Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 28 February 2022
Sec. Urban Ecology
This article is part of the Research Topic Ecological and Evolutionary Processes in Neotropical Urban Ecosystems View all 8 articles

The Behavioral Responses of the Chiguanco Thrush to Urbanization in a Neotropical City Comes From Preadapted Behavioral Traits

  • 1Facultad de Ciencias Puras y Naturales, Instituto de Ecología, Universidad Mayor de San Andrés, La Paz, Bolivia
  • 2Carrera de Biología, Facultad de Ciencias Puras y Naturales, Universidad Mayor de San Andrés, La Paz, Bolivia
  • 3Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden

Several animal species can survive within cities by changing their behavior; such changes could be the result of evolutionary adaptation, epigenetic effects, or come from preadapted traits through phenotypic plasticity or non-random dispersal. Exploring whether behavioral preadapted traits are present in non-urbanized populations could improve our understanding of the processes that allow animals to cope with urbanization. We compared the boldness, neophobia, and solving-test skills of adult individuals of the Chiguanco Thrush (Turdus chiguanco) between urban and extra-urban habitats in La Paz (Bolivia), a high-altitude Neotropical city. The urban Chiguanco Thrushes were bolder, less neophobic, and performed better in problem-solving tests. Extra-urban individuals varied significantly more among them in boldness and neophobia, and although a smaller proportion of individuals were able to solve the simplest problem-solving test, they did so in the same way as the urban ones. This evidence suggests that the behavioral responses of the Chiguanco Thrush to urbanization in La Paz come from preadapted traits.

Introduction

Urbanization is an intense global phenomenon that is affecting the biota in several ways, therefore understanding how the biota responds to urbanization has applications from ecological and evolutionary theory to sustainable urban planning (McDonnell and Hahs, 2015; Alberti et al., 2017a; Johnson and Munshi-South, 2017). The phenotypic changes in the behavior of animals that allow them to colonize and survive within cities are among the most described responses (Sol et al., 2013, 2020; Alberti et al., 2017b; Miranda, 2017). They may come from preadapted characters present before urbanization in non-urbanized populations, from epigenetic changes, or from evolutionary adaptation to urban conditions through natural selection (Partecke, 2014; McDonnell and Hahs, 2015).

Preadapted learning and innovation skills could allow urban colonizing populations to adjust their behaviors quickly through phenotypic plasticity (Snell-Rood, 2013; Sol et al., 2013). But also, there are several evidences of genetic divergences in behavioral responses between urban and non-urban populations suggesting adaptive evolutionary processes after urban colonization (e.g., Partecke et al., 2006; Partecke and Gwinner, 2007; Miranda et al., 2013; Mueller et al., 2013; Costantini et al., 2014). However, genetic divergences between urban and non-urban populations can also be explained by non-random dispersal (Edelaar and Bolnick, 2012) or immigrant selection (Partecke, 2014), a sorting process of subsampling in that only non-urban individuals with the appropriate preadapted behaviors would be those who could colonize and establish in urban settings.

Therefore, it is of special interest for the better understanding of the processes that allow the animals to adjust their behaviors to the urbanization, to explore whether the behavioral patterns conductive to colonizing and surviving in cities are present in non-urbanized populations. Our objective is to compare some behaviors between urban and non-urban populations of a same species, exploring the existence of preadapted behavioral responses to urbanization in the natural variations of non-urbanized populations. We selected the Chiguanco Thrush (Turdus chiguanco), an Andean bird species reported as one of the most ubiquitous, frequent, and abundant inhabitants from the hardest urban core to the rural areas of La Paz (Bolivia), a high-altitude tropical city of South America (Villegas and Garitano-Zavala, 2010).

Studying species of the genus Turdus also allows comparing behavioral responses to urbanization with congeneric species, since this genus has a worldwide distribution, many species are in contact with people in cities and suburbs around the world (Evans et al., 2010), and there are several studies on behavioral responses to urbanization for various species. Between them, for example, tolerance to humans (e.g., Møller, 2010; Carrete and Tella, 2011; Møller and Liang, 2012; Díaz et al., 2013; Mikula et al., 2014; Møller and Tryjanowski, 2014; Piratelli et al., 2015), and neophobia and neophilia (Miranda et al., 2013; Batisteli et al., 2022).

We compared the boldness, neophobia and innovation of adult individuals that inhabit the hardest urban core of the city with individuals from extra-urban habitats. For the boldness, we measured flight initiation distance (FID), and for innovation and neophobia we worked with individuals captured temporarily who were subjected to experimental behavioral tests. We focus special attention to differences in intra-population variations. We hypothesize that urban individuals will have a shorter FID, spend less time approaching a novel object, and solve problem-solving tests in less time compared to their extra-urban counterparts. In addition, we hypothesize that the range of intra-populational variation in behavioral responses in urban individuals will be less wide and part of the intra-population variation of the extra-urban Chiguanco Thrushes, such that those responses could come from preadapted traits.

