Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Endocrinol., 27 August 2021
Sec. Translational and Clinical Endocrinology
This article is part of the Research Topic Primary Adrenal Insufficiency: Quality of Life and Long-Term Outcome with Current Treatment Strategies View all 5 articles

Sleep, Cognition and Cortisol in Addison’s Disease: A Mechanistic Relationship

  • 1Centre for Higher Education Development, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
  • 2ACSENT Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
  • 3Division of Endocrinology, Department of Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa

Sleep is a critical biological process, essential for cognitive well-being. Neuroscientific literature suggests there are mechanistic relations between sleep disruption and memory deficits, and that varying concentrations of cortisol may play an important role in mediating those relations. Patients with Addison’s disease (AD) experience consistent and predictable periods of sub- and supra-physiological cortisol concentrations due to lifelong glucocorticoid replacement therapy, and they frequently report disrupted sleep and impaired memory. These disruptions and impairments may be related to the failure of replacement regimens to restore a normal circadian rhythm of cortisol secretion. Available data provides support for existing theoretical frameworks which postulate that in AD and other neuroendocrine, neurological, or psychiatric disorders, disrupted sleep is an important biological mechanism that underlies, at least partially, the memory impairments that patients frequently report experiencing. Given the literature linking sleep disruption and cognitive impairment in AD, future initiatives should aim to improve patients’ cognitive performance (and, indeed, their overall quality of life) by prioritizing and optimizing sleep. This review summarizes the literature on sleep and cognition in AD, and the role that cortisol concentrations play in the relationship between the two.

Introduction

Sleep is a critical biological process, an inevitable and essential aspect of normal human physiology. Nonetheless, questions about the functions of sleep (e.g., whether it is necessary for more than simple physical and mental restoration) remained unanswered until relatively recently. Available neuroscience literature provides some of the sought-after answers, suggesting that healthy, uninterrupted sleep is vital to ensuring that, for instance, consolidation of memory traces acquired during waking hours occurs smoothly and efficiently. Of particular interest here is that cortisol appears to play a particularly important role in mediating the sleep-memory relationship, especially because of its function in maintaining the integrity of sleep architecture (1, 2).

Patients with Addison’s disease (AD) require lifelong glucocorticoid replacement therapy. However, replacement medication does not restore the natural circadian rhythm of cortisol and, despite adherence, patients experience sub/supra physiological cortisol concentrations, particularly during the night. Patients with AD report and experience both poor-quality sleep and cognitive difficulties (38). One possible (but as yet unexplored) explanation for the sleep disruptions and memory deficits experienced by these patients is that the periods of sub- and- supra-physiological cortisol concentrations they experience may have a specific negative impact on processes of sleep-dependent memory consolidation.

This review aims to summarize and integrate the literature on the relationships between cortisol concentrations, sleep disruption, and cognitive functioning in patients with AD. This review is needed because despite expanding scientific evidence suggesting that sleep is a critical biological process and its functional value extends well beyond simple physical and mental restoration, very few published studies have investigated whether disrupted sleep is a possible mechanism that underlies the memory deficits experienced by patients with AD.

A Brief Overview of Human Sleep

Human sleep is a natural state of reduced responsiveness accompanied by a partial loss of consciousness. Sleep is regulated by three different processes: the homeostatic process, which determines its need, the circadian process that influences its timing, and the ultradian process that determines its organization (9, 10).

Sleep is cyclical in nature, alternating between 4 and 6 repeated cycles of rapid eye movement (REM) and non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, with each cycle lasting approximately 90–120 120 minutes (1113). Human sleep patterns have some predictable characteristics. Sleep onset is characterized by rhythmic alpha waves, occurring particularly in the occipital regions as discerned by electroencephalogram (EEG). Sleep then follows with NREM (Stages 1-4 (N1-N4) before the first episode of REM. The first sleep cycle usually begins with Stage 1 (N1), which lasts for 1-7 minutes after sleep onset. Stage 2 (N2), which lasts for 10-25 minutes, is signaled by K-complexes. As N2 progresses, high-voltage slow-waves appear, signaling the start of SWS. Within SWS, Stage 3 (N3) lasts only a few minutes, whereas Stage 4 (N4) lasts for 20-40 minutes. The body may re-enter lighter stages of sleep (N1-N3) for approximately 5 minutes, before the first REM episode is initiated. This episode is short-lived, lasting only 1-5 minutes. Thereafter, NREM and REM continue to alternate in a cyclic manner throughout the night, with REM cycles becoming longer and slow-wave sleep (SWS) shorter as the night progresses. Brief waking episodes occur in the later night, usually near the transitions into REM sleep (13).

Circadian Rhythmicity – Control of Hormone Release

The release of nearly all hormones follows daily oscillations, which result from an interaction between 24-hour circadian rhythmicity and the sleep-wake cycle (9, 1416). Circadian rhythms are generated by the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) hypothalamus, by light, and by ultradian rhythms (17, 18). The internal master clock in the SCN ensures we anticipate and prepare for changes in our environment and act appropriately (19, 20). These oscillators are synchronized with each other by the SCN’s master clock (21). Regarding the circadian clock, its timing mechanism is located in the SCN and incorporates three different components: (a) input pathways that transmit light and other environmental signals to the clock, (b) an endogenous pacemaker that generates 24-hour rhythms, and (c) output pathways that project to other brain regions and peripheral organs (9). The circadian clock is responsible for daily variations in body temperature, melatonin, and cortisol secretion, and will align those rhythms with those of sleep and other physiological processes (22). Circadian oscillators are also located in numerous peripheral tissues, including the liver, lungs, heart, and adrenal glands. These oscillators are synchronized with each other by the master clock (21). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis plays an important role in the homeostatic processes of the body, the co-ordination of the organism’s ability to adequately cope with environmental stressors and sleep regulation. This physiological system regulates the secretion of various hormones; of particular concern to this review is the release of cortisol resulting from HPA-axis activity.

In healthy people, cortisol has a robust diurnal secretory pattern. The highest concentrations occur in the early hours of the morning, with a peak just after waking. Concentrations then decrease slowly throughout the day, with troughs in the mid-afternoon and at midnight; the daily nadir typically happens several hours after initiation of nocturnal sleep. Concentrations then begin to rise from 02h00 to 03h00 and continue to rise until awakening. The nocturnal rise in cortisol is thought be in response to the greater energy demands of the brain as the night ends (2326). The daily rhythm of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) occur in close parallel with the daily rhythm of cortisol, all highest in the morning and reaching a nadir during around midnight (12, 25, 27). In contrast, melatonin is secreted from the pineal gland and synchronized to light from retinal input and growth hormone (GH) concentrations and are highest during early sleep, suggesting a reciprocal relationship between the HPA and hypothalamo-pituitary-somatotrophic (HPS) systems (21).

The HPA Axis and Sleep

The HPA axis plays an important role in maintaining alertness and modulating sleep [see Figure 1; (24, 28, 29)]. In fact, there is a bidirectional relationship between sleep architecture and HPA-axis activity. For example, cortisol exerts specific effects on sleep, whereas changes to sleep affect the release of this hormone (25, 26, 30).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 The hormonal control of sleep. Red arrows: negative feedback on the indicated brain structure.

Endogenous HPA Hormones and Their Effect on Sleep

The circadian rhythmicity of specific hormones (e.g., cortisol, ACTH, CRH, GH releasing hormone, melatonin) plays an essential role in sleep timing and offset and in the distribution of sleep stages across the night (28, 3133). Inhibitory HPA-axis actions, particularly during SWS, are responsible for attenuated cortisol activity during the first half the night. The quiescent period of HPA-axis activity starts prior to sleep, and continues into the first half the night, when SWS occurs at a maximum. Cortisol concentrations decrease rapidly in the first 20 minutes after SWS onset, and there is a consistent inverse temporal relationship between low cortisol concentrations and high SWS (15, 24, 3437). The optimal cortisol levels during early sleep augments SWS via feedback inhibition of CRH (28, 33). In the second half of the night, when REM sleep predominates, inhibitory mechanisms are attenuated and HPA secretory activity slowly increases (15, 38). Cortisol, CRH, and ACTH secretion and SNS activity increase during the latter part of the night. During the last sleep cycle, increases in cortisol are paired with increases in REM (39). In summary, while the deepening of sleep during SWS is associated with decreasing cortisol concentrations and decreased sympathetic tone, high autonomic and high cortisol activity occur during REM cycles (25, 33).

The reciprocal relationship between growth-hormone releasing hormone (GHRH) and CRH also plays an important role in regulating sleep. GHRH inhibits HPA-axis activity during early sleep, stimulating NREM and promoting sleep, whereas CRH inhibits SWS, enhances REM and vigilance, and disrupts sleep (27, 40, 41). This pattern points to a reciprocal interaction between sleep architecture and hormones of the hypothalamic-pituitary-somatotrophic (HPS) and HPA systems. Confirming the sleep-promoting role of GHRH and the sleep-disrupting effect of CRH, in older adults the typical age-related reduction in GH levels is accompanied by reduced SWS, whereas in both older adults and in depressed younger adults increased CRH levels contribute to the typically-observed sleep disruptions (27, 42).

Finally, ACTH and melatonin also play a role in sleep regulation. ACTH is the prime stimulus for cortisol release during sleep, and primarily affects sleep through its impact on cortisol secretion (29, 35). The secretion of melatonin, which has a sleep-promoting effect, is dependent on the light-dark cycle and is maximal during sleep periods (43). In fact, melatonin can induce sleep even when there is an insufficient homeostatic drive to sleep. Hence, melatonin administration has been used to treat insomnia and circadian rhythm disorders as it can preclude the drive for wakefulness and produce shifts in the circadian clock so that sleep occurs at a desired time (44).

Effects of Sleep on HPA Hormones

Sleep appears to have a direct impact on cortisol secretion. Specifically, sleep onset is associated with inhibitory effects on cortisol secretion. These effects persist for 1–2 hours after sleep onset (34, 45). In contrast, awakenings and the end of sleep are accompanied by cortisol increases (29, 46). Nocturnal awakenings are associated with releases of cortisol and subsequent inhibition of cortisol secretion (34, 40, 46).

Nocturnal awakenings and final morning awakening elicit a rapid increase in both ACTH and cortisol. Unlike nocturnal awakenings, this cortisol awakening response (CAR), includes a 50–60% increase in cortisol secretion, lasting an hour, with a peak at about 30 minutes after awakening (4750). Some research suggests that the release of ACTH and cortisol during late sleep is precipitated by the physiological expectation that sleep will end at a certain time, and/or by the anticipation of the stress of waking (5153).

Effects of Sleep Disruption on Circadian Rhythms

Acute shifts in the sleep-wake cycle (such as daytime sleeping or napping or the consequences of jetlag and shift-work), reduced sleep quality, and sleep deprivation all lead to HPA-axis activation and hence can alter the normal circadian pattern of cortisol secretion (34, 5459).

Regarding poor sleep quality, its experience (and, in fact, even its mere perception) is associated with increases in basal cortisol levels. Such increases stimulate arousal and suppress sleepiness, thus increasing sleep disturbances [which, in empirical studies, are characterised by increased wake time and reduced REM sleep; (16, 24, 60, 61)].

Regarding sleep deprivation, several studies report that elevated cortisol concentrations are present during both the sleep deprivation period and the subsequent day and evening (33, 59, 6266). Some researchers explain this physiological pattern by speculating that the initial sleep deprivation period activates the HPA axis as part of the stress response and may also reflect a decrease in the negative feedback regulation of the HPA axis. Thereafter, prolonged wakefulness increases sleep pressure (the increased need to sleep after periods of wakefulness), leads to fatigue and sleepiness, and causes a blunting of HPA-axis activity (34).

However, sleep disruptions do more than just impact the circadian rhythm of HPA-axis hormones. Disrupted sleep has detrimental effects on health, quality of life, mood and cognition, which is not surprising given the central role of sleep in physiological restorative processes, emotion regulation and memory consolidation (1, 10, 24, 67).

Sleep and Memory

The ability to effectively remember relies upon three broad cognitive processes: encoding (the transformation of new information into a form that can be stored in memory), consolidation (the stabilization of new memories in the brain), and retrieval (68, 69). One of the most important ways in which sleep affects cognition is by helping to consolidate memories (7072). The process of memory consolidation involves strengthening of memory traces, which represent information of our experiences, and the parallel integration of these experiences with previously acquired knowledge (69, 73).

Whereas encoding of environmental events (i.e., acquisition of information) and retrieval of those memories (i.e., reconstruction of previously acquired information) takes place during waking hours, the process of memory consolidation is incompatible with waking consciousness (1). Hence, when the organism effectively loses consciousness for several hours during sleep, physiological conditions are optimal for memory consolidation to take place. This is why theories regarding the function of sleep have gradually come to accept that a central aspect of this stage of consciousness is to strengthen memories encoded during waking and to subsequently transfer their traces into long-term storage (1, 74). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation (see Figure 2) appears to involve (a) repeated reactivation of information encoded during waking, and (b) transformation of newly acquired unstable memories into stable representations that become integrated into existing knowledge networks, thus forming long-term memories. In other words, during sleep the organism experiences “off-line” periods (i.e., periods that do not feature the kinds of interference experienced during waking) during which newly encoded memories are transferred from temporary to long-term stores (75, 76). The details of how these steps are accomplished, and which neural regions and neurobiological processes support them, remains somewhat controversial, however [for reviews, see (1, 2, 7779)].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 The hippocampal-to-neocortical dialogue. (A) During NREM sleep, memories temporarily stored in the hippocampus are transferred to the long-term store in the neocortex. (B) The dialogue involves the interaction between the slow oscillations, sleep spindles and hippocampal ripples to create spindle-ripple events (magnified circle). From Born et al. (75). System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychological Research, 76, 192-203.

Evidence for sleep-dependent memory consolidation is provided by numerous studies indicating that sleep enhances retention of information learned during waking hours, and that a sleep-filled delay enhances performance on a variety of declarative and non-declarative memory tasks (2, 8089). In contrast, when sleep is disrupted memory performance is poorer than when individuals are allowed to sleep uninterrupted (9092).

Cortisol: A Functional Role in Memory

Adequate concentrations of cortisol are essential for optimal cognitive functioning (9397). The hippocampus plays a vital role in memory consolidation and in new learning, encoding, and retrieval of declarative memories (97105), while the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is similarly important for integrating sensory information, evaluating the significance of environmental stimuli, and processing previously encoded materials (106110). Because both these structures contain particularly high concentrations of glucocorticoid receptors (111), any alterations in cortisol secretion have marked effects on their functioning. A substantial body of data indicates that elevated cortisol concentrations negatively impact performance on hippocampal-dependent memory tasks [e.g., word list and paragraph recall tasks; (94, 112120)] and on PFC-dependent working memory and executive functioning tasks [e.g., tests assessing set-shifting, attention, abstract thinking, cognitive flexibility, mental rotation; (120129)].

Studies have consistently demonstrated that chronically elevated glucocorticoids impair hippocampal-dependent memory (130134)1. The negative impact of elevated cortisol on verbal declarative memory performance has been demonstrated (a) following increases in endogenous levels of the hormone (through laboratory-based stress induction procedures; 127, 139, 141143), and (b) in studies that featured exogenously administered corticosteroids (128, 144148). For example, Kirschbaum et al. (94) found that stress-induced cortisol increases, and (separately) administration of 10 mg hydrocortisone orally, were associated with poorer recall of verbal material. Similarly, De Quervain and colleagues (149, 150) found that oral administration of 25mg of cortisone acetate significantly impaired both free and cued recall of verbal material, while leaving recognition memory (which is not dependent on hippocampal substrates) unaffected, and that the same dose of cortisone acetate impaired cued recall of a series of word pairs. In that study, stress-level doses of cortisone acetate reduced cerebral blood flow to the medial temporal lobe (MTL), a memory network that broadly includes the hippocampus. Several studies have also documented impaired performance on spatial memory and navigation tasks in the presence of elevated cortisol levels in humans (94, 127, 146, 151162). However, investigations of the impact of cortisol on spatial memory are more abundant in the animal literature. Furthermore, studies investigating spatial memory and cortisol in humans have produced more variable results, compared to the robust literature on impaired verbal memory in the presence of elevated cortisol levels.

Although hippocampal-dependent forms of memory are impaired by increased cortisol concentrations, non-hippocampal forms of memory (e.g., procedural memory), appear unaffected (e.g., 117, 163, 164). For example, Kirschbaum and colleagues (94) found that oral administration of 10mg cortisol to healthy subjects impaired performance on a declarative (a word list) but not a procedural (a word priming test) memory task. Furthermore, increased cortisol levels impair verbal declarative memory whereas non-verbal memory appears unaffected (165, 166). For example, Newcomer and colleagues (146) found that a 4-day period of oral administration of cortisol to young adults impaired their performance on a paragraph recall task, but did not significantly affect their recall of previously presented geometric line drawings or their performance on a spatial location task.

Regarding the PFC’s involvement in memory processing, this brain structure plays an important role in the encoding and retrieval of declarative memories (167). Specifically, after retrieval, the PFC determines whether an event occurred in a particular setting (168, 169), allowing accurate memories to be reconstructed. The PFC is also involved in working memory (WM). Specifically, it allows humans to (a) keep a mental “sketch” of information and protect this information from internal and external distractions, (b) inhibit inappropriate responses and behaviour, and (c) regulate attention. As such, the PFC allows for cognitive flexibility and goal-directed behaviour (121, 122).

Chronically elevated cortisol concentrations lead to dendritic atrophy in the PFC (170), and stengthens the noradrenalin system, which reduces neuronal firing within the structure (122, 171). Stress-induced cortisol increases also increase dopaminergic activity and glutamate levels in the PFC (129, 172). Glutamate receptor-mediated synaptic transmission in the PFC is particularly important for WM (173, 174). While acute elevations in glutamate have a positive effect on WM (129), excessive elevations cause impairment. Each of these hormones (noradrenalin, dopamine and glutamate) has an inverted-U influence on WM, with either too little or much impairing PFC functioning (122).

Glucocorticoid Receptors and Memory

Glucocorticoids affect the human brain by their interaction with two intracellular receptors (134). Glucocorticoids that enter the brain change gene expression by binding to type 1 mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) and type 2 glucocorticoid receptors (GRs). These two receptors bind cortisol with different affinities (135). MRs have a high affinity for cortisol and become heavily occupied at low cortisol concentrations (including the evening nadir of the cortisol circadian profile, when 90% of MRs, but only 10% of GRs, are occupied). In contrast, GRs have a lower affinity for cortisol and only become heavily occupied when cortisol levels reach a peak [e.g., after a stressor or after the post-awakening cortisol surge; (97, 134, 146, 175)].

MRs are found predominantly in the hippocampus, whereas GRs are distributed throughout the brain. Both play important roles in cognitive function, however (176, 177). MRs are located in brain regions involved in behavioral reactivity to new events, which enables the encoding of new information and subsequent retrieval, whereas GRs are located in brain regions involved in the consolidation and storage of information learned (94, 132, 178, 179). Hence, the activation of both receptors is a necessary for optimal memory functioning. For instance, de Kloet et al. (135) showed that when cortisol levels were mildly elevated (and therefore all MRs, but only some GRs, were activated), long-term-potentiation (LTP; the reinforcement of synaptic connections necessary for information storage) was enhanced. However, at higher cortisol levels (when GRs were over-activated and MR occupation was low), LTP was impaired. MRs play a particularly important role in hippocampal-dependent memory, executive function, and attention (126, 180184). In confirmation of the latter, Schultebraucks and colleagues (185) found that, during high MR occupation, verbal memory was significantly better and there were trends towards better executive functioning.

Variations in Cortisol Concentrations and Their Effects on Cognition

Most studies have focused on the deleterious effects of elevated cortisol levels on cognitive functioning. The negative effects of supra-physiological cortisol levels on brain structure and cognitive functioning is well known and evident in both healthy individuals and in patients known to experience chronically elevated cortisol levels [e.g., Cushing’s syndrome, depression, Alzheimer’s disease; (132, 175, 186191)].

Elevated cortisol concentrations impair cognitive function due to their effect on specific neurobiological systems. Specifically, the relationship between glucocorticoids and cognition usually follows an inverted-U shaped pattern (192195), where cognitive functioning is enhanced by a certain concentration of cortisol (114, 196, 197) and concentrations that are either too low or too high having impairing effects (114, 115, 135, 155, 178, 197).

Given that altered cortisol secretion plays a role in the etiology of many diseases marked by cognitive impairments [e.g., Addison’s disease, Cushing’s syndrome, Alzheimer’s disease, major depressive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, and metabolic syndrome; (130, 181, 185, 198202)], and given the known alterations in cortisol concentrations in patients with AD on replacement therapy, it is important to determine the physiological mechanisms by which chronically altered circadian rhythms impact cognitive functioning. One such mechanism may be through sleep, given that a bidirectional relationship exists between circadian rhythmicity and the sleep-wake cycle, and because successful memory consolidation of information learned during the day is known to rely on sleep (1, 90, 203).

Addison’s Disease

The diagnosis of AD is based on the measured presence of low plasma cortisol, low aldosterone levels, high renin levels, and elevated ACTH [loss of endogenous ACTH drive; (204)]. Patients with AD need to be on glucocorticoid (GC) replacement therapy for life, which is essential for survival (205). Cortisol is usually replaced with oral hydrocortisone, prednisone, or cortisone acetate (all of which activate predominantly GRs, predominantly), plus a mineralocorticoid (fludrocortisone) for sodium and potassium regulation (206, 207). Given the bidirectional relationship between cortisol and the sleep-wake cycle, the dosage, timing, and type of medication regimens used by patients may impact their general well-being and sleep patterns (28) due to the influence of GCs on circadian rhythmicity. Typically, GCs are replaced in 2-3 daily doses (see Figure 3), with the total daily dose ranging from 15-30mg. The highest dose (one-half to two-thirds) is taken in the morning, a reduced dose is taken in the afternoon, and (if required) a third dose in the late afternoon/evening [typically around 5pm; (208210)]. Such a dosing schedule of GC replacement is meant to imitate the normal diurnal cortisol rhythm, to reflect the peak cortisol rise in the morning, and to avoid over-replacement in the nadir of cortisol secretion during the night (208, 211). However, despite efforts to find the best replacement regimen in terms of dosage and timing (211213), none mimic the physiological circadian rhythm; there are still supra-physiological peaks during the day and lower-than-expected concentrations during the early hours of the morning (214219). This over- and under-replacement results from the biochemical properties of replacement medications. Oral hydrocortisone (HC) is absorbed rapidly, reaching maximum concentrations an hour after intake (220). However, HC replacement produces extremely variable peak concentrations within a supra-physiological range, followed by rapid declines to <100 nmol/l at 5-7 hours after ingestion (221) due to its short plasma half-life (around 1.5-1.8 hours; 215, 222). This means that patients require regular dosing, and that they nonetheless experience periods of cortisol deficiency, particularly between midnight and early morning (223). Another problem with GC replacement therapy is that it does not adequately replicate the morning rise in cortisol levels experienced by healthy individuals. In healthy individuals, the natural peak of cortisol starts during the onset of REM sleep in the early hours of the morning, whereas the peak level resulting from an early morning dose of hydrocortisone comes several hours after the medication has been taken (206). This temporary early-morning cortisol insufficiency in patients with AD can account for commonly reported symptoms such as fatigue, nausea, and headaches, which are alleviated within an hour after taking the morning dose of hydrocortisone (224). Overall, conventional GCs do not restore the normal circadian rhythm of hormone release (215, 225). Instead, patients are over-replaced immediately following therapeutic administration, and then under-replaced within a few hours of that administration (211, 226), which may have important implications for sleep regulation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Simulated cortisol profile for a patient [broken line] following thrice-daily hydrocortisone administration [10mg at 06:00, 5mg at 12:00 and 2.5mg at 18:00, shown as solid arrows]. The normal circadian rhythm of cortisol [solid line]. From Mah et al. (211). Weight‐related dosing, timing and monitoring hydrocortisone replacement therapy in patients with adrenal insufficiency. Clinical Endocrinology, 61(3), 367-375.

New Advancements in Treatment

As standard replacement therapy does not mimic the natural circadian rhythm, newer treatments aim to imitate physiological cortisol rhythms. These new treatments attempt to improve biochemical control of the release of cortisol and to reduce the long-term adverse effects typically associated with standard replacement regimens (220). Continuous subcutaneous HC infusion (CSHI) and modified release HC (MR-HC) tablets are two promising new treatments.

Infusion of HC in patients with AD has been shown to mimic normal circadian rhythmicity and improve quality of life (QoL; 226, 227). A crossover randomized clinical trial (N = 33 patients with AD who received CSHI or thrice-daily conventional therapy for a 3-month period), found that a 10mg/m2 daily dose of CSHI normalized cortisol and ACTH levels in the morning, and patients 24-hour cortisol curves approached normal circadian variation compared with conventional oral replacement. The 24-hour area under the curve (AUC) did not differ between infusion and conventional oral therapy, but daytime AUC (8am-midnight) was higher for oral replacement therapy, and night-time AUC (midnight-8am) was higher for CSHI. Infusion improved vitality and physical functioning (228) but did not improve sleep (except that sleep length increased, as measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) and actigraphy). Another randomized double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trial (N = 10 patients with AD) assessed whether CSHI improved QoL and fatigue, compared to standard GC therapy (229). CSHI did not improve health status in AD patients who had mild deficits in well-being at baseline. Overall, it appears that CSHI benefits some, but not all, patients in that it restores the usual circadian cortisol rhythmicity and improves QoL (226).

A significant disadvantage of subcutaneous infusions is their impracticality. An alternative is for patients using HC to wake up at 3am and take a dose of medication. This alternative is perhaps even more impractical and, moreover, may cause more daytime fatigue as well as supra-physiological peaks. MR-HC offers a more practical and sustainable approach to normalizing cortisol circadian rhythms due to its immediate and extended hormone release characteristics. MR-HC has been shown to mimic natural physiological cortisol circadian rhythm (230, 231). Johannsson and colleagues (231) demonstrated that taking a once-off morning dose of either 5 or 20mg MR-HC led to a closer mimicking of physiological cortisol circadian rhythms, except for the early-morning cortisol peak. However, if MR-HC is taken late at night (thus allowing for a delayed and sustained release), it can mimic the rise in cortisol that typically occurs during the early hours of the morning. For example, Debono et al. (230) showed that taking 15-20mg of MR-HC at 23h00 and 10mg at 07h00 reproduced the normal physiological cortisol circadian rhythm in healthy controls (see Figure 4). In that study, participants’ cortisol concentrations peaked, on average, at 08h32 and decreased throughout the day, reaching a nadir, on average, at 00h18. Dual-release hydrocortisone (DR-HC; Plenadren) has both an immediate-release coating and extended-release core. This form of replacement therapy better mimics the normal cortisol profile (218) and improves patients’ quality of life (232234). However, despite normalizing cortisol patterns, DR-HC has shown to have little effect on cognitive functioning or sleep (235). Although Krekeler and colleagues (235) showed that patients with adrenal insufficiency treated with DR-HC tended to show better executive functioning compared to patients on conventional HC, other cognitive domains appear unaffected, and they found no between-group differences in terms of sleep.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Concentration-time profiles for modified-release hydrocortisone (MR-HC) 5mg, 10mg, 15mg and 30mg compared with immediate-release hydrocortisone (IRHC). Graph showing delayed and sustained release characteristic of MR-HC (to convert values from mcg/dl to nmol/l x 27.59). From Debono et al. (230). Modified-release hydrocortisone to provide circadian cortisol profiles. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 94(5), 1548-1554.

Sleep Disruptions in AD

Numerous studies suggest that, in patients with AD, clinically relevant fatigue persists despite replacement therapy. For instance, Løvâs, Logeŧ, and Husebye (236) found that patients with AD self-reported reduced general health perception and vitality despite receiving replacement therapy with cortisone acetate and fludrocortisone. Similarly, van der Valk and colleagues (237) found that 48% of their patient sample (N = 328) self-reported abnormal fatigue; 61% reported severe fatigue. Researchers postulate that reports of increased daytime fatigue may be due to reduced quality of sleep in patients with AD and that relatively increased doses of HC may contribute to these sleep disruptions (3, 236, 238).

Løvâs, Husebye, Holsten, and Bjorvatn (3) found that 34% of the 60 patients with AD in their sample self-reported weekly sleep disturbances (difficulties falling asleep [13%], repeated awakenings [14%], and early morning awakenings [20%]). Similarly, Henry et al. (7) found that 60 patients with AD self-reported poorer sleep quality and efficiency, longer sleep latency, shorter sleep duration, more sleep disturbances, and greater daytime dysfunction compared to healthy controls. In terms of objectively measured sleep quality, Henry et al. (5) found, using actigraphy, that patients with AD experienced interrupted sleep characterized by worse sleep efficiency and a greater amount of time spent awake compared to healthy controls, who achieved a fuller night of uninterrupted sleep. These data present a pattern showing that patients with AD report frequent sleep disturbances, including difficulty falling asleep, nighttime awakenings, and a lower sleep quality. In all of the abovementioned studies, although patients were on replacement therapy, their cortisol secretion differed from healthy individuals. Because of cortisol’s key role in regulating our circadian rhythms and ensuring the transitions between sleep stages (33), it is unsurprising patients on replacement therapy still experience poor-quality sleep.

In terms of objectively measured sleep quality via polysomnography, limited information exists on the impact of low cortisol concentrations on sleep architecture. Similarly, few studies report on sleep in patients with AD when replacement medication is administered by conventional replacement. In terms of low cortisol concentrations, Gillin et al. (239) reported that patients with AD whose replacement medication was withheld for longer than 24 hours (and who therefore had undetectably low concentrations of cortisol at bedtime), showed increased time spent in SWS and correspondingly reduced time spent in REM sleep. Similarly, Garcia-Borreguero et al. (238) reported that patients with AD who were deprived of glucocorticoid medication for 1.5 hours prior to bedtime (and who therefore had undetectably low levels of cortisol at bedtime) showed increased wake after sleep onset (WASO) and REM latency and decreased amount of time spent in REM sleep, compared to patients who took their medication just before bedtime. These results suggest that the initiation and maintenance of REM sleep is facilitated by cortisol. In contrast, high cortisol concentrations appear to reduce the amount of time spent in SWS (27, 35, 240), and, consistent with this, relatively lower cortisol concentrations in healthy controls (administration of metyrapone) and patients with AD (replacement medication was withheld) are significantly associated with increased delta sleep (239).

Gillin et al. (239) reported that when medication was administered by conventional replacement, patients with AD had similar sleep to controls, except that patients took significantly longer to fall asleep and spent significantly more time in delta sleep (i.e., SWS). More recently, Henry and colleagues (4) found that when medication was administered by conventional replacement, patients with AD (compared to healthy matched controls) spent significantly less time in SWS and that there was a trend towards patients experiencing significantly shorter REM latency and more time in Stage 2 sleep.

Overall, few studies have objectively assessed sleep in patients with AD, despite that fact that an abundance of scientific evidence suggests that these patients experience disruptions to the cortisol circadian rhythm and, consequently, are at risk for experiencing negative effects on sleep architecture. For instance, patients with psychiatric conditions that exhibit elevated cortisol concentrations (e.g., depression and post-traumatic stress disorder) experience less time in SWS, shortened REM latency, increased REM sleep and density, and sleep discontinuity (238, 241244). Similarly, in patients with Cushing’s disease (who produce excessive amounts of cortisol), findings show that SWS is decreased, REM latency shortened, REM density is elevated, and aberrances in sleep continuity occur (244, 245). One study found that elevated cortisol concentrations in 11 patients with Cushing’s disease were associated with lower REM activity, and more awakenings during sleep (245). Similar patterns of decreased REM latency and/or increased time spent in REM sleep have been found in patients with AD who took hydrocortisone before bedtime and in healthy controls with artificially increased cortisol concentrations (41, 238). There are comparatively few data on sub-physiological cortisol levels and sleep, and similarly, few studies on sleep of patients with AD when replacement medication is administered as part of routine replacement. Therefore, effects on sleep quality and architecture of the illness itself, and of replacement therapy, remains largely unexplored in patients with AD.

Because of the central role the HPA axis plays in sleep regulation (28, 35), either low or high night-time cortisol, alongside high night-time ACTH and CRH, may lead to sleep disturbances in patients with AD (228). However, the implications of exposure to altered circadian cortisol patterns and consequent sleep disruptions have not been adequately addressed in the available literature. For instance, disrupted sleep may impede sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Because changes in sleep and memory are associated with the use of corticosteroids, further research is necessary to help understand the impact that replacement medication used by patients with AD has on the processes that influence sleep-dependent memory consolidation.

Cognitive Functioning in AD

Despite replacement therapy, patients with AD frequently present with both subjective cognitive complaints and objective cognitive impairments, including poor memory and impaired concentration (206, 207, 221). Due to the affinity between variations in cortisol concentrations and impaired performance on tests of memory, attention and executive functioning (115, 146, 246), understanding how these domains are affected is relevant in patients with AD. However, very few studies have characterized cognitive function in AD.

Klement et al. (247) reported that patients with AD on replacement therapy performed significantly more poorly than healthy controls on a declarative memory test. Similarly, Schultebraucks and colleagues (185) found that patients performed significantly more poorly than controls on a test of verbal learning, and Henry et al. (6) found that patients performed significantly more poorly than controls on tests of both verbal learning and memory (and that patients made significantly more false alarms [incorrectly saying a word on the list when it was not present] when recalling information). Henry et al. (5) found that healthy controls learned and retained more information than patients with AD on two different tasks of declarative memory, but that there were no significant between-group differences for procedural memory tasks. Tiemensma and colleagues (248) found that patients performed significantly more poorly on tests of both verbal and visual memory than healthy controls. The latter study also found mild executive impairment and significantly slower processing speed in their patient group. Interestingly, in this study, delaying HC intake in another group of patients with AD, which resulted in significantly lower cortisol concentrations at the time of cognitive testing, had no impact on cognitive performance. In terms of disease characteristics and cognition, Henry et al. (6) also found that patients who had AD for a longer interval had a slower speed of processing and that patients who were diagnosed later in life had poorer declarative and working memory, a slower speed of processing, and an overall greater cognitive impairment. Blacha et al. (249) found patients with AD (20 PAI and 20 SAI) showed significantly worse performance on a test of attention compared to controls (but found no difference in memory and other cognitive domains). They also found that higher HC doses impaired attention, visuo-motor skills and executive function, but that duration of therapy had no impact on cognitive performance. Similarly, Harbeck et al. (223) found that higher cortisol levels were associated with impaired short-term memory in patients who underwent short-term hydrocortisone infusion during the night. Overall, it appears that cognitive deficits in patients with AD are primarily in the domain of declarative memory (both verbal and visual memory), but also extend to executive functioning (including attention and processing speed).

Impaired declarative memory performance likely emerges from two main sources. First, indirectly, disrupted cortisol secretion patterns impact circadian rhythms and lead to sleep disturbances. Second, directly, due to supra-physiological cortisol levels experienced by patients taking short-acting hydrocortisone (250). Supra-physiological glucocorticoid increases impact on brain regions such as the hippocampus and the PFC which have a high concentration of glucocorticoid receptors (105, 251). These effects include, but are not limited to, degeneration of hippocampal neurons (252), altered organization of dendrites in the PFC (253), and, as such, impaired performance on tasks involving declarative memory (121, 254). In support of this, elevated cortisol concentrations associated with normal aging have been linked to ventricular enlargement, neuronal loss, and decreased volume in the hippocampus alongside a decline in cognitive performance (154, 252, 255257). Exogenous administration of hydrocortisone (occurring as a once off to a few days) to healthy subjects raising serum cortisol concentrations, impairs verbal memory, working memory, visuo-spatial memory, and executive functioning (96, 146, 150, 191, 258, 259). Similarly, exogenous administration of dexamethasone or prednisone to healthy subjects impairs memory performance (117, 191, 259, 260). Prolonged levels of increased hydrocortisone may cause permanent death of hippocampal neurons, reduce hippocampal glucose uptake (255) and neuronal excitability (261), impair synaptic plasticity (262, 263), decrease the amount of newly-generated neurons, alter synaptic density in the CA1 and CA3 regions (264), and cause death of dendrites in hippocampus (252).

Another neurobiological mechanism that may explain the memory deficits observed in patients with AD is the differential activation of the two types of receptors discussed earlier, MRs and GRs. Cortisol’s effects on the hippocampus and PFC are mediated by the interaction of glucocorticoids with MRs and GRs (134). Activation of MRs is essential for successful encoding, whereas activation of GRs is essential for successful consolidation and retrieval of memory (94, 135). Activation of both receptors is required for optimal memory performance (135). In one study providing empirical support for this proposed neurobiological mechanism, Tytherleigh et al. (207) found that adequately treated AD patients, performed significantly better a declarative memory recall task, when both receptor types were activated, compared to when only one or the other was activated. While some cortisol is needed to enhance cognition (a shift towards predominant MR activation and minimal GR activation), prolonged exposure and/or high concentrations of cortisol (predominant GR activation) have deleterious effects (135, 265267). In support of the beneficial effects of MRs on cognition, Schultebraucks et al. (185) used a repeated-measures crossover design and either administered patients with AD fludrocortisone (resulting in high MR occupation) or withheld the same drug from them (resulting in low MR occupation). Verbal memory performance was significantly better when MR occupation was high, and there also were trends towards better executive functioning in the condition.

Previous studies have shown that, in healthy adults across the lifespan, elevated cortisol levels impair cognitive functioning in ways that are predictable and that can be explained neurobiologically (105, 142, 166, 252). In AD, cortisol concentrations fluctuate –elevated far above basal levels (e.g., after hydrocortisone administration) or low [e.g., several hours after hydrocortisone medication has been taken and due to the fact that this medication has a relatively short half-life of roughly 1.5 hours; (223)]. Since the relationship between cognition and GCs usually follows an inverted-U shaped pattern, cortisol concentration variability in patients with AD may play an important role in their cognitive functioning. Furthermore, due to the known association between altered cortisol and impaired performance on standardized memory tests, between altered cortisol and disrupted sleep, and between sleep and memory consolidation, assessment of other contributors (e.g., disrupted sleep) that may contribute to deficient memory performance in patients with AD needs to be understood.

The Relationship Between Sleep and Cognition in AD

The orderly night-time sequence and transition between SWS and REM sleep provides optimal conditions for memory consolidation (1). Consolidation begins during SWS, when specific physiological conditions (e.g., slow oscillations in neocortical networks, HPA axis suppression) allow the reactivation of memories encoded during wakefulness (268). During REM sleep, physiological conditions (e.g., suppression of norepinephrine, increased levels of acetylcholine and serotonin, ponto-geniculo-occipital and theta waves) allow reactivated memories to be integrated with pre-existing knowledge, thereby facilitating long-term potentiation (269). Cortisol’s influence on successful memory consolidation during healthy sleep is accounted for because it plays a pivotal role in sleep stage initiation and maintenance (68). Although a well-known relationship between healthy sleep and optimal memory performance is noted (270), limited studies have explored this association in patients with AD.

Henry and colleagues (7) obtained data from self-reported questionnaires and suggest that memory impairment may be mediated by sleep disruptions in AD. Henry et al. (5) investigated the relationship between adrenal function, and objectively measured sleep and cognitive performance. Results showed that periods of sleep rather than wake benefited declarative memory retention in healthy controls’ but not in patients with AD. These findings concur with a large body of literature indicating that a full night of uninterrupted sleep has positive effects on memory. Because patients with AD do not have normal circadian rhythmicity, the sequence and transitions of sleep stages may not have occurred in such a way that is required for successful memory consolidation. Another possible explanation is that patients with AD are generally fatigued, and that therefore poor performance occurs whether one sleeps or not. The design of this study did not allow consideration that patients with AD may suffer from global fatigue and therefore poor performance. Another finding in this study was that, on a story recall test, patients had greater recall when a period of wake rather than wake separated learning from recall (counterintuitive to the body of literature that sleep is an offline process beneficial for the consolidation of learned information). This result corroborates in patients with AD that sleep may not be providing an optimal period for consolidation of previously learned material. In contrast to the patterns of data on declarative memory tests, no significant between-group (AD versus controls) or between-condition (Sleep versus Wake) were found on a test of procedural memory. These results may have emerged because declarative memory tasks are hippocampal-dependent, whereas procedural tasks are not. Since hydrocortisone affects hippocampal integrity but not areas typically associated with response-based sequence learning (e.g., motor cortex, caudate nucleus), it is possible that procedural memory performance of patients with AD is unimpaired. No prior study had investigated procedural memory in patients with AD, and hence this suggestion that procedural memory is not impaired in patients with AD is a novel finding.

The Relationship Between Sleep, Emotion Regulation, and Cognition

While it is well established that sleep plays a crucial role in various aspects of health, and cognition, sleep also plays an important role in the processing and regulation of emotion (1, 69, 79, 271). The experience of sleep deprivation or poor sleep quality makes people more sensitive to emotional and stressful events on the following day, elevates negative emotions (including feeling more irritable, angry, and anxious), and reduces positive emotions (272275). Short sleep duration and poor-quality sleep is also associated with elevated depressive symptoms (276). REM sleep plays a particularly important role in emotion regulation (79, 277), with research showing that patients with mood disorders have altered REM sleep (278). Because of cortisols key role in sleep stage initiation and maintenance, it has an important influence on the affect regulation that takes place during healthy sleep (73). HPA hyperactivity (and consequent elevated cortisol levels, for example) plays a crucial role in the pathogenesis of medical and psychiatric disorders (e.g., major depressive disorder (MDD)) that are marked by sleep disturbances alongside mood problems (34, 279). The co-occurring presence of HPA-axis hyperactivity, sleep disturbance and mood problems in these disorders is not coincidental. Clinical studies have implied that patients with nearly all neurological and psychiatric mood disorders have co-occurring sleep abnormalities, and specifically, problems with REM sleep (79, 271). Empirical studies show that when people are deprived of REM sleep they have intensified experience of negative emotions, show increased anxiety during stressful events, and exhibit less positive reactions to positive events (275, 280).

Previously published studies in patients with AD have consistently shown that, despite being on replacement therapy, these individuals still report and experience depression and anxiety, as well as reduced stress tolerance and reduced ability to cope with daily demands (5, 236, 281). Regarding affective disorders, a Danish study of 989 patients with AD found they were 2.68 times more likely to suffer from depression than a control group with osteoarthritis (281).

Generally depressed patients self-report difficulty falling and staying asleep, and early morning awakenings; (282), and experience both SWS and REM disruptions (278). Interestingly, individuals who take hydrocortisone late at night also have decreased REM latency and increased REM sleep time, a pattern similar to that found in depressed patients. As such, altered circadian rhythms in patients with AD may explain the high presence of affective disorders in this population.

In addition to the wealth of knowledge linking sleep and affect regulation, numerous studies illustrate that affect and cognition are interrelated (283, 284), and specifically that low mood is related to impaired cognitive functioning (285, 286). The high presence of affective symptoms in patients with AD may be related to patients sleep disturbances. The high presence of affective symptoms in patients with AD may also negatively impact cognitive functioning. That is to say, the co-occurring presence of sleep disturbances, depressive symptoms, and impaired cognitive functioning in patients with AD may not be coincidental. However, no published study has explored the relationship between affect dysregulation, sleep disturbances and cognitive impairment in AD.

Conclusion

In this article, we have summarised the current knowledge on sleep, cognition, and the association between the two in patients with AD. Numerous studies indicate that (i) healthy sleep benefits memory consolidation, (ii) alterations in cortisol activity has negative effects on sleep architecture, and (iii) sleep disruptions (e.g., as might be present in individuals with abnormal night-time cortisol concentrations) might impede the beneficial effects of sleep on memory consolidation. Sub- and supra-physiological cortisol concentrations resulting from immediate release hydrocortisone replacement therapy can have negative effects on sleep architecture and sleep-dependent memory consolidation processes. Therefore, disrupted circadian rhythms are suspected to be a major cause of sleep disturbances and cognitive impairment in patients with AD. It is well established that cortisol plays a key role in maintaining the integrity of sleep architecture and that sleep plays an important role in cognitive functioning, emphasizing the interrelationship between sleep, cognition and intact cortisol secretion.

The literature suggests that patients with AD experience disruptions to cognition, primarily in the domain of declarative memory (both verbal and visual memory), but also extending to executive functioning (specifically, attention and processing speed). Procedural memory does not appear to be impaired in these patients (although very few studies have investigated this). They also experience a reduced quality of sleep and altered sleep architecture. Hydrocortisone immediate release may lead to disrupted sleep patterns which impair optimal consolidation of learned information. Moreover, general fatigue may contribute to the presence of cognitive deficits in patients with AD. Furthermore, prolonged replacement therapy may have deleterious effects on brain regions required for optimal cognitive functioning (e.g., the hippocampus and PFC). However, studies using brain scans are needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Although prior research suggests that both cognitive and sleep complaints are frequently reported by patients with AD, only a few have used objective measures to assess either sleep patterns or memory impairments experienced by patients. Patients with AD encounter sub- and supra-physiological cortisol concentrations due to imperfect replacement therapy, ultimately altering sleep architecture and impairing cognition. It is conceivable that through modifying the pharmacokinetics of replacement therapy that these modalities in patients with AD may be improved. From a broader neuroscientific perspective, patients with AD provide a unique opportunity to simultaneously study the effects of hyper- and hypo-cortisolism on sleep quality, memory performance, and sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Careful study of these patients can help unravel the distinct roles that sub- and supra-physiological GC concentrations play in sleep regulation/structure and in sleep-dependent memory consolidation.

Although current replacement therapy aims to mimic the natural circadian rhythm of cortisol, periods of sub- and supra-physiological cortisol concentration are experienced. Both low and high cortisol concentrations can negatively impact cognition and sleep. More research is needed on the effects of dosage, duration and type of GC therapy used in patients with AD and how these impact cortisol concentrations, sleep and cognition.

Food and caffeine intake, smoking, intense exercise, and encountering stressful situations may all influence cortisol concentrations, sleep, and cognitive performance (287). Studies investigating sleep and cognition in AD should be careful to control for these potentially confounding factors. Another important contributor to cognition and sleep in patients with AD could be life-threatening events such as nocturnal hypoglycemia and adrenal crises (288). However, hardly any studies take this into account when investigating cognition and sleep. It is important to differentiate between impairments caused by the illness itself, the complications going with it or the therapy received by patients.

More studies are needed to characterize the relationship between sleep and memory, using objective measures to examine the hypothesis that poor sleep is a biological mechanism underlying memory impairment in patients with AD. More polysomnographic studies are needed to comprehensively investigate sleep architecture in patients with AD. Such studies may help explain for instance, memory consolidation in not enhanced by sleep in patients as in healthy controls. Intervention studies and clinical trials might seek to confirm this association and investigate whether the same pattern of sleep and memory deficits are present in patients, using modified-release or dual-release hydrocortisone. If it is confirmed that disrupted sleep is a vital mechanism underlying the impaired consolidation of previously learnt information, clinicians should prioritize treatment of disrupted sleep in patients with AD.

Author Contributions

Review written by MH and IR and edited by KT. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ On the other hand, acutely elevated GCs can either enhance (114, 134138) or impair memory depending on several factors, including but not limited to, the time of day of cognitive testing, the stage at which a stressor is applied (i.e., at encoding, consolidation or retrieval), and the dose of GC administered (139, 140).

References

1. Diekelmann S, Born J. The Memory Function of Sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci (2010) 11(2):114–26. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Diekelmann S, Wilhelm I, Born J. The Whats and Whens of Sleep-Dependent Memory Consolidation. Sleep Med Rev (2009) 13(5):309–21. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Løvås K, Husebye ES, Holsten F, Bjorvatn B. Sleep Disturbances in Patients With Addison’s Disease. Eur J Endocrinol/Eur Fed Endocr Soc (2003) 148(4):449–56. doi: 10.1530/eje.0.1480449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Henry M, Ross IL, Thomas KGF. Reduced Slow-Wave Sleep and Altered Diurnal Cortisol Rhythms in Patients With Addison’s Disease. Eur J Endocrinol/Eur Fed Endocr Soc (2018) 179(5):319–30. doi: 10.1530/EJE-18-0439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Henry M, Ross IL, Wolf PSA, Thomas KGF. Impaired Quality and Efficiency of Sleep Impairs Cognitive Functioning in Addison’s Disease. Psychoneuroendocrino (2017) 78:237–45. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Henry M, Thomas KGF, Ross IL. Episodic Memory Impairment in Addison’s Disease: Results From a Telephonic Cognitive Assessment. Metab Brain Dis (2014) 29(2):421–30. doi: 10.1007/s11011-014-9511-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Henry M, Wolf PS, Ross IL, Thomas KG. Poor Quality of Life, Depressed Mood, and Memory Impairment may be Mediated by Sleep Disruption in Patients With Addison’s Disease. Physiol Behav (2015) 151:379–85. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Klement J, Hubold C, Hallschmid M, Loeck C, Oltmanns KM, Lehnert H, et al. Effects of Glucose Infusion on Neuroendocrine and Cognitive Parameters in Addison Disease. Metabolism (2009) 58(12):1825–31. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2009.06.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Borbély AA, Daan S, Wirz-Justice A, Deboer T. The Two-Process Model of Sleep Regulation: A Reappraisal. J Sleep Res (2016) 25(2):131–43. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Vassalli A, Dijk DJ. Sleep Function: Current Questions and New Approaches. Eur J Neurosci (2009) 29(9):1830–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06767.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Normal Human Sleep: An Overview. In: Kryger T, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 5th ed. St. Louis: Elsivier Saunders (2011). p. 16–26.

Google Scholar

12. Chokroverty S. Overview of Normal Sleep. In: Sleep Disorders Medicine. New York: Springer (2017). p. 5–27.

Google Scholar

13. Markov D, Goldman M. Normal Sleep and Circadian Rhythms: Neurobiologic Mechanisms Underlying Sleep and Wakefulness. Psychiatr Clinics (2006) 29(4):841–53. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2006.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Van Cauter E, Turek F. Roles of Sleep-Wake and Dark-Light Cycles in the Control of Endocrine, Metabolic, Cardiovascular and Cognitive Function. Section (2000) 7:313–30. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp070415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Van Reeth O, Weibel L, Spiegel K, Leproult R, Dugovic C, Maccari S. Physiology of Sleep (Review)–Interactions Between Stress and Sleep: From Basic Research to Clinical Situations. Sleep Med Rev (2000) 4(2):201–19. doi: 10.1053/smrv.1999.0097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Vgontzas AN, Chrousos GP. Sleep, the Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis, and Cytokines: Multiple Interactions and Disturbances in Sleep Disorders. Endocrinol Metab Clinics (2002) 31(1):15–36. doi: 10.1016/S0889-8529(01)00005-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mistlberger RE, Rusak B. Circadian Rhythms in Mammals: Formal Properties and Environmental Influences. In: Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4th ed. Springer, Berlin (2005). p. 321–34.

Google Scholar

18. Pauls SD, Honma K-I, Honma S, Silver R. Deconstructing Circadian Rhythmicity With Models and Manipulations. Trends Neurosci (2016) 39(6):405–19. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Chung S, Son GH, Kim K. Circadian Rhythm of Adrenal Glucocorticoid: Its Regulation and Clinical Implications. Biochim Biophys Acta (BBA)-Molec Basis Dis (2011) 1812(5):581–91. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Koch C, Leinweber B, Drengberg B, Blaum C, Oster H. Interaction Between Circadian Rhythms and Stress. Neurobiol Stress (2017) 6:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Angelousi A, Kassi E, Nasiri-Ansari N, Weickert MO, Randeva H, Kaltsas G. Clock Genes Alterations and Endocrine Disorders. Eur J Clin Invest (2018) 2018:e12927. doi: 10.1111/eci.12927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Takahashi JS. Transcriptional Architecture of the Mammalian Circadian Clock. Nat Rev Genet (2017) 18(3):164–79. doi: 10.1038/nrg.2016.150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Benedict C, Kern W, Schmid SM, Schultes B, Born J, Hallschmid M. Early Morning Rise in Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Activity: A Role for Maintaining the Brain’s Energy Balance. Psychoneuroendocrino (2009) 34(3):455–62. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hirotsu C, Tufik S, Andersen ML. Interactions Between Sleep, Stress, and Metabolism: From Physiological to Pathological Conditions. Sleep Sci (2015) 8(3):143–52. doi: 10.1016/j.slsci.2015.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Steiger A. Neurochemical Regulation of Sleep. J Psychiatr Res (2007) 41(7):537–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Steiger A. Sleep and the Hypothalamo–Pituitary–Adrenocortical System. Sleep Med Rev (2002) 6(2):125–38. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Steiger A. Sleep and Endocrine Regulation. Front Biosci (2003) 8:s358–76. doi: 10.2741/1055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Buckley TM, