Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Endocrinol., 18 December 2023
Sec. Obesity
This article is part of the Research Topic Exercise, Diet, Cytokines and Obesity View all 8 articles

Cardiorespiratory fitness as a mediator between body fat rate and executive function in college students

Lina Zhu&#x;Lina Zhu1†Aihong He&#x;Aihong He2†Dandan ChenDandan Chen1Xiaoxiao DongXiaoxiao Dong1Xuan XiongXuan Xiong1Aiguo Chen,*Aiguo Chen1,3*
  • 1College of Physical Education, Yangzhou University, Yangzhou, Jiangsu, China
  • 2College of Physical Education, Yangzhou Polytechnic College, Yangzhou, Jiangsu, China
  • 3Nanjing Sport Institute, Nanjing, Jiangsu, China

Purpose: To examine whether body fat rate (BF%) is associated with cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) and whether cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) mediates the association between BF% and Executive function (EF) in young adults.

Methods: In this cross-sectional study, 226 college students were recruited from an university. Flanker, 2-back, and odder and shifting tasks were used to assess EF. The incremental cardiopulmonary exercise tests were performed, and maximal oxygen consumption was recorded during test. The body composition measuring instrument was used to evaluate the participants’ BF%.

Results: The BF% of college students was negatively correlated with each EF, BF% was negatively correlated with CRF, and CRF was negatively correlated with EF (P< 0.001). Structural equation modeling (SEM) and simultaneous analysis of several groups were used to construct mediator model. The CRF of college students plays a partial mediating role between BF% and EF, and the mediating effect accounts for 48.8% of the total effect value. Sex has no moderate effect on the relationship between BF%, CRF, and EF.

Conclusions: College students with high BF% can improve their CRF by strengthening physical exercise, thereby indirectly improving their EF. Therefore, college students who have a higher body fat percentage should be compensated for engaging in physical exercise in order to enhance their CRF and mitigate the detrimental effects of obesity and overweight on EF.

1 Introduction

In recent years, significant changes have occurred in people’s lifestyles, including their dietary habits, work routines, and rest patterns, due to the improvement of the economic level (1). However, daily physical activities and exercise have gradually decreased, leading to an increase in the incidence of obesity, which has become a serious social and public health issue.

Executive Function (EF) refers to the advanced cognitive processes that individuals use to control and adjust other cognitive processes when completing complex cognitive tasks, including inhibition, updating, and shifting (2, 3). As the core of an individual’s cognitive, emotional, and intellectual activities, EF plays a crucial role in learning behavior, physical and mental health, and the development of future achievements (4, 5). Therefore, investigating the factors that affect the development of individual EF has become a forefront research topic.

High body fat is one of the important characteristics of obese and overweight individuals, and it is also a significant risk factor that affects the healthy development of individual EFs. Currently, the potential biological mechanism of the relationship between obesity and overweight and executive dysfunction is not clear. However, a large number of clinical and animal model studies suggest that impaired regulatory function of insulin (6), glucose (7), increased systemic and central inflammation (8, 9), brain structural damage (10), and abnormal functional network activity (11) may be the potential mechanisms for excessive body fat to cause individuals to perform abnormal functions. Additionally, studies have suggested that levels of fat-related sex hormones may moderate the performance of EFs (1215). However, whether there are gender differences in the relationship between the two is still debated in the available evidence (15, 16), and further research is necessary. Studies have shown that high body fat in adulthood is associated with an increased risk of executive dysfunction and dementia in later age (1719).

High body fat has a detrimental impact on cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) in children and adolescents (2022). CRF, also known as aerobic fitness and aerobic capacity, is a reflection of an individual’s physical health status and exercise ability. It encompasses the ability of the cardiovascular circulation system, respiratory system, and muscle system to absorb and utilize oxygen to perform work during the continuous increase in physical activity load. Studies have shown that obese individuals who engage in regular physical exercise exhibit better metabolic function than those who do not, indicating that maintaining higher CRF reduces the risk of obesity-induced metabolic and cardiovascular diseases (23, 24). Furthermore, CRF is closely linked to the healthy development of EFs. A growing body of evidence suggests that adolescents with higher CRF exhibit more effective abilities to suppress non-task-related interference information, better performance in working memory accuracy, and more sensitive task switching (2530). Therefore, an individual’s CRF is affected by both body fat and EF, and body fat can directly affect EF performance. There is a pairwise correlation among the three. However, there is no study on the interaction among the three and whether there are sex differences in the relationship between the three. The 2014 report on the physical health of college students in China highlighted the rising rates of overweight and obesity among this population, as well as a decline in their physical fitness and cardiopulmonary function, resulting in various health, social, and psychological problems (31). Against this backdrop, this paper aims to explore the correlation between BF%, CRF, and EF of college students. Furthermore, a structural equation model is used to investigate the mediator effect of CRF between the percentage of body fat and EF, as well as gender differences. The findings of this paper will provide new evidence for fully understanding the relationship between obesity and EF of college students, as well as its potential influencing factors. Ultimately, this paper aims to lay a practical foundation for relevant departments to take preventive intervention measures to improve the physical and mental health of college students.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

