Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Endocrinol., 04 March 2024
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Current Advances in Hormone Replacement Therapy: From Basic to Clinical Research View all 4 articles

Systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of menopause hormone therapy on cognition

Caroline AndyCaroline Andy1Matilde NerattiniMatilde Nerattini2Steven JettSteven Jett2Caroline CarltonCaroline Carlton2Camila ZarateCamila Zarate2Camila BoneuCamila Boneu2Francesca FauciFrancesca Fauci2Trisha AjilaTrisha Ajila2Michael BattistaMichael Battista2Silky Pahlajani,Silky Pahlajani2,3Paul ChristosPaul Christos1Matthew E. FinkMatthew E. Fink2Schantel WilliamsSchantel Williams2Roberta Diaz BrintonRoberta Diaz Brinton4Lisa Mosconi,*Lisa Mosconi2,3*
  • 1Department of Population Health Sciences, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Neurology, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Radiology, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 4Department of Neurology and Pharmacology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States

Introduction: Despite evidence from preclinical studies suggesting estrogen’s neuroprotective effects, the use of menopausal hormone therapy (MHT) to support cognitive function remains controversial.

Methods: We used random-effect meta-analysis and multi-level meta-regression to derive pooled standardized mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (C.I.) from 34 randomized controlled trials, including 14,914 treated and 12,679 placebo participants.

Results: Associations between MHT and cognitive function in some domains and tests of interest varied by formulation and treatment timing. While MHT had no overall effects on cognitive domain scores, treatment for surgical menopause, mostly estrogen-only therapy, improved global cognition (SMD=1.575, 95% CI 0.228, 2.921; P=0.043) compared to placebo. When initiated specifically in midlife or close to menopause onset, estrogen therapy was associated with improved verbal memory (SMD=0.394, 95% CI 0.014, 0.774; P=0.046), while late-life initiation had no effects. Overall, estrogen-progestogen therapy for spontaneous menopause was associated with a decline in Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) scores as compared to placebo, with most studies administering treatment in a late-life population (SMD=-1.853, 95% CI -2.974, -0.733; P = 0.030). In analysis of timing of initiation, estrogen-progestogen therapy had no significant effects in midlife but was associated with improved verbal memory in late-life (P = 0.049). Duration of treatment >1 year was associated with worsening in visual memory as compared to shorter duration. Analysis of individual cognitive tests yielded more variable results of positive and negative effects associated with MHT.

Discussion: These findings suggest time-dependent effects of MHT on certain aspects of cognition, with variations based on formulation and timing of initiation, underscoring the need for further research with larger samples and more homogeneous study designs.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia in Western societies, with twice as many women suffering from the disorder as men (1). Postmenopausal women account for over 60% of AD patients (2, 3), a gender disparity that is not solely explained by longevity factors (49). Therefore, development of gender-specific AD preventative strategies is vital to combat the impending epidemic.

The discovery that the pathological process of AD may begin decades before symptom onset indicates a substantial window of time during which we may be able to intervene to halt or delay disease progression (10). This prodromal phase may start as early as midlife, thus coinciding with the menopause transition in women (1116). There is strong biological plausibility linking menopause with AD, as estrogen, particularly 17β-estradiol, is known to have neurotrophic and neuroprotective effects (1721), and modulatory effects on metabolic and biochemical pathways implicated in AD (9, 19, 2225). The onset of menopause is characterized by decreased production and activity of estradiol in both the body and the brain (18), with the subsequent establishment of a hypoestrogenic state, which has been proposed as a female-specific risk factor for AD (19, 22, 2426).

Although primarily viewed as a reproductive event, the menopause transition (perimenopause) is a neuroendocrine event associated with several neurological symptoms stemming from disruptions in estrogen-regulated thermoregulation, circadian rhythms and sensory processing, mood and cognitive functions (19). Many of these changes are risk factors for AD in turn (9, 19, 27). In particular, the fact that over 60% of menopausal women report memory changes (2830) is concerning as memory declines, especially in verbal memory, are among the earliest neuropsychological indicators of AD (3134).

Several studies sought to examine where estrogen supplementation by means of menopause hormone therapy (MHT) is protective against cognitive decline and AD. This hypothesis arose in large part from observational studies and small-scale clinical trials showing that MHT users had better cognitive function, especially verbal memory (35, 36), as well as a lower risk of AD or dementia than non-users (3747). However, larger randomized controlled trials (RCTs), such as the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS), reported an increased risk of all-cause dementia with estrogen-plus-progestogen therapy (EPT), and a non-significant risk increase with estrogen-only therapy (ET) for postmenopausal women ages 60 and older (48, 49). Additionally, EPT worsened cognition among older women, whereas ET had neutral effects (50).

To reconcile this discrepancy, it has been hypothesized that there may be a critical perimenopausal and early postmenopausal window or period during which estrogen exerts neuroprotection (5155), within the context of a “healthy cell bias” of estrogen action (22). However, results from RCTs of younger menopausal women have also reported contrasting findings. Large-scale trials, such as the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study of younger women (WHIMS-Y), the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Cognitive and Affective Ancillary Study (KEEPS-cog), and the Early versus Late Intervention Trial with Estradiol-Cognitive Endpoints (ELITE-cog), predominantly indicated neutral effects of MHT on cognitive function (5659). On the other hand, several smaller studies reported positive effects of MHT on memory function (6066) and, to some extent, verbal fluency (67).

As most studies included different populations, treatment types, and methodologies, meta-analytical examination of existing literature is warranted to systematically consolidate data from multiple sources. Herein, we conducted an updated systematic review and meta-analysis of data linking MHT to cognitive performance, encompassing findings from 34 randomized, placebo-controlled trials including 14,914 treated and 12,679 placebo participants. We examined cognitive outcomes as continuous measures; employed multi-level meta-regression analysis to examine sources of heterogeneity; and considered from the outset the impact of variables such as formulation, timing of initiation, type of menopause, and treatment duration.

Methods

Search criteria

In compliance with the Systematic Reviewers and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (68), we carried out a systematic literature search in PubMed/MEDLINE, Web of Science, and Cochrane databases from 1975 through September 2023. Key words included [‘hormone replacement therapy’, ‘estrogen therapy’, ‘estrogen replacement therapy’, ‘postmenopausal hormone therapy’] and [‘cognition’, ‘cognitive performance’, ‘memory’, ‘dementia’, ‘Alzheimer’s disease’] and [‘randomized controlled trial’, ‘clinical trial’]. This search was supplemented by a manual search of bibliographies from selected articles, reviews, and previous meta-analyses. Screening of studies was conducted by three independent authors (MN, SJ, LM). Any discrepancies were resolved by the senior author (LM).

Inclusion criteria

We selected only publications in the English language which met the following inclusion criteria: 1) the study was published in a peer-reviewed journal; 2) the cohort was well defined; 3) participants included medically healthy women (no comorbidities such as cardiovascular disease, stroke, or dementia) or patients with a hysterectomy/oophorectomy; 4) outcomes included measures of cognitive function; 5) studies were randomized placebo-controlled trials with at least 2 weeks duration (clinical trials without a placebo group, case reports, review papers, editorials, letters to the editor, personal communications, preclinical studies and in vitro research were excluded); 6) treatment was systemic estrogen with or without progestogen (studies of vaginal estradiol, tibolone, progesterone/progestin without estrogen, testosterone, and mixed preparations were excluded); 7) the outcomes were MHT vs. placebo; 8) included studies reported at least one estimate of association and one corresponding measure of statistical uncertainty such as P value, standard error, confidence interval, or data required for derivation of these estimates.

Data extraction

For each study, we extracted the following information: year of publication, country, study design, number of participants, participants’ ages, cognitive endpoints, treatment characteristics (e.g., timing of use, duration of use, route of administration, formulation, dosage), covariates, and summary estimates, e.g. mean or median at baseline and follow-up or change from baseline, with associated standard error, standard deviation or 95% confidence intervals (C.I.). The fully adjusted models were primarily used for analysis.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R 4.2.2 statistical software (R Core Team). To fully capitalize on the available data from the included studies, we conducted a quantitative analysis of both cognitive domains and individual cognitive tests. In sensitivity analysis, we additionally grouped study findings on the basis of selected characteristics included in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Randomized controlled clinical trials investigating effects of menopausal hormone therapy on cognition.

Meta-analysis with Robust Variance Estimation

Only RCTs that reported mean outcome scores at baseline and follow-up, raw or standardized mean change, and/or standardized mean difference with corresponding standard error, standard deviation, or 95% C.I, were included in analysis. Meta-analysis was performed for cognitive domains and individual tests available across at least four studies reporting comparable outcomes and exposure groupings (91, 92). As several studies include multiple effect estimates for different exposure types, as well as multiple outcome measures, we applied Robust Variance Estimation (RVE) to compute the pooled effect size (93). This method accounts for intra-study dependent effect sizes while mitigating the impact of outliers, unequal variances, and other sources of heterogeneity (93). We evaluated the presence of heterogeneity using Cochran’s Q, I2 and tau2 statistics (94). Because different study designs and outcome measures were used across studies, we interpreted data using random-effect models, which use a weighting scheme that incorporates study sample size and within- and between-study variance to account for study heterogeneity (9597).

