Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Med., 26 June 2018
Sec. Geriatric Medicine
This article is part of the Research Topic Longevity and Healthy Aging View all 11 articles

Objective vs. Subjective Health in Very Advanced Ages: Looking for Discordance in Centenarians

  • 1Center for Health Technology and Services Research, Institute of Biomedical Sciences Abel Salazar (CINTESIS-ICBAS), University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
  • 2School of Education, Polytechnic Institute of Viseu, Viseu, Portugal
  • 3Department of Education and Psychology, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal

Background: Living beyond 100 years of age is associated with several functional and health constraints but their impact depends on one's perception of the situation. Associations between self-rated health (SRH) with sociodemographic and psychosocial variables have been explored in several studies, revealing that one's health appraisal depends of factors beyond the objective health condition. There is a large body of literature concerning SRH in later life but lack of evidence about centenarians' perception of health and its associated factors, which could increase the available knowledge on the strengths and resources individuals in very advanced ages have for facing daily life limitations.

Objective and Methods: This study aims to analyse the relationship between subjective and objective health status in a sample of centenarians (n = 127). Subjective health was assessed by a single-item health measure, and objective health by considering the number of reported diseases and a functional capacity scale (BADL and IADL). Main health characteristics are described as well as examined the association between objective and subjective health.

Results: 46.5% of the sample has a good, very good, or excellent appraisal of their own health. SRH was associated (p < 0.05) with BADL and IADL scores and with the total number of diagnosis; when analyzing SRH according to the level of functional capacity, results revealed that most individuals with severe and moderate dependence have a reasonable to excellent SRH (p > 0.05).

Conclusion: Having diseases and functional dependence at 100 years old may not mean to have a bad SRH. The high variability in SRH and the discordance between objective and subjective measures are a proof of centenarian's capacity of adaptation and the existence of individual resources, which may be decisive for one' perception and handling of health situation at such an advanced age.

Introduction

One of the great achievements of the Twenty-first century will undoubtedly be the increase in exceptional longevity. As projected by the Population Division of the United Nations (1), at the beginning of 2100 there will be more than 25 million of individuals reaching 100 years old, which is more than 50 times the size of the population of centenarians in 2015. Japan is the leading country in terms of number of centenarians, but also in Europe, due to low fertility rates and decreasing old age mortality, there is an emergence of oldest old groups (2). In 2011, the total number of centenarians in Europe was 89,156, with France, Italy, and Greece presenting ratios higher than 20 centenarians per 100,000 inhabitants (3).

Gerontology and geriatric research has been affirming the importance of distinguishing various age subgroups within old age, in the sense that there are striking differences among young-old and the old-old or very-old, i.e., between the third and the fourth ages (4). Centenarians' studies have been revealing that reaching 100 years old brings several challenges at an individual level, but also to formal and informal carers since age-related adversities and age-related needs can be particularly demanding. Functional decline (e.g., physical health/activities of daily living restrictions, mobility, sensory impairment), and psychological and social losses (e.g., loss of loved ones and appreciated activities, dependency have been reported as being often present in such an advanced ages (5, 6).

As long life and longevity increases, an interest toward positive aspects of aging is gaining strength. Due to the difficulty in preventing and avoiding major age-related constrains, there is an increased investment in knowing how individuals manage adversity and in discovering the resources and protective factors of positive functioning (7). The ability to maintain positive self-perceptions and well-being despite hardships is considered an important paradox in advanced life, closely related with one's psychological resilience (8).

Self-rated health (SRH) is one of the most recognized subjective measures in aging research, clinical settings, and population's surveys. With a single and simple question (“In general, would you say your health is…”) it is possible to know if the person considers his/her health as “excellent,” “very good,” “good,” “fair,” or “poor” (9, 10). The importance of this question also relates to its predictive value and independent effect on mortality, which has been demonstrated in numerous studies and diverse populations (11, 12).