A total of 226 college students, ranging in age from 18 to 22, were recruited and chosen from an university located in Yangzhou. The sample consisted of 118 male and 108 female participants. The selection criteria included being right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Test (32) and Han nationality. Additionally, individuals with abnormal mental health levels as evaluated by Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (33), a history of mental disorders or genetic diseases, poor current mental condition, severe physical illnesses, brain trauma or nervous system diseases, as well as drug or alcohol dependence were excluded. The experiment was conducted in Yangzhou, China, with approval from the Ethical and Human Protection Committee of the Affiliated Hospital of Yangzhou University (2017-YKL045-01). Participants signed an informed consent form. All study procedures were in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2 CRF test

The evaluation of individual CRF often relies on the measurement of maximum oxygen uptake (VO2max). In this particular study, a progressive load exercise program was implemented, wherein subjects underwent an incremental load exercise on a pedal power bicycle (Elmed EGT1000). The measurement of maximum oxygen uptake was conducted using a CORTEX METALYZER-II desktop gas metabolism analyzer, which is manufactured in Germany. The test results were then used to infer the individual’s CRF. Additionally, the change in heart rate during exercise was monitored using a POLAR heart rate band. Prior to commencing the experiment, all participants were mandated to complete the Physical Activity Preparation Questionnaire (PAR-Q) in order to ascertain their lack of prior exposure to vigorous exercise or consumption of substances that stimulate the nervous system within the 24 hours preceding the test. Additionally, all participants were engage in a preparatory activity lasting between 3 to 5 minutes to mitigate the risk of sports-related injuries. The baseline heart rate (HR) of each participant was recorded prior to the commencement of the test. The test officially commenced once the participants’ heart rates had returned to a state of rest. The initial power load of the power bicycle is set at 50 watts, and participants are required to maintain a rhythm of 55-60 revolutions per minute. Subsequently, the load is increased by 50 watts every 3 minutes until participants report exhaustion. VO2 max is considered to be reached if any of the following criteria are met: (a) Oxygen intake remains relatively stable or slightly decreases (1500 ml/min) with the increase in load on the power bicycle. (b) Respiratory quotient is equal to or greater than 1.1. (c) Maximum heart rate exceeds 180 beats per minute. (d) Despite repeated encouragement, participants are unable to sustain a bicycle speed of 50-60 revolutions per minute. Following the completion of the test, the primary pathway documented the measurement, and the participants engaged in a period of rest lasting approximately 5 minutes. Subsequently, the subjects departed after confirming the absence of any anomalous reactions. The computation of CRF entailed the multiplication of the participants’ VO2 max (L/min) by 1000, which was then divided by their weight (kg) to yield the relative maximum oxygen intake (ml/min/kg). This value serves as an indicator of the participants’ CRF.

2.3 BF% measurement

This study employed the Health Keeper-Xcan body composition measuring instrument, a human body composition measuring instrument produced by Bionet Company in South Korea, featuring 12 points of contact electrodes. The bioelectric impedance analysis and 6-frequency multiloop method were utilized to comprehensively assess various body components. Participants were instructed to hold a hand-held electrode and stand on the foot electrode for approximately 40 seconds to complete the test. Subsequently, immediate measurements of human body moisture, protein, muscle, and fat, as well as measurements of the left and right upper limbs and left and right lower limbs, were obtained. This study examines the criteria established by the World Health Organization (WHO) for evaluating obesity, as well as the commonly utilized criteria for determining overweight and obesity based on BF%.

