Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Endocrinol., 10 January 2024
Sec. Experimental Endocrinology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuropeptide Actions in Arthropod Biology View all 7 articles

Neural mechanisms involved in female mate choice in invertebrates

  • 1Laboratorio de Ecología Evolutiva. Escuela Nacional de Estudios Superiores. Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, Mexico
  • 2Departamento de Biología Celular y Fisiología, Instituto de Investigaciones Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, Mexico
  • 3Institute for Evolution and Biodiversity, University of Münster, Münster, Germany

Mate choice is a critical decision with direct implications for fitness. Although it has been recognized for over 150 years, our understanding of its underlying mechanisms is still limited. Most studies on mate choice focus on the evolutionary causes of behavior, with less attention given to the physiological and molecular mechanisms involved. This is especially true for invertebrates, where research on mate choice has largely focused on male behavior. This review summarizes the current state of knowledge on the neural, molecular and neurohormonal mechanisms of female choice in invertebrates, including behaviors before, during, and after copulation. We identify areas of research that have not been extensively explored in invertebrates, suggesting potential directions for future investigation. We hope that this review will stimulate further research in this area.

1 Introduction

Mate choice is one of the most critical decisions organisms make because of its direct impact on their fitness. Mate choice is an evolutionary process that often favors the evolution of conspicuous traits in individuals (1). These traits, called secondary sexual characters (SSCs), include structures, colors, odors, and behavior (Box 1). Mate choice occurs when the evolution of SSCs in one sex leads to non-random mating with members of the opposite sex based on those characters (7, 8). Although mate choice was recognized over 150 years ago (3), many gaps exist in our understanding of its underlying mechanisms. For example, most studies on mate choice focus on the evolutionary causes of behavior, with less attention given to the physiological and molecular mechanisms involved (9, 10). Furthermore, although female choice is recognized as the more frequent type of mate choice (1, 11, 12), there have been more studies focusing on male behavior during the process of mate choice compared to female behavior (13). This bias may exist because male behaviors tend to be more conspicuous than female behaviors, resulting in a misinterpretation of females in a passive role in sexual selection. However, it is important to understand that the information conveyed by males through their behavior is interpreted within the female’s nervous system (14, 15) (Figure 1). Studying how female brains work during mate choice is crucial for fully understanding these behaviors from mechanism to evolutionary consequences.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 General description of neural pathways during female choice in insects. (A) Pre-copulation: Female mate choice is influenced by gene expression cascades that respond to stimuli from both the potential mate (e.g., sounds, colors, structures, pheromones) and the environment. The process begins with neural pathways that carry information from the sensory organs to integration points in the corresponding brain areas. There, synaptic plasticity genes activate primarily to produce suitable, plastic, and immediate responses based on the intensity of the signals presented by males (16, 17). If appropriate, such neural activity will prompt arousal, motivation, and reward systems mainly modulated by neurotransmitters (18). Furthermore, all the mentioned steps are subject to epigenetic regulation, while simultaneously being able to influence the same epigenetic regulators. Finally, the neural responses will result in behaviors that favor mating. (B) Copulation: During copulation, mechanical stimulation from the aedeagus (or pennis-like structure) intrusion stimulates abdominal neurons that carry the signal and triggers neural release of neurotransmitters involved in reward systems (1820), such as dopamine (21). Motivation and reward systems facilitate sexual receptivity and copulation. The mechanical stimulus could inhibit re-mating through muscle inhibitor peptides or by blocking rewarding systems. In certain species, neurotransmitters may modulate pair bonding and memory formation mediated by oxytocin-like peptides in the brain (22). Changes in brain areas responsible for memory could trigger partner preference but may also lead to rejection of less attractive males or to former mates in the case of polyandrous females. In addition to mechanic stimulation, male ejaculate contains peptides that have the potential to act in the brain and release other neurotransmitters (23, 24). Other seminal peptides also have effects on the neural control of the female reproductive tract. Some of the neural process occurring during copula may be regulated by epigenetic enzymes, such as methyltransferases (17). (C) Post-copulation: Following copulation, additional mechanisms enable females to bias the paternity of their offspring or counteract male manipulation prior to or during copulation (25). Because the neural mechanisms of this stage are not well understood, a general description of what occurs in this stage is not yet possible. However, some works provide insights into the neural mechanisms of certain female behaviors. For example, hormones such as Diuretic hormone 44 in Drosophila, responsible for controlling neurons in the muscle of the female tract to control sperm retention or dump (26). Octopamine and octopamine receptors located in the decision-making brain areas of insects are required for ovulation, egg laying and muscular contraction of spermathecae (2729). Vitellogenin is required for egg production and maturation, but is also located in the brain of social insects during reproduction and mate choice (17, 30). Females bias fertilization through sperm activation and/or deactivation, for example by glandular secretions that preserve the stored sperm (31) or with spermicidal action (32), it is unknown whether females exercise glandular control via neural actions from decision-making areas. Created with BioRender.com.

Box 1. Sexual selection.
Darwin described sexual selection as “a struggle not for existence concerning other organic beings or external conditions, but a struggle between individuals of the same sex, usually males, for possession of the other sex” [(2), p. 59]. The result of this fight is not death but the lack of offspring of individuals. This implies that males with the most exaggerated secondary sexual characteristics (SSCs) (coloration, plumage, antlers, pheromones, etc.) will be favored by mate choice to leave more progeny. Sexual selection has three components. The first two, mate competition and mate choice were described by Darwin (3). The third, sexual conflict between mating pairs, occurs when males and females compete with their partners to obtain an optimum between the costs and benefits of mating and reproducing, with this optimum differing between the sexes (46).

While a growing number of studies in vertebrates reveal the neural mechanisms involved in the process of selecting a mate, our comprehension of the neural mechanisms of mate choice in invertebrates remains limited. DeAngelis and Hofmann reviewed the most relevant works in vertebrates on the neural and molecular mechanisms of female choice, focusing mainly on decision-making (9). Although such studies in invertebrates are limited, the existing research provides an opportunity to explore several important research avenues. 1) What are the specific locations and mechanisms within the nervous system that govern the intricate process of mate choice decision-making? and where in the brain does the process of mate choice occur: before, during, or after mating? 2) Given the diversity of mating strategies invertebrates possess (33, 34), is there a corresponding diversity in their neural mechanisms, or do a few mechanisms control such diversity? 3) Are the neural mechanisms underlying behaviors that are less explored or absent in invertebrates, such as selective use of stored sperm or sexual cannibalism, controlled by similar mechanisms to better studied behaviors in vertebrates? 4) The mechanistic understanding of mate choice behavior in invertebrates may provide hints to their origin and evolution, and this knowledge may be important for understanding the variety of ways that such behaviors can occur and may additionally allow a better understanding of the evolution and mechanisms of behaviors that we observe in vertebrates.

Here, we review the neural mechanisms of female choice in invertebrates during mate choice. We specifically include works that focus on the role of genes, molecules (such as neurotransmitters, hormones, and proteins), and neurons that participate in female choice. We consider female choice to be a behavior that contributes to non-random mating and/or fertilization, including behaviors before, during, and after copulation. Furthermore, we identify areas of research that have not been extensively explored in invertebrates, suggesting potential directions for future investigation.

2 Precopulatory neural mechanisms

A crucial early phase in mate choice involves the accurate perception of signals from a potential mate, with a primary function being the identification of the sender as a member of the opposite sex (1). The mechanisms behind this stage will depend mainly on the nature of the signals and the type of organs and sensory cells used to receive the stimulus, e.g. chemoreceptors in the antennae of fruit flies (35, 36), the cuticle of nematodes (37, 38) or the ventral mesosoma of scorpions (39); the mechanoreceptors of Drosophila (40), crickets (41) and arachnids (39, 42); UV sensitive and other wavelength photoreceptors in odonates (43) and spiders (44, 45). The type of male signals will determine the neurobiological pathway that will carry this information to the integration centers in the female brain (Box 1) (46, 47). After the individual has been identified as a male of the same species, the female will be able to discriminate between potential mates.

