Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Genet., 08 January 2021
Sec. Applied Genetic Epidemiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Population Neuroscience of Development and Aging View all 14 articles

Studying the Interplay Between Apolipoprotein E and Education on Cognitive Decline in Centenarians Using Bayesian Beta Regression

  • 1Department of Biostatistics, Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Section of Geriatrics, Department of Medicine, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
  • 3Institute for Clinical Research and Health Policy Studies, Tufts Medical Center, Boston, MA, United States

Apolipoprotein E (APOE) is an important risk factor for cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease in aging individuals. Among the 3 known alleles of this gene: e2, e3, and e4, the e4 allele is associated with faster cognitive decline and increased risk for Alzheimer’s and dementia, while the e2 allele has a positive effect on longevity, and possibly on preservation of cognitive function. Education also has an important effect on cognition and longevity but the interplay between APOE and education is not well-characterized. Previous studies of the effect of APOE on cognitive decline often used linear regression with the normality assumption, which may not be appropriate for analyzing bounded and skewed neuropsychological test scores. In this paper, we applied Bayesian beta regression to assess the effect of APOE alleles on cognitive decline in a cohort of centenarians with longitudinal assessment of their cognitive function. The analysis confirmed the negative association between older age and cognition and the beneficial effect of education that persists even at the extreme of human lifespan in carriers of the e3 allele. In addition, the analysis showed an association between APOE and cognition that is modified by education. Surprisingly, an antagonistic interaction existed between higher education and APOE alleles, suggesting that education may reduce the positive effect of APOE e2 and increase the negative effect of APOE e4 at extreme old age.

Introduction

Declines of certain cognitive abilities are common complications of aging and identifying risk factors for cognitive decline is essential to search for therapeutic interventions. Known risk factors for cognitive decline include older age, lower education, and genes such as apolipoprotein E (APOE) that plays an important role in the risk for Alzheimer’s disease (Fan et al., 2019). APOE is involved in the transport of cholesterol and other lipids between cellular structures (Mahley, 1988). The gene has three well-characterized alleles e2, e3, and e4 that are defined by the combinations of the genotypes of the single nucleotide polymorphisms rs7412 and rs429358. The three alleles differ in two amino acids and result in proteins produced by the e2 and e3 alleles that bind to high-density lipoprotein cholesterol (HDL) while e4 binds to very-low-density lipoprotein (VLDL) (Belloy et al., 2019). Studies have shown that e4-carriers have increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease and accelerated cognitive decline compared to non-carriers (Staehelin et al., 1999; Bondi et al., 2003; Wetter et al., 2005; Belloy et al., 2019), while e2-carriers appears to have a reduced risk for age-related neurodegenerative disease (Henderson et al., 1995; Raber et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2017). The review by O’donoghue et al. (2018) lists 40 studies of the association between APOE and cognition in longitudinal studies, but none of these studies examined this association among individuals at extreme ages (e.g., centenarians). Investigating the association of this gene with cognitive decline in centenarians is important for informing about the extent of the protective and deleterious effect of this gene on cognitive function at the extreme of human lifespan. In addition, examining the interaction between education and APOE alleles could help to better characterize the long term effect of education on cognition and the interplay between genetic and environmental risk factors. Therefore, in this work, we examined the association of e2 and e4 alleles of APOE with cognitive function in centenarians enrolled in the New England Centenarian Study (NECS) (Sebastiani and Perls, 2012), who are enriched for carriers of the e2 allele, have varying levels of education, and for whom we have longitudinally collected assessments of cognitive function.

Typically, longitudinal studies of the effect of APOE on cognitive decline use linear mixed models of data collected from a variety of neuropsychological tests (Blair et al., 2005; Caselli et al., 2009; Kim et al., 2017). These models rely on the assumption that errors follow a normal distribution, which is unbounded and symmetrically distributed. However, the outcome of any neuropsychological test can easily violate the normality assumption in two aspects: (1) the neuropsychological test scores are usually defined in a limited interval between 0 and a maximum test score, and (2) the distribution of neuropsychological test scores is often skewed. Violating the normality assumption when modeling cognitive test scores could lead to a poor estimation of the genetic effect on cognitive function, and predict scores that are either negative or exceed the maximum test value. To address this problem, we propose using a regression model based on the assumption that the response follows a beta distribution. Beta distribution is defined in a finite interval and can accommodate distribution with different shapes. Hence this distribution is well suited to model test scores and questionnaire outcomes that can take values in a finite range (Smithson and Verkuilen, 2006).

