Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Med., 27 June 2022
Sec. Geriatric Medicine
This article is part of the Research Topic Aging-Friendly Environments and Healthy Aging View all 28 articles

Cognitive Trajectories in Community-Dwelling Older Adults and Incident Dementia, Disability and Death: A 10-Year Longitudinal Study

\nZimu WuZimu Wu1Robyn L. WoodsRobyn L. Woods1Trevor T. J. Chong,,Trevor T. J. Chong2,3,4Suzanne G. OrchardSuzanne G. Orchard1Raj C. ShahRaj C. Shah5Rory WolfeRory Wolfe1Elsdon StoreyElsdon Storey1Kerry M. SheetsKerry M. Sheets6Anne M. MurrayAnne M. Murray7John J. McNeilJohn J. McNeil1Joanne Ryan
Joanne Ryan1*
  • 1School of Public Health and Preventive Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 2Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 3Department of Neurology, Alfred Health, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 4Department of Clinical Neurosciences, St Vincent's Hospital, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 5Department of Family Medicine and Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 6Division of Geriatrics and Palliative Medicine, Department of Medicine, Hennepin Healthcare, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 7Berman Center for Outcomes and Clinical Research, Hennepin Healthcare Research Institute, Minneapolis, MN, United States

Objective: The inter-individual variability in cognitive changes may be early indicators of major health events. We aimed to determine whether late-life cognitive trajectories were associated with incident dementia, persistent physical disability and all-cause mortality.

Methods: Data came from a cohort of older community-dwelling individuals aged 70 years or above in Australia and the United States. Global cognition, verbal fluency, episodic memory and psychomotor speed were assessed regularly at up to seven waves between 2010 and 2017. Dementia, disability in activities of daily living, and death were adjudicated between 2017 and 2020. Latent classes of cognitive trajectories over seven years were determined using group-based trajectory modeling. Multivariable logistic regression was used for the prospective associations between cognitive trajectories and these outcomes.

Results: Cognitive trajectories were defined for 16,174 participants (mean age: 78.9 years; 56.7% female) who were alive and without incident dementia or disability by 2017, among which 14,655 participants were included in the association analysis. Between three and five trajectory classes were identified depending on the cognitive test. Cognitive trajectories were strongly associated with the risk of dementia. For example, compared to those in the highest-functioning trajectory, the worst performers of episodic memory had a 37-fold increased risk of dementia (95% CI: 17.23–82.64). The lowest trajectories of both global cognition and episodic memory also predicted increased mortality risk (OR: 1.80, 95% CI: 1.28–2.52; OR: 1.61, 95% CI: 1.09–2.36, respectively), while only slow psychomotor speed was marginally associated with physical disability (OR: 2.39, 95% CI: 0.99–5.77).

Conclusions: In older individuals, cognitive trajectories appear to be early indicators of clinically relevant health outcomes. Systematic cognitive assessments as part of routine geriatric evaluation may facilitate early identification and interventions for those individuals at highest risk.

Introduction

Maintaining good cognitive function is an important component of healthy aging (1, 2). There is heterogeneity in late-life cognitive aging across individuals, with distinct cognitive trajectories observed in older populations (3). Gradual decline in cognitive function at old age has been thought to be a ‘normal' part of the aging process (4). However, some individuals experience more advanced cognitive decline dementia (5), while others appear to sustain high cognitive function over time even with advancing age (6).

A decline in global cognition and memory is associated with an increased risk of brain atrophy and dementia (7, 8), and some preliminary evidence suggests that worse cognitive trajectories also predict the risk of other geriatric outcomes (3). For example, one study observed that lower trajectories in global cognition were associated with a higher burden of hospitalization (9), and another two studies reported that declining global cognitive trajectories increased the incidence risk of physical disability and mortality (10, 11). However, most previous studies only examined global cognition, often using the Mini-Mental State Examination, which is a relatively crude measure (9, 1215). It is unknown whether changes in specific domains of cognitive function are early indicators of important health outcomes.