Materials and Methods

Study Area

The city of La Paz is the administrative capital of Bolivia (Figure 1). The urban limits ranges from 16°26′ to 16°33′ S, 68°02′ to 68°10′ W, and 3,300 to 4,100 m. The climate is predominantly dry and cold, with a long arid season (March-October) and a short rainy season (November-February) that corresponds to the breeding season of the birds.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Location of the study sites for behavioral studies of the Chiguanco Thrush in the city of La Paz (Bolivia). In (A) the city of La Paz with the limits of the urban fringe in November 2013 (red polygon), the upper right square details the position of the study area in the Bolivian Andes (solid red square); the position of the study sites is marked with white circles, U1 to U3 for the urban zone, and E1 to E4 for the extra-urban zone. Urban site U2 (B) and extra-urban site E2 (C) are shown in detail as examples of general landscapes. The red arrows in (B) mark the urban capture places for adult individuals. Satellite images from Google Earth Pro 7.3.2, 2019, (11/10/2013).

The city was founded as a Spanish colonial city in the mid-16th century on a pre-Hispanic agrarian settlement located in an inter-Andean valley. It experienced a very low expansion for three centuries maintaining the surroundings as agro-ecosystems. From the mid-twentieth century to the present, La Paz is a sprawling city reaching an urbanized area of 80 km2 with more than 835 thousand inhabitants (United Nations, 2020). Although urban sprawl is recent, the current effect of urbanization on the erosion of native bird species richness is the most severe compared to other cities in South America (Leveau et al., 2017).

For this study, we used the definitions of urban-gradient habitat types proposed by Marzluff et al. (2001) and MacGregor-Fors (2011). We selected the two extremes of the urban gradient, the most urbanized urban core and the extra-urban environment, hereafter called “zones.” Various landscape and demographic parameters could describe the level of urbanization, in our case, the quantification of the level of urbanization does not require a high level of precision or the conjunction of several parameters. We select the percentage of impervious surface as the only parameter, which includes the total surface covered by any human infrastructure (houses, buildings, roads and cemented surfaces). For the estimation of the impervious surface estimation of each of the study sites, we used free Google Earth satellite images corresponding to the period in which we obtained the behavior data, and with good contrast between the vegetation and the impervious surfaces (end of the rainy season in March 2014). We evaluated the impervious surface for each study site in areas of 1 km2 (100 hectares), for this we located a central point for each site and delimited a square of 1000 × 1000 m, with a superimposed grid of 100 squares of 1 ha (Supplementary Figures 1, 2). We visually estimated the proportion of impervious surface in each 1-ha square in four discrete ranges: 0–25%, 26–50%, 51–75%, and 76–100%, and for quantification, we assigned the mean value for each range (12.5, 37.5, 62.5, and 87.5, respectively). We obtained the value for each plot of 1 km2 by the average of the 100 values of the plots of one-hectare.

Our sites in the urban zone had high values of impervious surface (more than 75%, see Supplementary Table 1), high rates of pedestrian and vehicle traffic, multi-family houses, multi-story buildings, and small and scarce public and private green areas dominated by exotic species of trees, shrubs, and herbs. Our sites in the extra-urban zone had less than 20% of impervious surface (Supplementary Table 1), dominated by a matrix of secondary shrub vegetation of native species, mixed with small crops and exotic trees.

Boldness Measurement

We studied the boldness of the Chiguanco Thrush using the Flight Initiation Distance (FID) method in two different events in the non-reproductive period. For the first, we selected six study sites, three for the urban zone (U1, U2, U3) and three for the extra-urban zone (E1, E2, E3) (Figure 1), evaluated in the mornings (06:45 to10:00), between March and June of 2012 and 2013. In order to compare the possible daily variations, we carried out a second measurement event in two urban sites (U1, U2) and two extra-urban sites (E1, E4) (Figure 1), between July and November 2016, in the afternoons (16:30 to 19:00). At each site, we used one or more linear-transects that summed approximately 1.5 km, traveled in one direction to avoid measuring the same individuals, and were repeated at each site three to five times with an interval of at least six weeks between them to avoid the habituation of birds. The home range reported for the species of the genus Turdus is from 2 to 10 ha (Ferry et al., 1981; Calegaro-Marques and Amato, 2014; Moraes et al., 2018); in our study, the linear distance between the central points (see Supplementary Material), the linear transects, and the capture sites (see next section), was at least 500 m with respect to the urban fringe, so we consider that the probability of movement of individuals between urban and extra-urban zones was very low.