Beta regression was proposed by Ferrari and Cribari-Neto (2004) and was further developed by others (Smithson and Verkuilen, 2006; Simas et al., 2010; Figueroa-Zúniga et al., 2013). Studies in multiple fields have utilized beta regression to model different variables such as ischemic stroke lesion volume (Swearingen et al., 2011), genetic distance (Branscum et al., 2007), and understory vegetation communities (Eskelson et al., 2011). In this paper, we use a Bayesian hierarchical beta regression model to fit longitudinally collected neuropsychological test scores, and we compare the results of this analysis with the conventional linear mixed model. Using the Bayesian beta regression to analyze the association between APOE and cognitive decline among centenarians enrolled in the NECS is an important novelty of our analysis (Sebastiani and Perls, 2012).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The NECS is an ongoing study that began in 1994 as a population-based study of centenarians living within eight towns in the Boston area and expanded enrollment to include centenarians, their siblings and offspring as well as controls to North America in 2000. Enrolled participants provide socio-demographic data, medical history, and physical function ability (Barthel Index, Mahoney and Barthel, 1965; Sinoff and Ore, 1997). Participants are also administered the Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration (BIMC) test (Blessed et al., 1968; Kawas et al., 1995), which is a brief test of global cognition that can be administered over the phone to the participant or with the help of a proxy. The BIMC has a maximum total score of 37 points. Scores of 33 or greater represent no impairment, 27–32 indicate mild impairment, 21–26 signify moderate impairment, and less than 20 are associated with severe impairment (Kawas et al., 1995). NECS participants are followed annually to collect new medical events, changes in medication and physical function ability, and are administered the BIMC annually. In this analysis, we use the data collected through November 2019. The data include information about sex, education, race, ages, APOE genotypes, and BIMC scores of participants who agreed to complete the test. Participants with Alzheimer’s disease or dementia who could not complete the test were not included. Genotype data for APOE were inferred from the combinations of the SNPs s7412 and rs429358 as described in Sebastiani et al. (2019a).

Statistical Analysis

Beta Regression Specification

A variable y defined in the interval (0, 1) follows the beta distribution if the density function is proportional to

f ( y | μ , ϕ ) y μ ϕ - 1 ( 1 - y ) ( 1 - μ ) ϕ - 1 ,

where μ represents the mean: 0 < μ < 1; the function μ(1−μ)/(1 + ϕ) is the variance, and ϕ > 0 is the precision parameter (Ferrari and Cribari-Neto, 2004). To parameterize the mean as a function of covariates, it is convenient to use the logit function

g ( μ ) = l o g ( μ / ( 1 - μ ) ) = x i T β ,

where β is a vector of coefficient and xiT is a vector of covariates (Smithson and Verkuilen, 2006; Zeileis et al., 2010). The beta distribution is defined in the open interval (0, 1). To fit this model to data defined in the range (a,b), we use the transformation y′ = (ya)/(ba). In our analysis, the BIMC scores range from 0 to 37. By adding/subtracting a small correct term from the minimum/maximum values of the BIMC scores, we rescaled the data to the interval [0.01, 0.99] to avoid zeros and ones.

Bayesian Beta Regression Modeling

We were interested in modeling the association of the following main covariates with BIMC scores: age, sex, education, and APOE alleles. We standardized the continuous variables age and education to generate parameters on the same scale and coded the dichotomous variable sex 0 for female and 1 for male. Since homozygote carriers of e2 or e4 are rare, we used the following allele groupings in the analyses:

1. “e2 group” comprising carriers of the APOE genotypes e2e2 or e2e3;

2. “e3 group” comprising carriers of the APOE genotype e3e3;

3. “e4 group” comprising carriers of the APOE genotypes e3e4 or e4e4.

We used dummy codings regarding these three groups, and we selected “e3 group” as the reference group in all of our analyses because it is the most frequent genotype in Whites. We performed two analyses to distinguish the beneficial effect of the e2 group allele relative to e3 group from lack of carrying the deleterious e4 allele. One analysis estimated the association between APOE and BIMC scores of the e2 group relative to the e3 group by including only the e2 groups and the e3 groups. The other analysis estimated the association between APOE and BIMC scores of the e4 group relative to the e3 group. For completeness, we also performed an additional analysis that included all three APOE groups.