Despite being considered a measure to examine subjective health status, several studies have shown that SRH reveals much more information. SRH is related to disease and functional status (13), but also with mental health [e.g., depression; (14)], suggesting that SRH can reflect the states of the human body and mind (12). Also psychosocial variables (e.g., life satisfaction and social support) and socioeconomic status have shown correlation with SRH (11, 15, 16), as well as personality traits (17) and other internal (e.g., as optimism and perceived control) and external (e.g., education, financial status) resources (18).

Due to the observed relation between SRH and objective health aspects, a decrease of SRH with advancing age is to be expected. However, evidence does not irrefutably confirm this hypothesis. Two reasons have to be considered. First, the relationship between subjective and objective health is complex and not direct or independent. Second, in old and very-old age there is a paradoxical pattern of discrepancy between subjective and objective indicators of health. Although there are some contradictions across studies, it seems that SRH does not decline only due to age-related decrease in health status (19). In Pinquart's Pinquart's (10) meta-analysis, for instance, there was a larger association with physical illness and functional limitations in those aged 60–75 years when compared to those aged above 75. Also in the oldest group, an increased association between mental health and SRH was verified (10).

Henchoz et al. (19), French et al. (14), and Zikic et al. (20), have studied objective and subjective measures of health in individuals with 80, 85, and 90 plus years old, respectively. Their findings point to a weaker relationship between objective health measures (e.g., medical diagnoses) and SRH, and to a less rapidly decline of the perception of health with advancing years than the one occurring in physical and functional health status. This weakening relationship between subjective and objective measures of health can be accounted for by several factors, including the capacity to adapt and be resilient, and can be an important indicator of the presence or absence of external and internal resources that might influence life's appraisal.

Despite some differences across studies, the available evidence on the oldest-old show that is possible to reach 100 years old in a relatively good health condition (21, 22). Centenarians have been presented as robust and resistant individuals, since they tend to survive, delay, or escape to the major age-related diseases, such as cancer, and cardiovascular diseases among others (23, 24). But there are many centenarians living in a frail and morbidity situation as well (25, 26). Also great difficulties in sensory domains and basic and instrumental daily living activities (BADL and IADL) have been widely reported in this population [cf. (2729)]. Nevertheless, several investigations are looking to this age group as a prototype of successful aging (30, 31) due to their ability to maintain a positive outlook about life.

Centenarians can be a very interesting group to examine objective vs. subjective health appraisals because they have to face several health and functional capacity problems. The study of this relation may reveal if SRH can serve as an indicator of centenarians' objective health status and if the discrepancy between the two dimensions of health still exists in such an advanced age. In this study, we sought to examine the association between centenarians' own subjective evaluations of health and their objective health status.

Methods

Data Collection

Data came from two centenarians studies, the Oporto Centenarian Study (PT100) and the Beira Interior Centenarian Study (PT100 Beira Interior) which were conducted in two distinct geographical regions of Portugal, each one with an area of approximately 60 km. Individuals aged 100 years and older between December 2013 and December 2014 were identified through voter registration files, churches, nursing homes, local media newspapers, and through snowball sampling. This first step of recruitment resulted in 291 potential participants; all of these were contacted, and a final sample of 241 participants was face-to-face interviewed. Fifty centenarians were excluded because they died in the interim or their relatives refused participation because of advanced dementia and other major health problems or due to lack of interest in the study. Since this study requires centenarians' own perceptions, information was only assessed if the individual was not affected by severe cognitive impairment and was willing to present information on these aspects (n = 127).

Data was collected during one or two sequential interview sessions directly with the centenarian and/or with a proxy respondent. Age was verified by following a protocol entailing personal identity document verification (e.g., birth certificate) and milestones assessments (e.g., wedding date, date of firstborn, subsequent birthdates of children) following best research practices in this field (32). An informed consent previously approved by the National Commission on Data Protection was used. More information about the methodological procedures of both centenarian studies can be found elsewhere (33).