To avoid factors that could alter the consistency of the results such as level of alertness, previous activities, number of human observers, flock size or starting distance (Blumstein et al., 2015), we performed FID measurements considering only single adult individuals (without gender distinction), observed relaxed and not vigilant on the ground at least 30 m from the observer. Throughout the study period, which includes the dry season and the transition to the wet season, herbaceous vegetation cover at ground level is short in both urban and extra-urban zones, which does not compromise the FID measurements. One of us, wearing in all cases the same clothing, approached the bird in a straight line, without interception of vegetation, at a constant speed (0.5–1.0 m/s) taking care that initially the bird was undisturbed by the presence of the observer or other external factors, such as the presence of dogs or sudden sounds. We measured the FID in centimeters when the bird flew, from the observer’s position to the exact starting point of the bird with a tape measure, we did not consider escapes by walking or jumping.

In the city of La Paz and its surroundings another species of the genus Turdus is present, the Great Thrush (T. fuscater), but is less abundant and frequent than the Chiguanco Thrush (Villegas and Garitano-Zavala, 2010). Although both species are very similar, it is possible to distinguish them because the Great Thrush is larger, with a longer tail, a darker blackish color on the back and a brighter eye-ring (Fjeldså and Krabbe, 1990; Collar, 2005). We excluded any individual observed with the combination of these morphological characters.

Neophobia and Problem-Solving Abilities Measurement

Captures and Captivity Conditions

For the experimental tests of neophobia and problem solving, we captured with mist nets a total of 20 adult individuals of Chiguanco Thrush without distinction of gender, ten in two urban places and ten in two extra-urban places. The urban places were two squares within site U2, the first extra-urban place was within site E1, and the other in site E4 (Figure 1, Supplementary Figures 1,2 and Supplementary Table 2). We opened the mist nets in the mornings (07:00–11:00), and in the afternoons (16:00–19:00); one of us, an expert in bird handling, untangled the bird from the net without causing any damage, placed it in a fabric bag and immediately transported it to the captivity facilities on the campus of the Universidad Mayor de San Andrés. The birds were housed in individual metal mesh covered aviaries (4.60 m length × 1.40 m width × 1.7 m height), with natural vegetation and soil.

The individuals were visually but not acoustically isolated from each other, the enclosures were surrounded by outdoors located in a natural area without human disturbance. We cleaned and disinfected the enclosures before and after the entry of a new bird. We kept each individual in captivity for only four days; on the fourth day, we released individuals in the same place where they were caught, with a temporary mark on the leg (nail polish), to avoid recapture. No individual died or showed any obvious signs of weakness or illness after captivity. We carried out the captures and tests before the birds entered the breeding season, between August and November 2016. The General Direction of Biodiversity and Protected Areas of the Bolivian Government (administrative resolution 026/09 and technical report 0214/2016) approved the captures and all the procedures.

We consider that our outdoor aviary would replicate the conditions of the Chiguanco Thrush natural habitat, so the responses would be more similar to those found in nature. However, we gave two days of adaptation before behavioral testing began as suggested by Audet et al. (2016). During these days, we fed the birds ad libitum with birdseed, fresh fruits (apple, grapes and oranges) and bread on a dish, there was always water available in another dish. Because non-urbanized birds may be less likely to eat from feeders (Tryjanowski et al., 2016), we pay particular attention to the feeding of the captured individuals. All the birds after two days’ adaptation fed adequately with no obvious signs of stress. On the third and fourth day, we carried out the behavioral tests in the mornings starting at 09:00. The day before the behavioral tests, we starved the birds overnight from 18:00 to increase their motivation when they faced the tests. After each test, the birds were fed again ad libitum until the next food deprivation.

Running the Tests

We used two different problem boxes, the “drawer test” and the “tunnel test” (Supplementary Figure 3), similar to those used by Audet et al. (2016), made of translucent white plastic. Our drawer test (7 cm height × 7 cm width × 7 cm length) had a hook in one side; the birds had to realize that to access the food they had to pull out the drawer by pulling the hook. Our tunnel test (5 cm height × 5 cm width × 20 cm length), had an opening on one side, inside the tunnel was a wooden stick (25cm) with a hook attached to the end that was outside the tunnel, we placed the food on the other end of the stick, the one that was inside the tunnel. The birds needed to realize that the only way to reach the food was by pulling the stick with their beak using the hook.

For the evaluation of neophobia, we presented the drawer test as a novel object on the third day, in the same place where we normally provided food ad libitum, placing fresh bread inside as a reward (determined as a favorite food based on previous observations). We did this as recommended by Greenberg and Mettke-Hofmann (2001) to exclude neophilic responses since the willingness to feed would compete with the phobia toward the new object. Looking behind the enclosure door through a hole, we measured the bird’s latency (seconds) to approach and peck at the object from the moment the bird entered the enclosure.

