Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 01 November 2017
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

The Influence of Negative Emotion on Cognitive and Emotional Control Remains Intact in Aging

  • 1International Max Planck Research School on Neuroscience of Communication, Leipzig, Germany
  • 2Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 3Department Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
  • 4Department of Social Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
  • 5Institute of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
  • 6Institute of Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
  • 7Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

Healthy aging is characterized by a gradual decline in cognitive control and inhibition of interferences, while emotional control is either preserved or facilitated. Emotional control regulates the processing of emotional conflicts such as in irony in speech, and cognitive control resolves conflict between non-affective tendencies. While negative emotion can trigger control processes and speed up resolution of both cognitive and emotional conflicts, we know little about how aging affects the interaction of emotion and control. In two EEG experiments, we compared the influence of negative emotion on cognitive and emotional conflict processing in groups of younger adults (mean age = 25.2 years) and older adults (69.4 years). Participants viewed short video clips and either categorized spoken vowels (cognitive conflict) or their emotional valence (emotional conflict), while the visual facial information was congruent or incongruent. Results show that negative emotion modulates both cognitive and emotional conflict processing in younger and older adults as indicated in reduced response times and/or enhanced event-related potentials (ERPs). In emotional conflict processing, we observed a valence-specific N100 ERP component in both age groups. In cognitive conflict processing, we observed an interaction of emotion by congruence in the N100 responses in both age groups, and a main effect of congruence in the P200 and N200. Thus, the influence of emotion on conflict processing remains intact in aging, despite a marked decline in cognitive control. Older adults may prioritize emotional wellbeing and preserve the role of emotion in cognitive and emotional control.

Introduction

In everyday social interactions, we observe multiple non-verbal cues such as facial expressions, gestures, and vocalizations (e.g., during an informal talk with a colleague, in a job interview). For instance, simple vocalizations can describe people’s emotions, feelings, and even social attitudes (Mohammadi and Vinciarelli, 2011). Therefore, the ability to accurately perceive emotional vocalizations is an important aspect of social communication and hampered recognition of emotional states may lead to inappropriate social behavior and poor interpersonal communication (Shimokawa et al., 2001; Blair, 2005; Chaby et al., 2015). However, this task becomes even more challenging when there is conflicting emotional information, for instance, when the affective meaning of vocal and facial expressions are incongruent (e.g., in irony; Wang et al., 2007; Pexman, 2008; Watanabe et al., 2014; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Such conflict usually results in prolonged reaction times (RTs) and increased error rates (Stroop, 1935; Simon and Rudell, 1967; Egner and Hirsch, 2005; Ochsner et al., 2009) reflecting increased demand on limited attentional resources when processing incongruent information (West, 2003).

We recently showed that negative emotion can facilitate emotional conflict processing (Zinchenko et al., 2015) similarly to its influence on cognitive conflict processing (e.g., Stroop task) where a conflict arises between opposing non-emotional stimulus dimensions (Kanske and Kotz, 2010, 2011a,b). Negative emotions lead to faster processing of both cognitive and emotional conflicts, and to a conflict-specific event-related potentials (ERPs; Zinchenko et al., 2015).

More specifically, our previous study showed a conflict specific dissociation for the N100 (Zinchenko et al., 2015). The N100 is a negative deflection occurring approximately 100 ms after stimulus onset. The N100 is modulated by attention (Hillyard et al., 1973), emotion (Scott et al., 2009), and congruence (Pourtois et al., 2000; Atkinson et al., 2003). We reported a larger conflict effect for emotional than for neutral trials in the cognitive task and a larger conflict effect for neutral than for emotional trials in the emotional task (Zinchenko et al., 2015). Furthermore, the P200 (positive-going deflection approximately 200 ms after stimulus onset) has been shown to increase for emotional stimuli (Paulmann and Kotz, 2008; Kanske et al., 2011), while processing of incongruity leads to a decreased P200 amplitude (Kokinous et al., 2014). Additionally, previous findings showed a reduced P200 response to incongruent compared to congruent trials in both cognitive and emotional tasks (Kokinous et al., 2014; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Finally, the N200 has been repeatedly reported as a neural marker of cognitive control and conflict processing with a larger N200 amplitude in response to incongruent than congruent stimuli (Kopp et al., 1996a; Van Veen and Carter, 2002; Luck, 2005; Azizian et al., 2006). This effect has been found at fronto-central (Kopp et al., 1996b; Heil et al., 2000; Kanske and Kotz, 2011a) as well as at posterior electrode-sites (Hughes et al., 2009; Zinchenko et al., 2015).

However, we know little about age-related changes in how emotion affects the two types of conflict. As people get older their ability to process cognitive conflict declines (Hasher et al., 1991). Studies reported that older adults show larger conflict effects and exhibit consistently poorer performance on tasks that require inhibition of interference (Stoltzfus et al., 1993). Importantly, this increased conflict effect in the elderly is not the result of general slowing, but is thought to reflect age-related decline in executive control and inhibition of interference (Langenecker et al., 2004; Zysset et al., 2007). On the other hand, inhibition of affective interference seems improved in older adults. For instance, Samanez-Larkin et al. (2009) showed that older but not younger adults show no emotional conflict cost when the task is to judge the emotional valence of a target word flanked by congruent or incongruent emotional distractors (i.e., negative, positive). Nonetheless, both age groups showed interference on control trials that required processing of non-emotional targets and distractors (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2009). Importantly, the results showed that older adults had no difficulties in identifying the emotional valence of stimuli in general. These findings are consistent with the growing body of literature suggesting that the ability to process and regulate emotions effectively remains stable or even improves across the adult lifespan (Charles and Carstensen, 2007). According to the socio-emotional selectivity theory, older adults prioritize processing and regulation of emotional information; their goals increasingly emphasize emotional components of life (Carstensen, 1993, 2006; Carstensen et al., 1999). Thus, older adults can better overcome negative emotions and thoughts (Carstensen et al., 2000), regulate emotions and display less physiological arousal when experiencing negative emotions (Tsai et al., 2000; Samanez-Larkin and Carstensen, 2011) than younger adults.

Conversely, some studies showed that older adults display decreased accuracy in emotion recognition portrayed in facial expressions (pictures of emotional faces; e.g., Williams et al., 2009; Orgeta, 2010; Lambrecht et al., 2012), conveyed through prosodic cues (Kiss and Ennis, 2001; Paulmann et al., 2009; Mitchell et al., 2011; Lambrecht et al., 2012; Templier et al., 2015) and, particularly, through non-verbal vocal cues (e.g., screams or laughter; Hunter et al., 2010; Lima et al., 2014; Chaby et al., 2015).

Thus, there is mixed evidence regarding the processing of emotions in older adults. Most importantly, it is not clear whether and how the emotional quality of a target (emotional, neutral) influences cognitive and emotional conflict processing as a function of age. Moreover, when using an emotional conflict task, previous studies employed stimuli where both the target and non-targets were emotional and there was no neutral baseline control (Egner et al., 2008; Samanez-Larkin et al., 2009). Therefore, the present study tested whether in older adults negative compared to neutral stimuli influence cognitive and emotional conflict processing differentially.

We used an experimental protocol that varied the source of conflict between non-emotional (cognitive conflict) and emotional (emotional conflict) stimulus dimensions while using identical stimuli in the exploration of both conflict types. We also measured ERPs to study neural correlates of early conflict-specific perceptual processes (e.g., N100; Zinchenko et al., 2015) and the influence of emotion on the two types of conflict (N200; Bruin and Wijers, 2002; Kanske and Kotz, 2011a). In two ERP studies, participants saw multisensory dynamic stimuli: short video clips of actors pronouncing different vocalizations in a negative or neutral tone of voice. Auditory and visual coding of the stimuli could be either congruent or incongruent with respect to vocalization (cognitive conflict task, Experiment 1) or emotion (emotional conflict task, Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, participants named the vowel expressed by the voice (i.e., “A” or “O”) irrespective of its emotional quality (emotional or neutral). In Experiment 2, participants were asked to report whether the voice was emotional or neutral, irrespective of the emotional valence of the face (i.e., the source of conflict was emotional). Importantly, while stimuli were identical in both tasks, the source of conflict was either emotional or cognitive and the target dimension was either emotional or neutral. This allowed testing whether the affective quality of the target (emotional, neutral) influences executive control when it is task-irrelevant (cognitive conflict, Experiment 1) and task-relevant (emotional conflict, Experiment 2).

