Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 19 June 2023
Sec. Chemical Ecology
This article is part of the Research Topic Mixtures & Modulations: Responses to Odorant Chemicals in Humans and Other Animals View all 5 articles

Editorial: Mixtures & modulations: responses to odorant chemicals in humans and other animals

  • 1Université Paris Cité, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Unité de Biologie Fonctionnelle et Adaptative, Paris, France
  • 2Fédération des Entreprises de la Beauté (FEBEA) département scientifique et règlementaire, Paris, France

Odorant processing by the olfactory system is a complex and fascinating process that still poses many challenges. One reason is that the sense of smell is unique and distinct from other sensory systems. First the nature of the signal itself is challenging. Contrarily to light or sound that possess measurable physical properties as frequency or duration, odorants can be composed of one to hundreds of molecules with different volatile properties. These molecules may or may not be individually odorous and their combination is not predictable. In addition, unlike other sensory systems, from the olfactory epithelium to central olfactory regions, the olfactory system does not have a clear-cut spatial organization or topographic map, hence the challenge of neuroscientists is to find the code, the organization that relates molecules to percepts (Murthy, 2011; Uchida et al., 2014; Kurian et al., 2021). In this regard, one strategy employed to decipher odorant coding is breaking down olfactory stimulations into their component parts by using monomolecular odorants. However, not so surprisingly, psychophysical experiments, behavioral testing and brain activity recordings in both humans and animals have demonstrated that processing of odorant mixtures is very far from processing the sum of its parts (Xu et al., 2020).

Finally, starting from the olfactory bulb, odorant processing is strongly influenced by the predicted value of the odorant, i.e. by the experience of the animal, and also by other sensory traits of the stimulus or the source of the stimulus. Hence, despite significant efforts to understand the sense of smell, the mechanisms of odorant perception remain elusive and we are far from understanding what makes a chemical smell in a particular way and how this relates to molecules and mixtures.

This Research Topic is composed of 2 original research articles, 1 conceptual analysis and 1 review, which tackle these questions from diverse angles and open new challenging perspectives.

As the number of odorous molecule is uncountable, modelling efforts using artificial intelligence and machine learning, have been conducted, to establish the unique link between chemical features of molecules and psychophysical perception of the odorant. Still, the prediction of the perception of chemical molecules remains unattainable. In their highly thoughtful review article Barwich and Lloyd claim that the current models, which rely exclusively on chemical properties and miss the biological complexity of odorant-receptor interaction, target the wrong question and could be misleading for a comprehensive understanding of odor coding and perception.

As an illustration of the claim of Barwich and Lloyd and based on their pivotal former study showing complex receptor modulation at the level of peripheral olfactory sensory receptors (Xu et al., 2020), Xu et al. challenge the current understanding that we have of olfactory coding. Indeed, if a specific odorant receptor can exhibit opposing activity when its ligand is associated in a mixture with other odorants, this raises questions about the coding mechanisms in the central olfactory system. Xu et al. proposes the compelling hypothesis that the quest for the olfactory code may be vain and that new methods should be used such as medicinal chemistry to advance our understanding this problem.

Li et al. went deeper into the complexity of mixtures by pointing the trigeminal content of odorants. In natural environments, odorants are not only complexes mixtures of molecules with subtle interactions, but also they are very likely to stimulate the trigeminal nerve, which carries somatosensory perception, in the nasal epithelium. Even though the trigeminal sense is under looked in physiological studies, olfaction and trigeminal stimulation are fully interdependent. Does trigeminal stimulation influence odorant mixture perception? Li et al. question the role of trigeminal stimulation in overshadowing the effect of one odorant by the other in binary mixtures.

Finally, Vandroux et al. examined interactions between sex pheromones and plant volatiles in moths. In these insects, finding a mate relies on the female’s release of sex pheromones and detections of this signals by males who have to navigate through a complex olfactory environment. Previous studies have shown that the interactions between sex pheromones and plant volatiles can occur in the peripheral olfactory system of moths at the level of the olfactory receptor neurons, implying that the olfactory receptor neurons that detect sex pheromones would also be able to detect certain volatile plant compounds. Vandroux et al. obtained unexpected results that appear to be in discrepancy with the dogma that each olfactory receptor neurons expresses only one type of olfactory receptor.

In conclusion, the articles published in this Research Topic although enlightening, reveal the thickness of shadows we yet have to explore in our journey to understand the perception of fragrant mixtures. So while your brain is silently processing the pleasant flavor coming out of the warm coffee cup you just pull yourself, ride your glasses, and walk with us though them, in order to catch up with your nose!

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Acknowledgments

We want to thank all the authors who took part in this Research Topic and the reviewers for their valuable evaluation of the manuscripts.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Kurian S. M., Naressi R. G., Manoel D., Barwich A.-S., Malnic B., Saraiva L. R. (2021). Odor coding in the mammalian olfactory epithelium. Cell Tissue Res. 383, 445–456. doi: 10.1007/s00441-020-03327-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murthy V. N. (2011). Olfactory maps in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 233–258. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uchida N., Poo C., Haddad R. (2014). Coding and transformations in the olfactory system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 363–385. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu L., Li W., Voleti V., Zou D.-J., Hillman E. M. C., Firestein S. (2020). Widespread receptor-driven modulation in peripheral olfactory coding. Science 368, eaaz5390. doi: 10.1126/science.aaz5390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: olfaction, odor mixture, perception, trigeminal, pheromone, odorant receptor, machine learning

Citation: Martin C and Poivet E (2023) Editorial: Mixtures & modulations: responses to odorant chemicals in humans and other animals. Front. Ecol. Evol. 11:1227877. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2023.1227877

Received: 23 May 2023; Accepted: 08 June 2023;
Published: 19 June 2023.

Edited and Reviewed by:

Cesar Gemeno, Universitat de Lleida, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Martin and Poivet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Claire Martin, Y2xhaXJlLm1hcnRpbkB1LXBhcmlzLmZy; Erwan Poivet, RXBvaXZldEBmZWJlYS5mcg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.