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between global and domain-specific trajectories of cognitive function, and the incidence risk of dementia, persistent physical disability and mortality. We also examined the association between cognitive trajectories and a composite measure of these three outcomes - disability-free survival that reflects a healthy lifespan. Using data collected from a large cohort of older adults, the findings disentangle the population heterogeneity of late-life cognitive trajectories and demonstrate how these can be used as an early marker for major health outcomes.

Materials and Methods

Study Sample

Study participants came from the ASPREE (ASPirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly) clinical trial – a randomized placebo-controlled trial that investigated the long-term effects of daily low-dose aspirin on health outcomes of older adults (16). A total of 19,114 participants aged 65+ years (African American and Hispanic/Latino) and 70+ years (Australian White, American White and all other ethnicities) were recruited from Australia and the United States between March 2010 and December 2014. At enrolment, eligible participants were required to be without a diagnosis of dementia and scored over 77 in the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS). Additionally, participants were without any severe difficulty performing any of the basic activities of daily living (ADL), established cardiovascular diseases, or any life-threatening illness (17).

Assessment of Cognitive Function

Cognitive function was assessed at baseline and annually over follow-up, at up to six waves. The battery included 1) 3MS for global cognitive function (18), 2) single-letter (F) Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-F) for verbal fluency (19), 3) Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised delayed recall task (HVLT-R) for episodic memory (20), and 4) Symbol-Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) for psychomotor speed (21). In addition, an overall score was generated by adding up the z-scores of the four cognitive tests (6, 22).

Ascertainment of Study Endpoints

The endpoints of this study included dementia, persistent physical disability, all-cause mortality and disability-free survival. Dementia was adjudicated according to the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (23). This was done in conjunction with several evaluations including Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive subscale (24), Color Trails (25), Lurian overlapping figures (26), and the Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study Activities of Daily Living scale (27), as well as other relevant information such as laboratory tests, neuroimaging tests, clinical case notes and hospital medical records. Persistent physical disability was defined as the inability to perform or severe difficulty in performing one or more basic ADLs (28). Persistence was determined as the loss of the same ADL for at least 6 months, or adjudicated admission to nursing care facility due to physical disability (17). Death was confirmed upon verification with at least two independent sources (e.g., family or Primary Care Partnerships report, or clinical record, or public death notice) (17), with linkage to the National Death Index (17). Disability-free survival, which is a composite endpoint analyzed as the primary outcome in the ASPREE clinical trial, was defined as the first occurrence of dementia or persistent physical disability or all-cause mortality, whichever came first (29).

Statistical Analysis

Group-based trajectory modeling (GBTM) was used to identify the latent classes of cognitive trajectory, for each cognitive test. GBTM is an application of finite mixture modeling, which captures the underlying subgroups based on the longitudinal patterns of an indicator (30). The goodness of fit and model adequacy was used to select the best model (30, 31) (details in Supplementary Material). Individuals were required to have cognitive data at baseline and at one or more subsequent visits, to be eligible for trajectory modeling.

To ensure temporality, cognitive trajectories were modeled over the trial period (from March 2010 until June 2017), and incident health outcomes were examined between June 2017 and November 2020. As such, participants included in this analysis needed to be alive at the end of the trial period, and without dementia or physical disability.

Basic characteristics were presented as numbers and proportions, and compared between subgroups using chi-squared test. Multivariable logistic regression was used to analyze the association between cognitive trajectories and subsequent health outcomes, using the highest-functioning trajectory class as the reference group. The models adjusted for self-reported socio-demographic characteristics including age at the last visit (continuous), gender (men, women), ethnicity (Australian White, African American, Hispanic/Latino, US White, others) and education (≤12 years, >12 years), as well as smoking status (never, former, current), alcohol intake (never, former, current), living situation (at home alone, at home with someone or in a residential home), body mass index (underweight or normal, overweight, obese), hypertension (no, yes - defined as being on treatment for high BP or BP >140/90 mmHg), diabetes mellitus (no, yes - defined as being on treatment for diabetes, or fasting glucose≥126 mg/dL (≥7 mmol/L), or self-report), dyslipidemia (no, yes - defined as taking cholesterol-lowering medications, or serum cholesterol ≥212 mg/dL (≥5 mmol/L; Australia) and≥240 mg/dL (≥6.2 mmol/L) or LDL >160 mg/dL (>4.1 mmol/L)), depression (no, yes - defined as a score of 8 or higher in the 10-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale), frailty index (continuous, constructed based on 67 health deficits) (32), weak grip strength (no, yes - defined as grip strength in the lowest 20% at baseline adjusted by gender and BMI) (33), slow gait speed (no, yes - defined as gait speed in the lowest 20% at baseline adjusted by gender and height) (33). Grip strength and gait speed were not included in the models for persistent physical disability, as they are closely related to this dependent variable. The information of all covariates was ascertained at baseline (gender, ethnicity and education) or the last study visit when cognitive function was assessed.

