Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Mar. Sci., 24 May 2021
Sec. Marine Megafauna

Humpback Whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) Song on a Subarctic Feeding Ground

  • 1Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 2Department of Arctic and Marine Biology, Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
  • 3Faculty of Biosciences, Fisheries and Economics, Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
  • 4Norwegian Polar Institute, Tromsø, Norway
  • 5Department of Marine Ecosystem Acoustics, Institute of Marine Research, Bergen, Norway
  • 6Department of Marine Mammals, Institute of Marine Research, Tromsø, Norway

Male humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) are known to produce long complex sequences of structured vocalizations called song. Singing behavior has traditionally been associated with low latitude breeding grounds but is increasingly reported outside these areas. This study provides the first report of humpback whale songs in the subarctic waters of Northern Norway using a long-term bottom-moored hydrophone. Data processed included the months January–June 2018 and December 2018–January 2019. Out of 189 days with recordings, humpback whale singing was heard on 79 days. Singing was first detected beginning of January 2018 with a peak in February and was heard until mid-April. No singing activity was found during the summer months and was heard again in December 2018, continuing over January 2019. A total of 131 song sessions, including 35 full sessions, were identified throughout the study period. The longest and shortest complete sessions lasted 815 and 13 min, respectively. The results confirm that singing can be heard over several months in winter and spring on a high latitude feeding ground. This provides additional evidence to the growing literature that singing is not an explicit behavior confined to low latitude breeding grounds. The peak of song occurrence in February appears to coincide with the reproductive cycle of humpback whales. Finally, this study indicates that song occurrence on a subarctic feeding ground likely aids the cultural transmission for the North Atlantic humpback whale population.

Introduction

Humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) are a highly migratory species, undertaking extensive annual migrations between high latitude summer feeding grounds in subpolar waters and low latitude winter breeding grounds in tropical waters (Dawbin, 1966). In the Eastern North Atlantic, the feeding grounds encompass subarctic waters of Iceland, Jan Mayen, Greenland, and Northern Norway to the Barents Sea (Stevick et al., 2006). Using sighting and catch data, humpback whales have been documented in Icelandic waters from May to September, peaking in June-July (Sigurjónsson and Víkingsson, 1997). Further north, in the Barents Sea, they have been observed during the summer months with a peak abundance in September and along the Norwegian coast mainly between October and February (with a peak in January) (Ramm, 2020). Humpbacks have been reported to generally arrive on their breeding grounds in the West Indies and Cape Verde between February and April (Stevick et al., 2003, 2016). However, some studies proposed that not all humpbacks migrate annually but overwinter on feeding grounds (Van Opzeeland et al., 2013; Magnúsdóttir et al., 2015), thus, migratory behavior appears to vary by individuals (Magnúsdóttir and Lim, 2019).

Humpback whales frequently display long series of structured sound sequences known as song. To date, singing has only been observed for males and is strongly linked to breeding behavior (Payne and McVay, 1971; Cerchio et al., 2001; Cholewiak, 2008). The song of humpbacks is highly stereotypical and arranged in a nested hierarchy; the shortest, most basic element is called a “unit” which combine and form a string of individual sounds termed “phrase.” Phrases, usually 2–20 units long, are then repeated in succession to form a “theme.” A “song” is defined as the combination of distinct themes joined in a predictable sequence, which are then repeated multiple times in a “song session,” lasting up to several hours (Payne and McVay, 1971; Cholewiak et al., 2013).