2.4 Executive function test tasks

The computerized neuropsychological test battery, known as the assessment’s battery, comprises three tests designed to assess a range of abilities associated with EF. Previous studies have utilized the EF assessments battery, which can be administered in a group format. In the present study, we imposed a requirement for all participants to promptly press the button, while simultaneously ensuring accuracy. The determination of outcome parameters was based on reaction time (RT).

Inhibition performance was evaluated using a modified Erickson Flanker task, a task known for its sensitivity to the effects of both acute and chronic exercises (3436). The participants were provided with instructions to respond promptly and accurately to each trial by selecting the center letter in each array. Any incorrect responses, including pressing the wrong button or responding within 150 ms or timeout, were deemed as errors. Prior to the main task, participants were required to complete 12 practice trials. The main task consisted of two blocks, each comprising 48 trials, with a one-minute rest period between the blocks. The congruent and incongruent trials were presented in a random order, with an equal likelihood of occurrence in each block. The overall duration of the task was approximately six minutes. The calculation of the difference in response times involved subtracting the reaction times for the congruent trail from those of the incongruent trail. A smaller difference in reaction time and a higher accuracy rate indicated superior inhibition performance.

In a previous study conducted by our researcher group (37), the 2-back task was employed. The 2-back condition served as a working memory task. If a participant’s task performance fell below 60% accuracy (ACC), the same practice was repeated. The mean reaction time (RT) was used to evaluate the behavioral performance in this working memory (WM) task, with shorter RT indicating better WM performance.

More-odd shifting task was used to assessment the shifting. Participants were asked to switch back and forth between two different tasks that used the same numeric stimuli, which appeared in the center of the computer screen (38). Participants received three blocks of stimuli. The first two blocks were the homogenous conditions in which only one task was performed. The task contained two conditions. In one condition, participants determined whether the digit presented was greater or less than 5. In the other condition, participants determined whether the digit presented was odd or even. During each trial, a solid or dashed outline cue appeared simultaneously with the numeric stimuli instructing participants to make a decision (greater/less than 5, or odd/even). The two blocks were counterbalanced across participants. The third block consisted of the task-heterogeneous condition in which participants were required to switch between equiprobable task sets on some trials and repeatedly perform the same task over trials in other cases. The two tasks alternated randomly in the heterogeneous block, with a maximum of seven consecutive trials performed repeatedly for each task. Therefore, all trials in the heterogeneous block were categorized into either switch or non-switch conditions. White numeric stimuli were presented on a black background for 200 ms, with a 2000 ms interstimulus interval from stimulus offset to onset. Participants completed 50 and 256 trials in homogenous and heterogeneous conditions, respectively.

2.5 Statistical analysis

The data obtained in this paper were analyzed using SPSS25.0 statistical software, which facilitated the execution of independent sample t-tests and Pearson correlation analysis. The validation of the structural equation model was conducted using Amos 25.0, and further analysis was performed on the multi-group structural equation model. Additionally, the significance of the intermediate effect was assessed using the Bootstrap method, with a statistical significance level set at P< 0.05.

3 Results

3.1 Behavioral performance and correlated analysis

According to obesity and overweight criteria (39, 40), men are considered overweight if their BF% falls within the range of 20% to 25%, while a BF% exceeding 25% indicates obesity. For women, a BF% between 25% and 30% is classified as overweight, while a percentage exceeding 30% is indicative of obesity. The study findings reveal that out of the total sample, 37 men were classified as overweight, while 20 men were categorized as obese. Similarly, 36 women were identified as overweight, whereas 22 women were classified as obese.

Sex differences in BF%, CRF, and EF sub-functions RT of college students were analyzed by using an independent sample t-test. It was found that there were sex differences in VO2 max (P< 0.001), BF% (P< 0.001), CRF (P< 0.001), and updating RT (P = 0.017), BMI (P = 0.192), inhibition RT (P = 0.055), and Shifting RT (P = 0.088) (see Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Behavioral performances of BF%, CRF and EF sub-functions in college students.

Pearson correlation analysis showed that the BF% was positively correlated with the RT of each sub-function of EF; BF% is negatively correlated with CRF. CRF is negatively correlated with the RT of each sub-function of EF, and the correlation is statistically significant (see Table 2 and Figure 1).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Correlation coefficients of BF%, CRF and EF sub functions.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 (A–C) showed the correlations among BF% and EF sub- functions; (D) showed the correlation between CRF and BF%; (E–G) showed the correlations among CRF and EF sub- functions.