We used backward selection for model fitting, and started with the hierarchical regression model:

y i j Beta ( μ i j ϕ , ϕ ( 1 - μ i j ) )
logit ( μ i j ) = μ b 0 I s i n g l e + b i 0 ( 1 - I s i n g l e ) + β a g e a g e i j +
β a g e 2 a g e i j 2 + β s e x s e x i + β e d u e d u i + β A P O E A P O E i +
β a g e . e d u a g e i e d u i + β a g e . A P O E a g e i A P O E i +
β e d u . A P O E e d u i A P O E i ,

where yij denotes the jth cognitive test score of the ith participant, the β coefficients are fixed effects, and bi0bi0 is the random intercept that we used to account for within participant correlation of the repeated measurements. Besides the covariates of the main effects of age, sex, education, and APOE, we also included a squared term of age and two-way interactions of these main effects in our model. We used a piecewise random intercept μb0Isingle + bi0⋅(1−Isingle) to accommodate participants with different number of test administration, where the indicator variable Isingle is 0 for participants with only one cognitive test score and Isingle = 1 otherwise. The random intercepts bi0 were assumed to be independent and normally distributed, i.e., bi0N(μb2b). We specified a normal prior for the mean parameter μb of the random intercept that μbN(0, 1000), and a gamma distribution with the shape and scale parameters both equal to 1 for the precision parameter ϕ and the precision of random intercept 1/σ2b. With this parameterization, the participants with only one test score would be assigned the fixed intercept μb in the regression, while participants with more than one test score have their own random intercept bi0. We used normal priors for all the fixed effect parameters.

We implemented the backward model selection algorithm using the deviance information criterion (DIC) (Spiegelhalter et al., 2002, 2014), which is particularly useful for selection of hierarchical models. Since DIC has a tendency to overfit (Clarke and Clarke, 2018), we then refined the model selected by this search by retaining only interactions and main effects with a posterior credible interval (CI) that did not include 0. Once we selected the final model, we also conducted a sensitivity analysis with respect to the prior distributions. For regression coefficients and the mean parameter of the random intercept that use the normal priors, we reduced the variance from 1,000 to 10 and 100. For precision parameters of the beta distribution and the random intercept that use the gamma priors, we modified the variance of gamma priors from 1 to 100. We ran each case to assess if the parameter estimates altered after we changed the scale of the prior parameters. We estimated the explained variance of the model using the regression sum of squares divided by the total sum of squares. All analyses were conducted in R3.6 and all Bayesian models were analyzed using Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) implemented in the “rjags” package (Plummer, 2016). The posterior estimates of the parameters are derived from at least 8,000 burn-in adaptions and 4,000 iterations.

Interpretation of the Results

A limitation of beta regression is that the magnitude of the regression coefficients is not directly interpretable in terms of changes of the outcome. To better understand the association of APOE with BIMC scores and the interplay with education, we estimated the fitted means (marginal effect) using inverse transformation of the logit function, and then we rescaled the fitted means to the original scale of the BIMC scores (0, 37). We calculated fitted means for the e2, e3, and e4 groups for three different education levels: low education (25% quantile of study population’s education; 8 years), median education (median of study population’s education; 12 years), and high education (75% quantile of study population’s education; 15 years). For each allele in each education level, we also calculated the corresponding age of onset of moderate cognitive impairment (BIMC score = 26) using the fitted trajectories of the cognitive test scores. These ages provide a quantitatively more interpretable metric of the genetic effects.

Comparison With the Conventional Method

For comparison with the conventional method, we performed two analyses that focus on the effect of e2 or e4 separately using linear mixed models. For a fair comparison, we used the same variables as selected by Bayesian beta regression, and we kept the random intercept term to adjust for the subject effect. The linear mixed model was conducted in R3.6 using the package “lme4.” We visualized the results of these two methods by plotting the fitted BIMC scores (marginal effect) with their credible/confidence intervals against age. In the Bayesian beta regression, the credible interval of the fitted value was computed using the 2.5th and 97.5 percentiles from the MCMC samples. In the linear mixed model, the confidence interval of the fitted values was derived using bootstrap. We also computed a residual sum of squares (RSS) for each method.

Results

Applying Bayesian Beta Regression on the NECS

Out of 768 total participants in the NECS dataset with APOE genotypes, we excluded 167 participants with missing test scores, 111 participants with missing education information, and 4 participants with APOE genotype e2e4. Table 1 summarizes the demographic characteristics and test scores at baseline of the 486 participants used in our analysis. The age range of the data used in our analysis was 91–113 years. The APOE e3 group was the most prevalent and used as the reference group in all subsequent analyses. Approximately 24% of centenarians in the study carried at least one e2 allele, while only 8% were carriers of an e4 allele. We did not find any e4 homozygous centenarians.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and the BIMC scores at baseline of the participants in the New England Centenarian Study.