Study-specific estimates were used to calculate pooled estimated effect sizes (standardized mean difference [SMD]) with 95% C.I. Results were considered significant at P < 0.05. When interpreting SMDs, the magnitude is a measure of effect size: a SMD of approximately 0.2 represents a small effect size, e.g. a small difference between the groups. A SMD of approximately 0.5 represents a medium effect size, e.g. a moderate difference between the groups. A SMD of 0.8 or greater indicates a large effect size, e.g. a substantial difference between the groups.

Our primary analysis focused on cognitive categories or domains, defined as different tests measuring the same cognitive ability which are first standardized and then analyzed as a group. As done in previous Cochrane and other meta-analyses (36, 98101), we grouped together assessments evaluating the same type of cognitive function into separate domains: verbal memory [e.g., paragraph recall/logical memory, paired associates, California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)]; working memory [e.g., digit symbol substitution (DSST), digit span forward and backward], visuospatial memory [e.g. Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT), Rey Visual Learning Test (RVLT)], verbal fluency (e.g., COWAT, animal naming, FAS), executive function (e.g. Stroop, Trail Making Test, TMT-B], visuospatial abilities [e.g. mental rotation, card sorting test], and global cognition [e.g., Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE), Montreal cognitive Assessment (MoCA), Cambridge Cognitive Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly (CAMCOG)]. The full list of tests included in each domain is found in Supplementary Table 1. For tests exhibiting an inverse association with performance (e.g. Stroop, TMT-B), the sign of the calculated standardized mean difference was inverted before grouping. As different studies categorized certain tests differently, we based our grouping on generally accepted criteria and insights from previous meta-analyses and Cochrane reviews. The process of grouping tests into domains was a collaborative effort involving three authors (MN, SJ, CC).

Secondarily, we tested whether specific cognitive tests are more sensitive to MHT effects than others, with a focus on those known to have estrogen-sensitive effects (79, 81, 102). Our primary outcomes of interest were as follows: short- and long-delayed recall assessed using the CVLT; immediate and delayed recall of paired associates; FAS; TMT-B; digit span forward and backward. We also examined MMSE and digit span total as enough studies reported these estimates.

We then conducted a sensitivity analysis testing for effects of MHT formulation (ET, EPT), initiation timing (midlife or age ¾65 years, late-life or age >65 years), menopause type (surgical, spontaneous), and treatment duration (¾1 year, >1 year). Analyses were repeated after excluding data from post-intervention studies (50, 59), as only ~4% of participants reported using hormone therapy after the trials termination (103).

Multi-level meta-regression

To further examine and account for heterogeneity in our dataset, we employed a multi-level meta-regression analysis incorporating potential modifier variables using the ‘metafor’ package in R4.2.2. This allowed us to obtain estimates for the effect of each modifier on the pooled estimate and thus identify drivers of the observed heterogeneity. The multi-level approach allowed inclusion of multiple estimates per study, incorporating hierarchical structuring to account for induced correlations of estimates that may arise from any given study and for within- and between-study dependencies across those parameters. An unstructured variance-covariance matrix was used for random effects variances (104). Covariates included in the model were MHT formulation (ET, EPT), initiation timing (midlife, late-life), menopause type (surgical, spontaneous), treatment duration (¾1 year, >1 year), and study size (<100, 100-500, >500). Changes to effect sizes due to confounders were assessed at P < 0.05.

Examination of publication bias

For analyses including 10 or more studies, possible publication bias was evaluated using Egger’s tests and funnel plots (105). For subgroups with significant publication bias, the Duval and Tweedie Trim and Fill method was applied which imputes the effects of missing studies using a random-effect modeling framework, and then re-computes the pooled effect size (106).

Results

Literature search and characteristics of included studies

Our PRISMA flow diagram is shown in Figure 1. Our systematic search initially identified 5,502 papers, of which 3,320 were found to be duplicates. From the remaining studies, 1,987 papers failed to meet the inclusion criteria and were excluded during title and/or abstract screening. The remaining 204 articles were selected for full-text inspection. Among these, a total of 34 eligible studies were pooled together for analysis.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 PRIMSA Flow chart.

Demographics

Key characteristics of pooled studies are found in Table 1. Thirty-four studies including quantitative data were examined for associations between MHT and cognition in this meta-analysis. The final dataset included a total of 14,914 treated and 12,679 placebo participants. However, the participant distribution in these studies exhibited significant variability, with an average of 719 participants per study, but a median of only 65.

The duration of the studies also showed a wide range. On average, the studies spanned a period of 24 months, yet the median was notably shorter at 6 months. Duration of treatment was ¾1 year in 58.8% of studies, while the remaining 41.2% extended beyond 1 year.

Regarding the menopausal status of the participants, 29.4% of the studies reported data on women who had undergone menopause spontaneously, and 20.6% focused on women who had undergone hysterectomies. The remaining studies included mixed groups. Additionally, 35.3% of studies included women who were ¾6 years post-menopause, another 44.1% included women who were >6 years post-menopause, 17.6% included mixed groups, and 2.9% did not report this information.

About a third of the studies (35.3.5%) reported on MHT as their primary outcome. Of those which reported on specific formulations, 44.1% utilized ET as the only therapy, and 20.6% utilized EPT. The mode of administration was predominantly oral (73.5%), followed by transdermal applications in 20.6% of the cases, while 2.9% used either oral or transdermal methods, and 2.9% employed intramuscular bolus injections. In terms of the specific hormones used, estradiol was the primary hormone used in 55.9% of studies. Conjugated equine estrogen (CEE) was the primary hormone in 41.2% of the studies). Additionally, 2.9% of studies used either estradiol (E2) or CEE. Among estradiol studies, 35.0% were in combination with a progestogen, chiefly micronized progesterone, as well as norethindrone/norethisterone, drospirenone, and dienogest. Among CEE studies, 73.3% were in combination with medroxyprogesterone acetate (MPA).

Almost half (47.1%) of the studies did not provide information on presence of menopausal symptoms. Among those that reported these data, menopausal symptoms were absent in 1 study and present, at least for some women, in the remaining 17 studies (50.0%). However, the average frequency of menopausal symptoms was 32.5%, with a median of only 16%.

By location, the majority of RCTs were conducted in America (58.8%), followed by Northern Europe (18%), Canada (5.9%), Brazil (5.8%), Iran (2.9%) and Australia (2.9%) (Table 1).

Primary analysis: meta-analysis of cognitive domains

Results are summarized in Table 2. Due to presence of heterogeneity across most cognitive domains (P ¾ 0.031), we interpreted the random effect model estimates. Pooled estimates from random-effects meta-analysis indicated no significant effects of MHT on cognitive domain scores compared to placebo (P ≤ 0.130; Supplementary Figures 1-8). Excluding the two post-intervention studies left results largely unchanged (Table 2). Possible sources of heterogeneity were investigated in the sensitivity analysis and meta-regression analysis, below.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Effects of MHT on cognitive domain scores.

Effects of treatment duration

Results are found in Table 3. Pooled estimates from random effects meta-analysis indicated no changes in cognitive domain scores with shorter duration of MHT treatment, and a worsening in visual memory with longer treatment duration (SMD = -1.577, 95% CI -2.261, -0.892; P = 0.022).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Effects of MHT on cognitive domains by duration of treatment.

Effects of timing

Results are found in Table 4. Pooled estimates from random effects meta-analysis indicated no significant effects of MHT during midlife. In contrast, MHT in late-life was associated with mild reductions in global cognition (SMD = -0.071, 95% CI -0.077, -0.065; P = 0.004).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Effects of MHT on cognitive domains by initiation timing.

Effects of menopause type and formulation

Results are found in Table 5. MHT for surgical menopause, mainly ET, was associated with moderate improvements in global cognition (SMD = 1.575, 95% CI 0.228, 2.921; P = 0.043). MHT for spontaneous menopause had no significant effects on cognition.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Effects of MHT on cognitive domains by menopause type and formulation.

Effects of formulation and timing

There were a sufficient number of studies to perform exploratory meta-analyses of verbal memory (overall and delayed recall) and visual memory domains based on type of treatment and treatment initiation. Results are summarized in Table 6, Figure 2. In midlife, we observed specific effects related to treatment type: ET was associated with mild improvements in verbal memory (SMD = 0.394, 95% CI 0.014, 0.774; P = 0.046), whereas EPT did not exhibit significant associations with verbal or visual memory. In late-life, effects varied: ET did not show significant effects on verbal memory or visual memory, while EPT was associated with improvements in verbal memory (P = 0.049) and no changes in visual memory.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6 Effects of MHT on cognitive domains by formulation and initiation timing.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 MHT effects on cognitive domains cores by initiation timing and formulation. Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials investigating effects of systemic MHT on verbal memory, delayed verbal memory, visual memory and working memory domains. Values are standardized mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (C.I.). ET, estrogen-only therapy; EPT, estrogen-progestogen therapy.