Emotional and cognitive information processed via multisensory channels optimizes behavioral responses in both younger (de Gelder and Vroomen, 2000; Kreifelts et al., 2007; Klasen et al., 2011) and older adults (Laurienti et al., 2006; Peiffer et al., 2007; Diederich et al., 2008; Hugenschmidt et al., 2009; DeLoss et al., 2013). Therefore, we used dynamic multisensory videos in order to achieve optimal performance (e.g., Jessen and Kotz, 2011; Klasen et al., 2012; Donohue et al., 2013). Based on previous findings and the aging literature, we hypothesized that older compared to younger adults may be more susceptible to interference in the cognitive conflict task (Langenecker et al., 2004; Zysset et al., 2007) and be less susceptible to affective interference in the emotional conflict task (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2009). We also expected that emotional targets would facilitate both types of conflict processing in younger adults (Kanske and Kotz, 2011a; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Whether or not we would observe similar facilitation in older adults was less clear as this has not yet been investigated. However, as several previous studies indicated preserved capabilities to process emotions in older participants (Mikels et al., 2005; Charles and Carstensen, 2007) and showed that both age groups use similar neural mechanisms to process emotions (Ebner et al., 2012), we expected that emotional targets would facilitate both types of conflicts in older adults as well.

While we found a larger conflict effect for emotional compared to neutral trials in the cognitive task and a larger conflict effect for neutral compared to emotional trials in the emotional task (Zinchenko et al., 2015), a comparable study on cognitive and emotional conflict processing did not observe a similar N1 dissociation (Alguacil et al., 2013). On the other hand, in line with previous findings, we expected to observe a reduced P200 response to incongruent than congruent trials in both cognitive and emotional tasks (Kokinous et al., 2014; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Furthermore, we expected to observe conflict-related N100 and P200 components either over anterior (Liu et al., 2012; Ho et al., 2015; Kokinous et al., 2015) or posterior electrode-sites (Gerdes et al., 2013; Ho et al., 2015).

Finally, we expected to observe larger N200 responses for incongruent compared to congruent stimuli in both age groups across the scalp (Zinchenko et al., 2015; Korsch et al., 2016). Since the modulation of the conflict-sensitive N200 response has been reported for unimodal stimuli (Kanske and Kotz, 2011b), but not for multisensory stimuli (Zinchenko et al., 2015), the question remains whether we would observe a larger N200 conflict effect for negative compared to neutral stimuli in the two conflict types and age groups.

Experiment 1

Methods

Participants

Twenty-six younger adults (female = 14, mean age = 25.2 years, range = 21–35) and 26 older adults (female = 13, mean age = 69.4 years, range = 63–78), all right-handed (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory score ME = 88.6, SD = 12.1) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and self-reported normal hearing participated in the two experiments. Participants underwent medical screening for any neurological history and were screened for past and present psychiatric disorders using the Structured Clinical Interview in DSM-IV (SCID-I; Wittchen et al., 1995) at the Day Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig. None of participants had a history of psychiatric/neurological disorders, alcoholism, or drug abuse. Drug abuse was further controlled in both age groups by an instant dipstick drug test (Drogentest Multi-8/2-DT, Diagnostik Nord) that examined the use of eight drugs (amphetamine, buprenorphine, benzodiazepines, cocaine, methamphetamine, morphine/opiates, methadone, and cannabis). Younger and older adults differed in mean years of education: younger adults (all 12 years), older adults [mean = 10.9 years, SD = 1.56, t(22) = 3.27, p < 0.01]. Both age groups were recruited from the database of the “Leipzig Cohort for Mind-Body-Emotion Interactions” (LEMON). Older adults completed a pure-tone audiometric screening: participants were included only if they had thresholds equal to or lower than 30 dB in both ears at frequencies crucial for speech perception (500–4000 Hz, Lima et al., 2014). Participants filled out the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (effortful and attentional control subscales, ATQ; Derryberry and Rothbart, 1988) and Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). Older adults showed lower levels of inhibitory control (4.5) relative to younger adults [5.29; t(50) = -4.49, p < 0.0001], lower levels of activation control [younger = 4.5, older = 5.58; t(50) = -3.84, p < 0.0001], lower attentional control: attentional shifting [younger = 4.7, older = 5.51; t(50) = -2.8, p < 0.01], and effortful control [younger = 4, older = 4.6; t(50) = -3.07, p < 0.01]. We also observed that older adults displayed higher levels of stress (13.31) than younger adults [7.76; t(50) = -3.39, p < 0.001], as well as higher levels of anxiety [younger = 2.3, older = 8.4; t(50) = -5.71, p < 0.0001] and depression [younger = 4.46, older = 9.85; t(50) = -3.17, p < 0.01]. Participants also rated the complete videos, video streams alone, and audio streams alone on a 7-point Likert scale using Self-Assessment Manikins for expressiveness, arousal, and emotion identification (Bradley and Lang, 1994; see Table 1). In order to control for the correct emotion identification in the younger and older adults, we used the ratings of the neutral stimuli by the younger adults from a previous study (Zinchenko et al., 2015) to extract the upper and lower boundaries of the confidence interval. If the neutral videos’ ratings of participants in the current study fell within the boundaries of the confidence interval, and the ratings of emotional videos fell outside of these boundaries, the participant identified emotion expressed in the stimuli correctly. This procedure ensured that participants were able to identify emotional stimuli, as the task required processing of emotions (three participants were not able to identify emotion of voices and were not included in further tests). Both groups showed no differences at the level of perceived expressiveness and arousal of the stimuli and rated emotional material as more emotional compared to neutral material (see Supplementary Material for details). All participants signed a consent form and received payment for their participation. The experiment followed the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Results of the video rating.

Stimulus Material

Stimuli, design, and procedure in the current study were identical to and validated in our previous work (Zinchenko et al., 2015). Stimuli consisted of short video clips of a male (27 years old) and a female (24 years old) semi-professional actors pronouncing the interjections “A” and “O” in a neutral and negative (i.e., angry) tone of voice (see Figure 1A). The sound in all videos was normalized to 70 dB using root mean square in Final Cut Pro. We created eight congruent and eight incongruent videos by matching or mismatching vocalizations of the face and voice (e.g., voice pronouncing “A” with facial lip movement corresponding to “A” vs. “O,” respectively). Videos were overlaid with the incongruent sound in Final Cut Pro 7 (Apple Inc.) using the onset of the original video sound for alignment. The duration of congruent and incongruent videos ranged between 1 and 2 s (Table 2). There were no time differences between conditions before the audio onset and the total video durations (see Supplementary Material for details).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A) Examples of video stimuli in Experiments 1 and 2: the female actor (the actor signed a consent form for the publication of these images) vocalized the interjections “A” and “O” in either a negative or a neutral tone of voice. Incongruence was created by mismatches in the vocalizations of audio and video components in Experiment 1, and mismatches in the expressed emotion of audio and video components in Experiment 2. (B) Example of a trial sequence.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Timing of video stimuli of Experiment 1.

To check whether videos differed with regards to movement, we estimated the number of movements per video clip by quantifying the variation of light intensity (luminance) between pairs of frames for each pixel (Pichon et al., 2008). The two emotions (neutral and emotional) and two vowels (“A” and “O”) were compared with a Kruskal–Wallis test. Emotional videos showed a higher number of movements relative to neutral videos (χ2 = 5.33, p < 0.05). As it is an inherent feature of angry expressions to be more dynamic and intense (e.g., Weyers et al., 2006) differences in mean motion were expected in naturalistic stimuli. Most importantly, differences in movement should not affect the outcome of the results as we focused on the interaction of congruence and emotion. Motion differences were not significant for the different vowels (χ2 = 1.25, p > 0.2).

Experiment 1 used a 2 (emotional, neutral) × 2 (congruent, incongruent) factorial design and was split into four blocks. Each block comprised 52 videos (26 emotional and 26 neutral, half were congruent and the other half incongruent) presented in a pseudo-randomized order. Overall, there were 208 trials and testing took approximately 45 min per participant.

Procedure

Participants sat in a dimly lit sound-attenuated booth about 1 m from a computer screen. The sound was delivered via headphones. Each trial started with a fixation cross on a blank computer screen for 200 ms (see Figure 1B). Subsequently, a video was presented and played in full duration (i.e., video was not cut off after the response). Participants were instructed to report whether the vocalization of the voice was “A” or “O” irrespective of the emotional quality. The emotion of the face and voice was always matched. To ensure that faces were not ignored, participants were sometimes (10% of all trials) asked a second question about the vocalization (lip movement, i.e., “A” or “O”) of the face. These additional questions were presented randomly, the response time to these questions was not limited, and these trials were not used for further analysis (all participants answered >90% questions correctly and were included for further analysis). The response time-window to the main question was 1500 ms, starting from voice onset. If participants did not respond within the response time-window, the words “try to respond faster” appeared on the screen for 200 ms. If participants answered incorrectly the word “incorrect” appeared on the screen. Button presses were counterbalanced across participants. An ITI of 1000, 1250, 1500, 1750, and 2000 ms was used randomly before the onset of the next trial.