The histogram in Figure 1 shows that the distribution of the baseline BIMC scores is bounded and highly skewed. Hence we analyzed the rescaled test scores using beta regression. The combination of backward selection using the DIC criterion and refinement of the model using the posterior credible intervals produced the models that are summarized in Tables 2, 3. The diagnostic plots (trace plots, autocorrelation plots, and the Gelman plots of all MCMC chains) showed no indications of lack of convergence. The sensitivity analyses also showed that changing the prior distributions did not appreciably alter the parameter estimates. The diagnostic plots and sensitivity analyses are all included in Supplementary Material.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Histogram of baseline Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration scores from the New England Centenarian Study.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Parameter estimates and 95% credible intervals from the analysis of the BIMC scores in carriers of APOE e2 and e3 alleles using Bayesian beta regression.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Parameter estimates and 95% credible intervals from the analysis of the BIMC scores in carriers of APOE e4 and e3 alleles using Bayesian beta regression.

Table 2 summarizes the results of the analysis comparing APOE e2 with APOE e3, and Table 3 summarizes the results of the analysis comparing APOE e4 with APOE e3. The significant coefficients are marked with bold fonts. In both analyses, age, sex, and education were all significantly associated with BIMC scores. Older age was associated with significantly worse performance on the test. More years of education and male sex were associated with significantly higher scores.

In the analysis comparing e2 with e3, the interaction between education and e2 was significant, thus suggesting an effect modification of education on the association of e2 with BIMC scores. It is noteworthy that the estimates of the main effect of education (0.063) and the interaction term (−0.062) were opposite, so that the negative interaction between e2 and education numerically canceled out the positive effect of education on BIMC scores in carriers of the e2 allele. This is shown in Figure 2, in which the fitted BIMC scores in e2 carriers remain nearly the same in different education levels. Figure 2 also shows the positive effect of education on BIMC scores in the e3 group. Interestingly, only participants of the e3 group with low education had a worse BIMC score compared to carriers of one or more e2 alleles. The difference between the e2 and e3 groups became negligible in participants with median education level, and carriers of the e3e3 genotypes with a high level of education obtained a better score than carriers of the e2 alleles.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Fitted Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration scores in carriers of one or more e2 alleles (dashed lines) compared to homozygotes e3e3 (continuous lines) for low education level (25% quantile, 8 years, red), median education level (12 years, black), and high education level (75% quantile, 15 years, blue). The scores were fitted using the estimates from Bayesian beta regression, assuming sex = female. Note visually there is only one dashed line, because the fitted lines of e2 carriers overlap in all three education groups.

To better summarize the clinical implication of these results, we calculated the ages of onset of cognitive impairment predicted by the fitted model. From the fitted lines, we estimated that the ages of onset of moderate cognitive impairment were 99.5, 101.8, and 103.5 years in for e3 carriers with low, median, and high education groups, respectively. The age of onset of moderate cognitive impairment (BIMC score = 26) was 102.1 years in e2 carriers, independent of education. Therefore, e2 carriers were estimated to delay the onset of moderate cognitive impairment by approximately 2 years compared to e3 carriers with low education, but this advantage essentially disappeared with higher education.

In the analysis comparing e4 with e3, both the main effect term of e4 and the interaction with education were significantly negative, thus suggesting that higher education was not sufficient to remove the negative association of the e4 allele with BIMC scores. This is illustrated in Figure 3 that shows the fitted BIMC scores in the e3 and e4 groups stratified by education. Among individuals with the e3e3 genotype, more years of education was associated with higher BIMC scores, but the positive effect modification of higher education was reduced in carriers of e4 compared to e3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Fitted Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration test scores in carriers of one or more e4 alleles (dashed lines) compared to homozygotes e3e3 (continuous lines) for low education level (25% quantile, 8 years, red), median education level (12 years, black), and high education level (75% quantile, 15 years, blue). The scores were fitted using the estimates from Bayesian beta regression, assuming sex = female.

From the fitted lines, the ages of onset of moderate cognitive impairment in e4 carriers were 99.5, 98.9, and 98.57 years in participants with low, median, and high education, respectively. Hence, in centenarians carrying the e4 allele, more years of education was not associated with a delay of cognitive decline.

The results of the combined analysis including all three groups of APOE alleles are summarized in Supplementary Table 1. After using the same model selection strategy as the previous separated analyses, the selected model had exactly the same variables of the two separated analyses. The direction of the effects of APOE and the significance of the variables were consistent with the previous analyses. The major difference was that the magnitude of the effects of APOE decreased in the combined analysis. For example, in the separated analysis, the beta coefficients of e2 and e4 were 0.032 and −0.382, respectively, while in the combined analysis the beta coefficients of e2 and e4 were 0.026 and −0.326. Hence, the analysis that combined all APOE alleles together suggested slightly smaller but still significant effects of APOE.