After the first approach and pecking, the bird had the opportunity to access the food by pulling the drawer; this was the first problem solving test. We measured from the first peck if each bird was able to solve it or not and the latency time (seconds). On the fourth day, we presented the tunnel test as a second problem; we also measured from the first peck if each bird was able to solve it or not, and the latency time (seconds). We gave a maximum of 20 min to solve each test, after that we considered that the birds were unable to solve it. Additionally, we videotaped the tests with a GoPro HERO camera and then re-analyzed them to make sure the data was collected correctly. A video of an urban individual solving the tunnel test is included in the Supplementary Material.

Data Analysis

The data on FID, neophobia and problem-solving abilities were continuous in nature and normally distributed (Supplementary Table 3). For the evaluation of the FID, we applied a two-factor general lineal model taking as factors the zone (urban and extra-urban) and the time of day (morning and afternoon), appealing to the robustness of the test with respect to heteroscedasticity. Subsequently, we compared between urban and extra-urban zones (independently for mornings and afternoons in the case of FID), differences in deviation using Lavene’s test, and differences between means using Welch’s t-test (because we obtained significant differences in deviations for most of cases). We correlated latency times for neophobia and problem solving for individuals who were able to solve the tests using Pearson’s correlation test. We performed all the statistical analysis and graphs with IBM SPSS Statistics v 23 (IBM Corporation, 2015), considering a significance threshold value of 0.05.

Results

Urban individuals of Chiguanco Thrush showed a significantly shorter distance to initiate flight escape behavior compared to their extra-urban counterparts (F1,99 = 281.377, p < 0.001), with no variation between mornings and afternoons (F1,99 = 0.080, p = 0.778) and without interaction between factors (F1,99 = 0.894, p = 0.347). In the extra-urban zone, there was a significantly greater interindividual variation, but only the upper part of the urban population data range is part of the extra-urban population data range, both for the measurements of the first event carried out in the mornings, and in the second one carried out in the afternoons (Figure 2A, Table 1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Comparison between urban (gray bars) and extra-urban (green bars) individuals of Chiguanco Thrush in the city of Paz for flight initiation distance (FID) (A) and for performance in the neophobia test and two problem-solving tests (drawer test and tunnel test) (B). In (A) the comparison between the morning (a.m.) and the afternoon (p.m.) is included, and in (B) the numbers below the bars show the total number of individuals who were able to solve the tests (out of a total of ten per zone).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the behavioral performance in boldness, neophobia and two problem- solving tests carried out with individuals of Chiguanco Thrush from urban (U) and extra-urban (Eu) zones of the city of La Paz.

All captured urban and extra-urban individuals of Chiguanco Thrush who faced the drawer test as a novel object, came to explore and peck at it, but the urban individuals did so on average approximately 4.5 times faster than their extra-urban counterparts (Table 1). As reported for FID, the extra-urban individuals showed a significantly greater variation among them in neophobia (Figure 2B, Table 1), and the urban population data range falls in the lower part of the extra-urban population data range.

After scanning, not all individuals could solve the drawer test, 80% of urban individuals did and only 50% of extra-urban ones. Interestingly, the extra-urban individuals who solved the drawer test did so in the same latency time and with the same interindividual variation as their urban counterparts (Table 1). The tunnel test was more difficult to solve, only 40% of urban and 10% of extra-urban individuals did, the only extra-urban spent twice the average time of urban ones (Figure 2, Table 1). All individuals who successfully completed the tunnel test solved the drawer test previously, but there was no correlation in latency times (r = −0.302, p = 0.621) (Figure 3A). Less neophobic urban individuals were not necessarily capable or faster in solving the drawer test (r = 0.531; p = 0.175) or the tunnel test (r = 0.044; p = 0.956) (Figures 3B,C), but the five extra-urban individuals who solved the drawer test showed a significant positive correlation between the latency time of the neophobia test and the drawer test (r = 0.914; p = 0.030) (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Relationship between neophobia and performance in problem-solving tests of temporarily captive adult individuals of the Chiguanco Thrush. (A) Drawer test vs. tunnel test, (B) Drawer test vs. neophobia, and (C) Tunnel test vs. neophobia. Black crosses and black dotted fit line for urban individuals, open green circles and green dotted fit line for extra-urban individuals, and solid black fit line for urban and extra-urban individuals together.

Discussion

Urban Chiguanco Thrush individuals responded to urbanization in La Paz with a significantly shorter FID, they were also less neophobic and more innovative, spent significantly less time approaching and exploring a novel object and more individuals solved the problem-solving tests. Although we did not assess learning skills directly, the fact that 50% of urban individuals who solved the simplest test were able to solve the most complex test the next day suggests that they probably have better learning abilities as well.

The decrease of FID was reported as an urbanization response for a broad spectrum of bird species (Evans et al., 2010; Sol et al., 2013; Miranda, 2017; Samia et al., 2017), as well as for all other species of the genus Turdus studied to date on all continents (Cooke, 1980; Wang et al., 2004; Møller, 2010; Carrete and Tella, 2011; Clucas and Marzluff, 2012; Díaz et al., 2013; Møller and Tryjanowski, 2014; Piratelli et al., 2015). There is also relative consistency between studies to show that urban bird populations have better capacities for innovation and problem solving (Møller, 2009; Sol et al., 2013; Griffin et al., 2017; Sayol et al., 2020). Although no other urban-gradient studies on innovation and problem-solving abilities were conducted for other species of the genus Turdus, innovation skills were previously reported. It is the case of novel foraging strategies (reviewed in Lefebvre et al., 1997), the use of novel nesting sites (Batisteli et al., 2022), true use of tools (reviewed in Bentley-Condit and Smith, 2010), faster habituation to experimental situations (Walasz, 1990), and the exploration and consumption of novel foods sources and opportunities (Marples et al., 1998; Lafleur et al., 2007; Møller et al., 2014).

Unlike the behaviors mentioned above, the results on the variations of neophobia and neophilia in the urban-gradient with different species of birds are far from consistent (Griffin et al., 2017; Sol et al., 2020). Precisely, Miranda et al. (2013) reported contradictory to our results that hand-reared Eurasian Blackbirds (Turdus merula) from urban areas were more neophobic and seasonally less neophilic compared to individuals from rural areas. Although it was suggested that the different responses reported in the literature arose in the difficulties to disentangle between neophobic and neophilic measures (Greenberg and Mettke-Hofmann, 2001), is it most probable that neophobic responses depend on the conditions of each city, because they vary widely in predictability, complexity and predation pressures (Griffin et al., 2017).

The less neophobic extra-urban Chiguanco Thrushes were at the same time the most innovative. Batisteli et al. (2022) reported a similar situation for peri-urban females of the Pale-breasted Thrush (Turdus leucomelas), the most innovative capable of nesting in human buildings were also less neophobic compared to tree-nesting females. Curiously, in the urban Chiguanco Thrushes, innovation and neophilia were decoupled; Riyahi et al. (2017) described the same pattern in Great Tit (Parus major) and suggest that it is a sign of the process to better cope with the more variable urban habitat, an interesting subject to explore in the future.

The variance of the FID of the Chiguanco Thrush in the urban core of the city of La Paz was significantly lower, and the range of urban population data overlaps only with the lower part of the range of extra-urban population data, a situation that is also temporarily stable. Following Møller (2010), it is possible to interpret that in La Paz the mean urban FID values represent the lowest subset of escape behaviors previously present in extra-urban populations before the colonization process and that currently, urban individuals are capable of responding even more boldly. Our results on neophobia and problem-solving skills with Chiguanco Thrush also suggest that a specific subset of previous behaviors found in the extra-urban populations colonized the city. Individuals from extra-urban sites showed more inter-individual variation in their motivation to explore novel objects, and the few individuals who were able to solve the simplest test performed it in the same average time as the urban ones.

To our knowledge, no previous study simultaneously explored such a broad spectrum of behavioral responses to urbanization in a single species. Our results with the Chiguanco Thrush in La Paz, consistently support that the behavioral responses to urbanization studied here are not the result of an adaptive process, but rather correspond to preadaptation processes or adaptedness (McDonnell and Hahs, 2015) because the observed responses to urbanization were part of the interindividual variation and the natural mechanisms of extra-urban populations.

Although we do not have evidence of genetic differences between urban and extra-urban populations of the Chiguanco Thrush, there is evidence of this in the Eurasian Blackbird in relation to migratory parameters (Partecke and Gwinner, 2007), neophilic and neophobic behaviors (Miranda et al., 2013), SERT gene polymorphism (Mueller et al., 2013), response to acute stress (Partecke et al., 2006), and blood oxidative response to chronic stress (Costantini et al., 2014). A mechanism that could reconcile the evidence of genetic differences between urban and non-urban populations with preadaptation is “Non-random dispersal” (Edelaar and Bolnick, 2012) or “Immigrant selection” (Partecke, 2014), a sorting process of subsampling of the original genetic pool, which could explain that only non-urban individuals with the appropriate preadapted behaviors would be the ones who could colonize and establish in urban environments. To asses this possibility, future studies with the Chiguanco Thrush could focus on genetic differences between urban and extra-urban populations, intra-individual and inter-individual behavioral variations, as well as the inheritance of these traits.

Studies in the Southern Hemisphere, and particularly the Neotropics where the urbanization process is still in early stages, are very scarce (Evans et al., 2010; Marzluff, 2016; Lepczyk et al., 2017; Miranda, 2017; Blumstein, 2019), offering interesting opportunities to explore the processes that allow the emergence of the behavioral responses to urbanization (e.g., Tryjanowski et al., 2020).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by General Direction of Biodiversity and Protected Areas of the Bolivian Government (administrative resolution 026/09 and technical report 0214/2016).

Author Contributions

ÁG-Z, RC, and GE-H designed the study. RC obtained FID data in 2012 and 2013. GE-H obtained FID data in 2016, built the problem- solving boxes, and recorded latency times from direct observations and videotapes. ÁG-Z performed the statistical analysis. All authors participated in the writing of the manuscript and approved the final version.

Funding

The Instituto de Ecología financed the maintenance of the captive breeding facilities at the University Campus of the Universidad Mayor de San Andrés.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to all persons who helped us obtain the data in the field, especially Luciana Tellería, Stephanie Torrico, and Georgina Enriquez. Jordi Altimiras helped with GE-H’s trip to La Paz. Andrea Salazar helped us with the design of Figure 1. Lucas Leveau, Sergio Castro and four reviewers offered valuable comments on the first version.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fevo.2022.830902/full#supplementary-material

References

Alberti, M., Marzluff, J., and Hunt, V. M. (2017a). Urban driven phenotypic changes: empirical observations and theoretical implications for eco-evolutionary feedback. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160029. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberti, M., Correa, C., Marzluff, J. M., Hendry, A. P., Palkovacs, E. P., Gotanda, K. M., et al. (2017b). Global urban signatures of phenotypic change in animal and plan populations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 8951–8956. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606034114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Audet, J.-N., Ducatez, S., and Lefebvre, L. (2016). The town bird and the country bird: problem solving and immunocompetence vary with urbanization. Behav. Ecol. 27, 637–644. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batisteli, A. F., Pizo, M. A., and Sarmento, H. (2022). Female neophobia predicts the use of buildings as nesting sites in a Neotropical songbird. Anim. Behav. 183, 151–157. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.11.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentley-Condit, V. K., and Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour 147, 185–221. doi: 10.1163/000579509X12512865686555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumstein, D. T. (2019). What chasing birds can teach us about predation risk effects: past insights and future directions. J. Ornithol. 160, 587–592. doi: 10.1007/s10336-019-01634-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumstein, D. T., Samia, D. S. M., Stankowich, T., and Cooper, W. E. Jr. (2015). “Best practice for the study of escape behavior,” in Escaping from Predators: An Integrative View of Escape Decisions, eds W. E. Cooper and D. T. Blumstein (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 407–419. doi: 10.1017/cbo9781107447189.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calegaro-Marques, C., and Amato, S. B. (2014). Urbanization breaks up host-parasite interactions: a case study on parasite community ecology of rufous-bellied thrushes (Turdus rufiventris) along a rural-urban gradient. PLoS One 9:e103144. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrete, M., and Tella, J. L. (2011). Inter-individual variability in fear of humans and relative brain size of the species are related to contemporary urban invasion in birds. PLoS One 6:e18859. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clucas, B., and Marzluff, J. M. (2012). Attitudes and actions toward birds in urban areas: human cultural differences influence bird behavior. Auk 129, 8–16. doi: 10.1525/auk.2011.11121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collar, N. J. (2005). “Family Turdidae (Thrushes),” in Handbook of the Birds of the World. Cuckoo-shrikes to Thrushes, eds J. del Hoyo, A. Elliott, and D. A. Christie (Barcelona: Lynx Edicions), 514–807.

Google Scholar

Cooke, A. S. (1980). Observations on how close certain passerine species will tolerate an approaching human in rural and suburban areas. Biol. Conserv. 18, 85–88. doi: 10.1016/0006-3207(80)90072-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costantini, D., Greives, T. J., Hau, M., and Partecke, J. (2014). Does urban life change blood oxidative status in birds? J. Exp. Biol. 217, 2994–2997. doi: 10.1242/jeb.106450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz, M., Møller, A. P., Flensted-Jensen, E., Grim, T., Ibáñez-Álamo, J. D., Jokimäki, J., et al. (2013). The geography of fear: a latitudinal gradient in anti-predator escape distances of birds across Europe. PLoS One 8:e64634. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edelaar, P., and Bolnick, D. I. (2012). Non-random gene flow: an underappreciated force in evolution and ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 659–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. L., Hatchwell, B. J., Parnell, M., and Gaston, K. J. (2010). A conceptual framework for the colonization of urban areas: the blackbird Turdus merula as a case study. Biol. Rev. 85, 643–667. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00121.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferry, C., Frochot, B., and Leruth, Y. (1981). Territory and home range of the blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla) and some other passerines, assessed and compared by mapping and capture–recapture. Stud. Avian Biol. 6, 119–120.

Google Scholar

Fjeldså, J., and Krabbe, N. (1990). Birds of the High Andes. Svendborg: University of Copenhagen Zoological Museum.

Google Scholar

Greenberg, R., and Mettke-Hofmann, C. (2001). “Ecological aspects of neophobia and neophilia in birds,” in Current Ornithology, eds V. Nolan and Jr C. F. Thompson (Boston, MA: Springer), 119–178. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-1211-0_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffin, A. S., Netto, K., and Peneaux, C. (2017). Neophilia, innovation and learning in an urbanized world: a critical evaluation of mixed findings. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 16, 15–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corporation (2015). IMB SPSS Statistics Version 23.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corporation.

Google Scholar

Johnson, M. T. J., and Munshi-South, J. (2017). Evolution of life in urban environments. Science 358:eaam8327. doi: 10.1126/science.aam8327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafleur, N. E., Rubega, M. E., and Elphick, C. S. (2007). Invasive fruits, novel foods, and choice: an investigation of European Starling and American Robin frugivory. Wilson J. Ornithol. 119, 429–438. doi: 10.1676/05-115.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., and Finkelstein, A. (1997). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Anim. Behav. 53, 549–560. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepczyk, C. A., La Sorte, F. A., Aronson, M. F., Goddard, M. A., MacGregor-Fors, I., Nilon, C. H., et al. (2017). “Global patterns and drivers of urban bird diversity,” in Ecology and Conservation of Birds in Urban Environments, eds E. Murgui and M. Hedblom (Cham: Springer), 13–33. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-43314-1_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leveau, L. M., Leveau, C. M., Villegas, M., Cursach, J. A., and Suazo, C. G. (2017). Bird communities along urbanization gradients: a comparative analysis among three Neotropical cities. Ornitol. Neotrop. 28, 77–87.