EEG Recording and Pre-processing

EEG was recorded from 59 Ag/AgCl scalp electrodes (10–10 system). We used Brain Vision Recorder (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany) and a BRAINAMP amplifier (DC to 250 Hz). The sampling rate was 500 Hz. The left mastoid served as a reference, and the sternum as ground. The vertical and horizontal electro-oculogram was measured for artifact rejection purposes. The impedance was kept below 5 kΩ.

EEG data were analyzed using the FieldTrip (v0.20120501) toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011) running on Matlab 8.1 R2013a (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, United States). Electrodes were re-referenced offline to linked mastoids. Only correct trials were chosen for data processing. Extended epochs of 2000 ms before and after video onset were extracted. Epochs containing excessive muscle activity or jump artifacts were rejected. Data were band-pass filtered using a two-pass Butterworth IIR filter with a frequency pass-band of 0.1–100 Hz (order of 4). After the preprocessing, principal components analysis was applied in order to reduce dimensionality of the data and preserve α = 0.99 of the variance (Dien, 2012). An independent component analysis (ICA) was conducted using the fastica algorithm. Subsequently, components were visually inspected and those showing ocular, muscle, heart, and electrode artifacts were manually rejected (mean number of components removed = 11, SD = 4). In a subsequent step, individual epochs were visually scanned and epochs containing artifacts were manually discarded. Approximately 14% of trials (incorrect responses, artifacts) were excluded from further analysis.

Data Analysis

For the statistical analysis, smaller epochs were selected (-200 to 1000 ms time-locked to the voice onset). Single-subject averages were calculated for each session and condition. In accordance with previous literature (Kanske and Kotz, 2011a; Ho et al., 2015; Zinchenko et al., 2015), four regions of interest were defined: left anterior (FP1, AF3, AF7, F3, F5, F7, FC3, FC5, FT7), right anterior (FP2, AF4, AF8, F4, F6, F8, FC4, FC6, FT8), left posterior (CP3, CP5, TP7, P3, P5, P7, PO3, PO7, O1), and right posterior (CP4, CP6, TP8, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO8, O2). Peak latencies were detected separately for each participant and each condition within the following time windows: 70–110 ms (N100), 140–225 ms (P200) as suggested by Luck and Kappenman (2011). The time window for the N200 was manually selected after visual inspection of the ERPs: 240–340 ms for younger adults and 280–380 ms for older adults. This was done due to the delayed latency of the N200 in older adults (Enoki et al., 1993; Anderer et al., 1996; Luck and Kappenman, 2011). A 20 ms time window was applied before and after each of the individual peaks for a mean amplitude analysis. For each time window a repeated-measures ANOVA was calculated, using emotion (emotional, neutral), congruence (congruent, incongruent), region (anterior, posterior) and side (left, right) as within-subject factors and age (younger, older) as a between-subject factor. As videos prior to voice onset in Experiment 1 were identical (as emotion of face and voice was kept constant and only vocalization varied) no baseline correction was applied before the voice onset (Zinchenko et al., 2015, 2017). Only statistically significant main effects and interactions that involved the critical factors emotion, congruence, and group are reported in the section “Results.”

Finally, as previous findings found a correlation between participants’ interpersonal characteristics (e.g., level of effortful control, anxiety, etc.) and the influence of emotion on executive attentional control (Kanske and Kotz, 2012) we have also ran an exploratory correlation analysis between the emotion-modulated N100 conflict effect [(negative incongruent - negative congruent) - (neutral incongruent - neutral congruent)] and the questionnaires data (i.e., ATQ, DASS).

Results

Behavioral Data

RT data

The main effect of age was significant as older adults produced overall longer RTs than younger adults [see Figure 2 and Table 3; younger = 502 ms, SD = 108 ms; older = 626 ms, SD = 96 ms; F(1,50) = 21.528, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.301]. Emotional stimuli resulted in longer RTs than neutral stimuli [emotional = 585 ms, SD = 129 ms; neutral = 543 ms, 110 ms; F(1,50) = 66.27, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.570]. Furthermore, we observed an interaction of emotion by age [F(1,50) = 10.92, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.192]: the main effect of emotion was larger for older adults [54 ms, SD = 11 ms; F(1,25) = 41.89, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.626] than for younger adults [30 ms; SD = 17 ms; F(1,25) = 24.39, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.494]. Similarly, the RTs were prolonged for incongruent compared to congruent stimuli [congruent = 515 ms, SD = 111 ms; incongruent = 630 ms, SD = 135 ms; F(1,50) = 141.23, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.739]. An interaction of congruence by age was also significant [F(1,50) = 141.23, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.739] and confirmed that the conflict effect was larger for older [114 ms, SD = 11 ms; F(1,25) = 67.47, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.730] than for younger adults [81 ms, SD = 4 ms; F(1,25) = 85.99, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.775].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Reaction times (A) and error rates (B) data [mean + standard error of the mean (SEM)] for younger and older adults in the congruent and incongruent/emotional and neutral conditions of Experiment 1 (left) and Experiment 2 (right). Denotes a significant main effect or interaction at p < 0.05.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Behavioral RT results of Experiments 1 and 2 per age group.

Finally, congruence was modulated by emotion [F(1,50) = 5.92, p < 0.02, ηp2 = 0.106]: the conflict effect was smaller for negative [89 ms, SD = 2 ms; F(1,50) = 94.137, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.653] than for neutral stimuli [106 ms, SD = 13 ms; F(1,50) = 151.58, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.752]. This effect was comparable in both age groups as the interaction of emotion by congruence by age was not significant [younger = 23 ms, SD = 1 ms; older = 10 ms, SD = 20 ms; F(1,50) = 0.87, p > 0.3, ηp2 = 0.017].

Error rate

Participants made more errors in response to incongruent (2.9%) than to congruent (0.86%) trials [F(1,50) = 5.06, p < 0.03, ηp2 = 0.108]. No other effects reached a level of significance (all p > 0.1).

ERP Data

N100 range

The main effect of congruence was significant [Figure 3; F(1,50) = 28.75, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.365], as well as an interaction of congruence by emotion [F(1,50) = 9.32, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.166]. As hypothesized, the conflict effect was larger in negative emotion stimuli [F(1,50) = 33.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.399] relative to neutral stimuli [F(1,50) = 3.64, p > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.067] and this effect was comparable across the both groups, as an interaction of emotion by congruence by age was not significant [F(1,50) = 0.101, p > 0.7, ηp2 = 0.002]. Additionally, we found an interaction of emotion by age [F(1,50) = 23.99, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.324]. Follow-up analysis revealed that negative relative to neutral stimuli resulted in overall increased amplitudes in younger adults [F(1,25) = 9.46, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.275] and this effect was reversed in older adults [F(1,25) = 14.60, p > 0.001, ηp2 = 0.369].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. (A) ERP waveforms at selected averaged electrodes [left anterior (FP1, AF3, AF7, F3, F5, F7, FC3, FC5, FT7), right anterior (FP2, AF4, AF8, F4, F6, F8, FC4, FC6, FT8), left posterior (CP3, CP5, TP7, P3, P5, P7, PO3, PO7, O1), and right posterior (CP4, CP6, TP8, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO8, O2)] depicting the conflict effect for emotional and neutral trials in Experiment 1. (B) Conflict effect represented as a topographic distribution of amplitude differences (incongruent–congruent) for each of the ERP components (i.e., N100, P200, and N200).

P200 range

We found an interaction of congruence by age [F(1,50) = 7.45, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.130]. Follow-up analysis revealed that congruent relative to incongruent stimuli elicited enhanced amplitude in younger adults [F(1,25) = 6.58, p < 0.02, ηp2 = 0.208], but not in older adults [F(1,25) = 1.16, p > 0.2, ηp2 = 0.045].

N200 range

The main effect of congruence was not significant [F(1,50) = 1.97, p > 0.1, ηp2 = 0.038], but we observed an interaction of region by congruence [F(1,50) = 15.17, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.233]. We resolved this interaction by region incongruent relative to congruent stimuli elicited increased amplitude in the posterior brain region [F(1,50) = 8.29, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.249] and not anterior region [F(1, 50) = 1.24, p > 0.2, ηp2 = 0.047].