3.2 Mediating effect of CRF between BF% and EF

Structural equation modeling was used to establish a mediating model to test the role of mediating variables. The model included one latent variable, EF (taking inhibition, updating, and shifting as observation variables), and two explicit variables, BF% and CRF. The mediating effect was tested by the Bootstrap method of deviation correction. The fitting index of the model is χ2/df=1.252, GFI = 0. 992, NFI = 0. 961, CFI = 0. 993, RMSE = 0. 033, which indicates that the model fits well. BF% and CRF accounted for 48.8% of the variation in EF (R2 = 0.488). The mediation model path is shown in Figure 2. The total effect of BF% on EF was 0.584 [P< 0.001, 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.420 ~ 0.757], and the direct effect was 0. 384 (P<0.001, 95% CI: 0.195 ~ 0.583), and the mediating effect was 0.201 (P< 0.001, 95% CI: 0.120 ~ 0.302), accounting for 34.41% of the total effect. Therefore, the BF% not only directly affects the EF of college students but also indirectly affects it by reducing their CRF; that is, CRF plays a partial mediating role between the BF% and EF.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 The Mediation Effect of CRF between BF% and EF in College Students.*** means P < 0.001. The numerical values in the figure represent normalized path coefficients (direct effects).

3.3 Moderating effect of sex in the relationship among BF%, CRF and EF

Multi-group analysis was used to compare the relationship between EF, CRF, and BF% among different sex college students. When all the path coefficients were equal for different sex, the model fitted well. Compared with the unrestricted model, Δχ2/df (12.985) and Δdf (11) had no statistical significance (P= 0.294>0.05). The two models can be regarded as equal models (see Table 3). The mediating model has identity among different sex, which indicates that sex has no moderating effect on the relationship between BF%, CRF, and EF in college students.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Multiple group structural equation model fitting indicators.

4 Discussion

4.1 Correlations among BF%, CRF and EF in college students

This study revealed a significant disparity in the BF% between males and females, with males exhibiting lower levels. Conversely, no notable discrepancy in BMI by sex was observed, indicating incongruity between BMI and BF% as measures of obesity. This finding aligns with previous research, which similarly demonstrated limited agreement between BMI and BF% in assessing obesity among college students. Consequently, it is recommended that a comprehensive approach be adopted in practical settings to effectively identify and prevent obesity among college students. Nonetheless, the author acknowledges a limitation in the study. However, the author also highlights that the assessment of obesity relies on the measurement of BF%. By excluding the weight of muscle and other tissues, this measurement effectively discerns whether the increase in body mass is attributable to fat or muscle and other tissues, thereby providing a more precise reflection of the overall degree of obesity in individuals (39). Furthermore, this study additionally establishes the existence of sex disparities in CRF, with males exhibiting higher levels compared to females. The findings of this study confirm previous research indicating that men possess inherent advantages over women in terms of the oxygen transport system and muscle tissue content, resulting in superior performance in CRF (41). Furthermore, within the scope of EF analysis in college students, it is noteworthy that only the updating sub-function displays notable sex disparities, wherein males exhibit superior overall performance in comparison to females. Prior research has indicated that females tend to exhibit advantages in inhibition and shifting prior to reaching puberty, yet this sex disparity gradually diminishes after the onset of puberty (42). Voyer et al. discovered that the impact of sex differences on visuospatial working memory is contingent upon age and the specific evaluation tasks employed (43). Nevertheless, men consistently exhibit superior performance in visuospatial working memory. While certain studies have failed to identify any significant sex disparities in updating abilities, males consistently outperform females in this domain (15). Consequently, it is evident that sex exerts distinct effects on the inhibition, updating, and shifting aspects of EF among college students.

