. We also considered whether perceived improvements in attention and working memory differed between groups. Levels of motivation and engagement throughout the training were assessed from the participants' answers to the self-report questions at pretest, at the 1st, 8th, and 16th training sessions, and at post-test. Groups did not differ in motivation [t(53) = 0.35, p = 0.72], engagement [t(53) = 0.24, p = 0.81], or expectation [t(53) = −1.26, p = 0.72] at pretest.

Video Game Performance and Game Experience across Sessions

The difficulty of the non-action video games was modified using an adaptive algorithm within and across the training sessions. The results showed that video game performance improved across sessions (see Figure 2). Comparisons between the first and last sessions were conducted on the performance Z-scores (Table 4). As expected, the results showed that training improved accuracy in all the video games.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Average performance scores obtained in each video game across the training sessions in Z scores (mean 0; standard deviation 1).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Performance (Z-Scores) of the 30 participants in the experimental group in the first and last training session on each of the 10 practiced non-action video games.

Game Experience across Training Sessions

Responses to questions on motivation (How motivated were you to achieve the highest score on the game?) and engagement (How engaging was each game?) were analyzed separately using mixed ANOVAs with group (experimental vs. active control) as a between-subjects factor, and training session (1, 8, and 16) as the within-subjects factor. Rating scores to these questions were averaged across all the games. The ANOVA conducted on motivation scores showed that neither the main effect of group [F(1, 52) = 0.513, MSE = 8.350, p > 0.05] nor session [F(2, 104) = 0.863, MSE = 1, 204, p > 0.05] were statistically significant. More importantly, the group by session interaction was marginally significant [F(2, 104)=2.955,MSE=1.204, p=0.056, ηPartial2=0.054]. However, the simple effects analysis of this marginally significant interaction showed no effect whatsoever in the group variable across sessions, suggesting that the two groups were similarly motivated across sessions.

The ANOVA conducted on engagement showed that the two groups were similarly engaged [F(1, 52) = 0.411, MSE = 7.07, p > 0.05], but engagement differed across sessions [F(2, 104)=3.212, MSE=1.354, p>0.05,ηPartial2=0.058]. Pairwise comparisons showed that the scores at session 16 (post-test) were significantly lower (x = 7.588, SE = 0.241) than at session 8 (x = 7.971, SE = 0.213). The group by session interaction was also statistically significant [F(2, 104)=3.376, MSE=1.354, p>0.05, ηPartial2=0.061]. Simple effects analysis of this interaction did not show any effect of group on session, except the 16th, which showed a marginal p-value (p = 0.065), suggesting that the two groups were similarly engaged, although the experimental group showed a slight drop in engagement in the final part of the experiment.

Perceived Improvement: Post-assessment Survey

Transfer expectations of video-game training on a series of different activities were assessed with a 5-point Likert scale (1 expectation of no change, and 5 expectation that training will have strong positive effects). A series of one-factor ANOVAs (group: experimental, active control) were performed to assess the perceived improvement of video-game training on daily life activities, memory, processing speed, current studies, emotions, attention and visual acuity. The results showed that groups did not differ in their expectations of improvement in daily life activities [F(1, 52) = 0.91, MSE = 0.89, p > 0.05, nP2 = 0.02], memory [F(1, 52) = 1.12, MSE = 1.27, p > 0.05, nP2 = 0.02], processing speed [F(1, 52) = 2.48, MSE = 3.22, p > 0.05, nP2 = 0.05] or emotions [F(1, 52) = 2.22, MSE = 2.50, p > 0.05, nP2 = 0.0]. The experimental group had higher expectations of improved attention [F(1, 52) = 5.12, MSE = 6.23, p < 0.05, nP2 = 0.09] and visual acuity [F(1, 52) = 5.69, MSE = 7.50, p < 0.05] after training than the control group.

To assess possible differences in expectation of improvement after training on the experimental tasks (Oddball task, Stroop-NP task, Corsi task, N-back task), participants indicated their expected improvement after video-game training on a scale of 1 to 5. ANOVAs revealed that groups did not differ in their expectations of improving their performance on the Oddball [F(1, 53) = 0.91, MSE = 0.62, p > 0.05, nP2 = 0.02] and N-back tasks [F(1, 53) = 0.52, MSE = 0.27, p > 0.05, nP2 = 0.01]. The ANOVAs revealed a significant group effect for the Stroop task [F(1, 53) = 6.06, MSE = 3.69, p < 0.05, nP2 = 0.10] and the Corsi task [F(1, 53) = 9.52, MSE = 4.80, p < 0.05, nP2 = 0.15], with higher ratings in the experimental than the active control group.