Measures

Three variables were considered as objective health: the number of diseases, the functional capacity in basic activities of daily living (BADL) and in instrumental activities of daily living (IADL). Diseases were assessed with a list of common health problems in older ages: high blood pressure, heart condition, diabetes, chronic lung disease, ulcers or other serious stomach issues, cirrhosis or other liver problems, kidney condition, frequent urinary infections, incontinence, prostate problems, problems with vision or hearing, arthritis, osteoporosis, stroke, cancer, pneumonia, falls, and other. Conditions mentioned as “other” were later coded. Functional disability was assessed through the Older Americans Resources and Services (OARS) Multidimensional Functional Assessment Questionnaire (34, 35). The scale includes SEVEN items to assess basic daily living activities (BADL, e.g., the ability to talk on the phone, to travel, go shopping, prepare meals) and other seven items to evaluate IADL (e.g., the capacity for walking, bathing, eating, toileting). Respondents were asked how much difficulty they had performing each of these activities by rating them on a three-point scale (2 = no difficulty; 1 = do with some help; 0 = cannot do without help). Cronbach's alpha for this study was 0.909 for the BADL scale and 0.879 for the IADL scale. Information regarding these three variables was collected with the centenarian's proxies, in most cases a family member or in the case of institutionalized centenarians it was a professional (e.g., nurse) of the nursing home.

Self-rated health (SRH) was assessed directly with the centenarian through a single item: “In general, would you say your health is…?,” with five response options labeled as excellent, very good, good, reasonable, and bad. Responses were scored in 1 indicating a bad SRH, 2 for a reasonable SRH, and 3 for a positive SRH (excellent, very good, and good).

According previous work (36, 37), BADL, IADL, and number of diagnoses were categorized in three categories each. For ADL, the categories considered were: 1—Mild (IADL dependence only); 2—Moderate (dependent in 1–2 BADL); 3—Severe (dependence in 3 or more BADL). For the number of diagnoses, the three categories considered were: 0–1; 2–3; ≥4. Additionally, SRH was also considered as a three-point scale: 1—bad; 2—reasonable; 3—good, very good, or excellent.

Sociodemographic data was obtained from structured questions about age, gender, current marital status, living arrangements, having children, income per month, and income management.

Statistical Analysis

Description of the sample was performed using frequencies (absolute and relative), mean and standard deviation. Mean differences of objective health measures according to self-perception of health were performed considering a one-way ANOVA. To evaluate the association between categorical variables (objective health and SRH), Chi-square test was used. In all analysis, a significance level of 0.05 was considered.

Results

Sample Characteristics

The sample comprises 127 centenarians with a mean age of 101.1 years (sd = 1.5 years, range = 100–108). One hundred and twelve centenarians are female (88.2%) and only 15 are male (11.8%). The majority are widowed (n = 108, 85.0%), 13 (10.2%) never married, 5 (3.9%) are married, and only 1 (0.8%) is divorced. Forty-six (36.2%) lived in an institution, and 12 (9.4%) lived alone. One hundred and seven (84.3%) have children. Almost 50% of the sample never attended school (n = 59, 46.5%). Concerning income, 20 (16.9%) receive <250 €/month, 78 (66.1%) receive between 250 € and 500 €, 16 (13.6%) between 500 € and 750 €, and 4 (3.3%) more than 750 €. Forty-nine (40.5%) reveal that cannot make ends meet, 52 (43.0%) just manage to get by, 14 (11.6%) have enough money with a little extra, and only 6 (5.0%) refer that money is not problem (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

Subjective and Objective Health

Fifty-nine centenarians (46.5%) report their health as good, very good or excellent, 48 (37.8%) as reasonable, and 20 (15.7%) as bad (Table 2). Concerning objective health, the mean score of BADL and IADL is 8.6 (sd = 4.2) and 3.8 (sd = 3.3), respectively. The average number of diagnoses reported by the centenarians is 3.6 (sd = 2.0), ranging from 0 (minimum) and 9 (maximum).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Subjective and objective health of the sample.

Association Between Self-Perception of Health and Physical and Functional Health

Considering the three objective measures of health (continuous variables), and comparing the mean values according to the three groups of SRH, we can verify in Table 3 that differences between groups were found (p < 0.05 for the three variables). Both BADL and IADL scores increase with the improvement of self-perception of health (i.e., better functional capacity was related with better SRH). Considering the number of diagnoses, results revealed that the group with a reasonable self-perception of health presented a higher number of diseases than the group with a bad self-perception (Figure 1).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. BADL, IADL, and number of diagnoses (mean scores) according to SRH.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Association between SRH and the number of diseases.

The Table 4 presents the results obtained (the percentage presented are % of total). No significant association was found between ADL and SRH. The percentage of centenarians with some extent of agreement between the two measures was 42%. Additionally, 29% referred a worse SRH comparing with the ADL capacities, and 29% referred a better SRH comparing with the ADL capacities. A significant association was found between SRH and number of diagnoses. The percentage of some agreement of the two measures was 32.3%. Only 8.7% referred a worse SRH comparing with the number of diagnoses and 59% referred a better SRH comparing with the number of diagnoses.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Association between SRH and objective health measures (categorical variables).

Discussion

The present study analyzed different aspects of health in a sample of Portuguese centenarians, comparing their perceived health with measures of objective health status. Centenarians presented a mean number of diseases of 3.6, as well as several functional limitations. These results are in line with other international studies conducted with centenarians, such as the ones from Georgia and Fordham (USA), Denmark and Heidelberg (Europe) (6, 25, 28, 29). Nevertheless, almost half (46.5%) of our sample perceived their health positively (good/very good/excellent). This percentage is, however, lower than the results presented by Cho et al. (30), Jopp et al. (29), Tigani et al. (38), and Liu and Zhang (15) who revealed that 73, 67, 66.8, and 54.3% of the centenarians in their studies rated their health in similar positive ways. These results can be understood under the influence of age and culture in self-ratings of health (12).

When examining the association between centenarians' own perception of health and their functional capacity, results of our study revealed that in overall BADL and IADL scores are associated with SRH, but that when further analyzing the different levels of dependence according to SRH, the association is no longer statistically significant. The majority of centenarians with mild dependence (IADL dependence only) have a positive (good, very good, excellent) SRH, but also the centenarians with moderate and severe dependence have higher rates of reasonable and good-excellent SRH (rather than a poor SRH). The association between the number of diagnosis and the SRH is statistically significant and the pattern is very similar to the previous one. The majority of centenarians with zero or one disease have a positive SRH, but the same happens for the ones with two or three diseases. Even the majority of centenarians with four or more diseases have a reasonable or good-excellent SRH. Together, these results allow us to state that having more diseases and dependence is not necessarily a sign of having a bad or negative SRH.

As verified with very old individuals [e.g., (10, 14, 17, 39)], it seems that also centenarians hold a weaker emphasis on their physical and functional status in the appraisal of their health condition. Previous studies that have shown discordance between objective and subjective health measures have associated it with the contribution of external and internal resources as optimism and perceived control (18, 40). A positive SRH may reflect the greater importance of psychological adaptation in very advanced age. Also the influence of downward social comparison has been reported. It seems that comparing oneself with others from the same age group who are in poorer health enables oldest old individuals (aged 80 plus) to maintain a positive SRH (19, 25). This is an important and common mechanism for the oldest old, since it is more frequent to find congeners in poor health at the age of 85 than at the age of 20, 40, or even 60 (19); in the cases of centenarians, however, since most individuals of the same generation are already dead, the comparison may elicit a more positive appreciation of one's health as it focuses on the exceptionality of still being alive. To date, there is limited information on the social comparison processes underneath SRH at such an advanced age, particularly the age group target for comparisons and its consequences for well-being. Such psychosocial process deserves, therefore, further attention.