All statistical analyses were conducted between September 2021 and February 2022, using Stata version 16.0 (Stata Corp., College Station, Texas, USA) and the ‘Proc Traj' package, with statistical significance set as a two-sided p-value < 0.05.

Results

Identification of Cognitive Trajectories

A total of 16,174 individuals were included in the trajectory modeling (Figure 1). For most participants, data on all cognitive tests were available at three or more timepoints (Supplementary Tables 13). Between three and five trajectory classes were identified depending on the cognitive test (Figure 2, Supplementary Table 4).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of participants included in the analysis. *Incident dementia, persistent physical disability and all-cause mortality.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Trajectory plots of the composite score (A), and the raw scores of 3MS (B), COWAT-F (C), HVLT-R delayed recall (D) and SDMT (E) (N = 16,174). (1) the x-axis denotes the year of cognitive assessment at baseline as well as 1, 3, 4, 5 and 6 years of follow-up; (2) the y-axis denotes the composite score and raw scores of the four cognitive tests; (3) the percentages refer to the proportions of participants assigned into the corresponding classes. 3MS, Modified Mini-Mental State Examination; COWAT-F, Controlled Oral Word Association Test-F; HVLT-R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised (delayed recall); SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test.

The model identified five classes for the overall score, with higher classes having higher baseline scores and less decline (Figure 2A). For the 3MS test, three classes were identified (Figure 2B), including one class with the highest baseline and least decline that accounted for most participants (64.1%). The four classes identified for COWAT-F (Figure 2C) mainly differed in baseline scores, but all improved over time with varying degrees. Four classes were identified for HVLT-R delayed recall (Figure 2D). The higher two trajectories with higher baseline scores showed slight improvement, while the lower two declined over time. In terms of SDMT, the five classes were crudely parallel, and all showed minor decline at a similar rate (Figure 2E).

Cognitive Trajectories and Associated Health Outcomes

Of the 14,655 participants with complete covariate data and who were alive and without dementia and physical disability by June 2017, a total of 854 participants reached the endpoints during the post-trial phase. This includes 265 incident cases of dementia, 253 cases of incident physical disability and 568 deaths (Figure 1). Compared to those included in the current analysis, those excluded due to missing data were older, with fewer years of education, and more likely to have chronic conditions (Supplementary Table 5).

Table 1 shows the basic characteristics of participants according to trajectory classes of the overall score. In general, participants in lower-functioning classes were older, more likely to be males, and with notably fewer years of education. They were also more likely to live alone at home, be a current smoker, and never drink alcohol, although the differences were less marked. In addition, all comorbidities were more prevalent in lower-functioning classes, except dyslipidemia which had an opposite trend.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Basic characteristics of included participants at their last attended follow-up visit (n = 14,655).

The fully adjusted associations of cognitive trajectories with incident dementia and physical disability are shown in Table 2. Compared to the individuals in the highest-functioning trajectory, worse performers with lower baseline scores and/or faster rates of decline showed higher dementia risk in all cognitive tests, although this was less marked for COWAT-F. However, only the lowest class - Low/Decline trajectory of SDMT showed a marginally significant association with physical disability (OR: 2.39, 95% CI: 0.99–5.77, p = 0.05).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fully adjusted a associations of cognitive trajectories with incident dementia and persistent physical disability (n = 14,655).