Comparison With Conventional Linear Mixed Models

The results of the analyses using linear mixed models are summarized in Tables 4, 5. In both analyses, only the effects of age and education reached statistical significance. The direction of the main effects of e2 and e4 were consistent with those estimated using the Bayesian beta regression. However, the effects did not reach statistical significance.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Parameter estimates from the analysis of the BIMC scores in carriers of APOE e2 and e3 alleles using the linear mixed model.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Parameter estimates from the analysis of the BIMC scores in carriers of APOE e4 and e3 alleles using the linear mixed model.

To visualize the differences of the results from Bayesian beta regression and the linear mixed model, we plotted the fitted BIMC scores with their 95% credible/confidence intervals against age in Figure 4. The plot shows that the credible intervals of the Bayesian beta regression were slightly narrower compared with the confidence intervals of the linear mixed model. The linear mixed model did not fit the data of the youngest age group well, and the fitted values as well as their confidence interval were even greater than the maximum BIMC score (max score = 37). This result is consistent with our hypothesis that using a linear mixed model with normality assumptions is not suitable when the outcomes are bounded neuropsychological scores. In addition, the Bayesian beta regression appeared to better capture the gradually increasing rate of decline, but the linear mixed model did not capture this feature. The RSS of the Bayesian beta regression was 51032.5 for the e2 analysis and 43151.54 for the e4 analysis, while the RSS of the linear mixed models was slightly larger (52443.72 for the e2 analysis and 43652.30 for the e4 analysis), thus confirming that the Bayesian beta regression fit the data better.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Fitted Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration test scores using Bayesian beta regression and the linear mixed models with their credible/confidence bands. (A) Analysis in carriers of APOE e2 and e3. (B) Analysis in carriers of APOE e4 and e3.

Discussion

In this paper, we investigated the relationship between APOE alleles and change of cognitive function in a large cohort of centenarians enriched for carriers of the e2 allele. Instead of using the conventional linear mixed model, we used a Bayesian hierarchical beta regression model to characterize the effect of APOE on cognitive function assessed through the Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration test. Our analyses confirmed the decline of cognitive function and the positive effect of education on preservation of cognitive function at extreme old age. The analysis also showed that the APOE e4 allele has a negative association with cognitive function even at extreme old age, and the APOE e2 allele appears to be protective only in centenarians with low education.

Our findings are consistent with previous studies that showed a decline of cognitive function with older age in multiple domains (Harada et al., 2013), and the negative effect of APOE e4 on cognitive decline in centenarians (Arosio et al., 2017; Du et al., 2020). There is some evidence that, among centenarians, the e2 allele confers protection from cognitive decline (Kim et al., 2017; Sebastiani et al., 2020), in addition to increasing the chance for longevity (Sebastiani et al., 2019a) and conferring protection from aging-related diseases (Wolters et al., 2019; Kuo et al., 2020). Our studies also found that carriers of e2 with low education can delay their onset of moderate cognitive impairment. These results suggest that targeting e2 gene products could lead to important therapeutics for the preservation of cognitive function (Sebastiani et al., 2019b). Interestingly, the persistence of the e4 allele in the population has been linked to a survival advantage at a young age and treatments that target the effect of APOE alleles at an older age will need to consider the pleiotropic effect of this gene (Belloy et al., 2019).

Studies have shown the importance of early education on better cognitive function at an older age (Alley et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2009). However, our study showed an antagonistic interaction between higher education and APOE alleles that suggests higher education may reduce the positive effect of e2 and increase the negative effect of e4 at extreme old age. A study of Japanese centenarians detected an education by APOE e4 interaction on cognition that differed by sex (Ishioka et al., 2016). The study showed that highly educated centenarians who carried at least one e4 allele had worse performance in the Mini-Mental State Examination, compared to poorly educated centenarians. Our findings are consistent with this counter-intuitive result but go one step further and disentangle the negative association of the e4 allele from the positive association of the e2 allele.