Singing has traditionally been associated with breeding in low latitude areas, however, growing evidence reports humpback whale song at higher latitudes, outside of breeding grounds, during feeding and migration (Gabriele and Frankel, 2002; Clark and Clapham, 2004; Vu et al., 2012; Garland et al., 2013; Magnúsdóttir et al., 2014; Gridley et al., 2018). The peak of song occurrence on breeding grounds around February–March (Au et al., 2000) appears to be in line with reproduction-specific traits such as the female ovulation period and increased testis weight in male humpback whales (Nishiwaki, 1959, 1960). This suggests that singing plays a role in reproduction (Tyack, 1981; Mobley et al., 1988; Smith et al., 2008). In the northern hemisphere, singing has been detected on feeding grounds on multiple occasions across ocean basins (Baker et al., 1985; Mattila et al., 1987; McSweeney et al., 1989; Clark and Clapham, 2004; Vu et al., 2012; Magnúsdóttir et al., 2014). Magnúsdóttir et al. (2015) reported regular singing activity during three consecutive winters on a subarctic feeding ground in Iceland. Icelandic waters have also been identified as a feeding ground and occasional passage route for Norwegian humpback whales, most likely aiding cultural transmission of songs within the North Atlantic (Magnúsdóttir and Lim, 2019). Recent data collection in Northern Norway is showing extensive humpback whale vocal activity during the winter months, mainly consisting of song. The objective of this study was to identify and describe the occurrence of the humpback whale song in Norwegian waters during migration and/or feeding.

Materials and Methods

Data Collection

Launched in 2013, the Lofoten-Vesterålen (LoVe) Ocean Observatory was deployed in a cold-water coral area about 20 km offshore from the coast of Vesterålen, Norway, above the arctic circle (>66°33’ N) (Figure 1). A hydrophone (Ocean Sonics SB35 ETH, 10 Hz–64 kHz) continuously records biologically generated sounds and anthropogenic noise within the area (Godø et al., 2014). Passive acoustic data processed in this study included the months January–June 2018 and December 2018–January 2019 and audio files were downloaded using the LoVe Ocean archive (Equinor and IMR, 2020) and processed using a combination of automated and manual methods.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Map of the sampling-nodes of the LoVe Ocean Observatory. Data used in this study were collected by node 1 (Equinor and IMR, 2020).

Song Detection

To identify the occurrence of humpback whale vocalization, a long-term spectral average (LTSA) was generated using PamGuard software (version 2.01.04 beta; Gillespie et al., 2009) for every month with available data (Figure 2). Simultaneously with the LTSA, PamGuard’s Whistle and Moan detector (WMD) for automated detection of humpback whale vocalizations was applied with the following settings: frequencies of 120–15,000 Hz, 4096 FFT, 50% overlap, Hann Window, detection threshold 8dB, remaining parameters as default by PamGuard. Including a WMD ensured the manual evaluation of false positives and false negatives, while assessing the occurrence of whale vocalizations. Areas of high activity generated by the LTSA and the WMD (see Supplementary Figure 1) were then investigated in detail using spectrograms in Raven Pro (version 1.6; Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, 2019) to determine start and end time of vocal activity as well as vocal categories (song or non-song). A song was defined by units found in a rhythmic context like phrases and themes (Figure 3). Randomly occurring sounds with no rhythmic pattern were considered non-song (Dunlop et al., 2008). Song sessions are usually sung in a continuous bout (Winn and Winn, 1978) and were delineated by a significant silent interval. Additionally, the number of chorusing singers was estimated using visual and auditory perceptual characteristics of overlapping units and phrases as applied by previous studies (Payne and Payne, 1985; Magnúsdóttir et al., 2015; Magnúsdóttir and Lim, 2019).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Humpback whale song occurrence over the study period. The horizontal bottom bar indicates data availability.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Spectrographic view of an observed song cycle in January 2018; generated using Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) size 8092 Hann-window with a frequency resolution of 5.69 Hz and a 70% overlap. Audio is provided in Supplementary Audio 2.

Results

Song Occurrence

A total of 189 days of recordings were included in the analysis to investigate the presence of humpback whale song off the coast of Northern Norway. Humpback singing activity was detected on 42% of all recording days, occurring in six out of the eight months, with song detected in winter and spring between December and April (Figure 2).

The first occurrence of measurable song (units, phrases, themes, Figure 3) in the data available was on the 3rd of January 2018. In this month, a mean of 78 (± 147) minutes of singing activity per recording day was heard, the lowest activity throughout the study period (Table 1). This was followed by the peak month February, where song was detected on 15 out of 16 days, with a mean of 949 (± 440) min and the highest minimum of 233 min per recording day. March and April were also months of relatively high activity with singing detected for a mean of 448 (± 480) and 357 (± 504) minutes per recording day, respectively. On some occasions, singing was heard for more than 22 h within one day and in April, singing by multiple whales lasted one full recording day (1,440 min) and even beyond. No instances of singing were detected in May or June followed by a lack of data availability from July up to and including November. The next singing activity was detected on the 10th of December 2018 and continued until the end of January 2019 with no more data added to the analysis.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of humpback whale song occurrence per recording day and song sessions.

Song Sessions

Throughout the study period, a total of 131 song sessions were identified, with February and March having the highest numbers of more than 30 sessions (Table 1). With continuous data recordings available, it was possible to detect full song sessions (including start and end time) on multiple occasions. In total, 35 complete sessions were detected, with the highest amount of ten and twelve sessions in January 2019 and March 2018, respectively. For other months, such as January, February, and December 2018, only a small number of full song sessions were detected (≤3). This was most likely a result of poor-quality recordings and/or too few data to determine the start and end time of a session in a meaningful measure. The lengths of full song sessions varied between several minutes and more than 12 h. The longest complete sessions occurred in March and April 2018 with more than 13 h and the shortest of only 13 min in January 2019.

Throughout 79 days of measured singing activity, multiple whales were found contemporaneously singing at a given time, with peaks of more than four singers in asynchronous chorus in February and April 2018. Although single singers were generally predominant throughout the study period (65%), multiple singers were detected for 35% of all detected song sessions (n = 131). Complete sessions (n = 35) resulted in similar ratios, with 63% sung by single whales and 37% by multiple animals.

Discussion

This study provides the first account of humpback whale songs in the subarctic waters of Northern Norway. The results show that humpbacks can be heard singing over several months on a high latitude feeding ground, providing additional evidence to the growing literature that singing is not an explicit behavior confined to breeding grounds (e.g., Vu et al., 2012; Magnúsdóttir et al., 2014). The re-occurrence of singing activity over adjacent years demonstrates that this is most likely not an unusual but rather persistent event within this area. Whales were acoustically detected in coastal areas of Northern Norway in the period of December to April, with peak singing in February. This is in line with previous studies within the North-East Atlantic, with highest singing activity heard in February (Magnúsdóttir et al., 2014; Magnúsdóttir and Lim, 2019).

March and April were also months of relatively high song occurrence with the longest complete song sessions recorded. The observed variability in singing behavior might be driven by the abundance of whales in the area, specifically males which are the only ones that have been observed to engage in this vocal display (Winn and Winn, 1978; Baker and Herman, 1984; Darling and Bérubé, 2001; Darling et al., 2006; Herman et al., 2013). Recent studies showed that Norwegian fjords represent an important stopover in the southbound migration for humpback whales (Kettemer et al., 2019; Ramm, 2020). Peak sighting rates by Ramm (2020) using photo identification were generally highest in January with low to no observations in February and onwards. While visual studies report no humpback whale sightings in Norwegian waters after February (Broms et al., 2015; Ryan et al., 2015; Ramm, 2020), acoustic detections by Aniceto et al. (2020) and the present study reveal longer stays throughout spring. Although recordings in February only covered 57% of all days within this month, singing activity would still be highest if the missing 12 days were set as “song absent” (resulting in 542 min/recording day).

The increase of song occurrence in February is most likely correlated with the reproduction cycle as already suggested by Magnúsdóttir and Lim (2019). Peaks in singing overlap the ovulation periods of females (Nishiwaki, 1959) and might be driven by elevated testosterone levels in males (Clark and Clapham, 2004). However, other studies on North Atlantic feeding grounds provided opposing results with no song present in February and peaks in November (Vu et al., 2012). Furthermore, the increase in humpback whale song occurrence in February could identify the time period when most males arrive at Norwegian feeding grounds. Magnúsdóttir et al. (2014) reported visual observations of humpbacks without any song detected from spring to autumn. However, further abundance investigations including sex ratios in February and beyond are necessary to confirm this hypothesis.

Humpback whales from the North-East Atlantic have been documented to generally arrive at the breeding sites in the West Indies and Cape Verde sometime between February and May (Stevick et al., 2003, 2016; Kettemer et al., 2019). The occurrence of song from December throughout April within the study area demonstrates that humpbacks start singing well before they have reached their breeding grounds. This might indicate that males start to migrate with a delay as proposed by Vu et al. (2012). However, Stevick et al. (2003) demonstrated evidence for sex segregation in migration with males arriving earlier at breeding areas than females. Additionally, pregnant female humpbacks have been documented to depart last from feeding grounds and arrive latest at breeding areas (Dawbin, 1966). As Ramm (2020) put forward, this suggests a dominance by females in the later winter-feeding seasons in Norway. Based on a sex ratio highly skewed toward male humpback whales in breeding areas, it has been proposed that females most likely remain in feeding areas to improve their body condition (Brown et al., 1995; Craig and Herman, 1997). A delayed departure or even remaining whales in the area could explain the lasting singing behavior by male humpbacks until April, suggesting a trade-off strategy for both sexes as indicated by Magnúsdóttir et al. (2014). Song session duration on breeding grounds has shown to be linked to the hormonal cycle in humpbacks. Tyack (1981) reported an increase in song bouts length as a result of decreasing female reproductive activity. This could account for the increased duration of full song sessions observed in March and April off Norway’s coast. However, given the small number of identified complete song sessions in other months such as February, this result must be interpreted with caution.

The majority of singers detected at a given time within the area were single whales, much in contrast to the chorusing singers on low-latitude breeding grounds. This pre-dominance of solo singers is consistent with other studies of high latitude feeding grounds (Gabriele and Frankel, 2002; Vu et al., 2012), however, these findings are not in line with the high number of simultaneous singers in Icelandic waters (Magnúsdóttir and Lim, 2019). Following the trend of singing activity per day per month, it can be inferred that the number of males present within the study area seems to be the driver of this variation in song occurrence. Humpback whale songs are gradually and continuously evolving within any given population from year to year, but all males conform to the same version of song within the same area (Payne et al., 1983; Payne and Payne, 1985). This change of themes, phrases and even units over time is termed “song evolution” (Payne et al., 1983) and “song revolution” is characterized by the complete replacement of an existing song by a novel song (Noad et al., 2000). Another ongoing study of the same dataset (unpublished) indicates temporal variation in song structure in Northern Norway. As already proposed by Magnúsdóttir and Lim (2019), song exchange in high latitude feeding areas might be a key driving force behind cultural transmission for North Atlantic humpback whale populations. Detailed analyses of humpback song patterns over multiple years and comparisons of song types between feeding and breeding grounds as well as along migration routes may contribute to our understanding of singing behavior in subarctic waters. It would furthermore help to elucidate the importance of feeding grounds for song exchange at high latitude feeding grounds of the North Atlantic.

The results presented here are largely in line with previous findings of humpback whale song production in the North Atlantic. However, they also provoke further investigations in relation to feeding and breeding behavior and how humpback vocalizations might relate to environmental conditions in Norwegian waters.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author Contributions

AA conceived the original idea. ST and AA designed the study. ST processed the data, performed the analysis, and took the lead in writing the manuscript. AA and UL supervised the project. AA, HA, GP, and UL provided critical feedback and helped shape the research, analysis and manuscript. All authors have read and approved this submitted version.

Funding

Funding for this work was partially provided by a postdoc scholarship from VISTA–a research program in collaboration between the Norwegian Academy of Sciences and Letters and Equinor. Acoustic sampling was provided by Equinor AA funding LoVe phase 1, and the Research Council of Norway under contract no. 245843 and partners funding LoVe phase 2. This project was made possible with the support of the Erasmus+ programme of the European Union and open access funding enabled by UiT Arctic University of Norway. The authors declare that this project was partly funded by Equinor ASA. The funder was not involved in the study analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Guosong Zhang at the Norwegian Institute of Marine Research and Oliver Nilssen for helping with the Python code for downloading the dataset.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fmars.2021.669748/full#supplementary-material

References

Aniceto, A. S., Pedersen, G., Primicerio, R., Biuw, M., Lindstrøm, U., and Camus, L. (2020). Arctic marine data collection using oceanic gliders: providing ecological context to cetacean vocalizations. Front. Marine Sci. 7:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fmars.2020.585754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au, W. W. L., Mobley, J., Burgess, W. C., Lammers, M. O., and Nachtigall, P. E. (2000). Seasonal and diurnal trends of chorusing humpback whales wintering in waters off western Maui. Marine Mammal Sci. 16, 530–544. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2000.tb00949.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. S., and Herman, L. M. (1984). Aggressive behavior between humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) wintering in Hawaiian waters. Canad. J. Zool. 62, 1922–1937. doi: 10.1139/z84-282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. S., Herman, L. M., Perry, A., Lawton, W. S., Straley, J. M., and Straley, J. H. (1985). Population characteristics and migration of summer and late−season humpback whales megaptera novaeangliae in Southeastern Alaska. Marine Mammal Sci. 1, 304–323. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.1985.tb00018.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broms, F., Wezel, F., López Suárez, P., Stevick, P. T., Biuw, M., Jann, B., et al. (2015). “Recent research on the migratory destinations of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) from a mid-winter feeding stop-over area in Northern Norway,” in Recent Research on the Migratory Destinations of Humpback Whales, ed. F. W. Wenzel (Malta: European Cetacean Society Special Publication).

Google Scholar

Brown, M. R., Corkeron, P. J., Hale, P. T., Schultz, K. W., and Bryden, M. M. (1995). Evidence for a sex-segregated migration in the humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 259, 229–234. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Center for Conservation Bioacoustics. (2019). Raven Pro: Interactive Sound Analysis Software (Version 1.6.1) [Computer software]. New York, NY: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology.

Google Scholar

Cerchio, S., Jacobsen, J. K., and Norris, T. F. (2001). Temporal and geographical variation in songs of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae: synchronous change in Hawaiian and Mexican breeding assemblages. Animal Behav. 62, 313–329. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cholewiak, D. M. (2008). Evaluating the Role of Song in the Humpback Whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) Breeding System with Respect to Intra-Sexual Interactions. Ph. D, Theses. New York, NY: Cornell University.

Google Scholar

Cholewiak, D. M., Sousa-Lima, R. S., and Cerchio, S. (2013). Humpback whale song hierarchical structure: historical context and discussion of current classification issues. Marine Mammal Sci. 29, E312–E332. doi: 10.1111/mms.12005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C. W., and Clapham, P. J. (2004). Acoustic monitoring on a humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) feeding ground shows continual singing into late spring. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 271, 1051–1057. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. S., and Herman, L. M. (1997). Sex differences in site fidelity and migration of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) to the Hawaiian Islands. Canad. J. Zool. 75, 1923–1933. doi: 10.1139/z97-822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darling, J. D., and Bérubé, M. (2001). Interactions of singing humpback whales with other males. Marine Mammal Sci. 17, 570–584. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2001.tb01005.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darling, J. D., Jones, M. E., and Nicklin, C. P. (2006). Humpback whale songs: do they organize males during the breeding season? Behaviour 143, 1051–1101. doi: 10.1163/156853906778607381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawbin, W. (1966). “The seasonal migratory cycle of humpback whales,” in Whales, Dolphins and Porpoises, ed. K. S. Norris (Los Angeles: University of California Press), 145–170. doi: 10.1525/9780520321373-011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunlop, R. A., Cato, D. H., and Noad, M. J. (2008). Non-song acoustic communication in migrating humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). Marine Mammal Sci. 24, 613–629. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2008.00208.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Equinor and IMR (2020). Ocean Observatory Archive. Available online at: http://love.statoil.com/. (accessed October 15, 2020)

Google Scholar

Gabriele, C., and Frankel, A. (2002). The occurrence and significance of humpback whale songs in glacier bay, Southeastern Alaska. Arctic Res. U.S. 16, 42–47.