Due to the great individual differences and disparities among oldest old individuals, especially in self-appraisals, several researches have been considering the role of risk and protective factors in explaining such variability. These studies have been presenting as a common trait of this age group the high weight of mental health aspects and psychosocial well-being factors when considering the correlates of SRH (10, 14, 17, 38). Puvill et al. (40) when analyzing the correlates of SRH in a representative population of 85-year olds found a weaker association with mortality and a stronger with mental health and life satisfaction. Therefore, an underrated subjective health condition may be indicative of psychosocial distress or burden of physical disability (14) and these health pessimists may be prone to depressive feelings (39). SRH has also been linked to frailty and anxiety in centenarians. Ribeiro et al. (26), for instance, found that SRF was the only predictor of depression in frail and pre frail centenarians and that a worse SRH increased the odds of experiencing clinical anxiety (41). When comparing centenarians with sexagenarians and octogenarians, Quinn et al. (42) found personality and levels of control as unique set of SRH's correlates in the oldest-old group. An apprehensive personality and low levels of control over health were more important than physical health variables in predicting a poor subjective health. Also Ruthig and Chipperfield (43), in a study on health incongruence in later life (ages 79–98 years) found that perceived control was weaker among pessimists; Tigani et al. (38) in a sample of Greek centenarians found that high optimism, adaptability and internal health locus of control were independently associated with good SRH.

Conclusion

The analysis of associations between objective and subjective health allowed to conclude that not all centenarians with moderate/severe constrains of ADL and diseases have a bad SRH, which may be related with the existence of other factors that are weighted in one's self-perception of health at such and advanced age. Being in the limit of longevity brings several challenges at the health level, and these may demand specific developmental regulation processes for fostering well-being. Research on these long-lived individuals regarding their difficulties but also the variables promoting resilience (and inherent positive SRH) is a necessary investment in order to reach better years of life. Future studies should further examine the association between objective and subjective measures of health by considering the influence of adaptive resources, such as characteristics of personal disposition, and protective social comparison mechanisms which may be decisive for centenarians' perception and handling of health limitations.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas Abel Salazar (Universidade do Porto, Portugal) with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Portuguese national data protection commission.

Author Contributions

OR, LA, and LT were responsible for the study conception and design. OR supervised data collection and helped writing the manuscript. LA wrote the manuscript. LT performed the data analysis and CP critically revised the paper for important intellectual content.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Authors would like to thank Dr. Rosa Marina Afonso and her team at the University of Beira Interior for collecting data within the Beira Interior Centenarian Study (PT100 Beira Interior).

References

1. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs Population Division. World Population Prospects: The 2015 Revision, Key Findings and Advance Tables. Working Paper No. ESA/P/WP.241 (2015).

2. Robine JM, Cubaynes S. Worldwide demography of centenarians. Mechanisms of ageing and development, centenarians in europe. Maturitas (2017) 165:59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2017.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Teixeira L, Araújo L, Jopp D, Ribeiro O. Centenarians in Europe. Maturitas(2017) 104:90–5. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2017.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Baltes PB, Smith J. New frontiers in the future of aging: from successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology (2003) 49:123–35. doi: 10.1159/000067946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Jopp D, Rott C. Adaptation in very old age: exploring the role of resources, beliefs, and attitudes for centenarians' happiness. Psychol Aging (2006) 21:266–80. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Poon L, Martin P, Bishop A, Cho J, Rosa G, Deshpande N, et al. Understanding centenarians psychosocial dynamics and their contribution to health and quality of life. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. (2010) 2010:680657. doi: 10.1155/2010/680657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cosco T, Howse K, Brayne C. Healthy ageing, resilience and wellbeing. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. (2017) 26:579–83. doi: 10.1017/S2045796017000324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Fry P, Debats D. Sources of human life-strengths, resilience and health. In: P. Fry, C. Keyes editors. New Frontiers in Resilient Aging: Life Strengths and Well-being in Late Life. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (2010). p. 15–59.