Secondary analysis: meta-analysis of individual cognitive tests

Next, we conducted a meta-analysis of individual cognitive tests based on data from the above RCTs. Results are summarized in Figures 35, Table 7. Pooled estimates from random-effects meta-analysis indicated a small negative effect of MHT on TMT-B compared to placebo (SMD = -0.034, 95% CI -0.041, -0.027; P = 0.010). As in the above analysis, excluding the two post-intervention studies had minimal impact on the results (Table 7).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 MHT effects on Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE), Trail Making Test-B, and FAS tests. Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials investigating effects of systemic MHT on MMSE, TMT-B and FAS tests. Forest plots display individual and pooled estimates expressed as standardized mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (C.I.). Studies are ordered by year of publication.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 MHT effects on verbal memory tests. Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials investigating effects of systemic MHT on paired associates immediate and delayed recall from the Wechsler memory scale, and California Verbal Learning Test (SVLT). Forest plots display individual and pooled estimates expressed as standardized mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (C.I.). Studies are ordered by year of publication.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 MHT effects on working memory tests. Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials investigating effects of systemic MHT on digit span forward, backward, and total. Forest plots display individual and pooled estimates expressed as standardized mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (C.I.). Studies are ordered by year of publication.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7 Effects of MHT on individual cognitive tests.

Effects of treatment duration

Pooled estimates from random effects meta-analysis indicated no changes in test scores with shorter duration of MHT treatment, and a mild worsening in TMT-B with longer treatment duration (SMD = -0.018, 95% CI -0.022, -0.014; P = 0.012) (Supplementary Table 2).

Effects of timing

This analysis includes estimates for studies in which treatment initiation was specified to occur in midlife or late-life (Supplementary Table 3). Pooled estimates from random effects meta-analysis indicated no significant differences between MHT and placebo in midlife. In contrast, MHT use in late-life was associated with small reductions in TMT-B scores (SMD = -0.050, 95% CI -0.052, -0.048; P = 0.002).

Effects of menopause type and formulation

Results are shown in Supplementary Table 4. ET for surgical menopause was associated with a moderate to large improvement in Digit Span forward (SMD = 0.747, 95% CI 0.603, 0.890; P = 0.009) but small declines in CVLT free recall (SMD = -1.142, 95% CI -1.249, -1.036; P = 0.005) compared to placebo. EPT for spontaneous menopause was associated with significant declines in MMSE compared to placebo (SMD = -1.853, 95% CI -2.974, -0.733; P = 0.030).

Effects of formulation and initiation timing

Results are reported in Supplementary Table 5. Overall, few studies offered stratification based on formulation and timing of treatment to perform a meta-analysis based on these parameters. Herein, we descriptively report results based on available data. There weren’t enough studies of midlife EPT to perform a meta-analysis. In late-life, ET was associated with improved digit span forward compared to placebo (P = 0.008) and had no effects on digit span backward and FAS, whereas EPT had no effects on CVLT free recall.

Meta-regression analysis

As shown in Supplementary Table 6, none of the covariates had significant moderating effects on the association between MHT and cognitive performance. This suggests that none of the hypothesized confounders were the primary drivers of heterogeneity in the associations of MHT with cognition. To understand whether MHT effects on cognition were concentrated in particular cognition domains or specific tests, meta-regression models containing domain and test indicators were constructed. Model results found the executive function domain was associated with worsening in SMD as compared to the referent category of global cognition (P = 0.051). In analysis of individual tests, none of the tests were associated with significant changes in SMD as compared to the referent category of MMSE.

Publication bias

No significant publication bias was observed in analysis of cognitive domains for the entire cohort or in subgroup analyses. For individual tests, only analyses of digit span forward and backward included enough studies to allow testing for publication bias. As shown in Table 7, no significant publication bias was observed for those tests. None of the subgroup analyses included a sufficient number of studies to evaluate presence of publication bias.

Discussion

The present meta-analysis, examining data from 34 randomized, placebo-controlled trials encompassing 14,914 treated and 12,679 placebo participants, shows differential associations between MHT and cognitive function in some domains and tests of interest depending on formulation and timing of treatment. Overall, MHT had no significant effects on cognitive domain scores. Nonetheless, in sub-group analyses, treatment for surgical menopause, mostly using ET, improved global cognition compared to placebo. Additionally, ET initiated specifically in midlife or before age 65 was associated with improved verbal memory, while no effects were observed for ET initiated in late-life or after age 65. On the other hand, EPT was associated with MMSE declines compared to placebo, mostly with late-life treatment. However, while EPT had no significant effects in midlife, it was associated with improved verbal memory in late-life. Treatment duration >1 year was associated with worsening in visual memory as compared to shorter duration. Analysis of individual tests indicated more complex patterns of positive and negative effects of MHT with variations based on factors such as formulation and initiation timing.

Comparison with previous meta-analyses

To date, eight meta-analyses of MHT effects on cognition have been published (35, 36, 98101, 107, 108). Several were published before or relatively soon after the end of the WHI (35, 36, 98100). Additionally, the majority of studies evaluated cognitive outcomes as categorical measures (e.g., improvement vs. decline vs. no change) (35, 36, 98, 99, 101), while only three studies conducted meta-analyses involving continuous measures (100, 107, 108). Among the two most recent meta-analyses, (107) examined 23 RCTs and reported a small negative effect of MHT on global cognition among postmenopausal women over 60, with no effects on memory or attention. (108) analyzed 10 RCTs, finding no significance differences in memory function between MHT and placebo, except for a negative effect on the digit span forward test among women >5 years postmenopause. No effects were observed among women ¾5 years postmenopause (108). To note, in both studies, subgroup analyses of early menopause included a maximum of 4 studies, which limits statistical power and generalizability of these findings. Possibly due to the relatively small number of studies available for analysis, neither study reported on differential effects of MHT formulation.

The present meta-analysis includes reports up to the year 2023, and takes into account known sources of heterogeneity such as formulation, timing of treatment and duration from the outset. This enabled us to integrate data across various types of studies with a large enough sample of >4 studies per subgroup. A main finding in our meta-analysis was that ET for surgical menopause had moderate positive effects on global cognition as compared to placebo, independent of initiation timing (SMD = 1.575, 95% CI 0.228, 2.921; P = 0.043). Additionally, when initiated specifically in midlife, ET was associated with improved verbal memory (SMD=0.394, 95% CI 0.014, 0.774; P=0.046). Cohort studies have reported an almost doubled long-term risk of dementia for women undergoing surgical menopause (11, 109111), which was highest following bilateral oophorectomy and lower but significant with unilateral oophorectomy and partial hysterectomy (11, 35, 36, 98, 109, 110, 112). However, undergoing oophorectomy after menopause did show an increased risk of AD (113), providing supporting for the window of opportunity hypothesis. Notably, MHT initiated within 5 years post-surgery and continued for at least 10 years was also linked with less global cognitive decline (11).

Literature Review and Interpretation Relative to the ‘Window of Opportunity’ Framework

The first RCT investigating the effects of MHT on cognitive function was the WHIMS, which evaluated CEEs and CEEs plus MPA in postmenopausal women aged 65 years or older as compared to placebo (48, 49). Contrary to the study hypotheses, dementia risk was near doubled for women in the CEE/MPA group [HR: 2.05; 95%CI: 1.21-3.48] (48), and increased by about 50% for those in the CEE only group [HR: 1.49; 95%CI: 0.83-2.66] (49). The absolute risk increase was however small, with about 12 additional cases of dementia for 1,000 women using CEE/MPA for five years, and 6 additional cases per 1,000 women using CEE for five years. Additionally, CEE therapy, alone or with MPA, was associated with mild reductions in global cognition during the first 3-4 years of follow-ups and several years later (72, 73).

The ancillary WHI Study of Cognitive Aging (WHISCA) examined whether these formulations influenced specific cognitive domains over a total of 6 years on average. A limitation of this study is that the trial addressed change in cognitive performance from an on-treatment baseline. In a sample of 2,304 postmenopausal women aged 65 and older, small changes in cognitive test scores were found (79, 84), where the CEE/MPA treated group exhibited trends towards declines in verbal learning as compared to placebo, but improved performance on figural memory testing (79), whereas no differences were observed between CEE-only and matching placebo groups (84). Additionally, two smaller trials reported no effects on cognition using oral CEEs and MPA (70) or transdermal estradiol and micronized progesterone (82).

Together, these findings suggested that CEE/MPA treatment in late-life produced an initial decrement in at least some aspects of cognitive function during the first few years of use, whereas unopposed therapy did not have positive or negative effects. Several RCTs confirmed lack of negative effects of estrogen-only therapy on memory and other cognitive outcomes in older hysterectomized women using oral estradiol, low dose transdermal estradiol (0.25 mg/day) and ultra low-dose transdermal estradiol (0.014 mg/day) (69, 76, 78, 80).