Google Scholar

Garland, E. C., Gedamke, J., Rekdahl, M. L., Noad, M. J., Garrigue, C., and Gales, N. (2013). Humpback whale song on the Southern Ocean feeding grounds: implications for cultural transmission. PLoS One 8:e79422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillespie, D., Mellinger, D., Gordon, J., Mclaren, D., Redmond, P., McHugh, R., et al. (2009). PAMGUARD: semiautomated, open source software for real−time acoustic detection and localization of cetaceans. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125:2547. doi: 10.1121/1.4808713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godø, O. R., Johnsen, S., and Torkelsen, T. (2014). The LoVe ocean observatory is in operation. Marine Technol. Soc. J. 48, 24–30. doi: 10.4031/MTSJ.48.2.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gridley, T., Silva, M. F., Wilkinson, C., Seakamela, S. M., and Elwen, S. H. (2018). Song recorded near a super-group of humpback whales on a mid-latitude feeding ground off South Africa. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 143, EL298–EL304. doi: 10.1121/1.5032126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, L. M., Pack, A. A., Spitz, S. S., Herman, E. Y. K., Rose, K., Hakala, S., et al. (2013). Humpback whale song: who sings? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1653–1663. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1576-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kettemer, L., Rikardsen, A., Mul, E., Broms, F., Biuw, E. M., Blanchet, M.-A., et al. (2019). “Migration patterns of eastern North Atlantic humpback whales revealed by satellite tracks,” in Proceeding of the Poster presented at: World Marine Mammal Conference WMMC19, (Spain: Barcelona).

Google Scholar

Magnúsdóttir, E. E., and Lim, R. (2019). Subarctic singers: humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) song structure and progression from an Icelandic feeding ground during winter. PLoS One 14:e0210057. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnúsdóttir, E. E., Miller, P. J. O., Lim, R., Rasmussen, M. H., Lammers, M. O., and Svavarsson, J. (2015). Humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) song unit and phrase repertoire progression on a subarctic feeding ground. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 138, 3362–3374. doi: 10.1121/1.4935517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnúsdóttir, E. E., Rasmussen, M. H., Lammers, M. O., and Svavarsson, J. (2014). Humpback whale songs during winter in subarctic waters. Polar Biol. 37, 427–433. doi: 10.1007/s00300-014-1448-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattila, D. K., Guinee, L. N., and Mayo, C. A. (1987). Humpback whale songs on a north atlantic feeding ground. J. Mammal. 68, 880–883. doi: 10.2307/1381574

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McSweeney, D. J., Chu, K. C., Dolphin, W. F., and Guinee, L. N. (1989). North pacific humpback whale songs: a comparison of southeast alaskan feeding ground songs with hawaiian wintering ground songs. Marine Mammal Sci. 5, 139–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.1989.tb00328.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobley, J. R., Herman, L. M., and Frankel, A. S. (1988). Responses of wintering humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) to playback of recordings of winter and summer vocalizations and of synthetic sound. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23, 211–223. doi: 10.1007/BF00302944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishiwaki, M. (1959). Humpback whales in ryukyuan waters. Sci. Rep. Whales Res. Instit. 14, 49–87.

Google Scholar

Nishiwaki, M. (1960). Ryukyuan humpback whaling in 1960. Sci. Rep. Whales Res. Instit. 15, 1–15. doi: 10.1515/9781614511151.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noad, M. J., Cato, D. H., Bryden, M. M., Jenner, M.-N., and Jenner, K. C. S. (2000). Cultural revolution in whale songs. Nature 408, 537–538. doi: 10.1038/35046199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, K., Tyack, P., and Payne, R. S. (1983). Progressive changes in the songs of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): a detailed analysis of two seasons in Hawaii. Commun. Behav. Whales 13, 49–55.