Our study focused on the association between APOE alleles and cognitive decline in extreme old individuals. Our analysis included well-known risk factors of cognitive decline such as older age and education. However, many more factors may affect the onset and rate of cognitive decline together or independently of APOE. Nutrition and dietary habits, for example, may be important risk factors to be considered in future analyses, given the role of APOE in lipid metabolism (Belloy et al., 2019). Studies showed that place of living changes the association between APOE alleles and extreme human longevity, after controlling for the overall genetic background (Gurinovich et al., 2019), suggesting that lifestyle may modify the genetic effect of APOE. However, we do not have data about dietary patterns in NECS centenarians to perform these analyses. The explained variance of our model was 0.29 for the e2 analysis and 0.30 for the e4 analysis. These results suggest that age, sex, APOE, and education can only explain less than 1/3 of the variability in BIMC scores and there are many factors, yet to be discovered, that should account for the unexplained variance. In future work, it will be interesting to collect data about additional physiological, medical, nutritional, or social factors that may contribute to maintain good cognitive function as people age and investigate their interplay with APOE alleles.

The Bayesian beta regression we used in this analysis has several strengths. First, this method allowed us to fit the test scores in the range of admissible values, and to model non-linear relations of the score with age and education. We illustrated the advantages of Bayesian beta regression by comparing it with the conventional linear mixed model using the NECS data. Second, to increase statistical power, we included all participants with at least one cognitive function assessment in our model. A piecewise random intercept was used to include subjects with either repeated measurements or just one measurement. Finally, we carried out a model selection using the DIC criterion and then refined our model by credible intervals. From the diagnostic plots of the MCMC, the final models converged well. In addition, in previous studies (Caselli et al., 2009; Kulminski et al., 2015; Barral et al., 2017) APOE genotype was characterized as carriers of e4 (i.e., e2e4, e3e4, and e4e4), and non-carriers of e4 (i.e., e2e2, e2e3, and e3e3). This comparison could yield biased results since the non-carriers of the e4 group might not represent the most prevalent population. There are also studies (Kim et al., 2017) included e2 (e2e2 and e2e3), e3 (e3e3), and e4 (e3e4 and e4e4) in a combined analysis with dummy codings. In fact, we performed a combined analysis on the NECS data and found that it produced smaller effects of e2 and e4. To eliminate these potential bias, we divided the participants into subgroups, where we only compared e2 carriers to e3 carriers and e4 carriers to e3 carriers separately.

Notable limitations of this study include loss to follow-up and mortality. Due to the extreme age of the study population, the mortality rate is considerably high: 49% of participants included in our analysis only completed the baseline evaluation. The absence of longitudinal data may induce bias, and thus we used a piecewise random intercept to compensate for those only with one record.

In conclusion, we examined the association between APOE alleles and cognitive decline in a cohort of centenarians. We confirmed the negative correlation between the e4 allele and cognition even at the extreme of human lifespan, and newly found the carrying the e2 alleles appears to be beneficial only in centenarians with poor education. The interaction between APOE e4 and education produced a counter-intuitive result that is, however, consistent with other results in centenarians. The antagonistic relation between higher education and carrying the e4 allele may be confounded by other risk factors and warrants more in-depth studies.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The use of the data is restricted and needs approval. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to PS, psebastiani@tuftsmedicalcenter.org.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Boston University IRB. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

QX proposed the method used in the manuscript, conducted the statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript. SA and TP designed the study, collected the data from the study cohort, and helped editing the manuscript. PS designed the analytic strategy, supervised the work, and critically revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA cooperative agreements U19-AG023122, R01 AG061844, and K01 AG057798).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fgene.2020.606831/full#supplementary-material

References

Alley, D., Suthers, K., and Crimmins, E. (2007). Education and cognitive decline in older americans. Res. Aging 29, 73–94. doi: 10.1177/0164027506294245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arosio, B., Ostan, R., Mari, D., Damanti, S., Ronchetti, F., Arcudi, S., et al. (2017). Cognitive status in the oldest old and centenarians: a condition crucial for quality of life methodologically difficult to assess. Mech. Ageing Dev. 165, 185–194. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2017.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barral, S., Singh, J., Fagan, E., Cosentino, S., Andersen-Toomey, S. L., Wojczynski, M. K., et al. (2017). Age-related biomarkers in LLFS families with exceptional cognitive abilities. J. Gerontol.Ser. A 72, 1683–1688. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belloy, M. E., Napolioni, V., and Greicius, M. D. (2019). A quarter century of APOE and Alzheimer’s Disease: progress to date and the path forward. Neuron 101, 820–838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., Folsom, A., Knopman, D. S., Bray, M., Mosley, T., and Boerwinkle, E. (2005). APOE genotype and cognitive decline in a middle-aged cohort. Neurology 64, 268–276.