Results regarding death and the composite endpoint are presented in Table 3. The association with increased mortality risk was seen in worse performers of the overall score and the 3MS test, as well as the lowest class of HVLT-R delayed recall – the Low/Decline trajectory (OR: 1.61, 95% CI: 1.09–2.36, p = 0.02). For the composite endpoint, associations were observed in the trajectories of all cognitive tests, except COWAT-F.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Fully adjusted a associations of cognitive trajectories with incident all-cause mortality and the composite endpoint (n = 14,655).

The results from the minimally adjusted models that only included age, gender, ethnicity and education (Supplementary Tables 6, 7) are not materially different from those produced by the fully adjusted models.

Discussion

Among more than 16,000 community-dwelling older adults initially without dementia or ADL physical disability, we found that low cognitive performers had an increased risk of subsequent geriatric outcomes, compared to those who maintained high cognitive performance. The trajectories of all cognitive tests were associated with incident dementia, but this was less marked for verbal fluency. Death was predicted by low performance in global cognition and episodic memory, while persistent physical disability was only predicted by slow psychomotor speed. These findings suggest that cognitive aging trajectories reflect multiple aspects of health status in late life and could be used as early indicators of risk for major health events, beyond dementia.

Strengths of this study include the large sample size, as well as repeated cognitive assessments that enabled trajectory modeling across 6 years. More importantly, this is the first study to our knowledge that examines the trajectory profiles of multiple cognitive tests using a data-driven approach. This allows a robust comparison across cognitive domains for their associations with important geriatric outcomes and therefore helps to inform targeted strategies for healthy aging. In addition, we visualized the developmental patterns of cognitive changes on a timeframe along with detailed trajectory statistics for each distinct subgroup, which highlights the continuous and dynamic traits of cognitive aging. This longitudinal approach shifts the focus from cognitive function at a single point in time, to cognitive changes (i.e., maintenance, improvement, and decline) over a certain period. In this way, the developmental patterns of the suboptimal trajectories may help to inform when, to whom, and to what extent preventative strategies should be implemented.

We observed an association between low or declining cognitive trajectories in later life and incident dementia. This is in line with well-established evidence that decline may appear in various cognitive domains at the pre-clinical stage of dementia, many years before the diagnosis (34, 35). One study using a similar analytical approach also observed lower levels of hippocampal volume and entorhinal cortical thickness, as well as higher risk of dementia in those with declining memory trajectories (7). Our results showed that trajectories of global cognition, episodic memory and psychomotor speed were all strongly predictive of incident dementia, with an apparent dose-response relationship across the trajectory classes. In contrast, the difference in dementia risk was less marked in the trajectories of COWAT-F, with smaller effect sizes and no dose-response relationship as was seen with other cognitive tests. Interestingly, most participants showed improvements in verbal fluency over time, including those in the lowest COWAT-F trajectory, which possibly reflects practice effects. Indeed, verbal abilities, especially vocabulary, have been known to remain stable or gradually improve with age (4), and are less impacted by the neuropathology of dementia than other cognitive abilities (36). However, previous studies observed poorer verbal fluency in dementia patients compared to the controls (37, 38). Also, the lowest two trajectories of COWAT-F in this study predicted higher dementia risk. Therefore, assessments of verbal abilities may still add value to a comprehensive cognitive battery in dementia screening and prediction, although COWAT-F may be less reflective of brain aging than tests for other cognitive domains.

Although the interdependence between cognitive and physical functioning has been documented (39), evidence regarding how cognitive trajectories could predict future risk of physical disability remains scarce. In a study of 754 community-dwelling older adults with five trajectories of global cognition, the burden of physical disability increased incrementally with the degree of cognitive decline (9). We did not observe a significant association between global cognitive trajectories and physical disability, nor did we find an association in verbal fluency or episodic memory. In the latter two cases, there was a non-significant trend for increasing risk with worse trajectories. It is worth noting that we focused on persistent dysfunction in the same ADL item for over 6 months, which is more stringent than incidental disability investigated in the study of Han and colleagues (9). We did, however, find that the lowest trajectory of psychomotor speed elevated the risk of physical disability by over 2 folds. These results suggest that slowed psychomotor speed may reflect underlying deficits of physical function, although moderate slowing seems to be considerably prevalent in this study. Psychomotor speed may be a central marker of cognitive aging, as it is essential for successful execution of various mental activities (40). This is corroborated by the evidence that psychomotor speed is closely linked to memory and spatial perception, as well as sensory and motor function (39, 41, 42), which are all indispensable in basic daily activities. Therefore, psychomotor speed may be a more sensitive indicator for the future risk of physical disability than the other cognitive domains.

Prior investigation on the extent that cognitive trajectories impact subsequent mortality risk is limited. Four studies found increased mortality risk in those with greater decline in global cognition (10, 11). However, two of these classified participants based on thresholds (e.g., quintile) of cognitive change, which may not adequately reflect the population variance, and the other two were on specific population subgroups (15, 43). Our findings align with previous studies regarding global cognition and add new evidence about other cognitive domains. The worst performers in global cognition and episodic memory showed substantially higher risk of death, and weak associations were also observed in the lower trajectories of psychomotor speed. This suggests that low performance in these cognitive domains may provide a multi-dimensional implication for the aging process, beyond a reflection of neurodegeneration. Trajectories of verbal fluency were not significantly associated with risk of death, which is probably because phonemic fluency and vocabulary are crystalized abilities that are generally acquired via prior learning experiences and are relatively resilient to aging (4). However, we cannot rule out the possibility that observable impairment in verbal abilities may indicate severe disease in some cases.

Terminal decline usually begins several years before death, but the reason remains contentious (44, 45). One explanation is that cognitive decline may be a part of the biological compromise caused by undiagnosed diseases preceding death (44). For example, the decedents with underlying vascular conditions are more likely to experience severe neurological deficits than those without (46). Our findings suggest that cognitive decline can be slow and steady after the age of 70 in the absence of observable cognitive or ADLs dysfunction, even in those at greater risk of impending death. However, their cognitive trajectories, especially in global cognition and episodic memory, may already start to diverge from those of others several years before death.

Our findings highlight the value of strategies to promote cognitive function to help prevent geriatric diseases. To date, several multidomain strategies have proved effective in maintaining cognitive functioning for older individuals in real-world settings. These strategies covered a wide range of domains including but not limited to cognitive training, physical activity, dietary modification and vascular health monitoring (47, 48). In addition, regular interaction with professionals to adjust and optimize cognitive strategies in a timely manner is also essential (47, 49). Therefore, promotional strategies for cognitive function in older age should be advocated broadly. Further, the heterogeneity seen in cognitive aging also suggests the importance of personalized strategies. The early identification of those at risk of adverse outcomes based on their cognitive performance provides an opportunity to formulate preventive strategies at a pre-clinical stage. These should involve not only cognitive training interventions chosen according to cognitive trajectories, but also precision approaches specifically tailored based on the risk profile, health status, and genetic architecture of an individual with declining cognitive trajectories (50).

There are limitations to be acknowledged. First, the ASPREE study only recruited generally healthy participants, and those who died, developed dementia or ADL disability during the trial period were further excluded from the current analysis (to minimize reverse causation during the period when cognitive trajectories were assessed). This likely resulted in a study sample that is healthier than the general older population, and the heterogeneity of cognitive aging may have been underestimated with fewer classes. Therefore, the findings are probably not entirely generalizable to the wider community. Second, the trajectories showed cognitive changes within a specific timeframe, with most participants aged over 70 years. It remains unclear how these outcomes are predicted by cognitive trajectories over a longer period. Further investigation into this area will help characterize cognitive aging beyond the current timeframe and provide information for older adults from more diverse age groups.

Conclusion

Late-life cognitive trajectories are associated with incident geriatric outcomes, but these vary across cognitive domains. Decline across all cognitive tests was associated with increased risk of dementia. Global cognition and episodic memory were associated with the risk of mortality, while only slow psychomotor speed predicted incident physical disability. These findings indicate that cognitive trajectories are an important predictive marker for neurodegeneration, and the overall health status in older individuals. Therefore, regular global assessments covering multiple cognitive domains in either community routine care or clinical practice may provide important opportunities for early interventions to prevent major health events.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The ASPREE clinical trial was conducted according to the guidelines of the International Conference on Harmonization Good Clinical Practice. The participants provided written informed consent to participate in the trial. The current study was approved by the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee.

Author Contributions

Study concept and design: JR, ZW, RLW, and TC. Trial design and Acquisition of data: JM, AM, RLW, RS, RW, ES, and SO. Analysis of data: ZW. Drafting of the manuscript: ZW and JR. Approval of the final version to be submitted, critically revising and editing the manuscript, and interpretation of data: All authors.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging and the National Cancer Institute at the National Institutes of Health (grant numbers U01AG029824 and U19AG062682); and the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia (grant numbers 334047 and 1127060); and Monash University (Australia); and the Victorian Cancer Agency (Australia). JR was supported by an NHMRC Dementia Research Leader Fellowship (grant number APP1135727). ZW is a recipient of the RTP scholarship awarded by Monash University and the Australian Government. The funders had no role in study design and collection, analysis, and interpretation of the results.

Conflict of Interest

AM reports receiving consulting fees from Alkahest, Inc. and grants from the National Institute on Aging. RS reports grants for clinical research regarding dementia and Alzheimer's disease from the National Institutes of Health, the Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, the Department of Defense, and the Illinois Department of Public Health; being as a non-compensated member of the Board of Directors of the Alzheimer's Association–Illinois Chapter; and being as a site principal investigator or sub-investigator for clinical trials and research studies for which his institution (Rush University Medical Center) is sponsored (Amylyx Pharmaceuticals, Inc., Eli Lilly &amp; Co., Inc., Genentech, Inc., Lundbeck, Inc., Merck &amp; Co, Inc., Navidea Biopharmaceuticals, Novartis Pharmaceuticals, Inc., Roche Holdings AG, and Takeda Development Center Americas, Inc.).

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the significant contribution of the dedicated and skilled staff in Australia and the United States to the ASPREE clinical trial. The authors are also most grateful to the ASPREE participants, who so willingly volunteered for this study, and the medical staff and clinics who cared for the participants.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fmed.2022.917254/full#supplementary-material

References

1. National Institute on Aging. Cognitive Health and Older Adults Bethesda: National Institute on Aging. (2020). Available online at: https://www.nia.nih.gov/health/cognitive-health-and-older-adults

2. World Health Organization. Ageing: Healthy ageing and functional ability Geneva: World Health Organization; 2020. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/questions-and-answers/item/healthy-ageing-and-functional-ability

3. Wu Z, Phyo AZZ, Al-Harbi T, Woods RL, Ryan J. Distinct cognitive trajectories in late life and associated predictors and outcomes: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis Rep. (2020) 4:459–78. doi: 10.3233/ADR-200232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. (2013) 29:737–52. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the lancet commission. Lancet. (2020) 396:413–46. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wu Z, Woods RL, Wolfe R, Storey E, Chong TTJ, Shah RC, et al. Trajectories of cognitive function in community-dwelling older adults: a longitudinal study of population heterogeneity. Alzheimers Dement. (2021) 13:e12180. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Zahodne LB, Wall MM, Schupf N, Mayeux R, Manly JJ, Stern Y, et al. Late-life memory trajectories in relation to incident dementia and regional brain atrophy. J Neurol. (2015) 262:2484–90. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7871-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Marioni RE, Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Brayne C, Matthews FE, Dartigues JF, et al. Social activity, cognitive decline and dementia risk: a 20-year prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. (2015) 15:1089. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-2426-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Han L, Gill TM, Jones BL, Allore HG. Cognitive aging trajectories and burdens of disability, hospitalization and nursing home admission among community-living older persons. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2016) 71:766–71. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Vittinghoff E, Barnes D, Simonsick EM, Newman A, et al. The effect of maintaining cognition on risk of disability and death. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2010) 58:889–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02818.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yaffe K, Peltz CB, Ewing SK, McCulloch CE, Cummings SR, Cauley JA, et al. Long-term cognitive trajectories and mortality in older women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2016) 71:1074–80. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hayden KM, Reed BR, Manly JJ, Tommet D, Pietrzak RH, Chelune GJ, et al. Cognitive decline in the elderly: an analysis of population heterogeneity. Age Ageing. (2011) 40:684–9. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Marioni RE, Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Brayne C, Matthews FE, Dartigues JF, et al. Cognitive lifestyle jointly predicts longitudinal cognitive decline and mortality risk. Eur J Epidemiol. (2014) 29:211–9. doi: 10.1007/s10654-014-9881-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Liu Z, Han L, Gahbauer EA, Allore HG, Gill TM. Joint trajectories of cognition and frailty and associated burden of patient-reported outcomes. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2018) 19:304–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2017.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kim JH, Kim JM. Association of cognitive impairment and grip strength trajectories with mortality among middle-aged and elderly adults. Int Psychogeriatr. (2019) 31:723–34. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218001175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. McNeil JJ, Woods RL, Nelson MR, Murray AM, Reid CM, Kirpach B, et al. Baseline characteristics of participants in the ASPREE (ASPirin in reducing events in the elderly) Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2017) 72:1586–93. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wolfe R, Murray AM, Woods RL, Kirpach B, Gilbertson D, Shah RC, et al. The aspirin in reducing events in the elderly trial: Statistical analysis plan. Int J Stroke. (2018) 13:335–8. doi: 10.1177/1747493017741383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Teng EL, Chui HC. The modified mini-mental state (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry. (1987) 48:314–8.

19. Ross TP. The reliability of cluster and switch scores for the controlled oral word association test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2003) 18:153–64. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00192-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ryan J, Woods RL, Murray AM, Shah RC, Britt CJ, Reid CM, et al. Normative performance of older individuals on the Hopkins verbal learning test-revised (HVLT-R) according to ethno-racial group, gender, age and education level. Clin Neuropsychol. (2021) 35:1174–90. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2020.1730444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sheridan LK, Fitzgerald HE, Adams KM, Nigg JT, Martel MM, Puttler LI, et al. Normative Symbol Digit Modalities Test performance in a community-based sample. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2006) 21:23–8. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Graziane JA, Beer JC, Snitz BE, Chang CC, Ganguli M. Dual trajectories of depression and cognition: a longitudinal population-based study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2016) 24:364–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2015.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Cooper JE. DSM-IV: diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: fourth edition. Br J Psychiatry. (1995) 166:4–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.166.1.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Graham DP, Cully JA, Snow AL, Massman P, Doody R. The Alzheimer's disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale: normative data for older adult controls. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2004) 18:236–40.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

25. D'Elia LF. Color Trails Test: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (1996).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

26. Alegret M, Boada-Rovira M, Vinyes-Junque G, Valero S, Espinosa A, Hernandez I, et al. Detection of visuoperceptual deficits in preclinical and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2009) 31:860–7. doi: 10.1080/13803390802595568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fish J. Alzheimer's disease cooperative study ADL scale. In: Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer (2011). p. 111–2.