Google Scholar

Payne, K., and Payne, R. S. (1985). Large scale changes over 19 years in songs of humpback whales in bermuda. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychol. 68, 89–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1985.tb00118.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, R. S., and McVay, S. (1971). Songs of humpback whales. Science 173, 585–597. doi: 10.1126/science.173.3997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramm, T. (2020). Hungry During Migration? Humpback Whale Movement From the Barents Sea to a Feeding Stopover in Northern Norway Revealed by Photo-ID Analysis. Unpublished Master’s thesis. Norway: UiT Arctic University of Norway.

Google Scholar

Ryan, C., Whooley, P., Berrow, S., Barnes, C., Massett, N., Strietman, W., et al. (2015). A longitudinal study of humpback whales in Irish waters. J. Marine Biol. Assoc. UK 96, 877–883. doi: 10.1017/S0025315414002033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sigurjónsson, J., and Víkingsson, G. A. (1997). Seasonal abundance of and estimated food consumption by cetaceans in icelandic and adjacent waters. J. Northwest Atlantic Fish. Sci. 22, 271–287. doi: 10.2960/J.v22.a20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. N., Goldizen, A. W., Dunlop, R. A., and Noad, M. J. (2008). Songs of male humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, are involved in intersexual interactions. Animal Behav. 76, 467–477. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevick, P. T., Allen, J., Bérubé, M., Clapham, P. J., Katona, S. K., Larsen, F., et al. (2003). Segregation of migration by feeding ground origin in North Atlantic humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). J. Zool. 259, 231–237. doi: 10.1017/S0952836902003151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevick, P. T., Allen, J., Clapham, P. J., Katona, S. K., Larsen, F., Lien, J., et al. (2006). Population spatial structuring on the feeding grounds in North Atlantic humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). J. Zool. 270, 244–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00128.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevick, P. T., Berrow, S. D., Bérubé, M., Bouveret, L., Broms, F., Jann, B., et al. (2016). There and back again: multiple and return exchange of humpback whales between breeding habitats separated by an ocean basin. J. Marine Biol. Assoc. UK 1, 1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0025315416000321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyack, P. (1981). Interactions between singing hawaiian humpback whales and conspecifics nearby. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 8, 105–116. doi: 10.1007/BF00300822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Opzeeland, I., Van Parijs, S., Kindermann, L., Burkhardt, E., and Boebel, O. (2013). Calling in the cold: pervasive acoustic presence of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in antarctic coastal waters. PLoS One 8:e73007. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vu, E. T., Risch, D., Clark, C. W., Gaylord, S., Hatch, L., Thompson, M. A., et al. (2012). Humpback whale song occurs extensively on feeding grounds in the western North Atlantic Ocean. Aquat. Biol. 14, 175–183. doi: 10.3354/ab00390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winn, H. E., and Winn, L. K. (1978). The song of the humpback whale Megaptera novaeangliae in the West Indies. Marine Biol. 47, 97–114. doi: 10.1007/BF00395631 doi: 10.1007/bf00395631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: passive acoustics, song occurrence, cetacean, Mysticeti, North Atlantic, Norway

Citation: Tyarks SC, Aniceto AS, Ahonen H, Pedersen G and Lindstrøm U (2021) Humpback Whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) Song on a Subarctic Feeding Ground. Front. Mar. Sci. 8:669748. doi: 10.3389/fmars.2021.669748

Received: 19 February 2021; Accepted: 29 April 2021;
Published: 24 May 2021.

Edited by:

Rebecca Dunlop, The University of Queensland, Australia

Reviewed by:

Songzuo Liu, Harbin Engineering University, China
Robert McCauley, Curtin University, Australia

Copyright © 2021 Tyarks, Aniceto, Ahonen, Pedersen and Lindstrøm. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ulf Lindstrøm, dWxmLmxpbmRzdHJvZW1AaGkubm8=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.