It’s important to note that RCT results from older postmenopausal women are not necessarily applicable to younger women for whom MHT is intended for. In fact, MHT appears to have generally neutral or positive effects on cognitive function in younger women, particularly improved verbal memory among surgical menopausal cases. When the effect of CEE/MPA and CEE-alone on cognition were evaluated in postmenopausal women aged 50 to 55 years, there were no differences in verbal memory, working memory, verbal fluency, and executive functions between either treated group and placebo (56). Follow-up examinations also indicated no significant long-term effects on cognitive function among younger women who had been taking hormones for up to 6 years (59). On the other hand, 65 to 79 year-old women who had been using MHT for five years or longer exhibited a decline in global cognition, executive function, and working memory, which persisted for over ten years (59). In the Cognitive Complaints in Early Menopause Trial (COGENT), with a sample size of 180 women aged 45–55 years of age, there were no significant effects of CEE/MPA on memory performance after 4 months of treatment, although trends were noted toward declines in short and long-delay verbal memory compared to placebo (81). Similar results were found in a trial of women with moderate-to-severe hot flashes study who were randomized to 1 year treatment with CEE/MPA, black cohosh, or red clover vs. placebo (67). The CEE/MPA group showed some improvement in verbal fluency test but worsening in some memory tests compared to placebo (67).

Additional studies provided similar results with different MHT formulations. Among women aged 42–58 years, who were within 3 years of their final menstrual period, 4-year treatment with oral CEE (0.45 mg/day) or transdermal estradiol (50 microg weekly) with or without cyclic micronized progesterone (200 mg for 12 days each month) showed no changes compared to placebo (57). Likewise, after an average of 57 months of treatment, oral estradiol with or without micronized progesterone administered to postmenopausal women divided into early (mean age of 56 years) and late postmenopausal (mean age of 65 years), resulted in no effects on verbal memory, executive function, or global cognition compared to placebo (58).

Several smaller studies have also been conducted, showing either neutral or protective effects of MHT in younger surgically menopausal women (114116, 60, 65). Studies examining the cognitive effects of opposed therapy in younger women with a uterus are scarcer and report contrasting results. For example, EPT was associated with improvement in some memory tests but worsening on some executive function tests in one study (86) and with neutral effects in another study (66). One RCT that directly compared estradiol valerate to estradiol valerate plus dienogest and placebo showed a benefit of opposed therapy on verbal memory compared with both placebo and estradiol (71). This suggests that different forms of progestogen may have different effects on cognition, with dienogest exhibiting more positive or neutral effects than MPA.

The notion that estrogen therapy administered to midlife women may be beneficial to some aspects of cognition was supported by observational evidence of positive effects of midlife MHT use on cognition and AD risk (3546). For instance, in the prospective Cache County study, use of hormone therapy during the menopausal window was protective against AD, whereas use after age 61 was associated with 2.1-fold increased risk (41). Additionally, women who initiated MHT within five years of menopause exhibited better cognitive performance than those who initiated treatment alter on (117). Similar results were found in the Kaiser Permanente study, where women who used MHT in midlife had a 26% reduced risk of dementia compared to never-users, while those who used MHT in late-life experienced a 48% higher risk of dementia (43). While observational studies are subject to several limitations, the majority show that estrogen may help maintain some aspects of cognition (36, 118122), particularly verbal memory (52, 98, 123), when initiated early in the course of menopause.

Limitations and implications for future research

A limitation of this meta-analysis was the relatively small number of studies included in each subgroup analysis. Out of the 52 RCTs initially identified, only 34 provided continuous estimates suitable for meta-analysis. Additionally, lack of standardization and use of several different cognitive tests across studies limited data integration. To address this issue, we focused our primary analyses on cognitive domains rather than individual tests, thus maximizing available data. It is important to clarify that we use the term ‘domain’ to refer to groupings of specific cognitive tests assessing the same cognitive function. We are thus not creating novel cognitive constructs but rather examining standardized data from different cognitive tests within a unified category, so as to streamline analysis by considering related tests collectively. Our aim was to provide a broader perspective on cognitive function, which could have mitigated the impact of specific test-level variations, while enhancing statistical power. This approach, which was used in some previous meta-analyses (36, 98101), likely allowed us to capture a more comprehensive view of cognitive performance, potentially smoothing out the fluctuations seen at the individual test level. However, it may have conservatively reduced our power to detect significant effects of MHT on cognition. As different studies categorize certain tests differently, we based our grouping on generally accepted criteria as well as insights from prior studies (36, 98101). Nonetheless, more research is needed to confirm the validity of these groupings within the context of MHT and cognition.

For completeness, meta-analysis of individual cognitive tests was provided as a secondary analysis. This analysis was limited by the relatively smaller number of studies reporting on the same cognitive tests. While most of the results did not reach statistical significance, a few negative and positive effects were observed. MHT was associated with a worsening in TMT-B scores relative to placebo, which was associated with studies of late-life use and longer duration of use. EPT was associated with MMSE declines compared to placebo, while ET was associated with improvements in Digit Span forward, particularly among studies of late-life treatment. However, there were also small declines in CVLT free recall among ET users when compared to placebo. Caution is needed when interpreting these results, as certain large-scale ancillary studies within the WHI (59, 84) were heavily weighted in these sub-analyses and had a substantial impact on the pooled estimates. Overall, these findings underscore the variable effects of MHT on different cognitive tests, with outcomes influenced by factors such as the type of therapy, timing of initiation, and treatment duration.

Another limitation relates to sample size disparities within the subgroups. The sample size for studies involving early menopause was smaller compared to those focusing on late-life interventions. This discrepancy may have reduced our power to detect significant effects within the early menopause subgroup. Additionally, the heterogeneity detected in the meta-analyses suggests the need for a more detailed investigation into methodological variations across studies. We addressed heterogeneity by performing both subgroup analyses and multi-level meta-regressions assessing the impact of type of menopause, MHT formulation, initiation timing and treatment duration. While these analyses revealed interesting patterns of MHT effects, the persistence of significant heterogeneity evident in meta-regression analyses accounting for these factors suggests the influence of additional unaccounted factors. For instance, as with previous meta-analyses (107, 108), the insufficient number of included studies hindered further examinations of different types of estrogen (estradiol vs. CEE) or progestogen (synthetic progestins vs. micronized progesterone), and route of administration (oral, transdermal, or intramuscular), as well as between continuous vs. cyclic use of progestogen. These factors could yield valuable insights into the nuances of MHT effects on cognition. Additionally, variables such as education and socioeconomical status, which also can increase heterogeneity, were accounted for in many the studies, and we used the fully adjusted estimates in our meta-analysis. However, possible risks for menopause-associated cognitive decline such as smoking status, comorbidities like diabetes or thyroid disease, and lifestyle factors were not consistently screened for or accounted for in the included studies and could have contributed to the observed heterogeneity. Overall, presence of residual heterogeneity is an indication of limitations within the existing literature. While this does not undermine the validity of the analysis, it does emphasize the need for caution in interpreting the findings.

Finally, when conducting multiple meta-analyses, there is a potential for a multiple comparisons issue. We did not apply a correction for multiple comparisons because our study design a priori included the main meta-analysis followed by sensitivity analyses encompassing distinct questions, each focusing on different aspects of the relationship between MHT and cognition, such as timing, formulation, and type of menopause. Therefore, we frame our analysis as exploratory and hypothesis-generating, rather than confirmatory, with the primary aim of providing an overview of the available evidence. Consequently, our findings do not warrant clinical application but rather support research interest in MHT for cognitive support and underscore the need for more comprehensive research in this area.

While clinical trials utilizing standardized cognitive tests as primary endpoints offer valuable insights, some limitations should also be considered. These limitations encompass statistical, methodological, and conceptual aspects. One notable limitation is the small sample sizes (<1,000 participants) observed in 28 out of the 34 studies. This may constrain statistical power, thus hindering the ability to identify statistically significant effects, particularly when exploring cognitive tests and domains that exhibit subtle changes. Cognitive performance can also be influenced by practice effects, particularly when trials are relatively brief. Participants’ familiarity with cognitive tests may lead to improvements in scores over time, irrespective of the treatment under investigation, which can mask or confound the true cognitive impact of MHT.

The variation in cognitive test selection and sensitivity across different studies is another limitation. Some trials focused exclusively on one or few specific cognitive tests or specific components of a test. This selectivity in test choice may not fully capture the multifaceted nature of cognitive function, potentially overlooking effects that may be apparent in a broader cognitive assessment. Further, certain cognitive tests employed may not be inherently sensitive to hormone levels. For instance, several studies used the MMSE or similar global cognition test as their primary (and sometimes only) endpoint, although there is no clear evidence that these tests would be sensitive to estrogen changes. The variability in cognitive tests employed across studies is another possible source of heterogeneity. These discrepancies encompass factors such as variations in test length, difficulty, word relatedness, the use of early acquired or frequently used language, repeated administration of the same test or parallel versions (with concerns about reliability and comparability), as well as potential differences among testers in multi-setting studies. Finally, it remains to be established whether the observed changes in cognition were primarily due to the effects of MHT or the remission of menopausal symptoms following treatment.