2.1 Sexual receptivity

Before choosing a male, the sexual status of the female determines her receptivity to copulation. A receptive female often displays a series of behaviors facilitating reproduction (48), which may include the release of chemical signals, movements toward males, or ovipositor extrusion. The neural mechanisms for sexual receptivity have been studied in Drosophila melanogaster. When immature virgin or recently mated D. melanogaster females are not receptive, they do not respond to the male’s courtship and instead avoid him by performing specific stereotypical movements: immature virgins curl their abdomen tip downwards, a behavior referred to as “curling”; mated females and immature virgins engage in a behavior called “decamping”, which involves running, jumping, or flying away; and mature females and immature virgins kick backward, a behavior termed “kicking”. (49) When receptive, however, they pause their movements and interact with male partners to copulate, a behavior referred to as “pausing” (18, 49). Two primary genes regulate D. melanogaster’s receptivity, doublesex (dsx) and fruitless (fru). Both are transcription factors responsible for differences in male and female sexual behaviors, and these differences are also observed in the neuronal circuits (5053). The description of the neural circuitry of sexual receptivity in Drosophila has allowed the identification of dsx- and fru-expressing neurons, dendritic projections, and connection sites in the brain and reproductive tract (18, 52, 53). Zhou et al. found that the silencing of dsx-expressing neurons reduced female receptivity (53). In addition to dsx, Bussell et al. showed that the transcription factor Abdominal-B (Abd-B) regulates the female behavior of pausing in front of courting males and interacting with them to copulate (18). Abd-B is present in a group of neurons in the abdominal ganglion with projections to the brain and reproductive tract, which suggests that their function is to integrate the sensory information of courtship and output the observed motor activity, such as “pausing” (18). The nervous system of females can be altered by peptides produced by themselves, but also by those transferred by males in the spermatic fluid (23). As an example, the sex peptide (SP) is originated in the male accessory glands and transferred during insemination to females. SP elicits alterations in neural activity that reduce receptivity (23). Within the female’s reproductive tract, sensory neurons responsive to sex peptide detect its presence and convey the signal to the SP-abdominal ganglia neurons and to myoinhibitory peptide interneurons. Concurrently, SP suppresses the activity of serotonergic projection neurons, leading to a decline in female receptivity (5457).

2.2 Neural response to male courtship

The neural circuits of sexual behavior in invertebrates have been primarily studied in Drosophila. Immonen and Ritchie identified differences in gene expression in Drosophila simulans female brains exposed to courtship songs by conspecific and heterospecific males (36). They identified several antennal signaling genes that are part of neuropeptide signaling pathways, including the neuropeptide Corazonin (Crz) (36) which is involved in dopamine regulation and modulates female sexual receptivity in Drosophila (21). In addition, they found that exposure to courtship songs influences the expression of genes involved in chemical communication, such as odorant receptor and co-receptor genes and odorant-binding protein-coding genes, most of which are involved in binding pheromones (36). These findings suggest that male courtship sensorial stimulation facilitates receptivity in females by enhancing their sensitivity to male chemical signals.

Female response to courtship may change after mating, as evidenced by olfactometry studies that demonstrate a decreased response to male olfactory cues in mated females of Anastrepha fruit flies compared to their unmated counterparts (58). While the neural pathways governing female responses to male courtship have been extensively elucidated in Drosophila, studies on other invertebrate species have identified specific components involved in responding to various types of male signals, including visual and vibrational stimuli (conveyed through leg and pedipalp drumming) in the courtship of Schizocosa wolf spiders (42). Notably, Schizocosa females possess vibration receptor organs in their legs comprised of multiple slit sensilla, each innervated by two neurons (42). Research by Knowlton and Gaffin (42) revealed that neurons in all sensilla, both proximal and distal, responded to leg drumming, with proximal sensilla exhibiting a higher response to pedipalpal drumming. It’s worth noting that pedipalps are also employed for sperm transfer. Therefore, the diverse responses to vibrational signals from different sources provide clues into the precision and sensitivity involved in detecting signals during courtship. Studying the nervous system during courtship not only enhances our understanding of female responses but also helps determine more precisely which male signals elicit a response in the female. Recent evidence suggests that, during courtship, the leg movements of male Schizocosa retrorsa induce air particle movements, which females respond to during mate choice, rather than responding solely to the visual stimulus (59).

2.3 Neural responses during mate evaluation

Experience with different males causes changes in female brain activity and affects how they choose a partner. Experience and exposure to males with variation in their SSCs play an essential role in how females choose in future encounters. In flies and crickets, it has been observed that females increase the acceptance threshold of potential partners according to the type of males with whom they have had previous social experiences (60, 61). Although the mechanisms behind the increase in choice threshold remain unknown, one suggestion is that Drosophila females become more selective after mating because copulation increases their Juvenile Hormone (JH) levels, which reduces olfactory sensitivity of Or47b odorant receptor neurons that sense the male sex pheromone (62). Therefore, in a circumstance where a previously mated female encounters more than one male, the chosen male will be the one that produces enough sex pheromones to surpass the threshold, which could lead to strong intra and intersexual competition.

Sensory modalities in mate choice may vary across species. However, other processes involved in information integration and neural response modulation may be evolutionarily conserved. For instance, the process of mate choice in vertebrates includes alterations in gene expression related to neural plasticity (16, 63), along with epigenetic control (64). Hernandez-Villanueva et al. found similar responses in the brain of Tenebrio molitor females, consisting in an increase in proteins related to synaptic plasticity and changes in the levels of methyltransferases and subunits of the methyltransferase complexes when they evaluate and mate with males of different phenotypes (17).

2.4 Attention-directed behaviors of sexual signals from various males

During female choice, females receive male signals from various potential partners and should differentiate one signal from another to choose the best option, that is, the signal that favors their fitness. This could be challenging in systems where several males court females simultaneously. In these cases, the female must be able to differentiate the signals of potential partners and focus her attention only on one male while ignoring the others. In crickets (Mecopoda elongata), when females receive acoustic signals from several males that give similar calls at brief time intervals, they prefer the leading male, who generally sets the interval rate that other males follow (65). Omega neuron 1 (ON1) neurons are involved in perceiving acoustic signals and receive excitatory monosynaptic connections from ipsilateral afferent sensory cells. ON1 neurons also have inhibitory activity on their contralateral connections (41). When females receive identical signals from two males in opposite directions, the ON1 neurons that receive the signal from the leading male are excited and simultaneously inhibit the contralateral ON1 neurons, preventing them from perceiving the other males’ acoustic signals (41). This raises the question of how the female brain discerns between males in species in which males court females as a group (lek) and how these mechanisms vary in subsequent encounters.

3 Copulatory neural mechanisms

A species’ mating system can be classified as either monogamous or polygamous (Box 2) (66), which may differ in their copulatory mechanisms. However, there is currently a gap in our knowledge, as molecular studies investigating gene expression and neural changes in the female brain of invertebrates with different mating systems are notably lacking, and potential behavioral disparities between these mating systems may be rooted in variations in the neural mechanisms that drive frequent mating with one or more partners. This information is also essential for determining whether these mechanisms are analogous to those found in vertebrates [e.g. (69) and (70)]. Furthermore, females may influence the outcome of copulation in several ways, which may vary by mating system. Females may respond positively or negatively to copulatory stimulation, they may control the duration of copulation (71) or may bias sperm utilization by favoring certain males (72, 73).

Box 2. Mating systems.
A mating system refers to the way in which individuals within species establish and maintain sexual pairs, as well as how fertilizations are achieved and which individuals are involved (66, 67). These systems are frequently influenced by a range of factors, encompassing ecological, social, and evolutionary aspects. Studying mating systems is invaluable for comprehending the reproductive strategies of diverse animal species and the dynamics of their populations. In the animal kingdom, various mating systems are observed, and they can be broadly classified into the following main types (6668):

Monogamy: In a monogamous mating system, a single male and a single female form a long-term (often lifelong) bond. Monogamy ensures that both parents participate in the care and protection of their offspring. When females exhibit this behavior, it is referred to as monandry.

Polygamy: Polygamy is a mating system where both sexes mates with multiple individuals of the opposite sex. When females display this type of behavior, the term used is polyandry.

Promiscuity: In a promiscuous mating system, individuals have multiple, often brief, sexual encounters with multiple partners, without forming long-term bonds. This is common in many species, including many insects and some mammals.

3.1 Responses to stimulation during copulation

In vertebrates, there is ample evidence that mechanical stimuli received during copulation can exert changes in the neuronal response of the female reproductive tract (7476). In Drosophila, during copulation, intrusion by the male aedeagus has been shown to decrease the activity of neuron clusters in the central brain [possibly pC1 and pCd populations (53)], resulting in reduced sexual receptivity of females independent of the action of seminal molecules that males transfer (19, 20). Female abdominal neurons expressing Piezo mechanosensitive channels are stimulated by insertion of the aedeagus. Then, the signal is transmitted to ascending neurons called LSANs that connect in the central brain (20). Shao et al. proposed that LSAN neurons connect to other neurons that produce a peptide known as muscle-inhibitory peptide (also described as allatostatin-B in Drosophila) resulting in a reduction in female receptivity (20).

3.2 Control of copulation duration

Duration of copulation is an important factor in how much ejaculate is transferred from the male and how likely fertilization is to occur. Abdominal ganglia are heavily involved in regulating muscle contractions in the reproductive tract to influence copulation duration (20). Although the specific neuronal centers involved remain unidentified, the brain also plays a significant role, as supported by observations in three fly species [Musca domestica (77), Anastrepha suspensa (78), and Batrocera tryoni (71)] that copulation with decapitated or decerebrated females lasted longer than with intact females (71, 77, 78).