Google Scholar

9. Bombak A. Self-rated health and public health: a critical perspective. Front Public Health (2013) 1:15. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2013.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pinquart M. Correlates of subjective health in older adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging (2001) 16:414–26. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.3.414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty- seven community studies. J Health Soci Behav. (1997) 38:21–37. doi: 10.2307/2955359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Jylhä M. What is self-rated health and why does it predict mortality? Towards a unified conceptual model. Soc Sci Med. (2009) 69:307–16. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Meng Q, Xie Z, Zhang T. A single item self-rated health measure correlates with objective health status in the elderly: a survey in suburban Beijing. Front Public Health (2013) 2:27. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2014.00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. French D, Sargent-Cox K, Luszcz M. Correlates of subjective health across the aging lifespan: understanding self-rated health in the oldest old. J Aging Health (2012) 24:1449–69. doi: 10.1177/0898264312461151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Liu G, Zhang Z. Sociodemographic differentials of the self-rated health of the oldest-old Chinese. Pop Res Policy Rev. (2004) 23:117–33 doi: 10.1023/B:POPU.0000019921.20777.1b

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lee HL, Huang HC, Lee MD, Chen J, Lin KC. Factors affecting trajectory patterns of self-rated health (SRH) in an older population—a community-based longitudinal study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2012) 54:334–41. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2011.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Schneider G, Driesch G, Kruse A, Wachter M, Nehen HG, Heuft G. What influences self-perception of health in the elderly? The role of objective health condition, subjective well-being and sense of coherence. Arch Gerontol Geriat. (2004) 39:227–37. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2004.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Benyamini Y. Why does self-rated health predict mortality? An update on current knowledge and a research agenda for psychologists. Psychol Health (2011) 26:1407–13. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.621703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Henchoz K, Cavalli S, Girardin M. Health perception and health status in advanced old age: a paradox of association. J Aging Stud. (2008) 22:282–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2007.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Zikic L, Jankelic S, Milosevic D, Despotovic N, Erceg P, Davidovic M. Self-Perception of Health (SPH) in the oldest-old subjects. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2009) 49:245–9. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2009.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hitt R, Young-Xu Y, Silver M, Perls T. Centenarians: the older you get, the healthier you have been. Lancet (1999) 354:652. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)01987-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Darvini C, Demakakos P, Charizani F, Tigani X, Tsiou C, Chalamandaris AG, et al. Assessment of the health status of Greek centenarians. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2008) 46:67–78. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2007.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Franceschi C, Bonafé M. Centenarians as a model for healthy aging. Biochem Soc Trans. (2003) 31:457–61. doi: 10.1042/bst0310457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Evert J, Lawler E, Bogan H, Perls T. Morbidity profiles of centenarians: survivors, delayers, and escapers. J Gerontol Med Sci. (2003) 58A:232–7. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.3.M232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Andersen-Ranberg K, Schroll M, Jeune B. Healthy centenarians do not exist, but autonomous centenarians do: a population-based study of morbidity among danish centenarians. J Am Geriat Soc. (2001) 49:900–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49180.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