The study of MHT’s impact on cognition faces some basic challenges. Firstly, existing evidence suggests that estrogen therapy primarily maintains cognitive function rather than enhancing it. For instance, in surgical menopause, estrogen suppression has been linked to memory deficits, with subsequent treatment restoring performance to baseline levels (60, 115, 116). This creates a statistical hurdle, as it essentially requires the placebo group to deteriorate to a sufficient degree for the treatment group’s benefits to be statistically detectable. If the placebo group’s cognitive performance remains relatively stable or does not decline significantly, it is challenging to demonstrate improvement in the treatment group, even if estrogen therapy is indeed preserving cognitive function effectively. To this point, another important consideration arises from evidence that cognitive performance doesn’t appear to be impaired during menopause (124, 125). In statistical terms, if there is no pre-existing cognitive deficit, it may be impractical to expect MHT to improve cognitive function. Additionally, ‘ceiling effects’ may come into play when cognitive performance is already near the upper limits of measurement sensitivity, making it challenging to detect further improvements, even if they exist. This presents a conceptual challenge, as MHT interventions aim to enhance or maintain cognitive abilities, which may already be functioning at a satisfactory level. Further, although cognitive complaints are generally more frequent in the perimenopausal stage early postmenopausal than in the late postmenopausal stages (124126), no clinical trials have focused specifically on perimenopausal women. For these reasons, it may be challenging to detect a signal for estrogen benefits on cognition in postmenopausal women. Overall, more studies are needed that carefully consider the baseline cognitive status of participants, the sensitivity of selected cognitive endpoints, and the appropriateness of cognitive improvement as an achievable outcome.

Studies that incorporate biological markers of AD are also warranted to overcome some limitations of existing literature. First off, increasing research identifies increased biomarker indicators of AD risk among perimenopausal and postmenopausal women compared to premenopausal controls or age-controlled men, including higher amyloid-beta (Aβ) deposition (1116) and tau pathology (127), glucose hypometabolism (1215, 128) and lower gray matter volume in some AD-vulnerable regions (1215, 128131) Additionally, some studies report altered mitochondrial energy production (132, 133) and more white matter hyperintensities (134) in postmenopausal women.

While neuroimaging studies of MHT are scarce, several indicate a possible beneficial role of estrogen therapy on biomarkers such as glucose metabolism (14, 135138), cerebral blood flow (139, 140), Aβ deposition (14, 141) and tau pathology (142). Notably, a recent prospective study reported, over a 6-month period, a smaller reduction in Aβ42/p-tau231 in MHT users compared to non-users (143). Medical imaging examining presence of cerebrovascular and neurodegenerative insults prior to treatment initiation can further guide both research efforts in identifying the optimal treatment timeline, providing important information for individualized treatment.

Finally, the type of progestogen used in MHT formulations is also of interest. Various progestogens have been utilized in MHT preparations, including several synthetic progestins such as MPA, norethindrone, norethisterone, drospirenone, and dienogest. There is laboratory evidence that certain progestins, particularly MPA, may antagonize the effects of estrogen on brain regions involved in memory function, such as the hippocampus (144, 145). Additionally, MPA users exhibit a higher risk of breast cancer and venous thromboembolism as compared to users of dydrogesterone (146). This suggests that the progestin type may modulate the impact of opposed therapy on some cognitive functions. Oral MPA was the most common progestogen used in the studies included in this meta-analysis, which may have contributed to the declines in global cognition observed in late-life.

In the end, our findings highlight the need for further research in this area, preferably by means of clinical trials using cognitive tests and AD biomarkers as endpoints. Moreover, the inclusion of a larger number of studies, especially those reporting continuous estimates, will enhance the statistical power and generalizability of findings. Stratified analyses based on estrogen and progestogen types initiated in midlife, and with different treatment durations, are warranted. For clinical practice, achieving precision hormone therapy will likely require a priori identification of women appropriate for MHT and the type and dose of MHT appropriate for their individual characteristics and health risks (146).

Conclusions

This meta-analysis suggests time-dependent effects of MHT on certain aspects of cognition, with variations based on formulation and timing of initiation, underscoring the need for further research with larger samples and more homogeneous study designs. Continued research is needed to further refine our understanding of these effects, enabling more informed clinical decisions for menopausal women seeking cognitive benefits from MHT.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

CA: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft. MN: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. SJ: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. CC: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. CZ: Writing – review & editing. CB: Writing – review & editing. FF: Writing – review & editing. TA: Writing – review & editing. MB: Writing – review & editing. SP: Writing – review & editing. PC: Writing – review & editing. SW: Writing – review & editing, Supervision. MF: Writing – review & editing. RB: Writing – review & editing. LM: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by grants from NIH/NIA (P01AG026572, R01AG05793, R01AG0755122); the Women’s Alzheimer’s Movement; and philanthropic support to the Alzheimer’s Prevention Program. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fendo.2024.1350318/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Alzheimer’s Association. 2022 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. (2022) 18:700–89. doi: 10.1002/alz.12638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. Jama. (1997) 278:1349–56. doi: 10.1001/jama.278.16.1349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Altmann A, Tian L, Henderson VW, Greicius MD, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative I. Sex modifies the APOE-related risk of developing Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. (2014) 75:563–73. doi: 10.1002/ana.24135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Gao S, Hendrie HC, Hall KS, Hui S. The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1998) 55:809–15. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.9.809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Andersen K, Launer LJ, Dewey ME, Letenneur L, Ott A, Copeland JR, et al. Gender differences in the incidence of AD and vascular dementia: The EURODEM Studies. EURODEM Incidence Research Group. Neurology. (1999) 53:1992–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.9.1992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Fratiglioni L, Launer LJ, Andersen K, Breteler MM, Copeland JR, Dartigues JF, et al. Incidence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. Neurology. (2000) 54:S10–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

7. Lobo A, Launer LJ, Fratiglioni L, Andersen K, Di Carlo A, Breteler MM, et al. Prevalence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. Neurology. (2000) 54:S4–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

8. Ferretti MT, Iulita MF, Cavedo E, Chiesa PA, Schumacher Dimech A, Santuccione Chadha A, et al. Sex differences in Alzheimer disease - the gateway to precision medicine. Nat Rev Neurol. (2018) 14:457–69. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0032-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rahman A, Jackson H, Hristov H, Isaacson RS, Saif N, Shetty T, et al. Sex and gender driven modifiers of alzheimer’s: the role for estrogenic control across age, race, medical, and lifestyle risks. Front Aging Neurosci. (2019) 11:315. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sperling RA, Karlawish J, Johnson KA. Preclinical Alzheimer disease-the challenges ahead. Nat Rev Neurol. (2013) 9:54–8. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bove R, Secor E, Chibnik LB, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bennett DA, et al. Age at surgical menopause influences cognitive decline and Alzheimer pathology in older women. Neurology. (2014) 82:222–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mosconi L, Berti V, Quinn C, Mchugh P, Petrongolo G, Varsavsky I, et al. Sex differences in Alzheimer risk: Brain imaging of endocrine vs chronologic aging. Neurology. (2017) 89:1382–90. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mosconi L, Rahman A, Diaz I, Wu X, Scheyer O, Hristov HW, et al. Increased Alzheimer’s risk during the menopause transition: A 3-year longitudinal brain imaging study. PloS One. (2018) 13:e0207885. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Rahman A, Schelbaum E, Hoffman K, Diaz I, Hristov H, Andrews R, et al. Sex-driven modifiers of Alzheimer risk. Neurology. (2020) 95:e166. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000009781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Mosconi L, Berti V, Dyke J, Schelbaum E, Jett S, Loughlin L, et al. Menopause impacts human brain structure, connectivity, energy metabolism, and amyloid-beta deposition. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:10867. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90084-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Coughlan GT, Betthauser TJ, Boyle R, Koscik RL, Klinger HM, Chibnik LB, et al. Association of Age at Menopause and Hormone Therapy Use With Tau and beta-Amyloid Positron Emission Tomography. JAMA Neurol. (2023) 80:462–73. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2023.0455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Goodman Y, Bruce AJ, Cheng B, Mattson MP. Estrogens attenuate and corticosterone exacerbates excitotoxicity, oxidative injury, and amyloid beta-peptide toxicity in hippocampal neurons. J Neurochem. (1996) 66:1836–44. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1996.66051836.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mcewen BS, Alves SE, Bulloch K, Weiland NG. Ovarian steroids and the brain: implications for cognition and aging. Neurology. (1997) 48:S8–15. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.5_Suppl_7.8S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Brinton RD, Yao J, Yin F, Mack WJ, Cadenas E. Perimenopause as a neurological transition state. Nat Rev Endocrinol. (2015) 11:393–405. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2015.82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hara Y, Waters EM, Mcewen BS, Morrison JH. Estrogen effects on cognitive and synaptic health over the lifecourse. Physiol Rev. (2015) 95:785–807. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00036.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lai YJ, Yu D, Zhang JH, Chen GJ. Cooperation of genomic and rapid nongenomic actions of estrogens in synaptic plasticity. Mol Neurobiol. (2017) 54:4113–26. doi: 10.1007/s12035-016-9979-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Brinton RD. The healthy cell bias of estrogen action: mitochondrial bioenergetics and neurological implications. Trends Neurosci. (2008) 31:529–37. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Arevalo M-A, Azcoitia I, Garcia-Segura LM. The neuroprotective actions of oestradiol and oestrogen receptors. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:17–29. doi: 10.1038/nrn3856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Jett S, Malviya N, Schelbaum E, Jang G, Jahan E, Clancy K, et al. Endogenous and exogenous estrogen exposures: how women’s reproductive health can drive brain aging and inform alzheimer’s prevention. Front Aging Neurosci. (2022) 14:831807. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.831807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Jett S, Schelbaum E, Jang G, Boneu Yepez C, Dyke JP, Pahlajani S, et al. Ovarian steroid hormones: A long overlooked but critical contributor to brain aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci. (2022) 14:948219. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.948219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Morrison JH, Brinton RD, Schmidt PJ, Gore AC. Estrogen, menopause, and the aging brain: how basic neuroscience can inform hormone therapy in women. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:10332–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3369-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. (2020) 396:413–46. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Davis SR, Lambrinoudaki I, Lumsden M, Mishra GD, Pal L, Rees M, et al. Menopause. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2015) 1:15004. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Monteleone P, Mascagni G, Giannini A, Genazzani AR, Simoncini T. Symptoms of menopause — global prevalence, physiology and implications. Nat Rev Endocrinol. (2018) 14:199–215. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2017.180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. El Khoudary SR, Greendale G, Crawford SL, Avis NE, Brooks MM, Thurston RC, et al. The menopause transition and women’s health at midlife: a progress report from the Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation (SWAN). Menopause. (2019) 26:1213–27. doi: 10.1097/GME.0000000000001424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Linn RT, Wolf PA, Bachman DL, Knoefel JE, Cobb JL, Belanger AJ, et al. The ‘preclinical phase’ of probable Alzheimer’s disease. A 13-year prospective study of the Framingham cohort. Arch Neurol. (1995) 52:485–90. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1995.00540290075020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. (2004) 256:183–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Tierney MC, Yao C, Kiss A, Mcdowell I. Neuropsychological tests accurately predict incident Alzheimer disease after 5 and 10 years. Neurology. (2005) 64:1853–9. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000163773.21794.0B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Jack CR Jr., Knopman DS, Weigand SD, Wiste HJ, Vemuri P, Lowe V, et al. An operational approach to National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association criteria for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. (2012) 71:765–75. doi: 10.1002/ana.22628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Yaffe K, Sawaya G, Lieberburg I, Grady D. Estrogen therapy in postmenopausal women: effects on cognitive function and dementia. JAMA. (1998) 279:688–95. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.9.688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hogervorst E, Williams J, Budge M, Riedel W, Jolles J. The nature of the effect of female gonadal hormone replacement therapy on cognitive function in post-menopausal women: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience. (2000) 101:485–512. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00410-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Paganini-Hill A, Henderson VW. Estrogen replacement therapy and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Intern Med. (1996) 156:2213–7. doi: 10.1001/archinte.156.19.2213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tang MX, Jacobs D, Stern Y, Marder K, Schofield P, Gurland B, et al. Effect of oestrogen during menopause on risk and age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. (1996) 348:429–32. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)03356-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kawas C, Resnick S, Morrison A, Brookmeyer R, Corrada M, Zonderman A, et al. A prospective study of estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology. (1997) 48:1517–21. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.6.1517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Slooter AJ, Bronzova J, Witteman JC, Van Broeckhoven C, Hofman A, Van Duijn CM. Estrogen use and early onset Alzheimer’s disease: a population-based study. J Neurology Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1999) 67:779–81. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.67.6.779

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Zandi PP, Carlson MC, Plassman BL, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Mayer LS, Steffens DC, et al. Hormone replacement therapy and incidence of Alzheimer disease in older women: the Cache County Study. Jama. (2002) 288:2123–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.17.2123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Henderson VW, Benke K, Green R, Cupples L, Farrer L. Postmenopausal hormone therapy and Alzheimer’s disease risk: interaction with age. J Neurology Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2005) 76:103–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.024927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Whitmer RA, Quesenberry CP, Zhou J, Yaffe K. Timing of hormone therapy and dementia: the critical window theory revisited. Ann Neurol. (2011) 69:163–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.22239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Shao H, Breitner JC, Whitmer RA, Wang J, Hayden K, Wengreen H, et al. Hormone therapy and Alzheimer disease dementia: new findings from the Cache County Study. Neurology. (2012) 79:1846–52.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

45. Yoo J, Shin D, Han K, Kim D, Won HS, Lee J, et al. Female reproductive factors and the risk of dementia: a nationwide cohort study. Eur J Neurol. (2020) 27:1448–58. doi: 10.1111/ene.14315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kim YJ, Soto M, Branigan GL, Rodgers K, Brinton RD. Association between menopausal hormone therapy and risk of neurodegenerative diseases: Implications for precision hormone therapy. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). (2021) 7:e12174. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nerattini M, Jett S, Andy C, Carlton C, Zarate C, Boneu C, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of menopause hormone therapy on risk of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. Front Aging Neurosci. (2023) 15. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1260427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Shumaker SA, Legault C, Rapp SR, Thal L, Wallace RB, Ockene JK, et al. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. (2003) 289:2651–62. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Shumaker SA, Legault C, Kuller L, Rapp SR, Thal L, Lane DS, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. (2004) 291:2947–58. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Espeland Ma BR, Hogan Pa, Rapp Sr, Coker Lh, Legault C, Granek I, et al. Long term effects of conjugated equine estrogens therapies on domain-specific cognitive function: results from the women’s health initiative study of cognitive aging (WHISCA) extension. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2010) 28:1263–71.

Google Scholar

51. Maki PM. The timing of estrogen therapy after ovariectomy–implications for neurocognitive function. Nat Clin Pract Endocrinol Metab. (2008) 4:494+. doi: 10.1038/ncpendmet0901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Maki PM, Sundermann E. Hormone therapy and cognitive function. Hum Reprod Update. (2009) 15:667–81. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dmp022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Henderson VW, Rocca WA. Estrogens and Alzheimer disease risk: Is there a window of opportunity. American Academy of Neurology (AAN) (2012). doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318271f88f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rossouw JE, Manson JE, Kaunitz AM, Anderson GL. Lessons learned from the Women’s Health Initiative trials of menopausal hormone therapy. Obstet Gynecol. (2013) 121:172–6. doi: 10.1097/AOG.0b013e31827a08c8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Manson JE, Bassuk SS, Kaunitz AM, Pinkerton JV. The Women’s Health Initiative trials of menopausal hormone therapy: lessons learned. Menopause. (2020) 27:918–28. doi: 10.1097/GME.0000000000001553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Espeland MA, Shumaker SA, Leng I, Manson JE, Brown CM, Leblanc ES, et al. Long-term effects on cognitive function of postmenopausal hormone therapy prescribed to women aged 50 to 55 years. JAMA Intern Med. (2013) 173:1429–36. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.7727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Gleason CE, Dowling NM, Wharton W, Manson JE, Miller VM, Atwood CS, et al. Effects of hormone therapy on cognition and mood in recently postmenopausal women: findings from the randomized, controlled KEEPS-cognitive and affective study. PloS Med. (2015) 12:e1001833; discussion e1001833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Henderson VW, St John JA, Hodis HN, Mccleary CA, Stanczyk FZ, Shoupe D, et al. Cognitive effects of estradiol after menopause: A randomized trial of the timing hypothesis. Neurology. (2016) 87:699–708. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Espeland MA, Rapp SR, Manson JE, Goveas JS, Shumaker SA, Hayden KM, et al. Long-term effects on cognitive trajectories of postmenopausal hormone therapy in two age groups. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2017) 72:838–45. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Phillips SM, Sherwin BB. Effects of estrogen on memory function in surgically menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (1992) 17:485–95. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90007-T