4.2 The mediating role of CRF in the relationship between BF% and EF

The present study revealed a negative association between the BF% and CRF, as well as a positive association between the BF% and sub-functional RT performances of EF. Additionally, CRF was found to negatively predict sub-functional RT performances of EF. This finding aligns with Rauner et al.’s (2013) systematic review, which highlighted the inverse relationship between fat content resulting from overweight or obesity and CRF (44). Previous research has established that overweight or obesity significantly impacts both CRF and EF (45, 46), a finding that aligns with our own study results. Hence, our findings corroborate the established association between BF%, CRF, and EF as observed in prior research. Specifically, our results indicate that higher BF% in college students are linked to poorer CRF levels. Moreover, elevated BF% are associated with diminished performance across various sub-functions of EF. Conversely, higher levels of CRF are associated with improved performance in each sub-function of EF. The relationship between CRF and the performance of individual sub-functions of EF is positively correlated, with higher levels of CRF associated with improved performance. While previous research has examined the association between BF%, CRF, and EF, this study is the first to investigate the relationship between the three sub-functions of EF, BF%, and CRF specifically in college students.

This study employed structural equation modeling to establish a well-fitting mediating effect model, examining the relationship between BF%, CRF, and EF. The findings revealed that CRF partially mediated the association between BF% and EF in college students. This suggests that the impact of body fat on EF is not only direct but also indirect, mediated by CRF. Furthermore, the present study discovered that sex did not exert any influence on the association between BF%, CRF, and EF, but failure to detect a difference does not establish the absence of a sex difference. It is worth noting that elevated BF% are considered significant indicators of both obesity and overweight, and the detrimental impact of obesity on cognitive function has been substantiated across various age groups (4749). Meanwhile, an inverse relationship between EFs and % body fat cannot be disregarded. Obese individuals exhibit traits such as a strong inclination toward immediate gratification in their pursuit of appetizing food, as well as a limited ability to regulate this motivational inclination, resulting in challenges when confronted with external stimuli promoting palatable food (50). Recently observation has also posited that a higher baseline EF capacity may be linked to a more advantageous trajectory in terms of BMI and waist circumference. Conversely, a low EF at follow-up is correlated with obesity and increased uncontrolled eating (51). Consequently, identifying individuals with low EF could prove beneficial in identifying adolescents who are at risk of obesity at earlier stages. This supplementary information substantiates the importance of considering cognitive training strategies when making decisions regarding interventions for weight loss.

Currently, numerous studies have corroborated the beneficial influence of CRF on EF. For instance, Themanson (2006) discovered that young adults in the high fitness group exhibited superior monitoring skills for irrelevant and erroneous information compared to their low fitness counterparts, indicating enhanced inhibition abilities (52). According to Scott (2016), there exists a positive correlation between CRF levels and the efficacy of neural functioning within the frontal-parietal lobe network, ultimately leading to improved updating performance (28). Additionally, both Scisco (2008) and Themanson (2008) discovered that young adults belonging to the high fitness group exhibited superior shifting abilities compared to their counterparts in the low fitness group (27, 53). Neuroimaging studies have indicated that individuals who are overweight or obese exhibit notable alterations in gray matter structure and white matter fiber density, resulting in diminished cognitive performance compared to those with normal body fat levels (11, 54, 55). CRF serves as a mediating variable influencing the relationship between obesity and academic achievement (53, 54). In recent academic discourse, the notion of “obesity but health” has been introduced by researchers. Notably, the Sardinha study revealed that adolescents categorized as having high CRF but normal weight, as well as those with high CRF but overweight or obesity, exhibit superior academic performance when compared to their counterparts with low CRF (56, 57). Consequently, this study posits that the substantial proportion of body fat could potentially exert a detrimental influence on the EF of children and adolescents; however, the presence of enhanced CRF may mitigate or diminish this adverse effect.

There are limitations on interpreting the causality of the mediated moderation effect model because our study used the cross-sectional study design. Although previous evidence supports a rapid decline tendency of EF in obesity, we cannot conclude any causal effects of an inactive lifestyle on the neuropsychological processes observed in the current study. These findings can potentially serve as an initial step for future longitudinal and interventional investigations, aiming to provide additional insights into the temporal association between obesity and intricate cognitive processing in the developing brain, as well as to establish a cause-and-effect relationship. Another important consideration is that there may be additional factors, not examined in this study, that could potentially contribute to the observed relationship. Although we excluded participants with physical activity contraindications, these obesity individuals allocate a greater amount of time to the utilization of electronic devices and engagement in professional courses, it is possible that at least some of the included participants had ongoing pathological processes that are still not symptomatically expressed and therefore they were not aware of.

5 Conclusion

CRF plays a crucial role in mediating the relationship between BF% and EF. It is suggested that CRF may determine the adverse impact of BF% on EF. Therefore, for college students with high body fat or obesity, it is imperative for colleges and universities to focus on reforming the physical education curriculum while also emphasizing the training and promotion of CRF in the curriculum design. This approach can effectively mitigate the negative impact of overweight and obesity on college students’ EF.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Affiliated Hospital of Yangzhou University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AH: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DC: Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. XD: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing. XX: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AC: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was partially supported by grants awarded to Aiguo Chen from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31771243). Additionally, it was partially supported by grants awarded to LZ from the Jiangsu Funding Program for Excellent Postdoctoral Talent (2022M722696) and the Social Science Foundation of Jiangsu Province (22TYD015).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all our participants in Yangzhou University for their contributions and support in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Worldwide trends in insufficient physical activity from 2001 to 2016: a pooled analysis of 358 population-based surveys with 1·9 million participants. Lancet Global Health (2018) 6:e1077–86. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30357-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Funahashi S. Neuronal mechanisms of executive control by the prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Res (2001) 39:147–65. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00224-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Crone EA, Peters S, Steinbeis N. Executive function development in adolescence. In: Executive function: Development across the life span. New York: Routledge (2017).

Google Scholar

6. Nameni G, Farhangi MA, Hajiluian G, Shahabi P, Abbasi MM. Insulin deficiency: A possible link between obesity and cognitive function. Int J Dev Neurosci Off J Int Soc Dev Neurosci (2017) 59:15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2017.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Strachan MW, Reynolds RM, Marioni RE, Price JF. Cognitive function, dementia and type 2 diabetes mellitus in the elderly. Nat Rev Endocrinol (2011) 7:108–14. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2010.228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Walker KA, Gottesman RF, Wu A, Knopman DS, Gross AL, Mosley TH, et al. Systemic inflammation during midlife and cognitive change over 20 years: The ARIC Study. Neurology (2019) 92:e1256–67. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Cunningham C, Campion S, Lunnon K, Murray CL, Woods JFC, Deacon RMJ, et al. Systemic inflammation induces acute behavioral and cognitive changes and accelerates neurodegenerative disease. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 65:304–12. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Weise CM, Thiyyagura P, Reiman EM, Chen K, Krakoff J. Fat-Free Body Mass but not Fat Mass is Associated with Reduced Gray Matter Volume of Cortical Brain Regions Implicated in Autonomic and Homeostatic Regulation. NeuroImage (2012) 64:712–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Figley CR, Asem JS, Levenbaum EL, Courtney SM. Effects of body mass index and body fat percent on default mode, executive control, and salience network structure and function. Front Neurosci (2016) 10:234. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Cornblath EJ, Tang E, Baum GL, Moore TM, Adebimpe A, Roalf DR, et al. Sex differences in network controllability as a predictor of executive function in youth. NeuroImage (2019) 188:122–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Darcey VL, McQuaid GA, Fishbein DH, VanMeter JW. Relationship between whole blood omega-3 fatty acid levels and dorsal cingulate gray matter volume: Sex differences and implications for impulse control. Nutr Neurosci (2020) 23:505–15. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2018.1525477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Laurent JS, Watts R, Adise S, Allgaier N, Chaarani B, Garavan H, et al. Associations among body mass index, cortical thickness, and executive function in children. JAMA Pediatr (2020) 174(2):170–7. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Solianik R, Brazaitis M, Skurvydas A. Sex-related differences in attention and memory. Medicina (Kaunas Lithuania) (2016) 52:372–7. doi: 10.1016/j.medici.2016.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Grissom NM, Reyes TM. Let’s call the whole thing off: evaluating gender and sex differences in executive function. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol (2019) 44:86–96. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0179-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Budge M, Young J. Body mass index in midlife and late-life as a risk factor for dementia: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Obes Rev an Off J Int Assoc Study Obes (2011) 12:e426–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00825.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kiliaan AJ, Arnoldussen IA, Gustafson DR. Adipokines: a link between obesity and dementia? Lancet Neurol (2014) 13:913–23. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70085-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kivimäki M, Luukkonen R, Batty GD, Ferrie JE, Pentti J, Nyberg ST, et al. Body mass index and risk of dementia: Analysis of individual-level data from 1.3 million individuals. Alzheimer’s Dementia (2018) 14:601–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bürgi F, Meyer U, Granacher U, Schindler C, Marques-Vidal P, Kriemler S, et al. Relationship of physical activity with motor skills, aerobic fitness and body fat in preschool children: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study (Ballabeina). Int J Obes (2005) (2011) 35:937–44. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ekelund U, Poortvliet E, Nilsson A, Yngve A, Holmberg A, Sjöström M. Physical activity in relation to aerobic fitness and body fat in 14- to 15-year-old boys and girls. Eur J Appl Physiol (2001) 85:195–201. doi: 10.1007/s004210100460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Michalsky MP, Inge TH, Simmons M, Jenkins TM, Buncher R, Helmrath M, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors in severely obese adolescents: the teen longitudinal assessment of bariatric surgery (Teen-LABS) study. JAMA Pediatr (2015) 169:438–44. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wang P-G, Gong J, Wang S-Q, Talbott EO, Zhang B, He Q-Q. Relationship of body fat and cardiorespiratory fitness with cardiovascular risk in Chinese children. PloS One (2011) 6:e27896. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Weltman A, Weltman JY, Hartman ML, Abbott RD, Rogol AD, Evans WS, et al. Relationship between age, percentage body fat, fitness, and 24-hour growth hormone release in healthy young adults: effects of gender. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1994) 78:543–8. doi: 10.1210/jcem.78.3.8126124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Cancela J, Burgo H, Sande E. Physical fitness and executive functions in adolescents: cross-sectional associations with academic achievement. J Phys Ther Sci (2019) 31:556–62. doi: 10.1589/jpts.31.556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Åberg MA, Pedersen NL, Torén K, Svartengren M, Bäckstrand B, Johnsson T, et al. Cardiovascular fitness is associated with cognition in young adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci United States America (2009) 106:20906–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905307106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Scisco JL, Leynes PA, Kang J. Cardiovascular fitness and executive control during task-switching: an ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol Off J Int Organ Psychophysiol (2008) 69:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Scott SP, de Souza MJ, Koehler K, Petkus DL, Murray-Kolb LE. Cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with better executive function in young women. Med Sci sports Exercise (2016) 48:1994–2002. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Westfall DR, Gejl AK, Tarp J, Wedderkopp N, Kramer AF, Hillman CH, et al. Associations between aerobic fitness and cognitive control in adolescents. Front Psychol (2018) 9:1298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hogan MJ, O’Hora D, Kiefer M, Kubesch S, Kilmartin L, Collins P, et al. The effects of cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise on executive functioning and EEG entropy in adolescents. Front Hum Neurosci (2015) 9:538. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Tian Y, Jiang C, Wang M, Cai R, Zhang Y, He Z, et al. BMI, leisure-time physical activity, and physical fitness in adults in China: results from a series of national surveys, 2000–14. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol (2016) 4:487–97. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(16)00081-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Derogatis LR, Unger R. Symptom checklist-90-revised. In: Weiner IB, Craighead WE, editors. The Corsini encyclopedia of psychology, vol. . p . Hoboken NJ: Wiley (2010). p. 1–2.