26. Ribeiro O, Duarte N, Teixeira L, Paúl C. Frailty and depression in centenarians. Int Psychogeriatr. (2018) 30:115–24. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217001910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ribeiro O, Teixeira L, Araújo L, Paúl C. Health profile of centenarians in portugal: a census-based approach. Pop Health Metr. (2016) 14:13. doi: 10.1186/s12963-016-0083-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Jopp D, Boerner K, Rott C. Health and disease at age 100: findings from the second heidelberg centenarian study. Deutsch. Ärzteblatt Int. (2016) 113:203–10. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2016.0203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Jopp D, Park MK, Leherfedl J, Paggi M. Physical, cognitive, social and mental health in near-centenarians and centenarians living in New York City: findings from the Fordham Centenarian Study. BMC Geriatr. (2016) 16:1. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0167-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cho J, Martin P, Poon L. The older they are, the less successful they become? Findings from the georgia centenarian study. J Aging Res. (2012) 2012:695854. doi: 10.1155/2012/695854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Araújo L, Ribeiro O, Teixeira L, Paúl C. Successful aging at 100 years: the relevance of subjectivity and psychological resources. Int Psychogeriatr. (2016) 28:179–88. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Sachdev P, Levitan C, Crawford J. Methodological issues in centenarian research: pitfalls and challenges. Asian J Gerontol Geriatr. (2012) 7:44–8.

Google Scholar

33. Ribeiro O, Araújo L, Teixeira L, Brandão D, Duarte N, Paúl C. Oporto centenarian study. In: N. A. Pachana, editor. Encyclopedia of Geropsychology Singapore: Springer. (2015). p. 978–81.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Fillenbaum G, Smyer M. The development, validity, and reliability of the OARS Multidimensional Functional Assessment Questionnaire. J Gerontol. (1981) 36:428–34. doi: 10.1093/geronj/36.4.428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Rodrigues RM. Validação da versão em português europeu de questionário de avaliação funcional multidimensional de idosos. Rev Panamer Sal Públ. (2008) 23:109–15. doi: 10.1590/S1020-49892008000200006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. McCusker J, Bellavance F, Cardin S, Trkpanier S, Verdon J, Ardman O. Detection of older people at increased risk of adverse health outcomes after an emergency visit: the ISAR screening tool. J Am Geriatr Soc. (1999) 47:1229–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb05204.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Damián J, Pastor-Barriuso R, Valderrama-Gama E. Factors associated with self-rated health in older people living in institutions. BMC Geriatr. (2008) 8:5. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-8-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tigani X, Artemiadis A, Alexopoulos E, Chrousos G, Darviri C. Self-rated health in centenarians: a nation-wide cross-sectional Greek study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2012) 54:342–8. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2012.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hong T, Zarit S, Malmberg B. The role of health congruence in functional status and depression. J Gerontol. (2004) 59B:151–7. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.4.P151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Puvill T, Lindenberg J, Gussekloo J, de Craen A, Slaets J, Westendorp R. Associations of various health-ratings with geriatric giants, mortality and life satisfaction in older people. PLoS ONE (2016) 11:e0163499. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ribeiro O, Teixeira L, Araújo L, Afonso R, Pachana N. Predictors of anxiety in centenarians: health, economic factors, and loneliness. Int Psychogeriatr. (2015) 27:1167–76. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214001628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Quinn ME, Johnson MA, Poon LW, Martin P. Psychosocial correlates of subjective health in sexagenarians, octogenarians, and centenarians. Issues Mental Health Nurs. (1999) 20:151–71.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

43. Ruthig J, Chipperfield J. Health incongruence in later life: implications for subsequent well-being and health care. Health Psychol. (2007) 26:753–61. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.6.753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: centenarians, longevity, physical health, self-rated health, well-being paradox

Citation: Araújo L, Teixeira L, Ribeiro O and Paúl C (2018) Objective vs. Subjective Health in Very Advanced Ages: Looking for Discordance in Centenarians. Front. Med. 5:189. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2018.00189

Received: 02 January 2018; Accepted: 08 June 2018;
Published: 26 June 2018.

Edited by:

Gil Atzmon, University of Haifa, Israel

Reviewed by:

Paola Sebastiani, Boston University, United States
Francesco Prattichizzo, MultiMedica (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2018 Araújo, Teixeira, Ribeiro and Paúl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lia Araújo, liajaraujo@esev.ipv.pt

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.