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Duka T, Tasker R, Mcgowan JF. The effects of 3-week estrogen hormone replacement on cognition in elderly healthy females. Psychopharmacol (Berl). (2000) 149:129–39. doi: 10.1007/s002139900324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Shaywitz SE, Naftolin F, Zelterman D, Marchione KE, Holahan JM, Palter SF, et al. Better oral reading and short-term memory in midlife, postmenopausal women taking estrogen. Menopause. (2003) 10:420–6. doi: 10.1097/01.GME.0000060241.02837.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Joffe H, Hall JE, Gruber S, Sarmiento IA, Cohen LS, Yurgelun-Todd D, et al. Estrogen therapy selectively enhances prefrontal cognitive processes: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study with functional magnetic resonance imaging in perimenopausal and recently postmenopausal women. Menopause. (2006) 13:411–22. doi: 10.1097/01.gme.0000189618.48774.7b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Leblanc ES, Neiss MB, Carello PE, Samuels MH, Janowsky JS. Hot flashes and estrogen therapy do not influence cognition in early menopausal women. Menopause. (2007) 14:191–202. doi: 10.1097/01.gme.0000230347.28616.1c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Gorenstein C, Rennó J Jr., Vieira Filho AH, Gianfaldoni A, Gonçalves MA, Halbe HW, et al. Estrogen replacement therapy and cognitive functions in healthy postmenopausal women: a randomized trial. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2011) 14:367–73. doi: 10.1007/s00737-011-0230-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Moradi F, Jahanian Sadatmahalleh S, Ziaei S. The effect of hormone replacement therapy on cognitive function in postmenopausal women: An RCT. Int J Reprod BioMed. (2019) 16(12):ijrm.v16i12.3682. doi: 10.18502/ijrm.v16i12.3682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Maki PM, Rubin LH, Fornelli D, Drogos L, Banuvar S, Shulman LP, et al. Effects of botanicals and combined hormone therapy on cognition in postmenopausal women. Menopause. (2009) 16:1167–77. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181ace484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Page MJ, Mckenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wolf OT, Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Torber S, Mcewen BS, Kirschbaum C. Two weeks of transdermal estradiol treatment in postmenopausal elderly women and its effect on memory and mood: verbal memory changes are associated with the treatment induced estradiol levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (1999) 24:727–41. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00025-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Binder EF, Schechtman KB, Birge SJ, Williams DB, Kohrt WM. Effects of hormone replacement therapy on cognitive performance in elderly women. Maturitas. (2001) 38:137–46. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5122(00)00214-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Linzmayer L, Semlitsch HV, Saletu B, Bock G, Saletu-Zyhlarz G, Zoghlami A, et al. Double-blind, placebo-controlled psychometric studies on the effects of a combined estrogen-progestin regimen versus estrogen alone on performance, mood and personality of menopausal syndrome patients. Arzneimittelforschung. (2001) 51:238–45. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1300030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Rapp SR, Espeland MA, Shumaker SA, Henderson VW, Brunner RL, Manson JE, et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. (2003) 289:2663–72. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Espeland MA, Rapp SR, Shumaker SA, Brunner R, Manson JE, Sherwin BB, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. (2004) 291:2959–68. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Dunkin J, Rasgon N, Wagner-Steh K, David S, Altshuler L, Rapkin A. Reproductive events modify the effects of estrogen replacement therapy on cognition in healthy postmenopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2005) 30:284–96. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Greenspan SL, Beck TJ, Resnick NM, Bhattacharya R, Parker RA. Effect of hormone replacement, alendronate, or combination therapy on hip structural geometry: a 3-year, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. J Bone Miner Res. (2005) 20:1525–32. doi: 10.1359/JBMR.050508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Schiff R, Bulpitt CJ, Wesnes KA, Rajkumar C. Short-term transdermal estradiol therapy, cognition and depressive symptoms in healthy older women. A randomised placebo controlled pilot cross-over study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2005) 30:309–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Wolf OT, Heinrich AB, Hanstein B, Kirschbaum C. Estradiol or estradiol/progesterone treatment in older women: no strong effects on cognition. Neurobiol Aging. (2005) 26:1029–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Almeida OP, Lautenschlager NT, Vasikaran S, Leedman P, Gelavis A, Flicker L. A 20-week randomized controlled trial of estradiol replacement therapy for women aged 70 years and older: effect on mood, cognition and quality of life. Neurobiol Aging. (2006) 27:141–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Resnick SM, Maki PM, Rapp SR, Espeland MA, Brunner R, Coker LH, et al. Effects of combination estrogen plus progestin hormone treatment on cognition and affect. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2006) 91:1802–10. doi: 10.1210/jc.2005-2097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Yaffe K, Vittinghoff E, Ensrud KE, Johnson KC, Diem S, Hanes V, et al. Effects of ultra-low-dose transdermal estradiol on cognition and health-related quality of life. Arch Neurol. (2006) 63:945–50. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.7.945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Maki PM, Gast MJ, Vieweg AJ, Burriss SW, Yaffe K. Hormone therapy in menopausal women with cognitive complaints: a randomized, double-blind trial. Neurology. (2007) 69:1322–30. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000277275.42504.93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Pefanco MA, Kenny AM, Kaplan RF, Kuchel G, Walsh S, Kleppinger A, et al. The effect of 3-year treatment with 0.25 mg/day of micronized 17beta-estradiol on cognitive function in older postmenopausal women. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2007) 55:426–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01085.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Marinho RM, Soares JM Jr., Santiago RC, Maganhin CC, MaChado F, De Miranda Cota AM, et al. Effects of estradiol on the cognitive function of postmenopausal women. Maturitas. (2008) 60:230–4. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2008.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Resnick SM, Espeland MA, An Y, Maki PM, Coker LH, Jackson R, et al. Effects of conjugated equine estrogens on cognition and affect in postmenopausal women with prior hysterectomy. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2009) 94:4152–61. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-1340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Tierney MC, Oh P, Moineddin R, Greenblatt EM, Snow WG, Fisher RH, et al. A randomized double-blind trial of the effects of hormone therapy on delayed verbal recall in older women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2009) 34:1065–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Alhola P, Tuomisto H, Saarinen R, Portin R, Kalleinen N, Polo-Kantola P. Estrogen + progestin therapy and cognition: a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind study. J Obstet Gynaecol Res. (2010) 36:796–802. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0756.2010.01214.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Kocoska-Maras L, Zethraeus N, Radestad AF, Ellingsen T, Von Schoultz B, Johannesson M, et al. A randomized trial of the effect of testosterone and estrogen on verbal fluency, verbal memory, and spatial ability in healthy postmenopausal women. Fertil Steril. (2011) 95:152–7. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2010.05.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Davison SL, Bell RJ, Robinson PJ, Jane F, Leech J, Maruff P, et al. Continuous-combined oral estradiol/drospirenone has no detrimental effect on cognitive performance and improves estrogen deficiency symptoms in early postmenopausal women: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Menopause. (2013) 20:1020–6. doi: 10.1097/GME.0b013e318287474f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Berent-Spillson A, Briceno E, Pinsky A, Simmen A, Persad CC, Zubieta JK, et al. Distinct cognitive effects of estrogen and progesterone in menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2015) 59:25–36. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Jayachandran M, Miller VM, Lahr BD, Bailey KR, Lowe VJ, Fields JA, et al. Peripheral markers of neurovascular unit integrity and amyloid-β in the brains of menopausal women. J Alzheimers Dis. (2021) 80:397–405. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Mikolajewicz N, Komarova SV. Meta-analytic methodology for basic research: A practical guide. Front Physiol. (2019) 10:203. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Higgins Jpt ES, Li T. Chapter 23: Including variants on randomized trials. In: Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, 2nd edition. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester (UK (2022).

Google Scholar

93. Vanderweele TJ. Optimal approximate conversions of odds ratios and hazard ratios to risk ratios. Biometrics. (2020) 76:746–52. doi: 10.1111/biom.13197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. (2002) 21:1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando: Academic Press (1985).

Google Scholar

96. Dersimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. (1986) 7:177–88. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Jackson D, White IR, Thompson SG. Extending DerSimonian and Laird’s methodology to perform multivariate random effects meta-analyses. Stat Med. (2010) 29:1282–97. doi: 10.1002/sim.3602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Leblanc ES, Janowsky J, Chan BK, Nelson HD. Hormone replacement therapy and cognition: systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. (2001) 285:1489–99. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.11.1489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Yesufu A, Bandelow S, Hogervorst E. Meta-analyses of the effect of hormone treatment on cognitive function in postmenopausal women. Womens Health (Lond). (2007) 3:173–94. doi: 10.2217/17455057.3.2.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Lethaby A, Hogervorst E, Richards M, Yesufu A, Yaffe K. Hormone replacement therapy for cognitive function in postmenopausal women. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2008) 2008(1):CD003122. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003122.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Hogervorst E, Bandelow S. Sex steroids to maintain cognitive function in women after the menopause: a meta-analyses of treatment trials. Maturitas. (2010) 66:56–71. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Maki PM, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM. Enhanced verbal memory in nondemented elderly women receiving hormone-replacement therapy. Am J Psychiatry. (2001) 158:227–33. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Ockene JK, Barad DH, Cochrane BB, Larson JC, Gass M, Wassertheil-Smoller S, et al. Symptom experience after discontinuing use of estrogen plus progestin. JAMA. (2005) 294:183–93. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.2.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Cheung MW. Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: a structural equation modeling approach. Psychol Methods. (2014) 19:211–29. doi: 10.1037/a0032968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. (2000) 56:455–63. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Zhou HH, Yu Z, Luo L, Xie F, Wang Y, Wan Z. The effect of hormone replacement therapy on cognitive function in healthy postmenopausal women: a meta-analysis of 23 randomized controlled trials. Psychogeriatrics. (2021) 21:926–38. doi: 10.1111/psyg.12768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Chen L, Zheng W, Chen G, Liu LH, Yao J, Chen Y. Menopausal hormone therapy does not improve some domains of memory: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). (2022) 13:894883. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2022.894883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Rocca WA, Bower JH, Maraganore DM, Ahlskog JE, Grossardt BR, De Andrade M, et al. Increased risk of cognitive impairment or dementia in women who underwent oophorectomy before menopause. Neurology. (2007) 69:1074–83. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000276984.19542.e6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Phung TK, Waltoft BL, Laursen TM, Settnes A, Kessing LV, Mortensen PB, et al. Hysterectomy, oophorectomy and risk of dementia: a nationwide historical cohort study. Dement Geriatr Cognit Disord. (2010) 30:43–50. doi: 10.1159/000314681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Rocca WA, Grossardt BR, Shuster LT. Oophorectomy, estrogen, and dementia: a 2014 update. Mol Cell Endocrinol. (2014) 389:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2014.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Gilsanz P, Lee C, Corrada MM, Kawas CH, Quesenberry CP Jr., Whitmer RA. Reproductive period and risk of dementia in a diverse cohort of health care members. Neurology. (2019) 92:e2005–14. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Imtiaz B, Tuppurainen M, Tiihonen M, Kivipelto M, Soininen H, Hartikainen S, et al. Oophorectomy, hysterectomy, and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a nationwide case-control study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2014) 42:575–81. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Hackman BW, Galbraith D. Replacement therapy and piperazine oestrone sulphate (‘Harmogen’) and its effect on memory. Curr Med Res Opin. (1976) 4:303–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