Google Scholar

MacGregor-Fors, I. (2011). Misconceptions or misunderstandings? on the standardization of basic terms and definitions in urban ecology. Landsc. Urban Plan. 100, 347–349. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.01.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marples, N. M., Roper, T. J., and Harper, D. G. C. (1998). Responses of wild birds to novel prey: evidence of dietary conservatism. Oikos 83, 161–165. doi: 10.2307/3546557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzluff, J. M. (2016). A decadal review of urban ornithology and a prospectus for the future. Ibis 159, 1–13. doi: 10.1111/ibi.12430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzluff, J. M., Bowman, R., and Donnelly, R. (2001). “A historical perspective on urban bird research: trend, terms, and approaches,” in Avian Ecology and Conservation in an Urbanizing World, eds J. M. Marzluff, R. Bowman, and R. Donnelly (New York, NY: Kluwer Academic Publisher), 20–47. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-1531-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, M. J., and Hahs, A. K. (2015). Adaptation and adaptedness of organisms to urban environments. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 46, 261–280. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-112414-054258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikula, P., Hromada, M., Albrecht, T., and Tryjanowski, P. (2014). Nest site selection and breeding success in three Turdus thrush species coexisting in an urban environment. Acta Ornithol. 49, 83–92. doi: 10.3161/000164514X682913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, A. C. (2017). “Mechanisms of behavioural change in urban animals: the role of microevolution and phenotypic plasticity,” in Ecology and Conservation of Birds in Urban Environments, eds E. Murgui and M. Hedblom (Cham: Springer), 113–132. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-43314-1_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, A. C., Schielzeth, H., Sonntag, T., and Partecke, J. (2013). Urbanization and its effects on personality traits: a result of microevolution or phenotypic plasticity? Glob. Chang. Biol. 19, 2634–2644. doi: 10.1111/gcb.12258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P. (2009). Successful city dwellers: a comparative study of the ecological characteristics of urban birds in the Western Palearctic. Oecologia 159, 849–858. doi: 10.1007/s00442-008-1259-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P. (2010). Interspecific variation in fear responses predicts urbanization in birds. Behav. Ecol. 21, 365–371. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P., Jokimäki, J., Skorka, P., and Tryjanowski, P. (2014). Loss of migration and urbanization in birds: a case study of the blackbird (Turdus merula). Oecologia 175, 1019–1027. doi: 10.1007/s00442-014-2953-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P., and Liang, W. (2012). Tropical birds take small risks. Behav. Ecol. 24, 267–272. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ars163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P., and Tryjanowski, P. (2014). Direction of approach by predators and flight initiation distance of urban and rural populations of birds. Behav. Ecol. 25, 960–966. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moraes, A. L., Da Silveira, N. S., and Pizo, M. A. (2018). Nocturnal roosting behavior of the Pale-breasted Thrush (Turdus leucomelas) and its relation with daytime area of use. Wilson J. Ornithol. 130, 828–833. doi: 10.1676/17-106.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, J. C., Partecke, J., Hatchwell, B. J., Gaston, K. J., and Evans, K. L. (2013). Candidate gene polymorphisms for behavioural adaptations during urbanization in blackbirds. Mol. Ecol. 22, 3629–3637. doi: 10.1111/mec.12288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partecke, J. (2014). “Mechanisms of phenotypic responses following colonization of urban areas: from plastic to genetic adaptation,” in Avian Urban Ecology: Behavioural and Physiological Adaptations, eds D. Gil and H. Brumm (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 131–142. doi: 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199661572.003.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partecke, J., and Gwinner, E. (2007). Increased sedentariness in European blackbirds following urbanization: a consequence of local adaptation? Ecology 88, 882–890. doi: 10.1890/06-1105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partecke, J., Schwabl, I., and Gwinner, E. (2006). Stress and the city: urbanization and its effects on the stress physiology in European blackbirds. Ecology 87, 1945–1952. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2006)87[1945:satcua]2.0.co;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piratelli, A. J., Favoretto, G. R., and de Almeida Maximiano, M. F. (2015). Factors affecting escape distance in birds. Zoologia 32, 438–444. doi: 10.1590/S1984-46702015000600002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riyahi, S., Björklund, M., Mateos-Gonzalez, F., and Senar, J. C. (2017). Personality and urbanization: behavioural traits and DRD4 SNP830 polymorphisms in great tits in Barcelona city. J. Ethol. 35, 101–108. doi: 10.1007/s10164-016-0496-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samia, D. S. M., Blumstein, D. T., Díaz, M., Grim, T., Ibáñez-Álamo, J. D., Jokimäki, J., et al. (2017). Rural-urban differences in escape behavior of European birds across a latitudinal gradient. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:66. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayol, F., Sol, D., and Pigot, A. L. (2020). Brain size and life history interact to predict urban tolerance in birds. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8:58. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2020.00058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snell-Rood, E. C. (2013). An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity. Anim. Behav. 85, 1004–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sol, D., Lapiedra, O., and Ducatez, S. (2020). “Cognition and adaptation to urban environments,” in Urban Evolutionary Biology, eds M. Szulkin, J. Munshi-South, and A. Charmantier (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 253–267. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780198836841.003.0016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sol, D., Lapiedra, O., and Gonález-Lagos, C. (2013). Behavioural flexibility for a life in the city. Anim. Behav. 85, 1101–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tryjanowski, P., Kosicki, J. Z., Hromada, M., and Mikula, P. (2020). The emergence of tolerance of human disturbance in Neotropical birds. J. Trop. Ecol. 36, 1–5. doi: 10.1017/S0266467419000282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tryjanowski, P., Møller, A. P., Morelli, F., Biaduń, W., Brauze, T., Ciach, M., et al. (2016). Urbanization affects neophilia and risk-taking at bird-feeders. Sci. Rep. 6:28575. doi: 10.1038/srep28575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations (2020). Demographic Yearbook, 70th issue, 2019. New York, NY: Department of Economic and Social Affairs.

Google Scholar

Villegas, M., and Garitano-Zavala, A. (2010). Bird community responses to different urban conditions in La Paz. Urban Ecosyst. 13, 375–391. doi: 10.1007/s11252-010-0126-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walasz, K. (1990). Experimental investigations on the behavioural differences between urban and forest blackbirds. Acta Zool. Cracov. 33, 235–271.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y. P., Chen, S. H., and Ding, P. (2004). Flush distance: bird tolerance to human intrusion in Hangzhou. Zool. Res. 25, 214–220.

Google Scholar

Keywords: urban ecology, evolution, Turdus, FID, neophobia, problem-solving

Citation: Garitano-Zavala Á, Calbimonte R and Esteve-Herraiz G (2022) The Behavioral Responses of the Chiguanco Thrush to Urbanization in a Neotropical City Comes From Preadapted Behavioral Traits. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10:830902. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.830902

Received: 07 December 2021; Accepted: 02 February 2022;
Published: 28 February 2022.

Edited by:

Rosane Garcia Collevatti, Universidade Federal de Goiás, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Piotr Tryjanowski, Poznań University of Life Sciences, Poland
João Carlos Pena, São Paulo State University, Brazil
Stephen Harris, SUNY Purchase, United States
Erison Carlos dos Santos Monteiro, Instituto Nacional de Pesquisas Espaciais (INPE), Brazil

Copyright © 2022 Garitano-Zavala, Calbimonte and Esteve-Herraiz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Álvaro Garitano-Zavala, agaritanozavala@umsa.bo

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.