Transfer of Video-Game Training Gains

The main results obtained in the transfer tasks at pre-training and post-training by the experimental and the active control groups are displayed in Table 5 and Figure 3.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Pre and post-training performance on psychological measures for the experimental and active control groups.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean performance of trained and active control groups at pre-test and post-test. (A) Mean differences between conditions for distraction (novel—standard) and alertness (silence—standard) in ms. (B) Mean RTs for ignored and control trials in the Stroop task. (C) Mean proportion of correct sequences in the Corsi blocks task. (D) Mean hits—false alarms rates obtained in the n-back task. Error bars represent plus minus 1 standard error.

Attentional Functions

Cross-modal oddball task

The main dependent variables for this task were distraction and alertness. We did not apply an ANCOVA with the pre-test scores as covariates because the assumption of independence with the inter-subjects factor was not met. The 2 group (experimental vs. control) × 2 session (pre- vs. post-test) × 3 sound condition (silence, standard, novel) mixed ANOVA conducted on the mean of the RTs of the correct responses showed a main effect of sound condition [F(2, 104)= 33.79, MSe=1075.94,p < 0.001, ηp2= 0.394]. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were all significant: standard sound condition was faster (622 ms) than the silence (645 ms) and novel (658 ms) conditions (ps < 0.001), while mean RT of the silence condition was faster than that of the novel condition (p = 0.03). The analysis also yielded a main effect of group [F(1, 52)= 5.70, p = 0.021, MSe=26573.78, ηp2= 0.10]. This result indicates that the active control group was faster (620 ms) than the experimental group (664 ms) overall. The main effect of session was also significant [F(1, 52)= 11.01, p = 0.002, MSe =1087.59, ηp2= 0.175]. RTs were faster at post-test (636 ms) than pre-test (647 ms). Finally, the session × sound interaction was statistically significant [F(2, 104)=4.69,MSe=717.65, p = 0.017, ηp2= 0.08]. Post-hoc comparisons indicated that RTs were faster at post-test only in the standard (617 and 627 ms for pre- and post-test, respectively) and novel (646 and 670 ms for pre- and post-test, respectively) conditions (p = 0.05 and p < 0.001, respectively). Additional analyses were conducted on distraction and alertness. We computed the distraction effect as the difference between the RT of the novel trials and the RT of the standard trials. A 2 group × 2 session ANOVA was performed on the distraction effect. Only the two-way group × session interaction was significant [F(1, 52)= 5.7, MSe= 222.53, p = 0.020, ηp2= 0.10], indicating that the active control group improved at post-test (27.33 and 19.49 for pre- and post-test, respectively) but the experimental group did not (26.32 and 32.37 for pre- and post-test, respectively). We computed the alertness effect as the difference between the RT of the silence trials and the RT of the standard trials. The ANOVA conducted on alertness did not show any significant effect (all ps > 0.05). Figure 3A represents the difference in RTs between conditions for distraction and alertness.

Stroop—NP task

Stroop results Responses for the Stroop analysis were coded as a function of the congruency between the color and the meaning of the color word. Congruent trials were those in which the color of the word coincided with the color in which it was presented. Incongruent trials were those in which the color word did not coincide with the color in which it was displayed. Trials were also coded according to the congruency of the word in the previous trial (N-1) with the color in the current trial in order to compute the NP effect. Thirty participants in the experimental group and 25 participants in the control group were included in this analysis.

To analyze the results of the Stroop task, a 2 group × 2 session × 2 congruency (congruent and incongruent) mixed ANOVA was conducted on the mean RTs for correct trials as the dependent variable. The analysis showed a highly significant main effect of congruency (classic Stroop effect) [F(1, 53)= 345.62; MSe=109.86, p<0.001,  ηp2= 0.867]. Responses to the incongruent stimuli were slower (884 ms) than to the congruent ones (801 ms; p < 0.001). The main effect of group was significant [F(1, 53)= 4.70, MSe = 40729.29, p<0.05, ηp2= 0.081], showing that the control group was faster (813 ms) than the experimental group (872 ms). The significant group × congruency interaction qualifies the main effect of group [F(1, 53)= 4.53, MSe=1098.86,p<0.05, ηp2 = 0.08]. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the active control group was faster than the experimental group (850 and 919 ms for the control and the experimental group, respectively), but only for incongruent stimuli (p < 0.02). Finally, we computed the Stroop effect as the difference between Incongruent RT—Congruent RT for each participant. A 2 group × 2 session mixed ANOVA showed that only the main effect of group was significant [F(1, 53) = 4.53, MSe = 2197.72, p < 0.05, nP2 = 0.08]. No other main effect or interaction was statistically significant. See Figure 3B.