Google Scholar

Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., and Roth, M. (1968). The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br. J. Psychiatry 114, 797–811.

Google Scholar

Bondi, M. W., Houston, W. S., Salmon, D. P., Corey-Bloom, J., Katzman, R., Thal, L. J., et al. (2003). Neuropsychological deficits associated with Alzheimer’s disease in the very-old: discrepancies in raw vs. standardized scores. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 9, 783–795. doi: 10.1017/s1355617703950119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branscum, A. J., Johnson, W. O., and Thurmond, M. C. (2007). Bayesian beta regression: applications to household expenditure data and genetic distance between foot-and-mouth disease viruses. Austr. N. Z. J. Stat. 49, 287–301.

Google Scholar

Caselli, R. J., Dueck, A. C., Osborne, D., Sabbagh, M. N., Connor, D. J., Ahern, G. L., et al. (2009). Longitudinal modeling of age-related memory decline and theAPOEε4 effect. N. Engl. J. Med. 361, 255–263. doi: 10.1056/nejmoa0809437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, B. S., and Clarke, J. L. (2018). Predictive Statistics: Analysis and Inference Beyond Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Du, M., Andersen, S. L., Schupf, N., Feitosa, M. F., Barker, M. S., Perls, T. T., et al. (2020). Accepted. association between APOE alleles and change of neuropsychological tests in the long life family study. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. (in press). doi: 10.3233/JAD-201113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eskelson, B. N., Madsen, L., Hagar, J. C., and Temesgen, H. (2011). Estimating riparian understory vegetation cover with beta regression and copula models. For. Sci. 57, 212–221.

Google Scholar

Fan, J., Tao, W., Li, X., Li, H., Zhang, J., Wei, D., et al. (2019). The contribution of genetic factors to cognitive impairment and dementia: apolipoprotein E Gene, gene interactions, and polygenic risk. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20:1177. doi: 10.3390/ijms20051177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, S., and Cribari-Neto, F. (2004). Beta regression for modelling rates and proportions. J. Appl. Stat. 31, 799–815.

Google Scholar

Figueroa-Zúniga, J. I., Arellano-Valle, R. B., and Ferrari, S. L. P. (2013). Mixed beta regression: a bayesian perspective. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 61, 137–147. doi: 10.1016/j.csda.2012.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurinovich, A., Andersen, S. L., Puca, A., Atzmon, G., Barzilai, N., and Sebastiani, P. (2019). Varying effects of APOE alleles on extreme longevity in European Ethnicities. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 74(Suppl._1), S45–S51. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glz179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harada, C. N., Natelson Love, M. C., and Triebel, K. L. (2013). Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 29, 737–752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, A. S., Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Christensen, H., Jacomb, P. A., Easteal, S., et al. (1995). Apolipoprotein E allele &isin;4, dementia, and cognitive decline in a population sample. Lancet 346, 1387–1390. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(95)92405-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishioka, Y. L., Gondo, Y., Fuku, N., Inagaki, H., Masui, Y., Takayama, M., et al. (2016). Effects of the APOE ε4 allele and education on cognitive function in Japanese centenarians. AGE 38, 495–503. doi: 10.1007/s11357-016-9944-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawas, C., Karagiozis, H., Resau, L., Corrada, M., and Brookmeyer, R. (1995). Reliability of the blessed telephone information-memory-concentration test. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 8, 238–242.

Google Scholar

Kim, Y. J., Seo, S. W., Park, S. B., Yang, J. J., Lee, J. S., Lee, J., et al. (2017). Protective effects of APOE e2 against disease progression in subcortical vascular mild cognitive impairment patients: a three-year longitudinal study. Sci. Rep. 7:1910. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02046-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulminski, A. M., Arbeev, K. G., Culminskaya, I., Ukraintseva, S. V., Stallard, E., Province, M. A., et al. (2015). Trade-offs in the effects of the apolipoprotein E Polymorphism on risks of diseases of the heart, cancer, and neurodegenerative disorders: insights on mechanisms from the long life family study. Rejuvenation Res. 18, 128–135. doi: 10.1089/rej.2014.1616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, C.-L., Pilling, L. C., Atkins, J. L., Kuchel, G. A., and Melzer, D. (2020). ApoE e2 and aging-related outcomes in 379,000 UK Biobank participants. Aging 12, 12222–12233. doi: 10.18632/aging.103405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahley, R. (1988). Apolipoprotein E: cholesterol transport protein with expanding role in cell biology. Science 240, 622–630. doi: 10.1126/science.3283935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahoney, F. I., and Barthel, D. W. (1965). Functional evaluation: the Barthel Index: a simple index of independence useful in scoring improvement in the rehabilitation of the chronically ill. Md State Med. J. 14, 61–65.