28. Katz S. Assessing self-maintenance: activities of daily living, mobility, and instrumental activities of daily living. J Am Geriatr Soc. (1983) 31:721–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1983.tb03391.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. McNeil JJ, Woods RL, Nelson MR, Reid CM, Kirpach B, Wolfe R, et al. Effect of Aspirin on disability-free survival in the healthy elderly. N Engl J Med. (2018) 379:1499–508. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1800722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Nagin DS. Group-based trajectory modeling: an overview. Ann Nutr Metab. (2014) 65:205–10. doi: 10.1159/000360229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Andruff H, Carraro N, Thompson A, Gaudreau P, Louvet B. Latent class growth modelling: a tutorial. Tutor Quant Methods Psychol. (2009) 5:11–24. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.05.1.p011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ryan J, Espinoza S, Ernst ME, Ekram A, Wolfe R, Murray AM, et al. Validation of a deficit-accumulation frailty index in the Aspirin in reducing events in the elderly study and its predictive capacity for disability-free survival. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2022) 77:19–26. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glab225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2001) 56:M146–56. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Smits LL, van Harten AC, Pijnenburg YA, Koedam EL, Bouwman FH, Sistermans N, et al. Trajectories of cognitive decline in different types of dementia. Psychol Med. (2015) 45:1051–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Li G, Larson EB, Shofer JB, Crane PK, Gibbons LE, McCormick W, et al. Cognitive trajectory changes over 20 years before dementia diagnosis: a large cohort study. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2017) 65:2627–33. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Henry JD, Crawford JR, Phillips LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer's type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:1212–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lim KB, Kim J, Lee HJ, Yoo J, Kim HS, Kim C, et al. cowat performance of persons with alzheimer dementia, vascular dementia, and parkinson disease dementia according to stage of cognitive impairment. PM R. (2019) 11:737–44. doi: 10.1002/pmrj.12125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Malek-Ahmadi M, Small BJ, Raj A. The diagnostic value of controlled oral word association test-FAS and category fluency in single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2011) 32:235–40. doi: 10.1159/000334525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Clouston SA, Brewster P, Kuh D, Richards M, Cooper R, Hardy R, et al. The dynamic relationship between physical function and cognition in longitudinal aging cohorts. Epidemiol Rev. (2013) 35:33–50. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxs004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. (1996) 103:403–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Finkel D, Reynolds CA, McArdle JJ, Pedersen NL. Age changes in processing speed as a leading indicator of cognitive aging. Psychol Aging. (2007) 22:558–68. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lovden M, Wahlin A. The sensory-cognition association in adulthood: Different magnitudes for processing speed, inhibition, episodic memory, and false memory? Scand J Psychol. (2005) 46:253–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2005.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Hu X, Gu S, Sun X, Gu Y, Zhen X, Li Y, et al. Cognitive ageing trajectories and mortality of Chinese oldest-old. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2019) 82:81–7. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2019.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. MacDonald SW, Hultsch DF, Dixon RA. Aging and the shape of cognitive change before death: terminal decline or terminal drop? J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2011) 66:292–301. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bosworth HB, Siegler IC. Terminal change in cognitive function: an updated review of longitudinal studies. Exp Aging Res. (2002) 28:299–315. doi: 10.1080/03610730290080344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hassing LB, Johansson B, Berg S, Nilsson SE, Pedersen NL, Hofer SM, et al. Terminal decline and markers of cerebro- and cardiovascular disease: findings from a longitudinal study of the oldest old. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2002) 57:P268–76. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.3.P268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Andrieu S, Guyonnet S, Coley N, Cantet C, Bonnefoy M, Bordes S, et al. Effect of long-term omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation with or without multidomain intervention on cognitive function in elderly adults with memory complaints (MAPT): a randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. (2017) 16:377–89. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30040-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levalahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. (2015) 385:2255–63. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Richard E, Moll van Charante EP, Hoevenaar-Blom MP, Coley N, Barbera M, van der Groep A, et al. Healthy ageing through internet counselling in the elderly (HATICE): a multinational, randomised controlled trial. Lancet Digit Health. (2019) 1:e424–e34. doi: 10.1016/S2589-7500(19)30153-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ryan L, Hay M, Huentelman MJ, Duarte A, Rundek T, Levin B, et al. Precision aging: applying precision medicine to the field of cognitive aging. Front Aging Neurosci. (2019) 11:128. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, cognition, dementia, activities of daily living, death, longitudinal

Citation: Wu Z, Woods RL, Chong TTJ, Orchard SG, Shah RC, Wolfe R, Storey E, Sheets KM, Murray AM, McNeil JJ and Ryan J (2022) Cognitive Trajectories in Community-Dwelling Older Adults and Incident Dementia, Disability and Death: A 10-Year Longitudinal Study. Front. Med. 9:917254. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2022.917254

Received: 10 April 2022; Accepted: 30 May 2022;
Published: 27 June 2022.

Edited by:

Yao Yao, Peking University, China

Reviewed by:

Beatrice Arosio, University of Milan, Italy
Ruth Stephen, University of Eastern Finland, Finland

Copyright © 2022 Wu, Woods, Chong, Orchard, Shah, Wolfe, Storey, Sheets, Murray, McNeil and Ryan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joanne Ryan, joanne.ryan@monash.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.