We have further run a partial correlation (controlling for age) between the emotion-modulated N100 conflict effect [(negative incongruent - negative congruent) - (neutral incongruent - neutral congruent)] and the questionnaires data (i.e., ATQ, DASS). We found a significant negative correlation between the attentional shifting and the emotion modulated N100 conflict effect (r = -0.344, p = 0.007). This shows that lower levels of attentional shifting correlate with an increased benefit of negative emotions in the conflict sensitive N100. We also found a comparable correlation for effortful control (r = -0.259, p = 0.033), but this correlation did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Finally, we also observed that the levels of stress (r = -0.308, p = 0.02) and anxiety (r = -0.302, p = 0.022) negatively correlated with the emotional N100 conflict effect, and this correlation was absent for the neutral emotional N100 conflict effect (all ps > 0.3). More specifically, lower levels of stress and anxiety correlated with a larger emotional N100 conflict effect but not the neutral conflict effect.

In summary, Experiment 1 tested the influence of task-irrelevant emotion on cognitive conflict processing in two age groups. We show that emotion facilitates RT conflict processing by reducing the conflict effect in both age groups. Further, negative emotion modulates cognitive conflict in the N100 and P200 time-ranges. Interestingly, older adults showed a valence-specific N100 conflict effect only at posterior electrode-site. Finally, emotion facilitated the N200 conflict effect in the older but not in younger adults, possibly reflecting prioritized processing of emotional information in older adults. To test an age-specific influence of the task relevance of emotion on emotional conflict processing we conducted Experiment 2.

Experiment 2

Methods

Participants

Participants who took part in Experiment 1 also participated in Experiment 2.

Stimulus Material and Procedure

We modified the original videos of Experiment 1 and created 12 congruent and 12 incongruent videos by matching or mismatching the emotional valence of the face and voice (e.g., voice pronouncing a neutral “A” and the corresponding audio “A” was pronounced emotionally, Figure 1). To create incongruent trials with a neutral target voice (i.e., audio trace), the original emotional facial video streams were combined with neutral auditory streams. Similarly, the originally neutral facial video streams were combined with emotional auditory streams to create incongruent trials with an emotional target. The original voice onset was used to align the incongruent voice with the lip movement in both incongruent conditions. The vocalization of the face and voice was always matched. Participants were instructed to report whether the voice was either emotional or neutral.

The duration of congruent and incongruent videos ranged between 1 and 2 s (Table 4). There were no differences between conditions in time before the audio onset and total video durations (see Supplementary Material for details).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Timing of video stimuli of Experiment 2.

Experiment 2 used a 2 (emotional, neutral) × 2 (congruent, incongruent) factorial design and consisted of four blocks. Each block comprised 52 videos (26 emotional and 26 neutral, half were congruent and the other half incongruent) presented in random order. To ensure that faces were not ignored, probe questions were introduced (10% of all trials), where participants had to report the emotional expression of the face. These trials were not used for further analysis (all participants answered >90% of questions correctly and were included for further analysis). Overall, 208 trials were presented and testing took approximately 45 min per participant.

EEG Recording and Pre-processing

The EEG recording and pre-processing were identical to Experiment 1. Artifact components were removed after ICA (mean = 16, SD = 3.6). Approximately 17% of the trials (incorrect responses, artifacts) were excluded from further analysis.

Data Analysis

Data analysis was identical to that of Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, a voice target (emotional or neutral) was preceded by either a congruent or an incongruent video. As we found motion differences for emotional compared to neutral videos (see Supplementary Material for details), a 200 ms baseline correction was applied before the voice onset to account for these differences.

Results

Behavioral Data

RT data

The main effect of age was significant as older adults produced overall longer RTs than younger adults [see Figure 2; younger = 526 ms, SD = 66 ms; older = 740 ms, SD = 133 ms; F(1,50) = 63.918, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.561]. Furthermore, we observer an interaction of emotion by age group [F(1,50) = 9.32, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.157]. Emotional stimuli resulted in longer RTs in older adults [48 ms, SD = 10 ms; F(1,25) = 12.96, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.341] but not in younger adults [-9 ms, SD = 8 ms; F(1,25) = 1.39, p > 0.2, ηp2 = 0.053]. In a similar vein, the main effect of conflict varied as a function of age [interaction of congruence by age was significant, F(1,50) = 31.81, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.389]: the main effect of congruence was larger in older adults [132 ms, SD = 11 ms; F(1,25) = 93.78, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.790] than in younger adults [48 ms, SD = 6 ms; F(1,25) = 68.81, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.734].

Finally, negative emotion influenced emotional control in an age-specific manner [emotion by congruence by age, F(1,50) = 13.79, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.216]: the interaction of emotion by congruence was significant in older adults [F(1,25) = 17.95, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.418] but not in younger adults [F(1,25) = 2.13, p = 0.157, ηp2 = 0.078]. In older adults, the conflict effect was larger for neutral stimuli [170 ms, SD = 5 ms; F(1,25) = 160.25, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.865] than for emotional stimuli [94 ms, SD = 17 ms; F(1,25) = 25.23, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.502].

Error rate

We found a main effect of age as older adults made more errors than younger adults [F(1,50) = 4.14, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.081]. The interaction of congruence by age was also significant [F(1,50) = 10.29, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.179]: the main effect of congruence was larger in older adults [congruent = 1.5%, incongruent = 11.27%; F(1,25) = 21.49, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.452] than in younger adults [congruent = 2.5%, incongruent = 4.37%; F(1,25) = 7.14, p < 0.02, ηp2 = 0.254].

ERP Data

N100 range

As expected, the main effect of congruence was significant [Figure 4; F(1,50) = 12.86, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.205], as well as an interaction of congruence by emotion [F(1,50) = 5.52, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.099]. The conflict effect was larger in negative emotion stimuli [F(1,50) = 13.69, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.215] relative to neutral stimuli [F(1,50) = 0.904, p > 0.3, ηp2 = 0.018] and this effect was comparable across the both groups, as an interaction of emotion by congruence by age was not significant [F(1,50) = 0, p > 0.9, ηp2 = 0].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. (A) ERP waveforms at selected averaged electrodes [left anterior (FP1, AF3, AF7, F3, F5, F7, FC3, FC5, FT7), right anterior (FP2, AF4, AF8, F4, F6, F8, FC4, FC6, FT8), left posterior (CP3, CP5, TP7, P3, P5, P7, PO3, PO7, O1), and right posterior (CP4, CP6, TP8, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO8, O2)] depicting the conflict effect for emotional and neutral trials in Experiment 2. (B) Conflict effects are represented as a topographic distribution of amplitude differences (incongruent–congruent) for each of the ERP components (i.e., N100, P200, and N200).

P200 range

We found an interaction of region by congruence [F(1,50) = 8.84, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.150]. Follow-up analysis revealed that congruent stimuli elicited increased amplitude relative to incongruent stimuli in the posterior brain region [F(1,50) = 5.32, p < 0.03, ηp2 = 0.096], but not in the anterior brain region [F(1,50) = 0.165, p > 0.6, ηp2 = 0.003]. This effect was comparable across both age groups, as an interaction of region by congruence by age was not significant [F(1,50) = 2.55, p > 0.1, ηp2 = 0.049].

We also observed an interaction of region by emotion [F(1,50) = 15.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.234], as well as an interaction of region by emotion by age [F(1,50) = 11.37, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.185]. Follow-up analysis showed that an interaction of region by emotion was significant in older adults [F(1,25) = 19.76, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.442], but not in younger adults [F(1,25) = 0.223, p > 0.6, ηp2 = 0.009]. In older adults, emotional stimuli elicited reduced amplitude relative to neutral stimuli in the anterior brain region [F(1,25) = 16.81, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.402], but not in the posterior brain region [F(1,25) = 1.61, p > 0.2, ηp2 = 0.06].

N200 range

We observed an interaction of region by emotion [F(1,50) = 6.67, p < 0.02, ηp2 = 0.118] and region by emotion by age [F(1,50) = 5.1, p < 0.03, ηp2 = 0.093]. We resolved this interaction by age and found a significant two-way interaction of region by emotion in younger adults [F(1,25) = 13.72, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.354], but not in older adults [F(1,25) = 0.046, p > 0.8, ηp2 = 0.002]. In younger adults, neutral relative to emotional stimuli elicited increased amplitude in the anterior brain region [F(1,25) = 43.016, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.632] and posterior region [F(1,25) = 13.39, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.349].

Finally, we found an interaction of region by congruence [F(1,50) = 7.98, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.138] and region by congruence by age [F(1,50) = 7.24, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.127]. Further analysis showed an interaction of region by congruence in the older adults [F(1,25) = 24.39, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.494], but not in younger adults [F(1,25) = 0.006, p > 0.9, ηp2 = 0]. In older adults, congruent stimuli elicited increased amplitude relative to incongruent stimuli in the anterior brain region [F(1,25) = 4.36, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.149], but not posterior brain region [F(1,25) = 1.14, p > 0.2, ηp2 = 0.044].

With regard to the correlation analyses, in the emotional conflict task we only observed a positive correlation between activation control (i.e., the capacity to perform an action when there is a strong tendency to avoid it) and the emotionally modulated N100 conflict effect (r = 0.314, p = 0.012). That is, higher levels of activation control were associated with increased benefits of emotional stimuli in the N100 conflict effect.

In summary, Experiment 2 set out to test the role of emotionality of the target dimension in emotional conflict processing as a function of age. Consistent with our predictions, we show that emotional stimuli facilitated emotional conflict processing by reducing the RT conflict effect for older adults. Younger adults also showed a numerical trend in the same direction. The N100 responses showed an age-independent valence-specific reversal: incongruent negative stimuli elicited a larger amplitude than congruent stimuli, while neutral congruent trials elicited a larger amplitude than incongruent trials. Finally, we found an interaction of emotion and congruence in the P200 and N200 range across both age groups.

Discussion

The present study tested the role of aging and the influence of emotion on cognitive and emotional conflict processing by using behavioral measures and ERPs of the EEG. In the following, we separately discuss the results of each conflict type and conclude with a general discussion.

Cognitive Conflict

In the cognitive conflict task, participants named the vowel expressed by the voice (i.e., “A” or “O”) irrespective of its emotional quality. Consequently, older adults produced overall longer RTs and took longer to respond to emotional than to neutral stimuli. Emotion sped up the response to conflict in both age groups. Finally, emotion modulated the N200 conflict effect in older adults only, while we observed valence-specific conflict-sensitive N100 and P200 responses in both age groups.

First, we were able to replicate previous findings and observed that negative emotion facilitate cognitive conflict processing by reducing the RT conflict effect (Kanske and Kotz, 2010, 2011a; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Emotional stimuli attract attention due to their motivational relevance for survival (Ohman et al., 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2001) and trigger cognitive control processes (Norman and Shallice, 1986). Importantly, the magnitude of facilitation was similar in both younger and older adults. In other words, although older adults showed larger conflict effects and prolonged RTs to emotional stimuli, they preserved the emotional modulation of cognitive control (Dolcos et al., 2014).

In the EEG results we observed that the N100 conflict effect was larger for emotional than for neutral stimuli (Zinchenko et al., 2015). Importantly, we found this response pattern in both younger and older adults. In other words, negative emotion facilitates cognitive conflict processing as early as 100 ms after stimulus onset in both age groups, probably, by attracting additional attention to the target (Egner and Hirsch, 2005; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Interestingly, the conflict effect was absent in the N100 response to neutral stimuli of older but not younger adults (Hammerer et al., 2010). Conversely, the conflict-sensitive N100 was comparable across age groups when stimuli were emotional. A possible explanation could be that older adults benefit the most from processing of emotional compared to neutral stimuli (Scheibe and Carstensen, 2010). These results are also in line with findings that older rather than younger adults focus more on emotional information and that they can use emotional information during cognitive processing to compensate for their cognitive deficits (Scheibe and Blanchard-Fields, 2009; Mather, 2012; Mammarella et al., 2016). In other words, while negative emotion facilitates conflict processing in both age groups, older adults may be the primary beneficiaries in this process. In line with this idea, older adults also showed a reduced N100 for emotional relative to neutral trials, and this effect was reversed in younger adults. This shows that while emotional stimuli result in comparable neural and behavioral modulation of the conflict effect, the two groups may process emotional stimuli in an age-specific manner.

We also found a conflict effect in the P200 of younger but not older adults: incongruent P200 responses were smaller than congruent P200 responses. Increased allocation of attention has been linked to a decrease in the P200 amplitude (Crowley and Colrain, 2004; Kokinous et al., 2014). Therefore, we interpret the reduced P200 response in incongruent trials as an increase in attentional allocation to the target when it is incongruent (Etkin et al., 2006; Egner et al., 2008). Interestingly, while younger participants displayed an increased P200 response in incongruent trials, this effect was absent in older adults, that is, older adults showed no conflict effect in the P200 stimuli. Hammerer et al. (2010) reported that the magnitude of the conflict effect reduces with age, and older adults show a smaller ERP response in inhibition (also, see Kok, 2000 for a review). Therefore, the reduced ERP conflict effect goes along with an increased RT conflict cost.

Furthermore, we observed a conflict-related N200 response (Bruin and Wijers, 2002; Ho et al., 2015). Importantly, the N200 conflict effect was observed in both younger and older adults (Kokinous et al., 2014; Zinchenko et al., 2015). The N200 is evoked during tasks that require the inhibition of pre-potent responses, when two or more incompatible response tendencies are activated simultaneously (Van Veen and Carter, 2002; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003). Therefore, the current results reflect conflict processing in both age groups.

To summarize, although older adults showed an overall decline in cognitive control and prolonged responses to emotional stimuli, the influence of emotion on cognitive conflict processing was preserved in the elderly. Importantly, the neural correlates of affective facilitation of cognitive control point to the particular benefit for the elderly from processing emotional information.

Emotional Conflict

In the emotional conflict task, participants reported the emotional valence of a presented vowel irrespective of whether the emotion expressed in the face was congruent or not, while the lip movement (face) and vocalization (voice) were congruent and task-irrelevant. As a result, older adults responded more slowly and displayed larger conflict effects than younger adults. Further, the behavioral results reveal that emotion facilitates conflict processing but only in older participants. The N100 response showed a valence-specific conflict effect in both age groups: incongruent stimuli elicited larger N100 amplitude than congruent stimuli, and this effect was greater in negative relative to neutral stimuli. Furthermore, we found conflict- and valence-specific effects in the P200 and N200 responses.

Interestingly, emotional stimuli facilitated RT conflict processing in older adults, while younger adults only showed a numerical trend toward significance. A previous study showed that negative emotion targets facilitate emotional conflict processing (Zinchenko et al., 2015). Processing of emotional stimuli enhances the readiness to act (Oatley and Jenkins, 1996) and speeds up executive control not only in cognitive conflict but also in emotional conflict processing (Kanske, 2012; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Importantly, our findings confirm that the elderly preserve the beneficial role of negative emotion in the emotional conflict processing.

Contrary to the results of Samanez-Larkin et al. (2009), we observed a large conflict effect in emotional incongruence in both younger and older adults. In other words, older adults showed no advantage over younger adults in emotional conflict processing. One of the possible explanations for this is that the current study used dynamic multisensory stimuli, which made the task very demanding and resulted in larger conflict for older compared to younger adults. Indeed, Samanez-Larkin et al. (2009) observed no difference in the error rates between younger and older adults, while in our task this difference was substantial. Previous studies showed that older adults particularly benefit from processing of multisensory stimuli (Laurienti et al., 2006; Peiffer et al., 2007; Diederich et al., 2008; Hugenschmidt et al., 2009; DeLoss et al., 2013). However, our findings imply that incongruent multisensory stimuli may, in turn, be especially difficult to process for older adults.

Furthermore, we found valence-specific conflict effect in the N100: incongruent stimuli elicited a larger N100 response to incongruent trials in the negative emotion condition. Importantly, this effect was absent in the neutral condition. Zinchenko et al. (2015) suggested that the valence-specific dissociation in the N100 reflects a conflict-specific processing mechanism. In more detail, unlike cognitive conflict, emotional conflict processing has been found to involve facilitated inhibition of the task-irrelevant emotional distractor dimension of a stimulus (Etkin et al., 2006; Egner et al., 2008). Furthermore, studies showed that the inhibition of neutral task-irrelevant distractors is less demanding than to emotional distractors (Ohman et al., 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2001). Therefore, we assume that the observed N100 conflict effect for emotional rather than neutral stimuli may reflect a valence-specific mechanism of inhibition effective in emotional conflict processing.

In line with our previous findings, incongruent stimuli produced reduced P200 at posterior but not anterior electrode-sites (Zinchenko et al., 2015). A decreased P200 response has been linked to an increase in the allocation of attention (Crowley and Colrain, 2004). Therefore, a reduced P200 response in incongruent trials could imply attentional capture by mismatching vocalizations. As incongruent stimuli require more resources to be inhibited than neutral stimuli (Algom et al., 2004; Dolcos and McCarthy, 2006), we interpret the observed results as less allocation of attention to the vocalization when the distractor was incongruent rather than congruent. Importantly, we show that this effect holds true for older adults as well.

Finally, we also report a conflict-sensitive N200 (Bruin and Wijers, 2002; Kokinous et al., 2014; Ho et al., 2015; Zinchenko et al., 2015). Interestingly, this conflict effect appeared in older but not in younger adults. Moreover, unlike in the cognitive conflict task, incongruent stimuli resulted in a reduced N200 amplitude in older adults. Previous findings suggested that the N200 indexes conflict monitoring and its amplitude reflects the extent to which attentional control is required for conflict processing (Van Veen and Carter, 2002; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003; Dennis and Chen, 2007). Therefore, a reduced N200 in response to incongruent stimuli may reflect reduced effortful control and the impeded ability to engage executive processes in older relative to younger adults (Posner and Rothbart, 2007). This is in line with the increased number of incorrect responses to incongruent stimuli for older relative to younger adults.

General Discussion and Limitations

In contrast to substantial cognitive and physical decline, older adults seem to maintain high levels of affective well-being and emotional stability (Scheibe and Carstensen, 2010). Our data show that negative emotions can effectively modulate cognitive and emotional control in healthy aging, when using multisensory real-life stimuli. An important implication of the current findings is that healthy older adults preserve the ability to process conflicting emotional information (e.g., in irony; Wang et al., 2007; Pexman, 2008; Watanabe et al., 2014; Zinchenko et al., 2015). In this regard, it was shown that the correct recognition of emotional states facilitates social behavior and interpersonal communication (Shimokawa et al., 2001; Blair, 2005; Chaby et al., 2015). Therefore, the present results provide an important contribution to understanding of changes in emotional competencies in healthy aging (Doerwald et al., 2016).

Although emotion facilitated both cognitive and emotional conflict processing in older adults, the neural correlates of conflict processing were different. More specifically, in the cognitive conflict task we observed the main effect of congruence in the N200 over the posterior electrode-sites, while in the emotional conflict task the conflict-sensitive N200 was found at anterior electrode-sites. Various theories attempted to describe age-related decline in executive functions. The frontal aging theory postulates that reduced conflict-sensitive ERP activity over frontal electrode-sites may arise from structural and functional deterioration of the frontal lobes (Lucci et al., 2013). On the other hand, the age-related posterior–anterior shift theory argues that age-related increased frontal lobe activity is a consequence of decreased activity in posterior brain regions (Davis et al., 2008; St. Jacques et al., 2009). Our findings indirectly support both assumptions that vary as a function of how a task engages conflict-specific mechanisms (Egner and Hirsch, 2005; Etkin et al., 2006). In particular, cognitive conflict processing (Experiment 1) amplifies the processing of the stimulus target dimension (Egner and Hirsch, 2005), while emotional conflict processing (Experiment 2) seems to lead to facilitated inhibition of the non-target emotional distractor dimension of a stimulus (Etkin et al., 2006). Therefore, our findings add important evidence to the literature that aging does not affect conflict processes equally but rather shows conflict-specific modulations of interference control (Kawai et al., 2012; Sebastian et al., 2013). These results are particularly compelling as we used the same multisensory stimuli in the two different conflict tasks.

In cognitive conflict processing, task-irrelevant emotion prolonged RTs in both age groups. It has been shown that salient emotional stimuli capture attention and need longer to be processed (Compton, 2003; Vuilleumier, 2005). These findings are consistent with previous study that tested the role of negative emotion in cognitive conflict processing using the same multisensory audio-visual stimuli (Zinchenko et al., 2015), but not consistent with previous studies that used unimodal audio (Kanske and Kotz, 2011a) and visual stimuli (Kanske and Kotz, 2011b) and did not observe overall increased responses for emotional stimuli. Possibly, multisensory dynamic stimuli elicit stronger emotional responses and hinder the RTs relative to unimodal stimuli. Importantly, negative emotion has consistently been show to facilitate cognitive control and reduce the conflict effect (i.e., incongruent–congruent) irrespective of stimulus type. On the other hand, older, but not younger adults showed prolonged responses to negative relative to neutral stimuli in emotional conflict task. Older adults prioritize processing of emotional information and preserve the ability to process and regulate emotions, despite a general decline in executive control (Charles and Carstensen, 2007; St. Jacques et al., 2010). Possibly, the preserved ability to process emotional information in healthy aging happens at the expense of prolonged responses. Additionally, task-relevance may modulate the influence of negative emotion on the speed of responses in older adults and may possibly cause the observed differences across conflict types (Lichtenstein-Vidne et al., 2012). Therefore, the influence of negative emotion on response duration may potentially depend on the stimulus characteristics (unimodal, multisensory) and task relevance (task-relevant, -irrelevant), and future studies will need to address these points more systematically.

People of different ages are more likely to attend to and report more exposure to faces of their own than another age group (Anastasi and Rhodes, 2005; Lamont et al., 2005; Ebner and Johnson, 2010; Ebner et al., 2012). However, in the present study we only used videos of younger individuals, which could limit the observed effects as older adults may have processed faces of younger adults differently relative to their own-age faces (He et al., 2011). This factor should be further considered in future studies. In line with previous findings (Kanske and Kotz, 2012), our current results indicated that participants’ interpersonal characteristics modulate the role of emotions on cognitive and emotional conflict processing. We show that individual abilities to initiate and shift attention, as well as general levels of stress and anxiety modulate the role of emotion in conflict processing (Kanske and Kotz, 2012). Therefore, future studies should also consider inter-individual parameters when examining emotions and executive control. Lastly, as the current study concentrated on negative emotions, one question that remains is whether we would observe the same result in positive emotional conflicts in older participants as in younger adults (Zinchenko et al., 2017). For instance, while some previous studies reported a positivity effect: a trend in older adults to preferentially orient their attention to and remember more positive than negative or neutral stimuli (Kennedy et al., 2004; Mather and Carstensen, 2005; Sasse et al., 2014), other studies have failed to replicate such a positivity effect in older adults (Murphy and Isaacowitz, 2008; Steinmetz et al., 2010). Since previous studies showed that older adults benefit from processing of multisensory information (Hugenschmidt et al., 2009; DeLoss et al., 2013), we hypothesize that salient multimodal real-life stimuli (see Zinchenko et al., 2017) may clarify the role of positivity in processing of emotional information (Reed and Carstensen, 2012). It was proposed that the effect of positivity might operate during an active regulation of emotional states (Isaacowitz and Blanchard-Fields, 2012). Therefore, it is possible that the positivity effect can be observed when emotion is task-relevant (in an emotional conflict task), relative to a cognitive conflict task, when emotion is task-irrelevant. More specifically, it is plausible that older adults may show no cost of processing of neutral stimuli in the presence of a positive emotion distractor (Zinchenko et al., 2017). Alternatively, a meta-analysis by Reed et al. (2014) showed that positivity effect is larger in studies that do not constrain cognitive processing. As both cognitive and emotional conflict tasks tax executive functions, it is plausible that the age-specific positivity effect may be absent in these two tasks. Therefore, the question remains whether positive emotions influence cognitive and emotional control differently as a function of age. Future studies should address these questions in more detail.

Conclusion

While aging brings apparent changes to cognitive functions such as executive control, memory, attention, and processing speed, it seems to have little effect on the way processing of task-relevant and -irrelevant emotional information influences the two investigated control systems. The influence of emotion on cognitive and emotional conflict processing remains intact in age, possibly due to age-related changes in motivation and/or differential rates of brain deterioration across neural systems.

Author Contributions

AZ and SK conceived and designed the experiments. AZ performed the experiments. AZ, CO, and PK analyzed the data. AZ, SK, CO, PK, ES, and AV interpreted the results. AZ wrote first draft of paper. SK, CO, PK, ES, and AV revised the paper. AV accessed participants.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00349/full#supplementary-material

References

Algom, D., Chajut, E., and Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133, 323–338. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alguacil, S., Tudela, P., and Ruz, M. (2013). Cognitive and affective control in a flanker word task: common and dissociable brain mechanisms. Neuropsychologia 51, 1663–1672. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anastasi, J. S., and Rhodes, M. G. (2005). An own-age bias in face recognition for children and older adults. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 1043–1047. doi: 10.3758/Bf03206441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderer, P., Semlitsch, H. V., and Saletu, B. (1996). Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 99, 458–472. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(96)96518-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, C. M., Drysdale, K. A., and Fulham, W. R. (2003). Event-related potentials to Stroop and reverse Stroop stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00038-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azizian, A., Freitas, A. L., Parvaz, M. A., and Squires, N. K. (2006). Beware misleading cues: perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex. Psychophysiology 43, 253–260. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00409.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 698–718. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion - the self-assessment mannequin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruin, K. J., and Wijers, A. A. (2002). Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1172–1182. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00141-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact across the life-span - a theory of socioemotional selectivity. Nebr. Symp. Motiv. 40, 209–254.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 312, 1913–1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously - A theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am. Psychol. 54, 165–181. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.3.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., and Nesselroade, J. R. (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 644–655. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.4.644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaby, L., Luherne-du Boullay, V., Chetouani, M., and Plaza, M. (2015). Compensating for age limits through emotional crossmodal integration. Front. Psychol. 6:691. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, S. T., and Carstensen, L. L. (2007). “Emotion regulation and aging,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation, ed. J. J. Gross (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 307–327.

Google Scholar

Compton, R. J. (2003). The interface between emotion and attention: a review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2, 115–129. doi: 10.1177/1534582303255278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowley, K. E., and Colrain, I. M. (2004). A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 732–744. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1201–1209. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Gelder, B., and Vroomen, J. (2000). Bimodal emotion perception: integration across separate modalities, cross-modal perceptual grouping or perception of multimodal events? Cogn. Emot. 14, 321–324. doi: 10.1080/026999300378842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLoss, D. J., Pierce, R. S., and Andersen, G. J. (2013). Multisensory integration, aging, and the sound-induced flash illusion. Psychol. Aging 28, 802–812. doi: 10.1037/a0033289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, T. A., and Chen, C. C. (2007). Neurophysiological mechanisms in the emotional modulation of attention: the interplay between threat sensitivity and attentional control. Biol. Psychol. 76, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derryberry, D., and Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 55, 958–966. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.958

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diederich, A., Colonius, H., and Schomburg, A. (2008). Assessing age-related multisensory enhancement with the time-window-of-integration model. Neuropsychologia 46, 2556–2562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dien, J. (2012). Applying principal components analysis to event-related potentials: a tutorial. Dev. Neuropsychol. 37, 497–517. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2012.697503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doerwald, F., Scheibe, S., Zacher, H., and Van Yperen, N. W. (2016). Emotional competencies across adulthood: state of knowledge and implications for the work context. Work Aging Retire. 2, 159–216. doi: 10.1093/workar/waw013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., and McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. J. Neurosci. 26, 2072–2079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5042-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, S., Katsumi, Y., and Dixon, R. (2014). The role of arousal in the spontaneous regulation of emotions in healthy aging: an fMRI investigation. Front. Psychol. 5:681. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donohue, S. E., Todisco, A. E., and Woldorff, M. G. (2013). The rapid distraction of attentional resources toward the source of incongruent stimulus input during multisensory conflict. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 623–635. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebner, N., Rieckmann, A., Johnson, M., Johnson, M., and Hakan, F. (2012). Neural correlates of processing own-age versus other-age emotional faces. Int. J. Psychol. 47, 283–284.

Google Scholar

Ebner, N. C., and Johnson, M. K. (2010). Age-group differences in interference from young and older emotional faces. Cogn. Emot. 24, 1095–1116. doi: 10.1080/02699930903128395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., Etkin, A., Gale, S., and Hirsch, J. (2008). Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1475–1484. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1784–1790. doi: 10.1038/nn1594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enoki, H., Sanada, S., Yoshinaga, H., Oka, E., and Ohtahara, S. (1993). The effects of age on the N200 component of the auditory event-related potentials. Cogn. Brain Res. 1, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(93)90023-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., and Hirsch, J. (2006). Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron 51, 871–882. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerdes, A. B. M., Wieser, M. J., Bublatzky, F., Kusay, A., Plichta, M. M., and Alpers, G. W. (2013). Emotional sounds modulate early neural processing of emotional pictures. Front. Psychol. 4:741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammerer, D., Li, S. C., Muller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2010). An electrophysiological study of response conflict processing across the lifespan: assessing the roles of conflict monitoring, cue utilization, response anticipation, and response suppression. Neuropsychologia 48, 3305–3316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasher, L., Stolzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., and Rypma, B. (1991). Age and Inhibition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17, 163–169. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.17.1.163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., Ebner, N. C., and Johnson, M. K. (2011). What predicts the own-age bias in face recognition memory? Soc. Cogn. 29, 97–109. doi: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.1.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., and Hennighausen, E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: inhibitory executive processes? J. Psychophysiol. 14, 218–225. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.14.4.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., and Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science 182, 177–180. doi: 10.1126/science.182.4108.177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, H. T., Schroger, E., and Kotz, S. A. (2015). Selective attention modulates early human evoked potentials during emotional face-voice processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 798–818. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hugenschmidt, C. E., Mozolic, J. L., and Laurienti, P. J. (2009). Suppression of multisensory integration by modality-specific attention in aging. Neuroreport 20, 349–353. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328323ab07

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, G., Velmans, M., and De Fockert, J. (2009). Unconscious priming of a no-go response. Psychophysiology 46, 1258–1269. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00873.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, E. M., Phillips, L. H., and MacPherson, S. E. (2010). Effects of age on cross-modal emotion perception. Psychol. Aging 25, 779–787. doi: 10.1037/a0020528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., and Blanchard-Fields, F. (2012). Linking process and outcome in the study of emotion and aging. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 3–17. doi: 10.1177/1745691611424750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, S., and Kotz, S. A. (2011). The temporal dynamics of processing emotions from vocal, facial, and bodily expressions. Neuroimage 58, 665–674. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P. (2012). On the influence of emotion on conflict processing. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:42. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., and Kotz, S. A. (2010). Modulation of early conflict processing N200 responses to emotional words in a flanker task. Neuropsychologia 48, 3661–3664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., and Kotz, S. A. (2011a). Emotion speeds up conflict resolution: a new role for the ventral anterior cingulate cortex? Cereb. Cortex 21, 911–919. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., and Kotz, S. A. (2011b). Emotion triggers executive attention: anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala responses to emotional words in a conflict task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 198–208. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., and Kotz, S. A. (2012). Effortful control, depression, and anxiety correlate with the influence of emotion on executive attentional control. Biol. Psychol. 92, 456–463. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., Plitschka, J., and Kotz, S. A. (2011). Attentional orienting towards emotion: P2 and N400 ERP effects. Neuropsychologia 49, 3121–3129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawai, N., Kubo-Kawai, N., Kubo, K., Terazawa, T., and Masataka, N. (2012). Distinct aging effects for two types of inhibition in older adults: a near-infrared spectroscopy study on the Simon task and the flanker task. Neuroreport 23, 819–824. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283578032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychol. Sci. 15, 208–214. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiss, I., and Ennis, T. (2001). Age-related decline in perception of prosodic affect. Appl. Neuropsychol. 8, 251–254. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0804_9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klasen, M., Chen, Y. H., and Mathiak, K. (2012). Multisensory emotions: perception, combination and underlying neural processes. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 381–392. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klasen, M., Kenworthy, C. A., Mathiak, K. A., Kircher, T. T. J., and Mathiak, K. (2011). Supramodal representation of emotions. J. Neurosci. 31, 13635–13643. doi: 10.1523/Jneurosci.2833-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, A. (2000). Age-related changes in involuntary and voluntary attention as reflected in components of the event-related potential (ERP). Biol. Psychol. 54, 107–143. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00054-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokinous, J., Kotz, S. A., Tavano, A., and Schroger, E. (2015). The role of emotion in dynamic audiovisual integration of faces and voices. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 713–720. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokinous, J., Tavano, A., Kotz, S. A., and Schroger, E. (2014). Audiovisual facilitation is maintained under degraded visual stimulation during the perception of dynamic emotion expressions. Psychophysiology 51, S60–S60.

Google Scholar

Kopp, B., Mattler, U., Goertz, R., and Rist, F. (1996a). N2,P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 99, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/0921-884x(96)95617-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopp, B., Rist, F., and Mattler, U. (1996b). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology 33, 282–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korsch, M., Fruholz, S., and Herrmann, M. (2016). Conflict-specific aging effects mainly manifest in early information processing stages An ERP study with different conflict types. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:53. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreifelts, B., Ethofer, T., Grodd, W., Erb, M., and Wildgruber, D. (2007). Audiovisual integration of emotional signals in voice and face: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 37, 1445–1456. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambrecht, L., Kreifelts, B., and Wildgruber, D. (2012). Age-related decrease in recognition of emotional facial and prosodic expressions. Emotion 12, 529–539. doi: 10.1037/a0026827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, A. C., Stewart-Williams, S., and Podd, J. (2005). Face recognition and aging: effects of target age and memory load. Mem. Cogn. 33, 1017–1024. doi: 10.3758/Bf03193209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langenecker, S. A., Nielson, K. A., and Rao, S. M. (2004). fMRI of healthy older adults during Stroop interference. Neuroimage 21, 192–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laurienti, P. J., Burdette, J. H., Maldjian, J. A., and Wallace, M. T. (2006). Enhanced multisensory integration in older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1155–1163. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., Henik, A., and Safadi, Z. (2012). Task relevance modulates processing of distracting emotional stimuli. Cogn. Emot. 26, 42–52. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.567055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, C. F., Alves, T., Scott, S. K., and Castro, S. L. (2014). In the ear of the beholder: how age shapes emotion processing in nonverbal vocalizations. Emotion 14, 145–160. doi: 10.1037/a0034287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, T. S., Pinheiro, A., Zhao, Z. X., Nestor, P. G., McCarley, R. W., and Niznikiewicz, M. A. (2012). Emotional cues during simultaneous face and voice processing: electrophysiological insights. PLOS ONE 7:e31001. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, S. H., and Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, 2nd. Edn. Sydney, NSW: Psychology Foundation.

Google Scholar

Lucci, G., Berchicci, M., Spinelli, D., Taddei, F., and Di Russo, F. (2013). The effects of aging on conflict detection. PLOS ONE 8:e56566. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J. (ed.). (2005). “An introduction to event-related potentials and their neural origins,” in An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 2–50. doi: 10.1007/s10409-008-0217-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Kappenman, E. S. (2011). The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., and Fairfield, B. (2016). Noradrenergic modulation of emotional memory in aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 27, 61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M. (2012). The emotion paradox in the aging brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1251, 33–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06471.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychol. Aging 20, 542–553. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, R. L. C., Kingston, R. A., and Boucas, S. L. B. (2011). The specificity of age-related decline in interpretation of emotion cues from prosody. Psychol. Aging 26, 406–414. doi: 10.1037/a0021861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohammadi, G., and Vinciarelli, A. (2011). “Towards a technology of nonverbal behaviour,” in Affective Computing and Interaction: Psychological, Cognitive and Neuroscientific Perspectives, ed. G. Didem (Hershey, PA: IGI Global), 133–156. doi: 10.4018/978-1-61692-892-6.ch007

CrossRef Full Text

Murphy, N. A., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2008). Preferences for emotional information in older and younger adults: a meta-analysis of memory and attention tasks. Psychol. Aging 23, 263–286. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., van den Wildenberg, W., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 17–26. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.1.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, D. A., and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. Conscious. Self Regul. 4, 1–18. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-0629-1_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oatley, K., and Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding Emotions. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Hughes, B., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Neural systems supporting the control of affective and cognitive conflicts. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1841–1854. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohman, A., Flykt, A., and Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 466–478. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orgeta, V. (2010). Effects of age and task difficulty on recognition of facial affect. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 65, 323–327. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulmann, S., Jessen, S., and Kotz, S. A. (2009). Investigating the multimodal nature of human communication insights from ERPs. J. Psychophysiol. 23, 63–76. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.23.2.63

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulmann, S., and Kotz, S. A. (2008). An ERP investigation on the temporal dynamics of emotional prosody and emotional semantics in pseudo- and lexical-sentence context. Brain Lang. 105, 59–69. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peiffer, A. M., Mozolic, J. L., Hugenschmidt, C. E., and Laurienti, P. J. (2007). Age-related multisensory enhancement in a simple audiovisual detection task. Neuroreport 18, 1077–1081. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3281e72ae7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pexman, P. M. (2008). It’s fascinating research - The cognition of verbal irony. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 286–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00591.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., and Grezes, J. (2008). Emotional modulation of visual and motor areas by dynamic body expressions of anger. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 199–212. doi: 10.1080/17470910701394368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pourtois, G., de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., Rossion, B., and Crommelinck, M. (2000). The time-course of intermodal binding between seeing and hearing affective information. Neuroreport 11, 1329–1333. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004270-00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, A. E., and Carstensen, L. L. (2012). The theory behind the age-related positivity effect. Front. Psychol. 3:339. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, A. E., Chan, L., and Mikels, J. A. (2014). Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychol. Aging 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0035194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samanez-Larkin, G. R., and Carstensen, L. L. (2011). “Socioemotional functioning and the aging brain,” in The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience, eds J. Decety and J. T. Cacioppo (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 507–521.

Google Scholar

Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Robertson, E. R., Mikels, J. A., Carstensen, L. L., and Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Selective attention to emotion in the aging brain. Psychol. Aging 24, 519–529. doi: 10.1037/a0016952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasse, L. K., Gamer, M., Büchel, C., and Brassen, S. (2014). Selective control of attention supports the positivity effect in aging. PLOS ONE 9:e104180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheibe, S., and Blanchard-Fields, F. (2009). Effects of regulating emotions on cognitive performance: what is costly for young adults is not so costly for older adults. Psychol. Aging 24, 217–223. doi: 10.1037/a0013807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheibe, S., and Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Emotional aging: recent findings and future trends. J. Gerontol. 65B, 135–144. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., Leuthold, H., and Sereno, S. C. (2009). Early emotion word processing: evidence from event-related potentials. Biol. Psychol. 80, 95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastian, A., Baldermann, C., Feige, B., Katzev, M., Scheller, E., Hellwig, B., et al. (2013). Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 2183–2193. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimokawa, A., Yatomi, N., Anamizu, S., Torii, S., Isono, H., Sugai, Y., et al. (2001). Influence of deteriorating ability of emotional comprehension on interpersonal behavior in Alzheimer-type dementia. Brain Cogn. 47, 423–433. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, J. R., and Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility - effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. J. Appl. Psychol. 51, 300–304. doi: 10.1037/h0020586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St. Jacques, P., Dolcos, F., and Cabeza, R. (2010). Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the amygdala during negative evaluation: a network analysis of fMRI data. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 315–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St. Jacques, P. L., Dolcos, F., and Cabeza, R. (2009). Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the amygdala for subsequent memory of negative pictures: a network analysis of fMRI data. Psychol. Sci. 20, 74–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02258.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, K. R., Muscatell, K. A., and Kensinger, E. A. (2010). The effect of valence on young and older adults’ attention in a rapid serial visual presentation task. Psychol. Aging 25, 239–245. doi: 10.1037/a0018297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoltzfus, E. R., Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., Ulivi, M. S., and Goldstein, D. (1993). Investigations of inhibition and interference in younger and older adults. J. Gerontol. 48, 179–188. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.4.P179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Templier, L., Chetouani, M., Plaza, M., Belot, Z., Bocquet, P., and Chaby, L. (2015). Altered identification with relative preservation of emotional prosody production in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Geriatr. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 13, 106–115. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2015.0524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., and Carstensen, L. L. (2000). Autonomic, subjective, and expressive responses to emotional films in older and younger Chinese Americans and European Americans. Psychol. Aging 15, 684–693. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.4.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Veen, V., and Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 593–602. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30, 829–841. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., and Dapretto, M. (2007). Reading affect in the face and voice - Neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 698–708. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, T., Yahata, N., Kawakubo, Y., Inoue, H., Takano, Y., Iwashiro, N., et al. (2014). Network structure underlying resolution of conflicting non-verbal and verbal social information. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 767–775. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R. (2003). Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks. Neuropsychologia 41, 1122–1135. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00297-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weyers, P., Muhlberger, A., Hefele, C., and Pauli, P. (2006). Electromyographic responses to static and dynamic avatar emotional facial expressions. Psychophysiology 43, 450–453. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00451.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. M., Mathersul, D., Palmer, D. M., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., and Gordon, E. (2009). Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: I. Age effects in males and females across 10 decades. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 31, 257–277. doi: 10.1080/13803390802255635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittchen, H. U., Kessler, R. C., Zhao, S., and Abelson, J. (1995). Reliability and clinical validity of UM-CIDI DSM-III-R generalized anxiety disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 29, 95–110. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(94)00044-R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zinchenko, A., Kanske, P., Obermeier, C., Schröger, E., and Kotz, S. A. (2015). Emotion and goal-directed behavior: ERP evidence on cognitive and emotional conflict. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1577–1587. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zinchenko, A., Obermeier, C., Kanske, P., Schröger, E., and Kotz, S. A. (2017). Positive emotion impedes emotional but not cognitive conflict processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 665–677. doi: 10.3758/s13415-017-0504-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zysset, S., Schroeter, M. L., Neumann, J., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Stroop interference, hemodynamic response and aging: an event-related fMRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 937–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, executive control, conflict processing, cognitive conflict, emotional conflict, ERPs

Citation: Zinchenko A, Obermeier C, Kanske P, Schröger E, Villringer A and Kotz SA (2017) The Influence of Negative Emotion on Cognitive and Emotional Control Remains Intact in Aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:349. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00349

Received: 20 April 2017; Accepted: 16 October 2017;
Published: 01 November 2017.

Edited by:

Alessio Avenanti, Università di Bologna, Italy

Reviewed by:

Florin Dolcos, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, United States
Jacqueline Ann Rushby, University of New South Wales, Australia

Copyright © 2017 Zinchenko, Obermeier, Kanske, Schröger, Villringer and Kotz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sonja A. Kotz, kotz@cbs.mpg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.