Google Scholar

O’donoghue, M. C., Murphy, S. E., Zamboni, G., Nobre, A. C., and Mackay, C. E. (2018). APOE genotype and cognition in healthy individuals at risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Cortex 104, 103–123. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plummer, M. (2016). rjags: Bayesian Graphical Models Using MCMC. R Package Version 4 (6).

Google Scholar

Raber, J., Huang, Y., and Ashford, J. W. (2004). ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk and AD pathology. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 641–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastiani, P., Andersen, S. L., Sweigart, B., Du, M., Cosentino, S., Thyagarajan, B., et al. (2020). Patterns of multi-domain cognitive aging in participants of the long life family study. GeroScience 42, 1335–1350. doi: 10.1007/s11357-020-00202-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastiani, P., Gurinovich, A., Nygaard, M., Sasaki, T., Sweigart, B., Bae, H., et al. (2019a). APOE alleles and extreme human longevity. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 74, 44–51. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastiani, P., Monti, S., Morris, M., Gurinovich, A., Toshiko, T., Andersen, S. L., et al. (2019b). A serum protein signature of APOE genotypes in centenarians. Aging Cell 18:e13023. doi: 10.1111/acel.13023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastiani, P., and Perls, T. T. (2012). The genetics of extreme longevity: lessons from the New England centenarian study. Front. Genet. 3:277. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2012.00277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simas, A. B., Barreto-Souza, W., and Rocha, A. V. (2010). Improved estimators for a general class of beta regression models. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 54, 348–366. doi: 10.1016/j.csda.2009.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinoff, G., and Ore, L. (1997). The barthel activities of daily living index: self-reporting versus actual performance in the Old-Old (= 75 years). J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 45, 832–836. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1997.tb01510.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smithson, M., and Verkuilen, J. (2006). A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychol. Methods 11, 54–71. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.11.1.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., and Van Der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 64, 583–639. doi: 10.1111/1467-9868.00353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., and Van Der Linde, A. (2014). The deviance information criterion: 12 years on. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 76, 485–493. doi: 10.1111/rssb.12062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staehelin, H. B., Perrig-Chiello, P., Mitrache, C., Miserez, A. R., and Perrig, W. J. (1999). Apolipoprotein E genotypes and cognitive functions in healthy elderly persons. Acta Neurol. Scand. 100, 53–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1999.tb00723.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swearingen, C. J., Tilley, B. C., Adams, R. J., Rumboldt, Z., Nicholas, J. S., Bandyopadhyay, D., et al. (2011). Application of beta regression to analyze ischemic stroke volume in NINDS rt-PA clinical trials. Neuroepidemiology 37, 73–82. doi: 10.1159/000330375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetter, S. R., Delis, D. C., Houston, W. S., Jacobson, M. W., Lansing, A., Cobell, K., et al. (2005). Deficits in inhibition and flexibility are associated with the APOE-E4 allele in nondemented older adults. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 27, 943–952. doi: 10.1080/13803390490919001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Hebert, L. E., Scherr, P. A., Barnes, L. L., Mendes De Leon, C. F., and Evans, D. A. (2009). Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology 72, 460–465. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000341782.71418.6c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolters, F. J., Yang, Q., Biggs, M. L., Jakobsdottir, J., Li, S., Evans, D. S., et al. (2019). The impact of APOE genotype on survival: results of 38,537 participants from six population-based cohorts (E2-CHARGE). PLoS One 14:e0219668. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeileis, A., Cribari-Neto, F., Grün, B., and Kos-Midis, I. (2010). Beta regression in R. J. Stat, Softw. 34, 1–24.

Google Scholar

Keywords: apolipoprotein E, beta regression, blessed information-memory-concentration test, cognitive function, centenarians

Citation: Xiang Q, Andersen SL, Perls TT and Sebastiani P (2021) Studying the Interplay Between Apolipoprotein E and Education on Cognitive Decline in Centenarians Using Bayesian Beta Regression. Front. Genet. 11:606831. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2020.606831

Received: 15 September 2020; Accepted: 30 November 2020;
Published: 08 January 2021.

Edited by:

Tomáš Paus, University of Toronto, Canada

Reviewed by:

Anbupalam Thalamuthu, University of New South Wales, Australia
M. J. Mosher, Western Washington University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Xiang, Andersen, Perls and Sebastiani. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qingyan Xiang, qyxiang@bu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.