115. Sherwin BB. Estrogen and/or androgen replacement therapy and cognitive functioning in surgically menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (1988) 13:345–57. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(88)90060-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Sherwin BB. Estrogen and cognitive functioning in surgically menopausal women. Ann NY Acad Sci. (1990) 592:474–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb30379.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Matyi JM, Rattinger GB, Schwartz S, Buhusi M, Tschanz JT. Lifetime estrogen exposure and cognition in late life: the Cache County Study. Menopause. (2019) 26:1366–74. doi: 10.1097/GME.0000000000001405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Kampen DL, Sherwin BB. Estrogen use and verbal memory in healthy postmenopausal women. Obstet Gynecol. (1994) 83:979–83. doi: 10.1097/00006250-199406000-00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Robinson D, Friedman L, Marcus R, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage J. Estrogen replacement therapy and memory in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc. (1994) 42:919–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1994.tb06580.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Kimura D. Estrogen replacement therapy may protect against intellectual decline in postmenopausal women. Hormones Behav. (1995) 29:312–21. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1995.1022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Szklo M, Cerhan J, Diez-Roux AV, Chambless L, Cooper L, Folsom AR, et al. Estrogen replacement therapy and cognitive functioning in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study. Am J Epidemiol. (1996) 144:1048–57. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a008877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Resnick SM, Maki PM, Golski S, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB. Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and neuropsychological performance. Hormones Behav. (1998) 34:171–82. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Maki PM. A systematic review of clinical trials of hormone therapy on cognitive function: effects of age at initiation and progestin use. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2005) 1052:182–97. doi: 10.1196/annals.1347.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Greendale GA, Huang MH, Wight RG, Seeman T, Luetters C, Avis NE, et al. Effects of the menopause transition and hormone use on cognitive performance in midlife women. Neurology. (2009) 72:1850–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a71193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Maki PM, Henderson VW. Cognition and the menopause transition. Menopause. (2016) 23:803–5. doi: 10.1097/GME.0000000000000681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Woods NF, Mitchell ES, Adams C. Memory functioning among midlife women: observations from the Seattle Midlife Women’s Health Study. Menopause. (2000) 7:257–65. doi: 10.1097/00042192-200007040-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Buckley RF, O’donnell A, Mcgrath ER, Jacobs HIL, Lois C, Satizabal CL, et al. Menopause status moderates sex differences in tau burden: A framingham PET study. Ann Neurol. (2022) 92:11–22. doi: 10.1002/ana.26382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Mosconi L, Berti V, Quinn C, Mchugh P, Petrongolo G, Osorio RS, et al. Correction: Perimenopause and emergence of an Alzheimer’s bioenergetic phenotype in brain and periphery. PloS One. (2018) 13:e0193314. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Kim GW, Park K, Jeong GW. Effects of sex hormones and age on brain volume in post-menopausal women. J Sex Med. (2018) 15:662–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2018.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Schelbaum E, Loughlin L, Jett S, Zhang C, Jang G, Malviya N, et al. Association of reproductive history with brain MRI biomarkers of dementia risk in midlife. Neurology. (2021) 97:e2328–39. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Zeydan B, Tosakulwong N, Schwarz CG, Senjem ML, Gunter JL, Reid RI, et al. Association of bilateral salpingo-oophorectomy before menopause onset with medial temporal lobe neurodegeneration. JAMA Neurol. (2019) 76:95–100. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.3057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Jett S, Dyke JP, Andy C, Schelbaum E, Jang G, Boneu Yepez C, et al. Effects of sex and APOE ϵ4 genotype on brain mitochondrial high-energy phosphates in midlife individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease: a 31Phosphorus MR spectroscopy study. PloS One. (2023) 18(2):e0281302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Jett S, Dyke JP, Andy C, Schelbaum E, Jang G, Boneu Yepez C, et al. Sex and menopause impact (31)P-Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy brain mitochondrial function in association with (11)C-PiB PET amyloid-beta load. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:22087. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-26573-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Lohner V, Pehlivan G, Sanroma G, Miloschewski A, Schirmer MD, Stocker T, et al. Relation between sex, menopause, and white matter hyperintensities: the rhineland study. Neurology. (2022) 99:e935–43. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000200782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Eberling JL, Reed BR, Coleman JE, Jagust WJ. Effect of estrogen on cerebral glucose metabolism in postmenopausal women. Neurology. (2000) 55:875–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.6.875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Rasgon NL, Silverman D, Siddarth P, Miller K, Ercoli LM, Elman S, et al. Estrogen use and brain metabolic change in postmenopausal women. Neurobiol Aging. (2005) 26:229–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Silverman DH, Geist CL, Kenna HA, Williams K, Wroolie T, Powers B, et al. Differences in regional brain metabolism associated with specific formulations of hormone therapy in postmenopausal women at risk for AD. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2011) 36:502–13. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Rasgon NL, Geist CL, Kenna HA, Wroolie TE, Williams KE, Silverman DH. Prospective randomized trial to assess effects of continuing hormone therapy on cerebral function in postmenopausal women at risk for dementia. PloS One. (2014) 9:e89095. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Maki PM, Resnick SM. Longitudinal effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and cognition. Neurobiol Aging. (2000) 21:373–83. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(00)00123-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Slopien R, Junik R, Meczekalski B, Halerz-Nowakowska B, Maciejewska M, Warenik-Szymankiewicz A, et al. Influence of hormonal replacement therapy on the regional cerebral blood flow in postmenopausal women. Maturitas. (2003) 46:255–62. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5122(03)00144-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Kantarci K, Lowe VJ, Lesnick TG, Tosakulwong N, Bailey KR, Fields JA, et al. Early postmenopausal transdermal 17β-estradiol therapy and amyloid-β Deposition. J Alzheimers Dis. (2016) 53:547–56. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Wisch JK, Meeker KL, Gordon BA, Flores S, Dincer A, Grant EA, et al. Sex-related differences in tau positron emission tomography (PET) and the effects of hormone therapy (HT). Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2021) 35:164–8. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Depypere H, Vergallo A, Lemercier P, Lista S, Benedet A, Ashton N, et al. Menopause hormone therapy significantly alters pathophysiological biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. (2023) 19(4):1320–30. doi: 10.1002/alz.12759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Nilsen J, Brinton RD. Divergent impact of progesterone and medroxyprogesterone acetate (Provera) on nuclear mitogen-activated protein kinase signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2003) 100:10506–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1334098100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Nilsen J, Brinton RD. Impact of progestins on estradiol potentiation of the glutamate calcium response. Neuroreport. (2002) 13:825–30. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200205070-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Kim Y. Precision hormone therapy: gaps and opportunities. Gynecol Reprod Endocrinol Metab. (2020) 1:80–8.

Google Scholar

Keywords: HRT (hormone replacement therapy), Alzheimer disease, cognition, meta-analysis, menopause

Citation: Andy C, Nerattini M, Jett S, Carlton C, Zarate C, Boneu C, Fauci F, Ajila T, Battista M, Pahlajani S, Christos P, Fink ME, Williams S, Brinton RD and Mosconi L (2024) Systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of menopause hormone therapy on cognition. Front. Endocrinol. 15:1350318. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2024.1350318

Received: 05 December 2023; Accepted: 19 February 2024;
Published: 04 March 2024.

Edited by:

Yoshinori Okamoto, Meijo University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Alicia A. Walf, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, United States
Henrik Ryberg, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Sweden

Copyright © 2024 Andy, Nerattini, Jett, Carlton, Zarate, Boneu, Fauci, Ajila, Battista, Pahlajani, Christos, Fink, Williams, Brinton and Mosconi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lisa Mosconi, bGltMjAzNUBtZWQuY29ybmVsbC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.