Google Scholar

34. Chen A-G, Yin H-C, Yan J, Yang Yu. Effects of acute aerobic exercise of different intensity on executive function. Acta Psychologica Sin (2011) 43:1055–62. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2011.01055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Chen A-G, Yan J, Yin H-C, Pan C-Y, Chang Y-K. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on multiple aspects of executive function in preadolescent children. Psychol Sport Exercise (2014) 15:627–36. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Zhu L, Yu Q, Herold F, Cheval B, Dong X, Cui L, et al. Brain structure, cardiorespiratory fitness, and executive control changes after a 9-week exercise intervention in young adults: A randomized controlled trial. Life-Basel (2021) 11:292. doi: 10.3390/life11040292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chen A-G, Zhu L-N, Yan J, Yin H-C. Neural Basis of Working Memory Enhancement after Acute Aerobic Exercise: fMRI Study of Preadolescent Children. Front Psychol (2016) 7:1804. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Shi Y, Cai K, Zhu H, Dong X, Xiong X, Zhu L, et al. Football juggling learning alters the working memory and white matter integrity in early adulthood: A randomized controlled study. Appl Sci (2021) 11:3843. doi: 10.3390/app11093843

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Zhu W, Tian H, Zhang R. Comparison of body mass index, waist-hip ratio and body fat ratio in assessing obesity among college students. Chin J School Health (2019) 40:1703–1705 + 1708.

Google Scholar

40. Shi M, Zhu Y-B. Research progress on the diagnostic criteria of obesity and its clinical application. Food Nutr China (2014) 20:76–80.

Google Scholar

41. Mota J, Guerra S, Leandro C, Pinto A, Ribeiro JC, Duarte JA. Association of maturation, sex, and body fat in cardiorespiratory fitness. Am J Hum Biol Off J Hum Biol Council (2002) 14:707–12. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.10086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hyde JS. Sex and cognition: gender and cognitive functions. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2016) 38:53–6. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Voyer D, Voyer SD, Saint-Aubin J. Sex differences in visual-spatial working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bull Rev (2017) 24:307–34. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1085-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Rauner A, Mess F, Woll A. The relationship between physical activity, physical fitness and overweight in adolescents: a systematic review of studies published in or after 2000. BMC Pediatr (2013) 13:19. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-13-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Ronan L, Alexander-Bloch A, Fletcher PC. Childhood obesity, cortical structure, and executive function in healthy children. Cereb Cortex (New York N.Y. 1991) (2019) 30(4):2519–28. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Esteban-Cornejo I, Martinez-Gomez D, Gómez-Martínez S, del Campo-Vecino J, Fernández-Santos J, Castro-Piñero J, et al. Inflammatory biomarkers and academic performance in youth. The UP & DOWN Study. Brain behavior Immun (2016) 54:122–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Schwartz DH, Leonard G, Perron M, Richer L, Syme C, Veillette S, et al. Visceral fat is associated with lower executive functioning in adolescents. Int J Obes (2005) (2013) 37:1336–43. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Baumgartner NW, Walk AM, Edwards CG, Covello AR, Chojnacki MR, Reeser GE, et al. Relationship between physical activity, adiposity, and attentional inhibition. J Phys activity Health (2018) 15:191–6. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2017-0181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bove RM, Gerweck AV, Mancuso SM, Bredella MA, Sherman JC, Miller KK. Association between adiposity and cognitive function in young men: Hormonal mechanisms. Obes (Silver Spring Md.) (2016) 24(4):954–61. doi: 10.1002/oby.21415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol J normal Abnormal Dev childhood adolescence (2002) 8:71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Mayer MA, Catalani F, Fraire J, Deltetto N, Martín L, Beneitez A, et al. Inhibitory control and obesity in adolescents: A prospective cohort study. Appetite (2022) 171:105910. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2022.105910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Themanson JR, Hillman CH. Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action monitoring. Neuroscience (2006) 141:757–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Themanson JR, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH. Fitness and action monitoring: evidence for improved cognitive flexibility in young adults. Neuroscience (2008) 157:319–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Maayan L, Hoogendoorn C, Sweat V, Convit A. Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obes (Silver Spring Md.) (2011) 19:1382–7. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mujica-Parodi LR, Renelique R, Taylor MK. Higher body fat percentage is associated with increased cortisol reactivity and impaired cognitive resilience in response to acute emotional stress. Int J Obes (2005) (2009) 33:157–65. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Sardinha LB, Marques A, Martins S, Palmeira A, Minderico C. Fitness, fatness, and academic performance in seventh-grade elementary school students. BMC Pediatr (2014) 14:176. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-14-176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sardinha LB, Marques A, Minderico C, Palmeira A, Martins S, Santos D, et al. Longitudinal relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness and academic achievement. Med Sci sports Exercise (2016) 48:839–44. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: body fat rate, cardiorespiratory fitness, executive function, young adult, mediation effect

Citation: Zhu L, He A, Chen D, Dong X, Xiong X and Chen A (2023) Cardiorespiratory fitness as a mediator between body fat rate and executive function in college students. Front. Endocrinol. 14:1293388. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2023.1293388

Received: 13 September 2023; Accepted: 04 December 2023;
Published: 18 December 2023.

Edited by:

Junhao Huang, Guangzhou Sport University, China

Reviewed by:

Marta Sevilla-Sanchez, University of A Coruña, Spain
Bojan Masanovic, University of Montenegro, Montenegro
Marcos Mayer, CESIM Foundation, Argentina

Copyright © 2023 Zhu, He, Chen, Dong, Xiong and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aiguo Chen, YWdjaGVuQHl6dS5lZHUuY24=

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.