3.3 Monogamy: neuromodulatory mechanisms of pair bonding

After sexual attraction and choosing a mating partner, some species may form pair bonds characterized by selective attachments with some degree of durability (70, 79). Much of the knowledge about the neural mechanisms of pair bonding has been generated through the study of strictly monogamous species, mainly mammals such as the rodent Microtus ochrogaster (70, 79, 80). In this species, the peptides oxytocin and vasopressin modulate pair bonding (79, 80), and together with the dopaminergic system in the brain, promote continuous mating with the same partner (70). It has been proposed that the genes encoding oxytocin and vasopressin originated about 600 million years ago (81), with the predecessor molecules to these peptides having a similar role as modulators of social behaviors in different taxa (8286), including invertebrates (87). However, the role of oxytocin-like peptides during mate choice in invertebrates has yet to be explored, likely because they are absent in flies and bees, precluding their study in honey bees, the most widely studied social insect, and Drosophila (88). Most descriptions of the role of oxytocin-like peptides are about behaviors associated with male copulation in nematodes (88), hirudines (89), and gastropods (90, 91). However, its role in female pair bonding and copulation is poorly understood.

Some invertebrate species are described as monogamous, remaining with a single sexual partner for an extended period of time, usually marked by the period of parental care, or because they mate only once in their lives (92). Monogamous behavior and its neural mechanisms have been studied in the biparental beetle Lethrus apterus. In this species the expression of genes encoding the insect oxytocin-like peptide, inotocin (int), and its receptor (intr) in the brain increase during reproductive season and are highest at the beginning of pair formation and during the period of parental care (22). These findings suggest that, as observed with oxytocin in vertebrates, inotocin could modulate temporary mate-attachment behavior in insects. More research is needed on the role of inotocin in species typically described as monogamous or performing biparental care. Additionally, while it is intuitive to study monogamy in species that are commonly considered monogamous or exhibit biparental care, the mating systems in invertebrates are much more complex and flexible than typical descriptions found in other species. Excluding these models limits our understanding of how different mechanisms have evolved in similar behaviors. For example, in Gonodactylus bredini shrimps, it has been observed that males and females reduce their aggression towards their former mates when they meet again (93). Some shrimps of the genus Alpheus are considered socially monogamous (pair bond without implying sexual exclusivity between the two partners) since both sexes defend the territory and sometimes provide the nest with food, benefiting more from living in pairs than alone (94). Many molecular pathways can be conserved over long periods of evolutionary time, and understanding what molecules are involved in the modulation of mate attachment of invertebrates would allow a better understanding of the evolution of the molecular systems behind pair bonding that have been widely described in typically monogamous mammals and other socially monogamous vertebrates.

In addition to oxytocin-like peptides, other neuropeptides may be involved in regulating mate attachment. Cunningham et al. reported several neuropeptides that orchestrate biparental care in the beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides (95). For example, Natalisin FMRFamide and Sulfokinin have functions that promote mating in various taxa (9698); Tachykinin is involved in aggression (99); neuropeptide-like precursor 1 (NPLP-1) is involved in the division of social labor in honeybee workers (100); and Pheromone-Biosynthesis-Activating Neuropeptide (PBAN) activates pheromone synthesis (101). These neuropeptides might be important for pair bonding because biparental care is one of the most important selective pressures for the evolution of monogamy (66), and in Nicrophorus vespilloides pair bonding and parental care of the larvae are long-lasting.

3.3.1 Other molecules involved in social interactions and pair bonding

Another molecule related to insect social behavior is vitellogenin (Vg), whose primary known function is the production of yolk proteins in oviparous animals (102). Nevertheless, Vg has also been detected in the brains of social insects such as Apis mellifera (103) and N. vespilloides (30); as well as in the brains of female Tenebrio molitor, a polygamous species (17). Vitellogenin is also linked to parental care, with expression of it and its receptor decreasing during active parental care and varying throughout the reproductive cycle in both sexes of the subsocial beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides (30), and to the regulation of genes in the brain that are involved in division of labor in eusocial insects, such as insulin receptor precursor, JH epoxide hydrolase, Impl3 (ecdysone-inducible gene L3); PLRP2 (pancreatic lipase-related protein 2 precursor); Sirt6 (sirt 6 histone deacetylase); TRIP4 (thyroid hormone receptor interactor 4), the transcription factor fruitless in the brain (104, 105). The relationship between Vg expression in the brain and social behavior is not yet clear, but it is also known to function in the brain to regulate energy metabolism of glial cells (103) and to buffer against damage and oxidative stress (106), which may provide some clues. Memory and decision-making demand a large energy expenditure in the brain (107), and cognitive processes such as mate recognition may have a high metabolic demand. A potential area for future investigation could involve examining whether vitellogenin mitigates brain damage in species where females remember previous partners or engage in continuous partner assessment based on previous experiences with males. An example of this possibility is observed in T. molitor where vitellogenin levels were higher in the brains of females that evaluated more attractive males compared to those that evaluated less attractive males (17). Furthermore, females exposed to attractive males exhibited elevated levels of catalase in comparison to those interacting with non-attractive males (17). The increase in catalase levels implies the activation of an antioxidant defense mechanism within the brain. Whether the high metabolic rate in females evaluating different phenotypic males is connected to such a mechanism remains to be answered.

Evidence shows that the peptides and proteins mentioned in the above section are important candidate molecules to study their role as regulators of social behavior during mate choice in invertebrates. However, most approaches focus on analyzing brain gene expression during mate choice, pending behavioral observation when these molecules’ functions are altered, for example, through silencing or using agonist and antagonist drugs. Combining different experimental approaches would allow knowing if the selected molecules or genes are the only effectors of the behaviors mentioned or if it is a behavior that responds to more than one effector.

3.4 Neural mechanisms promoting polyandry

Polyandry is the system where a female mates with more than one partner (66). However, many mating systems are flexible, with females able to undergo phases of monandry or polyandry in the face of changing ecological contexts (108), such as the availability of mates in some schistosome species (109) and the damselfly Ischnura hastata (110); or environment resources as seen in the beetle Ips latidens (111) and the butterfly Pieris napi (112). In addition to the absence of mate recognition and attachment systems described in monogamy, the neural mechanisms of polyandry should address; 1) the motivation to seek and accept copulation with multiple males, and 2) behaviors promoting copulation with new males, such as aggression towards previous partners with the intention of increasing offspring variability (66). Re-mating motivation mechanisms usually involve the same genes and neurons as receptivity mechanisms (79, 113), and differences between mating systems might be due to different activational states of these circuits.

In addition to signals perceived before copulation, seminal proteins and peptides can affect female sexual receptivity circuitry (23, 54, 114), leading the female to change temporarily from polyandry to monandry or vice versa. In the cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus, the seminal proteins ToSfp022 and ToSfp0 appear to be responsible for reducing mate-seeking behavior, as females mated with males with a knockdown in the ToSfp022 and ToSfp01 genes left their nest significantly more frequently in response to male courtship songs compared to those mated with control males (24). While the mechanism of action of these proteins remains unclear, evidence suggests they interact with the female nervous system in a manner similar to sex peptide in Drosophila (23, 54, 57).

Finally, among the benefits of polyandry are increased genetic variability of the offspring and/or receiving more direct benefits from different males [i.e. nuptial gifts, nutrients and high sperm reserves (115, 116)]. Therefore, in studying the neural mechanisms of polyandry, it is essential to include the behaviors of rejection and aggression toward known males with whom they have previously mated. Such behaviors are described in different taxa, such as the pseudoscorpion Cordylochernes scorpioides (117), the spider Pholcus phalangioides (118), the moth Ephestia kuehniella (119), and the cricket Gryllodes sigillatus (120). Although for now this explanation remains hypothetical, these works suggest that females recognize their previous partners through chemical signals that they transfer to males during copulation. These chemical cues might trigger changes in female gene expression that could inactivate neural circuitry for sexual receptivity upon subsequent encounters.

4 Postcopulatory neural mechanisms

Females can skew the paternity of their offspring during and after copulation, a phenomenon called cryptic female choice (25, 121). The mechanisms involved include morphological traits in the reproductive tract or genitalia, physiology, and behaviors that non-randomly favor paternity in certain males (122, 123). Some behaviors include control of copulation duration, retention, or expulsion of the ejaculate, and cryptic elimination of spermatozoa by substances with spermicidal action (25). Below we describe neural mechanisms studied during cryptic female choice in invertebrates.

4.1 Neural control of spermathecal contraction

Female arthropods have sperm storage organs called spermathecae in which the sperm of one or several males can be stored for use long after copulation has finished. This results in males competing against the sperm of their rivals inside the female’s reproductive tract (sperm competition). Based on the neural control of the spermatheca by muscles in insects (124, 125), it has been suggested that females could actively bias fertilization through the nervous control of the spermatheca (27, 123, 126128). Although it is not yet clear how the spermatheca might bias male paternity, important advances have been made in studying the mechanisms that control spermatheca contractions in some insects. In Locusta migratoria, females release sperm stored in the spermatheca through contractions that begin when sensory cells located on the wall of the genital chamber are activated by mechanical stimulation from passage of the egg to be fertilized (129, 130). Joint action of octopamine and tyramine increases the frequency of muscular contractions of the spermatheca in D. melanogaster (123) and in L. migratoria (28, 131). Furthermore, in D. melanogaster, octopamine receptors in the mushroom bodies (OAMB) are required both for ovulation (29) and the release of sperm from spermatheca (123). In D. melanogaster, these results imply that the mushroom bodies are involved in controlling reproduction and ovulation. This makes sense with the fact that the mushroom bodies are the primary information integration centers in the insect brain, where learning processes, memory, odor, and size discrimination occur (132134). These results support the idea that females may bias the paternity of some males based on cues received from male evaluation before or during copulation.

4.2 Sperm dumping

Another behavior by which females may bias male paternity is the selective expulsion of sperm that the male has transferred to her. The neurobiological pathway behind this behavior has been studied in D. melanogaster through the dynamics of Diuretic Hormone 44 (DH44) and its receptor Diuretic Hormone 44 Receptor 1 (DH44R1) in the brain. By silencing transcription of the genes encoding DH44 and its receptor in neurons of the pars intercerebralis region with RNA interference, sperm expulsion occurred much more rapidly than in control females (26). The dh44 and dh44r1 genes are orthologous to the corticotropin-releasing factor (crf) and corticotropin-releasing factor receptor (crfr) genes that have stress response functions in vertebrates (135). Therefore, the authors suggested that sperm expulsion in Drosophila could be a stress response caused by the seminal peptides transferred during copulation (26, 136).

In addition to the possible effect of seminal peptides, control of sperm expulsion may occur based on the perceived genetic quality of the male before or during copulation as seen in females of the spider Physocyclus globosus. In this species, sperm expulsion occurs during or after mating, but females favor paternity for males who display increased courtship behaviors before and during copulation (73). The relationship between courtship intensity and sperm expulsion suggests the possibility that the neural pathways regulating sperm retention are affected by molecules and circuits implicated during courtship or copulation. Such neural pathways could be connected to reward circuits involving dopamine, which may regulate female decisions during mate choice either by promoting mate search and/or acceptance of mating after evaluation of males (137). Neural circuits promoting mating and courtship in male Drosophila have been identified, but female neural responses to courting males are poorly studied. Nonetheless, the transition from rejection to mate acceptance in virgin females may be controlled by the ellipsoid body (EB), a structure of the central complex (138). The interconnected ring neurons (R) in the EB receive input from the PPM3 clustered dopaminergic neurons situated in the superior medial protocerebrum. In the EB, Cholinergic R4d neurons promote rejection behaviors, while the activation of GABAergic and glutamatergic R2/R4m neurons promotes mating acceptance. Additionally, inhibiting R2/R4m neurons leads to an increase in mating latency (138, 139). Although this finding aligns with reports in various vertebrate dopaminergic circuits (137), further research on female neural responses is crucial and may be fruitful considering the possibilities for manipulating the Drosophila system (138, 139).

4.3 Control of oviposition

Even after fertilization has occurred, it is possible that females exert some control of oviposition, influencing which eggs are laid (121). Octopamine regulates oviposition in insects (140142), ticks (143) and nematodes (144, 145) by controlling the contraction rhythms of the oviduct. In Drosophila, the Octb2R and OAMB receptors present in octopaminergic and glutamatergic neurons in the abdominal ganglia project to the epithelial and reproductive cells of the oviducts (29, 142, 146). Recent studies indicate that activation of the Octb2R receptor causes relaxation, while activation of the OAMB receptor causes contraction (146). Additional neuromodulators, such as glutamate, tyramine, and other biogenic amines, may alter the impact of octopamine on the aforementioned neurons. However, their effects remain poorly comprehended and may vary among species (140146).

4.4 Other behaviors of postcopulatory female choice

There are several exciting behaviors to study in postcopulatory female choice for which there is not yet an approach to study the possible neural mechanisms behind them, for example:

Activation and inactivation of spermatozoa. In several invertebrate species, females may influence fertilization through substances secreted in their reproductive tract that either activate or disable sperm transferred by their partners. For instance, the female reproductive tract of Drosophila pseudoobscura provides a spermicidal environment that contributes to sperm competition (32). Another intriguing example comes from spiders, where glandular secretions activate and maintain the sperm stored within spermathecae (31, 147). It has been proposed that the female nervous system regulates the secretion of these glands (148), which may act as a mechanism to bias fertilization towards preferred males, such as those with higher courtship intensity (149).

Selective uptake of sperm in the spermatheca. Paternity may be biased by controlling the reception of sperm in the spermatheca (25). There is some research into the mechanism involved in the release of sperm stored in the spermatheca, but the mechanism controlling the uptake of sperm into the spermatheca remains unknown.

5 Female choice issues that have yet to be explored in invertebrates

5.1 Sexual cannibalism

One of the most extravagant behaviors in sexual selection is sexual cannibalism, when the female consumes the partner during or after mating and sometimes during courtship without mating. This behavior has only been documented in spiders and mantids. Sexual cannibalism might increase male reproductive success and, in many cases, results from female choice based on the quality of their partners (31, 150, 151). The neural mechanisms of this behavior are yet to be addressed. Based on descriptions of the behavior, it can be hypothesized that the visual sensory system as well as circuits and systems related to foraging and aggression are involved (31), but how these systems relate to mate choice is an open question.

Another example of sexual cannibalism is the mutual “partial” cannibalism of the cockroach Salganea taiwanensis. In this monogamous species, both sexes reciprocally consume the wings of their mate. The hypotheses for the evolutionary causes of this behavior propose that consumption of the wings promotes monogamy since, without wings, it is dangerous to leave the nest to look for another mate (152). Because this type of cannibalism promotes pair bonding and does not end in complete consumption of the partner, the neural mechanisms behind this behavior may differ from the aggression mechanisms proposed for sexual cannibalism in spiders and mantids.

5.2 Female neural responses to male manipulation

Sexual conflict arises when some of the following circumstances are in place (reviewed in: 147): 1) males try to overcome filters imposed by the females during mate choice, while the females respond with counter-adaptations to male strategies (153); 2) males exert sensory exploitation of pre-existing circuits in females (154); 3) females resist exploitation or manipulation by males (154, 155). However, sexual conflict is mostly detected through observation of behavior, and most of the conclusions do not address how the female’s nervous system responds to male manipulation. The investigation of the neural responses of females during sexual conflict would advance the comprehension of how sexual selection is taking place across species. For instance, in species where male manipulation occurs, analyzing behavior and reproductive success may not reflect females’ resistance to manipulation. However, looking through the nervous system of females could offer a different perspective. By exploring their neural responses, a “physiological attempt” of resistance may be revealed. While these responses are not yet considered counter-adaptive mechanisms, they have an evolutionary potential to develop and empower females to confront manipulation.

Some of the male manipulative behaviors are truly extreme, and studies on the neural responses of females when they occur are limited. One species where such a conflict occurs is the true bug Gerris gracilicornis. In this species, males attract predators via special leg movements when females they attempt to mount refuse their advances, leading to coercive copulation (156). How females decide to allow or deny copulation in the context of potentially being preyed on, and if the mechanisms of aggression and flight are coordinated and connected to those of sexual receptivity are interesting questions. Another extreme case of sexual conflict during mating is the traumatic insemination observed in the bed bug Cimex lectularius. In this species, males pierce the cuticle of females and inseminate them directly into their body cavity without penetrating their genitalia (157, 158). It is unknown whether the circuits connecting the female genitalia to the nervous system are active or inactive in such cases, or if other circuits are involved during insemination or when sperm is released from their storage organs. Comparison of this behavior with closely related species that do not display extra-genital copulation may provide insight into whether and how the neural response of these females to this type of male behavior has evolved.

In ants of the species Hypoponera opacior, some males mate with young nestmate queens even before they eclose from their cocoons (159, 160). It would be interesting to study any behavioral response to this in either the inseminated queens as pupae or post eclosion or in their egg-laying mother.

Mating plugs are another example of male manipulation, and it is not known what happens inside female brains when a mating plug prevents copulation with subsequent males in some species of arachnids (161163); insects (164); and even nematodes (165). A mating plug could do more than just physically block copulation; it could also mechanically stimulate neurons of the reproductive tract resulting in inhibition of receptivity (166). Mating plugs are also utilized by male vertebrates (167, 168), however, research on the neural responses of females to this behavior is scarce.

5.3 Possible costs of neural processes of female choice

Sexual differences in reproductive strategies are usually explained as arising from different reproductive costs for each sex (5, 169), but could this same argument be applied to neural mechanisms? In numerous species, females select mates using a cognitive process that may incur ecological costs (17, 107, 170, 171); however, males may not face equivalent consequences because of the absence of neural elements that enable females to be more selective in their mates. Nevertheless, if there is male choice, they too may experience comparable costs. One such cost is intoxicating male sperm in Drosophila females (172, 173). In this case, the role of the female detoxification systems is unknown. It is certainly possible to address these questions in vertebrates, but simpler nervous systems, shorter generation times and faster metabolic rates may make invertebrates better candidates.

4 Conclusions

Study of the neural mechanisms of mate choice is essential for a comprehensive understanding of the unusual behaviors we observe in nature. There are an increasing number of studies on the neural mechanisms of mate choice. However, those on female choice are less common and usually have a broad descriptive approach and not under the theoretical framework of sexual selection. Females of various species display a broad diversity of mate choice mechanisms, and it is possible that this variety in mechanisms corresponds to variation in mating system, life history and evolutionary history. For this reason, it is essential that models used to study female mate choice are equally diverse.

As with other social behaviors, the mechanisms behind female choice are complex and difficult to study because several often occur simultaneously. As techniques, including single-cell sequencing, genome and epigenome sequencing, proteomics, and access to invertebrate genomes advance, it will be possible to disentangle mate choice behaviors with careful experimental design. Working together to understand the immediate causes behind the behaviors will complete the picture of the evolution of mate choice.

Author contributions

SC-M: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization. IF-P: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JC-G: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by GCRLE-0620 Junior Scholar Award from the Global Consortium for Reproductive Longevity and Equality at the Buck Institute, made possible by the Bia-Echo Foundation, and by “Programa de Apoyo a Proyectos de Investigación e Innovación Tecnológica” UNAM-PAPIIT IA206922 to IF-P. SC-M is a doctoral student from Programa de Doctorado en Ciencias Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (UNAM) and received fellowship 632723 from CONAHCyT, México.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ian A. E. Butler for insightful comments on the manuscript and three reviewers that improved the quality of this paper.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Andersson M. Sexual Selection. New Jersey: Princeton University Press (1994).

Google Scholar

2. Darwin C. On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: Albemarle Street, London, John Murray (1859).

Google Scholar

3. Darwin C. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: Princeton University Press (1871).

Google Scholar

4. Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol Evol (2003) 18:41–7. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(02)00004-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Trivers RL. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B, editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man. Chicago: Aldine (1972). p. 795–838.

Google Scholar

6. Parker GA. Sexual selection and sexual conflict. In: Blum MS, Blum NA, editors. Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects. New York, NY: Academic Press (1979). p. 123–66. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2018.00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Halliday T. The study of mate choice. In: Bateson PPG, editor. Mate Choice. New York, USA:Cambridge University Press (1983). p. 3–32.

Google Scholar

8. Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Morley J. The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proc R Soc B (2003) 270:653–64. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. DeAngelis RS, Hofmann HA. Neural and molecular mechanisms underlying female mate choice decisions in vertebrates. J Exp Biol (2020) 223. doi: 10.1242/jeb.207324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ryan MJ, Page RA, Hunter KL, Taylor RC. ‘Crazy love’: nonlinearity and irrationality in mate choice. Anim Behav (2019) 147:189–98. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Edward DA, Chapman T. The evolution and significance of male mate choice. Trends Ecol Evol (2011) 26:647–54. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Pollo P, Nakagawa S, Kasumovic MM. The better, the choosier: A meta-analysis on interindividual variation of male mate choice. Ecol Lett (2022) 25:1305–22. doi: 10.1111/ele.13981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ah-King M. The history of sexual selection research provides insights as to why females are still understudied. Nat Commun (2022) 13:6976. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-34770-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fuss T. Mate choice, sex roles and sexual cognition: neuronal prerequisites supporting cognitive mate choice. Front Ecol Evol (2021) 9:749499. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.749499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ryan MJ. Darwin, sexual selection, and the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2021) 118:e2008194118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2008194118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Cummings ME, Larkins-Ford J, Reilly CRL, Wong R, Ramsey M, Hofmann HA. Sexual and social stimuli elicit rapid and contrasting genomic responses. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2008) 275:393–402. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hernández-Villanueva M, Cordero-Molina S, Macías García C, Contreras-Garduño J. The females’ response to male attractiveness: Mate choice, larvae production and differential brain protein expression. Ethology (2023) 129: 515–26. doi: 10.1111/eth.13389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bussell JJ, Yapici N, Zhang SX, Dickson BJ, Vosshall LB. Abdominal-B neurons control Drosophila virgin female receptivity. Curr Biol (2014) 24:1584–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Heifetz Y, Lindner M, Garini Y, Wolfner MF. Mating regulates neuromodulator ensembles at nerve termini innervating the drosophila reproductive tract. Curr Biol (2014) 24:731–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Shao L, Chung P, Wong A, Siwanowicz I, Kent CF, Long X, et al. A neural circuit encoding the experience of copulation in female Drosophila. Neuron (2019) 102:1025–1036.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Neckameyer WS. Dopamine modulates female sexual receptivity in Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurogenet (1998) 12:101–14. doi: 10.3109/01677069809167259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nagy NA, Németh Z, Juhász E, Póliska S, Rácz R, Kiss J, et al. Inotocin, a potential modulator of reproductive behaviors in a biparental beetle, Lethrus apterus. J Insect Physiol (2021) 132:104253. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2021.104253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Avila FW, Sirot LK, LaFlamme BA, Rubinstein CD, Wolfner MF. Insect seminal fluid proteins: identification and function. Annu Rev Entomol (2011) 56:21–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-120709-144823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moschilla JA, Tomkins JL, Simmons LW. Identification of seminal proteins related to the inhibition of mate searching in female crickets. Behav Ecol (2020) 31:1344–52. doi: 10.1093/beheco/araa090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Eberhard WG. Female Control. New Jersey: Princeton University Press (1996).

Google Scholar

26. Lee KM, Daubnerová I, Isaac RE, Zhang C, Choi S, Chung J, et al. A neuronal pathway that controls sperm ejection and storage in female Drosophila. Curr Biol (2015) 25:790–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Avila FW, Bloch Qazi MC, Rubinstein CD, Wolfner MF. A requirement for the neuromodulators octopamine and tyramine in Drosophila melanogaster female sperm storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2012) 109:4562–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117689109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Clark J, Lange AB. Octopamine modulates spermathecal muscle contractions in Locusta migratoria. J Comp Physiol A (2001) 189:105–14. doi: 10.1007/s00359-002-0375-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lee H-G, Seong C-S, Kim Y-C, Davis RL, Han K-A. Octopamine receptor OAMB is required for ovulation in Drosophila melanogaster. Dev Biol (2003) 264:179–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2003.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Roy-Zokan EM, Cunningham CB, Hebb LE, McKinney EC, Moore AJ. Vitellogenin and vitellogenin receptor gene expression is associated with male and female parenting in a subsocial insect. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2015) 282:20150787. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Herberstein ME, Wignall AE, Hebets EA, Schneider JM. Dangerous mating systems: Signal complexity, signal content and neural capacity in spiders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2014) 46:509–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Holman L, Snook RR. A sterile sperm caste protects brother fertile sperm from female-mediated death in Drosophila pseudoobscura. Curr Biol (2008) 18:292–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Eberhard WG. Postcopulatory sexual selection: Darwin’s omission and its consequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2009) 106:10025–32. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901217106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Yoshizawa K, Ferreira RL, Kamimura Y, Lienhard C. Female penis, male vagina, and their correlated evolution in a cave insect. Curr Biol (2014) 24:1006–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Stensmyr MC, Dekker T, Hansson BS. Evolution of the olfactory code in the Drosophila melanogaster subgroup. Proc Biol Sci (2003) 270:2333–40. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Immonen E, Ritchie MG. The genomic response to courtship song stimulation in female Drosophila melanogaster. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2011) 279:1359–65. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Choe A, Chuman T, von Reuss SH, Dossey AT, Yim JJ, Ajredini R, et al. Sex-specific mating pheromones in the nematode Panagrellus redivivus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2012) 109:20949–54. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1218302109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Fagan KA, Luo J, Lagoy RC, Schroeder FC, Albrecht DR, Portman DS. A single-neuron chemosensory switch determines the valence of a sexually dimorphic sensory behavior. Curr Biol (2018) 28:902–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Gaffin DD, Shakir SF. Synaptic interactions in scorpion peg sensilla appear to maintain chemosensory neurons within dynamic firing range. Insects (2021) 12:904. doi: 10.3390/insects12100904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Göpfert MC, Robert D. The mechanical basis of Drosophila audition. J Exp Biol (2002) 205:1199–208. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.9.1199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Römer H, Hedwig B, Ott SR. Contralateral inhibition as a sensory bias: the neural basis for a female preference in a synchronously calling bushcricket, Mecopoda elongata. Eur J Neurosci (2002) 15:1655–62. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02003.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Knowlton ED, Gaffin DD. Female wolf spider, Schizocosa avida1, vibration receptor responses to male courtship. Southwest Entomol (2019) 44:213–28. doi: 10.3958/059.044.0124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Huang SC, Chiou TH, Marshall J, Reinhard J. Spectral sensitivities and color signals in a polymorphic damselfly. PloS One (2014) 9:e87972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Peaslee AG, Wilson G. Spectral sensitivity in jumping spiders (Araneae, Salticidae). J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol (1989) 164:359–63. doi: 10.1007/BF00612995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Painting CJ, Rajamohan G, Chen Z, Zeng H, Li D. It takes two peaks to tango: the importance of UVB and UVA in sexual signaling in jumping spiders. Anim Behav (2016) 113:137–46. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.12.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell T. Principles of Neural Science. New York: McGraw-Hill Medical (2000).

Google Scholar

47. Ryan MJ, Cummings ME. Perceptual biases and mate choice. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst (2013) 44:437–59. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ringo J. Sexual receptivity in insects. Annu Rev Entomol (1996) 41:473–94. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.41.010196.002353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Aranha MM, Vasconcelos ML. Deciphering Drosophila female innate behaviors. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2018) 52:139–48. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cachero S, Ostrovsky AD, Yu JY, Dickson BJ, Jefferis GSXE. Sexual dimorphism in the fly brain. Curr Biol (2010) 20:1589–601. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.07.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Yamamoto D, Koganezawa M. Genes and circuits of courtship behavior in Drosophila males. Nat Rev Neurosci (2013) 14:681–92. doi: 10.1038/nrn3567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Chowdhury T, Calhoun RM, Bruch K, Moehring AJ. The fruitless gene affects female receptivity and species isolation. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2020) 287:20192765. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.2765

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Zhou C, Pan Y, Robinett CC, Meissner GW, Baker BS. Central brain neurons expressing doublesex regulate female receptivity in Drosophila. Neuron (2014) 83:149–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rezával C, Pavlou HJ, Dornan AJ, Chan Y-B, Kravitz EA, Goodwin SF. Neural circuitry underlying Drosophila female postmating behavioral responses. Curr Biol (2012) 22:1155–65. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Feng K, Palfreyman MT, Häsemeyer M, Talsma A, Dickson BJ. Ascending SAG neurons control sexual receptivity of Drosophila females. Neuron (2014) 83:135–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Jang YH, Chae HS, Kim YJ. Female-specific myoinhibitory peptide neurons regulate mating receptivity in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat Commun (2017) 8:1630. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01794-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Yang YT, Hu SW, Li X, Sun Y, He P, Kohlmeier KA, et al. Sex peptide regulates female receptivity through serotoninergic neurons in Drosophila. iScience (2023) 26:106123. doi: 10.1016/j.isci

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Córdova-García G, Sirot L, Abraham S, Díaz-Fleischer F, Flores-Estevez N, López-Ortega M, et al. Mating, but not male accessory gland products, changes female response to olfactory cues in Anastrepha fruit flies. Front Physiol (2021) 12:714247. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.714247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kundu P, Choi N, Rundus AS, Santer RD, Hebets EA. Uncovering ‘Hidden’Signals: previously presumed visual signals likely generate air particle movement. Front Ecol Evol (2022) 10:939133. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00742.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bailey NW, Zuk M. Acoustic experience shapes female mate choice in field crickets. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2008) 275:2645–50. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0859

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Dukas R. Learning in the context of sexual behavior in insects. Anim Biol (2006) 56:125–41. doi: 10.1163/157075606777304258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kohlmeier P, Zhang Y, Gorter JA, Su CY, Billeter JC. Mating increases Drosophila melanogaster females’ choosiness by reducing olfactory sensitivity to a male pheromone. Nat Ecol Evol (2021) 5:1165–73. doi: 10.1038/s41559-021-01482-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lynch KS, Ramsey ME, Cummings ME. The mate choice brain: comparing gene profiles between female choice and male coercive poeciliids. Genes Brain Behav (2011) 11:222–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183x.2011.00742.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kotenkova E, Romachenko A, Ambaryan A, Maltsev A. Effect of early experience on neuronal and behavioral responses to con- and heterospecific odors in closely related Mus taxa: epigenetic contribution in formation of precopulatory isolation. BMC Evol Biol (2019) 19: 55–72. doi: 10.1186/s12862-019-1373-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Hartbauer M, Kratzer S, Römer H. Chirp rate is independent of male condition in a synchronizing bushcricket. J Insect Physiol (2006) 52:221–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2005.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Emlen ST, Oring LW. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science (1977) 197:215–23. doi: 10.1126/science.32754240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Kokko H, Klug H, Jennions MD, Shuker DM, Simmons LW. Mating systems. In: Shuker DM, Simmons LW, editors. The evolution of insect mating systems. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2014). p. 42–58.

Google Scholar

68. Klug H. Why monogamy? A review of potential ultimate drivers. Front Ecol Evol (2018) 6:30. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2018.00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Grebe NM, Sharma A, Freeman SM, Palumbo MC, Patisaul HB, Bales KL, et al. Neural correlates of mating system diversity: oxytocin and vasopressin receptor distributions in monogamous and non-monogamous Eulemur. Sci Rep (2021) 11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83342-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Young LJ, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat Neurosci (2004) 7:1048–54. doi: 10.1038/nn1327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Pérez-Staples D, Weldon CW, Radhakrishnan P, Prenter J, Taylor PW. Control of copula duration and sperm storage by female Queensland fruit flies. J Insect Physiol (2010) 56:1755–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Eberhard WG. Evidence for widespread courtship during copulation in 131 species of insects and spiders, and implications for cryptic female choice. Evolution (1994) 48:711. doi: 10.2307/2410481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Peretti AV, Eberhard WG. Cryptic female choice via sperm dumping favors male copulatory courtship in a spider. J Evol Biol (2010) 23:271–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01900.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Pfaus JG, Heeb MM. Implications of immediate-early gene induction in the brain following sexual stimulation of female and male rodents. Brain Res Bull (1997) 44:397–407. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00219-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Pfaus JG, Damsma G, Wenkstern D, Fibiger HC. Sexual activity increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of female rats. Brain Res (1995) 693:21–30. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00679-k

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Tetel MJ, Celentano DC, Blaustein JD. Intraneuronal convergence of tactile and hormonal stimuli associated with female reproduction in rats. J Neuroendocrinol (1994) 6:211–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1994.tb00574.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Leopold RA, Terranova AC, Swilley EM. Mating refusal in Musca domestica: effects of repeated mating and decerebration upon frequency and duration of copulation. J Exp Zool (1971) 176(3):353–9. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-9-47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Fritz AH. Sperm storage is not subject to cephalic control in the Caribbean fruit fly, Anastrepha suspensa. J Insect Behav (2009) 22:412–22. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Johnson ZV, Young LJ. Neurobiological mechanisms of social attachment and pair bonding. Curr Opin Behavl Sci (2015) 3:38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Young KA, Gobrogge KL, Liu Y, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bonding: Insights from a socially monogamous rodent. Front Neuroendocrinol (2011) 32:53–69. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Insel TR, Shapiro LE. Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (1992) 89:5981–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Winslow JT, Hastings N, Carter CS, Harbaugh CR, Insel TR. A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in monogamous prairie voles. Nature (1993) 365:545–8. doi: 10.1038/365545a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Gwee P-C, Tay B-H, Brenner S, Venkatesh B. Characterization of the neurohypophysial hormone gene loci in elephant shark and the Japanese lamprey: origin of the vertebrate neurohypophysial hormone genes. BMC Evol Biol (2009) 9: 1–15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-9-47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Chu J, Marler CA, Wilczynski W. The effects of arginine vasotocin on the calling behavior of male cricket frogs in changing social contexts. Horm Behav (1998) 34:248–61. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Klatt JD, Goodson JL. Oxytocin-like receptors mediate pair bonding in a socially monogamous songbird. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2013) 280:20122396. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Ramsey ME, Fry D, Cummings ME. Isotocin increases female avoidance of males in a coercive mating system: Assessing the social salience hypothesis of oxytocin in a fish species. Horm Behav (2019) 112:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Fetter-Pruneda I, Hart T, Ulrich Y, Gal A, Oxley PR, Olivos-Cisneros L, et al. An oxytocin/vasopressin-related neuropeptide modulates social foraging behavior in the clonal raider ant. PloS Biol (2021) 19:e3001305. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Garrison JL, Macosko EZ, Bernstein S, Pokala N, Albrecht DR, Bargmann CI. Oxytocin/vasopressin-related peptides have an ancient role in reproductive behavior. Science (2012) 338:540–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1226201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Wagenaar DA, Hamilton MS, Huang T, Kristan WB, French KA. A hormone-activated central pattern generator for courtship. Curr Biol (2010) 20:487–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. De Lange RPJ, Joosse J, Van Minnen J. Multi-messenger innervation of the male sexual system of Lymnaea stagnalis. J Comp Neurol (1998) 390:564–77. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19980126)390:4<564::aid-cne8>3.0.co;2-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Van Kesteren R, Smit A, De Lange R, Kits K, Van Golen F, van der Schors R, et al. Structural and functional evolution of the vasopressin/oxytocin superfamily: vasopressin-related conopressin is the only member present in Lymnaea, and is involved in the control of sexual behavior. J Neurosci (1995) 15:5989–98. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.15-09-05989.1995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Wedell N. Monandry and polyandry as alternative lifestyles in a butterfly. Behav Ecol (2002) 13:450–5. doi: 10.1093/beheco/13.4.450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Caldwell RL. Recognition, signaling and reduced aggression between former mates in a stomatopod. Anim Behav (1992) 44:11–9. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80749-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Mathews LM. Territorial cooperation and social monogamy: factors affecting intersexual behaviors in pair-living snapping shrimp. Anim Behav (2002) 63:767–77. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Cunningham CB, Badgett MJ, Meagher RB, Orlando R, Moore AJ. Ethological principles predict the neuropeptides co-opted to influence parenting. Nat Commun (2017) 8. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Jiang H, Lkhagva A, Daubnerová I, Chae H-S, Šimo L, Jung S-H, et al. Natalisin, a tachykinin-like signaling system, regulates sexual activity and fecundity in insects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2013) 110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310676110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Liu T, Kim K, Li C, Barr MM. FMRFamide-like neuropeptides and mechanosensory touch receptor neurons regulate male sexual turning behavior in caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci (2007) 27:7174–82. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1405-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Nässel DR, Williams MJ. Cholecystokinin-like peptide (DSK) in Drosophila, not only for satiety signaling. Front Endocrinol (2014) 5:219. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Asahina K, Watanabe K, Duistermars BJ, Hoopfer E, González CR, Eyjólfsdóttir EA, et al. Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in drosophila. Cell (2014) 156:221–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Han B, Fang Y, Feng M, Hu H, Qi Y, Huo X, et al. Quantitative neuropeptidome analysis reveals neuropeptides are correlated with social behavior regulation of the honeybee workers. J Proteome Res (2015) 14:4382–93. doi: 10.1021/acs.jproteome.5b00632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Rafaeli A. Pheromone biosynthesis activating neuropeptide (PBAN): Regulatory role and mode of action. Gen Comp Endocrinol (2009) 162:69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Li H, Zhang S. Functions of vitellogenin in eggs. In: Oocytes: maternal information and functions. Houston, TX, USA: Springer, Cham (2017). p. 389–401.

Google Scholar

103. Münch D, Ihle KE, Salmela H, Amdam GV. Vitellogenin in the honey bee brain: Atypical localization of a reproductive protein that promotes longevity. Expl Gerontol (2015) 71:103–8. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2015.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Kohlmeier P, Feldmeyer B, Foitzik S. Vitellogenin-like A–associated shifts in social cue responsiveness regulate behavioral task specialization in an ant. PloS Biol (2018) 16:e2005747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Wheeler MM, Ament SA, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Robinson GE. Brain gene expression changes elicited by peripheral vitellogenin knockdown in the honey bee. Insect Mol Biol (2013) 22:562–73. doi: 10.1111/imb.12043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Corona M, Velarde RA, Remolina S, Moran-Lauter A, Wang Y, Hughes KA, et al. Vitellogenin, juvenile hormone, insulin signaling, and queen honey bee longevity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2007) 104:7128–33. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701909104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Mery F, Kawecki TJ. A cost of long-term memory in Drosophila. Science (2005) 308:1148–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1111331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Gowaty PA. Adaptively flexible polyandry. Anim Behav (2013) 86:877–84. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.08.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Beltran S, Boissier J. Schistosome monogamy: who, how, and why? Trends Parasitol (2008) 24:386–91. doi: 10.1016/j.pt.2008.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Cordero-Rivera A, Rivas-Torres A, Encalada AC, Lorenzo-Carballa MO. Sexual conflict and the evolution of monandry: The case of the damselfly Ischnura hastata (Odonata: Coenagrionidae) in the Galápagos Islands. Ecol Entomol (2023) 48:336–46. doi: 10.1111/een.13225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Reid ML. Monogamy in the bark beetle Ips latidens: ecological correlates of an unusual mating system. Ecol Entomol (1999) . 24:89–94. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2311.1999.00168.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Mellström HL, Wiklund C. What affects mating rate? Polyandry is higher in the directly developing generation of the butterfly Pieris napi. Anim Behav (2010) 80:413–8. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Wang K, Wang F, Forknall N, Yang T, Patrick C, Parekh R, et al. Neural circuit mechanisms of sexual receptivity in Drosophila females. Nature (2021) 589:577–81. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2972-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Scott D. Inhibition of female drosophila melanogaster remating by a seminal fluid protein (Esterase 6). Evolution (1986) 40:1084. doi: 10.2307/2408766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Jennions MD, Petrie M. Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc (2000) 75:21–64. doi: 10.1017/s0006323199005423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Simmons LW. The evolution of polyandry: sperm competition, sperm selection, and offspring viability. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst (2005) 36:125–46. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102403.112501

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Zeh JA, Newcomer SD, Zeh DW. Polyandrous females discriminate against previous mates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (1998) 95:13732–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.23.13732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Mestre L, Rodríguez-Teijeiro JD, Tuni C. Females of the cellar spider discriminate against previous mates. Ethology (2015) 121:994–1001. doi: 10.1111/eth.12414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Xu J, Wang Q. A polyandrous female moth discriminates against previous mates to gain genetic diversity. Anim Behav (2009) 78:1309–15. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Ivy TM, Weddle CB, Sakaluk SK. Females use self-referent cues to avoid mating with previous mates. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2005) 272:2475–8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Thornhill R. Cryptic female choice and its implications in the scorpionfly harpobittacus nigriceps. Am Nat (1983) 122:765–88. doi: 10.1086/284170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Cordero C. Ejaculate substances that affect female insect reproductive physiology and behavior: Honest or arbitrary traits? J Theor Biol (1995) 174:453–61. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1995.0111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Eberhard WG, Cordero C. Sexual selection by cryptic female choice on male seminal products - a new bridge between sexual selection and reproductive physiology. Trends Ecol Evol (1995) 10:493–6. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(00)89205-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Rodríguez V. Function of the spermathecal muscle in Chelymorpha alternans Boheman (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae: Cassidinae). Physiol Entomol (1994) 19:198–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1994.tb01043.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Villavaso EJ. Functions of the spermathecal muscle of the boll weevil, Anthonomus grandis. I Insect Physiol (1975) 21:1275–8. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(75)90099-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Fritz AH, Turner FR. A light and electron microscopical study of the spermathecae and ventral receptacle of Anastrepha suspensa (Diptera: Tephritidae) and implications in female influence of sperm storage. Arthropod Struct Dev (2002) 30:293–313. doi: 10.1016/s1467-8039(01)00038-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Hellriegel B, Bernasconi G. Female-mediated differential sperm storage in a fly with complex spermathecae, Scatophaga stercoraria. Anim Behav (2000) 59:311–7. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Snook RR, Hosken DJ. Sperm death and dumping in Drosophila. Nature (2004) 428:939–41. doi: 10.1038/nature02455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Clark J, Lange AB. The neural control of spermathecal contractions in the locust, Locusta migratoria. J Insect Physiol (2000) 46:191–201. doi: 10.1016/s0022-1910(99)00116-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Clark J, Lange AB. Evidence of a neural loop involved in controlling spermathecal contractions in Locusta migratoria. J Insect Physiol (2001) 47:607–16. doi: 10.1016/s0022-1910(00)00149-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. da Silva R, Lange AB. Tyramine as a possible neurotransmitter/neuromodulator at the spermatheca of the African migratory locust, Locusta migratoria. J Insect Physiol (2008) 54:1306–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Heisenberg M. What do the mushroom bodies do for the insect brain? An Introduction. Learn Mem (1998) 5:1–10. doi: 10.1101/lm.5.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Solanki N, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. Central complex and mushroom bodies mediate novelty choice behavior in Drosophila. J Neurogenet (2015) 29:30–7. doi: 10.3109/01677063.2014.1002661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Zars T. Behavioral functions of the insect mushroom bodies. Curr Op Neurobiol (2000) 10:790–5. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00147-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Lovejoy DA, Balment RJ. Evolution and physiology of the Corticotropin-Releasing Factor (CRF) family of neuropeptides in vertebrates. Gen Comp Endocrinol (1999) 115:1–22. doi: 10.1006/gcen.1999.7298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Hussain A, Üçpunar HK, Zhang M, Loschek LF, Grunwald Kadow IC. Neuropeptides modulate female chemosensory processing upon mating in Drosophila. PloS Biol (2016) 14:e1002455. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Lynch KS, Ryan MJ. Understanding the role of incentive salience in sexual decision-making. Integr Comp Biol (2020) 60:712–21. doi: 10.1093/icb/icaa054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Ishimoto H, Kamikouchi A. A feedforward circuit regulates action selection of pre-mating courtship behavior in female Drosophila. Curr Biol (2020) 30:396–407. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.11.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Ishimoto H, Kamikouchi A. Molecular and neural mechanisms regulating sexual motivation of virgin female Drosophila. Cell Mol Life Sci (2021) 78:4805–19. doi: 10.1007/s00018-021-03820-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Cook BJ, Wagner RM. Some pharmacological properties of the oviduct muscularis of the stable fly Stomoxys calcitrans. Comp Bioch Physiol C Comp Toxicol Pharmacol (1992) 102:273–80. doi: 10.1016/0742-8413(92)90111-j

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Monastirioti M, Linn CE Jr., White K. Characterization of Drosophila tyramine β-hydroxylase gene and isolation of mutant flies lacking octopamine. J Neurosci (1996) 16:3900–11. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.16-12-03900.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Orchard I, Lange AB. Evidence for octopaminergic modulation of an insect visceral muscle. J Neurobiol (1985) 16:171–81. doi: 10.1002/neu.480160303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Cossío-Bayúgar R, Miranda-Miranda E, Narváez Padilla V, Olvera-Valencia F, Reynaud E. Perturbation of tyraminergic/octopaminergic function inhibits oviposition in the cattle tick Rhipicephalus (Boophilus) microplus. J Insect Physiol (2012) 58:628–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Alkema MJ, Hunter-Ensor M, Ringstad N, Horvitz HR. Tyramine functions independently of octopamine in the Caenorhabditis elegans nervous system. Neuron (2005) 46:247–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Horvitz HR, Chalfie M, Trent C, Sulston JE, Evans PD. Serotonin and octopamine in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Science (1982) 216:1012–4. doi: 10.1126/science.6805073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Deshpande SA, Rohrbach EW, Asuncion JD, Harrigan J, Eamani A, Schlingmann EH, et al. Regulation of Drosophila oviduct muscle contractility by octopamine. iSiencie (2022) . 25:104697. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Vöcking O, Uhl G, Michalik P. Sperm dynamics in spiders (Araneae): Ultrastructural analysis of the sperm activation process in the garden spider Argiope bruennichi (Scopoli, 1772). PloS One (2013) 8:e72660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Herberstein ME, Schneider JM, Uhl G, Michalik P. Sperm dynamics in spiders. Behav Ecol (2011) 22:692–5. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Schneider JM, Lesmono K. Courtship raises male fertilization success through post-mating sexual selection in a spider. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2009) 276:3105–11. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0694

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Berning AW, Gadd RDH, Sweeney K, MacDonald L, Eng RYY, Hess ZL, et al. Sexual cannibalism is associated with female behavioral type, hunger state and increased hatching success. Anim Behav (2012) 84:715–21. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.06.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Buskirk RE, Frohlich C, Ross KG. The natural selection of sexual cannibalism. Am Nat (1984) 123:612–25. doi: 10.1086/284227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Osaki H, Kasuya E. Mutual wing-eating between female and male within mating pairs in wood-feeding cockroach. Ethology (2021) 127:433–7. doi: 10.1111/eth.13133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Parker GA. Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2006) 361:235–59. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Arnqvist G. Sensory exploitation and sexual conflict. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2006) 361:375–86. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Holland B, Rice WR. Perspective: chase-away sexual selection: antagonistic seduction versus resistance. Evolution (1998) 52:1. doi: 10.2307/2410914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Han CS, Jablonski PG. Male water striders attract predators to intimidate females into copulation. Nat Comm (2010) 1. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Predel R, Neupert S, Derst C, Reinhardt K, Wegener C. Neuropeptidomics of the bed bugCimex lectularius. J Proteome Res (2017) 17:440–54. doi: 10.1021/acs.jproteome.7b00630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Stutt AD, Siva-Jothy MT. Traumatic insemination and sexual conflict in the bed bug Cimex lectularius. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2001) 98:5683–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.101440698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Foitzik S, Heinze J, Oberstadt B, Herbers JM. Mate guarding and alternative reproductive tactics in the ant Hypoponera opacior. Anim Behav (2002) 63:597–604. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Kureck IM, Neumann A, Foitzik S. Wingless ant males adjust mate-guarding behavior to the competitive situation in the nest. Anim Behav (2011) 82:339–46. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Contreras-Garduno J, Peretti AV, Cordoba-Aguilar A. evidence that mating plug is related to null female mating activity in the scorpion Vaejovis punctatus. Ethology (2006) 112:152–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01149.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Oviedo-Diego MA, Mattoni CI, Vrech DE, Michalik P, Peretti AV. The morphology of mating plugs and its formation in scorpions: Implications for intersexual participation. J Morphol (2020) 281:620–35. doi: 10.1002/jmor.21125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Uhl G, Nessler SH, Schneider JM. Securing paternity in spiders? A review on occurrence and effects of mating plugs and male genital mutilation. Genetica (2009) 138:75–104. doi: 10.1007/s10709-009-9388-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Wedell N. Female receptivity in butterflies and moths. J Exp Biol (2005) 208:3433–40. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Canales-Lazcano J, Contreras-GarduÑo J, Cordero C. Strategic adjustment of copulatory plug size in a nematode. Curr Zool (2018) 65:571–7. doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Dickinson JL, Rutowski RL. The function of the mating plug in the chalcedon checkerspot butterfly. Anim Behav (1989) 38:154–62. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(89)80074-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Friesen CR, Uhrig EJ, Squire MK, Mason RT, Brennan PLR. Sexual conflict over mating in red-sided garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis) as indicated by experimental manipulation of genitalia. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci (2014) 281:20132694. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2694

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Ginsberg JR, Huck UW. Sperm competition in mammals. Trends Ecol Evol (1989) 4:74–9. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(89)90152-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Bateman AJ. Intra-sexual selection in drosophila. Hered (1948) 2:349–68. doi: 10.1038/hdy.1948.21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Burger JMS, Kolss M, Pont J, Kawecki TJ. Learning ability and longevity: a symmetrical evolutionary trade-off in Drosophila. Evolution (2008) 62:1294–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00376.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Dukas R. Costs of memory: ideas and predictions. J Theor Biol (1999) 197:41–50. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1998.0856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Lung O, Tram U, Finnerty CM, Eipper-Mains MA, Kalb JM, Wolfner MF. The Drosophila melanogaster seminal fluid protein Acp62F is a protease inhibitor that is toxic upon ectopic expression. Genetics (2002) 160:211–24. doi: 10.1093/genetics/160.1.211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Mueller JL, Page JL, Wolfner MF. An ectopic expression screen reveals the protective and toxic effects of Drosophila seminal fluid proteins. Genetics (2007) 175:777–83. doi: 10.1534/genetics.106.065318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mate choice, invertebrate brain, neural mechanisms, sexual selection, sexual conflict

Citation: Cordero-Molina S, Fetter-Pruneda I and Contreras-Garduño J (2024) Neural mechanisms involved in female mate choice in invertebrates. Front. Endocrinol. 14:1291635. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2023.1291635

Received: 09 September 2023; Accepted: 20 December 2023;
Published: 10 January 2024.

Edited by:

Qisheng Song, University of Missouri, United States

Reviewed by:

Laura Lavine, Washington State University, United States
Mauro S. B. Silva, Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School, United States
Luis Paiva, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Chile

Copyright © 2024 Cordero-Molina, Fetter-Pruneda and Contreras-Garduño. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sagrario Cordero-Molina, Y29yZGVybW9saW5hLnNAY29tdW5pZGFkLnVuYW0ubXg=; Ingrid Fetter-Pruneda, aWZldHRlckBpaWJpb21lZGljYXMudW5hbS5teA==; Jorge Contreras-Garduño, amNnQGVuZXNtb3JlbGlhLnVuYW0ubXg=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.