NP results Responses for NP analyses were coded as a function of the relationship between the color of the current target word and the color denoted by the word in the previous trial (distractor). Different types of trials were coded: (a) ignored repetition trials were those in which the word in the preceding trial denoted the color of the word color of the current stimulus; and (b) control trials were those in which both the target (color) and the distractor (word) in the current trial were different from the target and distractor in the previous trial. The ignored repetition condition and the control condition were always an incongruent trial preceded by an incongruent trial.

A 2 group × 2 session × 2 repetition (ignored repetition, control condition) mixed ANOVA was performed on the mean RTs for correct trials. The NP effect was computed as the difference between ignored repetition and control conditions. The analysis showed a significant main effect of group [F(1, 53)= 4.45, MSe = 58822.53, p<0.05, ηp2= 0.07], indicating that the experimental group was significantly slower (941 ms) than the control group (871 ms) in all conditions. The main effect of repetition was also significant [???, showing that ignored repetition trials were slower than control trials for both groups (for the experimental group: 968 and 913 ms for ignored repetition and control trials, respectively; for the control group: 893 and 849 ms for ignored repetition and control trials, respectively). The size of the NP effect was 55 ms for the experimental group and 44 ms for the control group. Finally, neither the main effect of session nor any interactions were significant (all ps > 0.05).

Effects of Training on Spatial Working Memory

Corsi blocks

We conducted a 2 group × 6 level ANCOVA with the pre-test scores as covariates conducted on the proportion of correct sequences obtained at each difficulty level. The assumption of no relationship between the inter-subjects factor (group) and the covariates was met (all ps > 0.05) as well as the equality of slopes (all ps > 0.05). The results showed a main effect of level [F(5, 235) = 13.706, MSe = 0.022, p < 0.001, ηp2= 0.382]. The mean correct proportions were 0.97, 0.78, 0.69, 0.41, 0.24, and 0.13 for levels 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6, respectively. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons showed that all levels differed significantly from each other (all ps < 0.05). Neither other main effects nor any interaction reached statistical significance. However, as the ANCOVA did not include session as a factor, we also performed an ANOVA to assess specifically the effect of session. This analysis showed that session was highly significant [F(1, 53) = 22.57, MSe = 0.027, p < 0.001, ηp2= 0.299]. The mean correct proportion at pre-test was 0.48 while at post-test the mean was 0.54. The interaction session × group was not significant, suggesting that both groups benefited equally after training (see Figure 3C).

N-back task

One participant in the experimental group and one in the control group were excluded from the analysis due to the large number of no responses (more than 50%). Thus, data from 29 participants in the experimental group and 24 participants in the control group were included in this analysis. We did not perform an ANCOVA with the pre-test scores as covariate because the assumption of no relationship between the inter-subjects group factor and the covariates was not met. The 2 group × 2 session × 3 level mixed ANOVA conducted on the means of Hits minus False Alarms yielded a significant main effect of Level [F(2, 102)=188,68, MSe = 13,21, p <0.001, ηp2= 0.79] with means of 25.39, 21.33, and 15.69 for 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back levels, respectively. A main effect of session also reached significance [F(1, 51)=13.91, MSe = 12.51, p< 0.001, ηp2= 0.21]. The Hits-FA mean was 20.06 at pre-test and 21.55 at post-test. The session × level interaction was marginally significant [F(2, 102)= 3.06, MSe = 9.48, p = 0.051,ηp2 =0.067], showing that only 1-back and 3-back levels improved between pre- and post-test. Finally, the group × session interaction was also marginally significant [F(1, 51)=3,162, MSe =12.518, p= 0.08,ηp2 = 0.06], suggesting that the experimental group improved marginally at post-test compared to the control group (19.18 and 20.94 at pre- and post-test for the experimental group; 21.37 and 21.72 at pre- and post-test for the control group). Figure 3D shows this marginal interaction.

Discussion

The present study yielded three main results: (1) Unsurprisingly, participants improved significantly in the video games across the training sessions; (2) the experimental group did not show greater improvements in measures of selective attention and working memory compared to the control group; and (3) a marginal training effect was observed for the N-back task, but not for the Stroop task in the experimental group while both groups improved similarly in the Corsi Blocks task. On the basis of these results, one can conclude that training with non-action video games provide modest benefits for untrained tasks (near transfer) that were not directly trained but were under the umbrella of the executive function. Moreover, the effect was not specific for that kind of training, since a similar effect was observed in the group trained with simulation strategy video games.

Older adult participants in the non-action adaptive video games improved on all the practiced games across the training sessions. This is in line with previous findings reported in a large number of intervention studies (e.g., Ackerman et al., 2010; Reddick et al., 2013; Ballesteros et al., 2014; Baniqued et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2016).

Transfer Effects

Motivated by results suggesting that training older adults with non-action video games improve aspects of cognition when compare performance with that of a passive control group (Ballesteros et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2016), and by others showing that executive control functions could improve with adaptive computerized training (Morrison and Chain, 2011; Nouchi et al., 2013; Hardy et al., 2015), we hypothesized that older adults trained with adaptive cognitive video games would show greater improvements in measures of executive function than an active control group trained with video games not designed specifically for cognitive training. The experimental group trained with the commercial adaptive games showed a trend to improve in the N-back task after training and a similar degree of improvement on the Corsi task than the active control group. The opposite occurred in the Oddball task in which only the active control group showed less distraction after training. Based on these results, one might conclude that training with non-action games provide modest benefits for untrained tasks compared with an active control group trained with non-adaptive simulation strategy video games.

A recent randomized controlled trial conducted with 64 healthy young adults, trained with the same commercial web-based cognitive training video games and 64 trained with web-based video games that do not target executive functions or adapt the difficulty during training (active control group) found that performance over time in the cognitive training group improved across sessions in the trained video games. However, both training conditions improved similarly in the cognitive assessment battery that included tests of attention, working memory (visual/spatial n-back), response inhibition, interference control (Stroop test), and cognitive flexibility, not directly trained but in the domain of executive function targeted by the training regimen (Kable et al., 2017).

It is central to consider the importance of the control group, as most previous intervention studies did not include an active control condition as similar as possible to the experimental condition (Lampit et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2014) and with similar levels of motivation, social contact, and engagement (Blacker et al., 2014; Motter et al., 2016). In the current RCT, we tried to equate task factors that might contribute to differential improvements. First, the number of older participants was almost double that of our previous intervention studies. They were randomly assigned to a group trained with video games from Lumosity or to an active control group playing The Sims and SimCity (two simulation strategy games) for the same number of sessions. The inclusion of an active control group is considered critical for inferences about the specific potential effects of the intervention (Dougherty et al., 2016; Motter et al., 2016; Simons et al., 2016). We selected an active control condition as similar as possible to the training condition in that the control group also played non-action games. In several recent intervention studies conducted with young adults to investigate whether training with action video games enhances aspects of cognition, including visual WM (Blacker et al., 2014), cognitive flexibility (Glass et al., 2013), plasticity in the visual system (Li et al., 2011) or several aspects of perception and cognition (Oei and Patterson, 2013), the active control participants also played non-action strategy video games.

Boot et al. (2011; see also Dougherty et al., 2016; Simons et al., 2016) warned that the use of a control group per se does not preclude the possibility of differential placebo effects contaminating the results of the trained group. The argument is that the experimental group might have higher expectations of their performance on the transfer tasks compared to the active control group. Recently, Blacker et al. (2014) collected measures of expectations in a group trained on action games and in an active control group trained with The Sims. Baniqued et al. (2014) used casual video games to train young adults. The intervention included an active control group that played several games not related to WM and reasoning, and responded to feedback questions about engagement, motivation, enjoyment, and perceived effort.

In order to match the expectations of our experimental group (trained with non-action games) with those of the active control group, we evaluated both groups' expectations for improvement on each outcome measure. Results showed that groups did not differ in their expectations of improvement in the attentional Oddball task and the verbal WM N-back task. The experimental group had higher expectations of improvement than the active control group in the response inhibition Stroop task and the visuospatial WM Corsi blocks task. The expectations and outcomes of the two groups were not aligned, so it is unlikely that the results were driven by a placebo effect.

There are discrepancies between the findings of the present training study and those of Toril et al.'s (2016) intervention study, which reported significant improvement by the experimental group after training with non-action video games in two computerized spatial WM tasks, the Corsi blocks and the Jigsaw puzzle task, and no change in a passive control group. In the present study, both groups improved their performance on the Corsi blocks after training. This suggests that playing strategy games also enhances visuospatial WM. This specific difference could be due to the fact that the games played by the active control group involve not only managing the characters' lives or a city but also traveling visually around the city to identify resources and opportunities. This visual navigation may be partly responsible for the results obtained in the visuospatial WM task. The discrepancy between the results of the two studies might thus be due to the type of control group, either passive (Toril et al., 2016) or active (the present study).

As indicated above, participants were randomly assigned to either the experimental group or the active control group before performing the cognitive tasks in the laboratory (pre-testing). However, the active control group was faster than the group trained with non-action cognitive video games in those cognitive tasks in which the dependent variable was response time (Oddball and Stroop-NP tasks). In the working memory tasks (Corsi Blocks and N-back tasks) in which the dependent variable was accuracy, experimental and control groups did not differ.

Limitations

A number of limitations of the present study need to be acknowledged. First, although the number of participants in the present study was larger than many previously published training studies, it is always desirable to include a large number of participants per condition to increase power. Null effects may reflect the lack of power and variability within the groups. For example, Melby-Lervåg et al. (2016) advised that studies with small sample sizes (<20 participants per condition) and passive (untreated) control groups produce a bias toward significant (although low-powered) results (see also Maraver et al., 2016). In the current study, there were more than 20 participants per group and the active control group was also trained with video games. Secondly, it is possible that the 16 training sessions were insufficient to show transfer, and that a longer or denser (more hours per week) training regime could have yielded greater enhancements. However, as mentioned in the Introduction, recent meta-analyses (Lampit et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2014) showed that shorter training regimes were better than longer ones that can lead to loss of motivation. For that reason, we decided to have only 16 training sessions in the current study. Thirdly, as mentioned above, in two previous studies, we included a passive control group. In the present study we did not include a passive control group to control for unspecific repetition effects. Instead, in this RCT, we included an active control group and almost double the number of participants compared to our previous studies (Ballesteros et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2016). The inclusion of a passive control group would not have determined whether the improvements observed were due to the specific video games used in the training regimes, to the use of iPads, or simply to social interaction with the trainer and the other participants during the training sessions (see Ballesteros et al., 2015b; Schmicker et al., 2016). Even if a significant group by session interaction would be found, the result could be due to different factors as mention in the section Introduction. Finally, the participants in the present study were older adults without cognitive impairment. It could be the case that these elders were performing at a high level and training with the video games would not produce greater benefits (Toril et al., 2014). In fact, Kable et al. (2017) in their RCT with young adults included both, an active control group and a passive control group assessed at pretest and post-test without any training. The results showed that the improvement observed in the passive group was comparable to that of the group trained with adaptive commercial cognitive games and the active control group (Kable et al., 2017).

Conclusion

In sum, further research is needed to ascertain whether computerized cognitive training improves executive functions, specifically selective attention and working memory, as well as everyday functioning in healthy older adults. While high levels of mental activity have been associated with both better cognitive performance and reduced risk of dementia (Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2006), in view of the modest benefits for untrained working memory and attentional functions of non-action video game training is vital to explore more deeply whether video games or other types of computerized cognitive training can improve executive functions in older adults (Foroughi et al., 2016; Motter et al., 2016; Simons et al., 2016) and whether there are stable relations between training with video games and cognitive abilities in general (McCabe et al., 2016). Special attention deserves multi-domain interventions that combine cognitive training with physical activity embedded in a social environment for supporting cognition and independent living of an increasing older adults population (Ballesteros et al., 2015a).

Ethics Statement

This clinical trial is registered on the ClinicalTrials.gov database (Clinicaltrials.gov ID: NCT02796508). The UNED's Ethical Review Board approved the trial. All the participants gave their written informed consent before the study started and were informed of their right to terminate participation at any time. The work described has not been published previously.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and study design: SB and JM; Programming experimental tasks; JM and AP; ER enrolled the participants, conducted the training sessions and collected the data with some help from other members of the group; Data analysis: JM, PT, AP, and JMR; Questionnaires: ER and JMR; Interpretation: All the authors; Manuscript preparation: SB with some support from JM, JMR, ER, AP, and PT; Final approval: All the authors.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by a grant from the Spanish Government (grant # PSI2013-41409-R) and by the Council of Madrid (S-BIO/0170/2006 and P2010/BMD-2349) to SB. AP is supported by an FPU grant and ER by an FPI fellowship (Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness) associated with this project. Lumosity provided free access to the video game training platform for the participants in this clinical trial. The funders and Lumosity had no role in study design, data collection and analyses, or preparation of the manuscript. The authors thank the UNED Associated Center Escuelas Pías (Madrid) for providing space to conduct the training sessions and to Vanessa Peinado for her help with the training.

References

Ackerman, P. L., Kanfer, R., and Calderwood, C. (2010). Use it or lose it? Wii brain exercise practice and reading for domain knowledge. Psychol. Aging 25, 753–766. doi: 10.1037/a0019277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrés, P., Guerrini, C., Phillips, L., and Perfect, T. (2008). Differential effects of aging on executive and automatic inhibitory mechanisms. Dev. Neuropsychol. 33, 101–123. doi: 10.1080/87565640701884212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrés, P., Parmentier, F- B. R., and Escera, C. (2006). The effect of age on involuntary capture of attention by irrelevant sounds: a test of the frontal lobe hypothesis of aging. Neuropsychology 44, 2564–2568. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., et al. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, J. A., and Gazzaley, A. (2015). Video games, cognitive exercise, and the enhancement of cognitive abilities. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 4, 160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

CrossRef Full Text

Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). “Working memory” in The Psychology of learning and Motivation, ed G. H. Bower (New York, NY: Academic Press), 47–89.

Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, D., Logie, R., and Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer's disease. A longitudianl study. Brain 114, 2521–2542. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.6.2521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288, 2271–2281. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, K., Edwards, J. D., and Ross, L. A. (2007). The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. 62, 19–31. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.special_issue_1.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., and Reales, J. M. (2004). Intact haptic priming in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease: evidence for dissociable memory systems. Neuropsychologia 44, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Kraft, E., Santana, S., and Tziraki, C. (2015a). Maintaining older brain functionality: a targeted review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 55, 453–477. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Mayas, J., Prieto, A., Toril, P., Pita, C., Ponce de León, L., et al. (2015b). A randomized controlled trial of brain training with non-action video games in older adults: results of the 3-month follow-up. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:45. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Mayas, J., Ruíz-Marquez, E., Prieto, A., Toril, P., Ponce de León, L., et al. (2017). Effects of video game training on behavioral and electrophysiological measures of attention and memory: protocol for a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res. Protoc. 6:e8. doi: 10.2196/resprot.6570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Prieto, A., Mayas, J., Toril, P., Pita, C., Ponce de León, L., et al. (2014). Training older adults with non-action video games enhances cognitive functions that decline with aging: a randomized controlled trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:277. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00277

CrossRef Full Text

Baniqued, P. L., Allen, C. M., Kranz, M. B., Johnson, K., Sipolins, A., Dickens, C., et al. (2015). Working memory, reasoning, and task switching training: transfer effects, limitations, and great expectations? PLoS ONE 10:e0142169. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baniqued, P. L., Kranz, M. B., Voss, M. W., Lee, H., Cosman, J. D., Severson, J., et al. (2014). Cognitive training with casual video games: points to consider. Front. Psychol. 4:1010. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baniquet, P. L., Lee, H., Voss, M. W., Basak, C., and Kramer, A. F. (2013). Selling points: what cognitive abilities are tapped by casual video games? Acta Psychol. 142, 74–86. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychol. Rev. 11, 1–29. doi: 10.1023/A:1009085417776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in real time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol. Aging 23, 765–777. doi: 10.1037/a0013494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bavelier, D., Gree, C. S., Schrater, P., and Pouget, A. (2012). Brain plasticity through the lifespan: learning to learn and action video games. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 391–416. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belleville, S., Chertkow, H., and Gauthier, S. (2007). Working memory and control of attention in persons with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropschology 21, 458–469. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. C., Martin, M., Zöllig, J., Röcke, C., Mérillat, A., Jäncke, L., et al. (2016). Multi-domain training enhances attentional control. Psychol. Aging 31, 390–408. doi: 10.1037/pag0000081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blacker, K. J., Curby, K. M., Klobusicky, E., and Chein, K. M. (2014). Effects of video game training on visual working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40, 1992–2004. doi: 10.1037/a0037556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boot, W. R., Blakely, D. P., and Simons, D. J. (2011). Do action video games improve perception and cognition? Front. Psychol. 2:226. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boot, W. R. (2015). Video games as tools to achieve insight into cognitive processes. Front. Psychol. 6:3. doi: 10.3389/978-2-88919-553-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boot, W. R., and Kramer, A. F. (2014). The brain-games conundrum: does cognitive training really sharpen the mind? Cerebrum 2014, 1–15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boot, W. R., Simons, D. J., Stothart, C., and Stutts, C. (2013). The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology: why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspect. Pychol. Sci. 8, 445–454. doi: 10.1177/1745691613491271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bopp, K. L., and Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. 60, 223–233. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.5.P223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borella, E., Carretti, B., Cantarella, A., Riboldi, F., Zavagnin, M., and Beni, R. (2014). Benefits of training visuospatial working memory in young-old and old-old. Dev. Psychol. 50, 714–727. doi: 10.1037/a0034293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borella, E., Carretti, B., Riboldi, F., and De Beni, R. (2010). Working memory training in older adults: of transfer and maintenance effects. Psychol. Aging 25, 767–778. doi: 10.1037/a0020683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brehmer, Y., Westerberg, H., and Backman, L. (2012). Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer and maintenance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:63. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buitenweg, J. I. V., Murre, J. M. J., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2012). Brain training in progress: a review of trainability in healthy seniors. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:183. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.0018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bürki, C., Ludwig, C., Chicherio, C., and de Ribaupierre, A. (2014). Individual differences in cognitive plasticity: an investigation of training curves in younger and older adults. Psychol. Res. 78, 821–835. doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0559-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., Hutchison, S., Perrig-Chiello, P., Däpp, C., Müller, M., et al. (2008). Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychol. Aging 23, 743–753. doi: 10.1037/a0014342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlin, E., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., and Stigsdotter Neely, A. (2008). Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: immediate gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol. Aging 23, 720–730. doi: 10.1037/a0014296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, N., Hongkeun, K., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Age-related Differences in brain activity during true and false memory retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 1390–1402. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dougherty, M. R., Hamovitz, T., and Tidwell, J. W. (2016). Reevaluating the effectiveness of n-back training on transfer through the Bayesian lens: support for the null. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 306–316. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0865-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. Practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–1998. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Foroughi, C. K., Monfort, S. S., Paczinski, M., McKnight, P. E., and Greenwood, P. M. (2016). Placebo effects in cognitive training. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 7470–7474. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601243113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glass, B. D., Maddox, W. T., and Love, B. C. (2013). Real-time strategy game training: Emergence of a cognitive flexibility trait. PLoS ONE 8:e70350. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423, 534–537. doi: 10.1038/nature01647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., Strobach, T., and Schubert, T. (2014). On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychol. Res. 78, 756–772. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0535-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, J. L., Nelson, R. A., Thomason, M. E., Stenberg, D. A., Katovich, K., Farzin, F., et al. (2015). Enhancing cognitive abilities with comprehensive training: a large, online, randomized, active-controlled trial. PLoS ONE 10:e0134467. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinzel, S., Schulte, S., Onken, J., Duong, Q.-L., Riemer, T. G., Heinz, A., et al. (2014). Working memory training improvemepnts and gains in non- trained cognitive tasks in Young WM training effects in aging and older adults. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 21, 146–173. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2013.790338

CrossRef Full Text

Huntley, J. D., and Howard, R. J. (2010). Working memory in early Alzeimer's disease: a neuropsychological review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25, 121–132. doi: 10.1002/gps.2314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kable, J. W., Caulfield, M. K., Falcone, M., McConnell, M., Bernardo, L., Parthasarathi, T., et al. (2017). No effect of commercial cognitive training on brain activity, choice behavior or cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 37, 7390–7402. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2832-16.2017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karback, J., and Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 25, 2027–2037. doi: 10.1177/0956797614548725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kueider, A. M., Parisi, J. M., Gross, A. L., and Rebok, G. W. (2012). Computerized cognitive training with older adults: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 7:e40588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lampit, A., Hallock, H., and Valenzuela, M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 11:e1001756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, R. W., Ngo, C., Nguyen, J., and Levi, D. M. (2011). Video-game play induces plasticity in the visual system in adults with amblyopia. PLoS Biol. 9:e1001135. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lussier, M., Bruillard, P., and Bherer, L. (2015). Limited benefits of heterogeneous dual-task training on transfer effects in older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 72, 1–13. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maraver, M. J., Bajo, M. T., and Gomez-Ariza, C. J. (2016). Training on working memory and inhibitory control in young adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:588. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayas, J., Parmentier, F. B. R., Andrés, P., and Ballesteros, S. (2014). Plasticity of attentional functions in older adults after non-action video game training. PLoS ONE 9:e92269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayeda, E. R., Whitmer, R. A., and Yaffe, K. (2015). Diabetes and cognition. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 31, 101–115. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2014.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, J., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. (2016). Brain training pessimism, but applied-memory optimism. Psychol. Sci. Public Int. 17, 187–191. doi: 10.1177/1529100616664716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., and Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 512–534. doi: 10.1177/1745691616635612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Br. Med. Bull. 27, 272–277. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monette, M. C. E., Baird, A., and Jackson, D. L. (2014). A meta-analysis of cognitive functioning in nondemented adults with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Can. J. Diabetes 38, 401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.jcjd.2014.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, A. B., and Chain, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 46–60. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0034-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motter, J. N., Devanand, D. P., Doraiswamy, P. M., and Sneed, J. R. (2016). Clinical trials to gain FDA approval for computerized cognitive training: What is the ideal control condition? Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:249. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mozolic, J. L., Long, A. B., Morgan, A. R., Rawley-Payne, M., and Laurienti, P. J. (2011). A cognitive training intervention improves modality-specific attention in a randomized controlled trial of healthy older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 655–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nap, H. H., de Kort, Y. A. W., and Ijselsteijn, W. A. (2009). Senior gamers: motivations, preferences, and needs. Gerontechnology 8, 247–262. doi: 10.4017/gt.2009.08.04.003.00

CrossRef Full Text

Nieboer, A., Lindenberg, S., Boosma, A., and Bruggen, A. C. V. (2005). Dimensions of wellbeing and their measurement: the SPF:IL Scale. Soc. Indic. Res. 73, 313–353. doi: 10.1007/s11205-004-0988-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nouchi, R., Taki, Y., Takeuchi, H., Hashizume, H., Nozawa, T., Kambara, T., et al. (2013). Brain training game boosts executive functions, working memory and processing speed in young adults: a randomized control trial. PLoS ONE 8:e55518. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oei, A. C., and Patterson, M. D. (2013). Enhancing cognition with video games: a multiple game training study. PLoS ONE 8:e58546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owsley, C., Sloane, M., McGwin, G., and Ball, K. (2002). Timed instrumental activities of daily living tasks: relationship to cognitive function and everyday performance assessments in older adults. Gerontology 48, 254–265. doi: 10.1159/000058360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Davidson, L., Lautenschlager, G., Smith, A. D., Smith, P., and Hedden, T. (2002). Models of visuo-spatial and verbal memory across the adult lifespan. Psychol. Aging 17, 299–320. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M., and Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information processing: meta-analytic investigation. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 20, 1055–1079. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0418-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences, and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddick, T. S., Shipstead, A., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., Kane, M. J., and Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized placebo-control study. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 359–379. doi: 10.1037/a0029082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redondo, M. T., Beltrán-Brotóns, J. L., Reales, J. M., and Ballesteros, S. (2016). Executive functions in patients with Alzheimer's disease, type 2 diabetes patients and cognitively healthy older adults. Exp. Gerontol. 83, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, B., and Fuller, B. (2004). N-Back Test Computer Software. Available online at: http://step.psy.cmu.edu/scripts-plus/ (Accessed June 21, 2012).

Schmicker, M., Schwefel, M., Vellage, A-K., and Müller, N. G. (2016). Training of attentional filtering, but not of memory storage, enhances working memory efficiency by strengthening the neural gatekeeper network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 636–642. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186. doi: 10.1177/1529100616661983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaniol, J., Davidson, P. S., Kim, A. S., Han, H., Moscovitch, M., and Grady, C. L. (2009). Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychology 47, 1765–1779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipper, S. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54, 321–343. doi: 10.1080/713755969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipper, S. P., and Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 37, 591–611. doi: 10.1080/14640748508400921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toril, P., Reales, J. M., and Ballesteros, S. (2014). Video game training enhances cognition of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Aging 29, 706–716. doi: 10.1037/a0037507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toril, P., Reales, J. M., Mayas, J., and Ballesteros, S. (2016). Brain training with video games enhances visuospatial working memory in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:206. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00206

CrossRef Full Text

Valenzuela, M. J., and Sachdev, P. (2006). Brain research and dementia: a systematic review. Psychol. Med. 36, 441–454. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Bastian, C. C., Langer, N., Jäncke, L., and Oberauer, K. (2013). Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Mem. Cognit. 41, 611–624. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0280-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, P., Liu, H. H., Zhu, X. T., Meng, T., Li, H. J., and Zuo, X. N. (2016). Action video game training for healthy adults: a meta-analytic study. Front. Psychol. 7:907. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wayne, K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 352–358. doi: 10.1037/h0043688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1999). WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Administration and Scoring Manual, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation and Harcourt Brace.

World Medical Association (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

CrossRef Full Text

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zinke, K., Zeintl, M., Eschen, A., Herzog, C., and Kliegel, M. (2012). Potentials and limits of plasticity induced by working memory training in old-old age. Gerontology 58, 79–87. doi: 10.1159/000324240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: selective attention, cognitive training, healthy aging, video games, working memory

Trial Registration: Clinicaltrials. gov ID: NCT02796508; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02796508

Citation: Ballesteros S, Mayas J, Prieto A, Ruiz-Marquez E, Toril P and Reales JM (2017) Effects of Video Game Training on Measures of Selective Attention and Working Memory in Older Adults: Results from a Randomized Controlled Trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:354. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00354

Received: 04 March 2017; Accepted: 17 October 2017;
Published: 01 November 2017.

Edited by:

Emil C. Toescu, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Elzbieta Szelag, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Poland
Carlo Sestieri, Università degli Studi “G. d'Annunzio” Chieti—Pescara, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Ballesteros, Mayas, Prieto, Ruiz-Marquez, Toril and Reales. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Soledad Ballesteros, mballesteros@psi.uned.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.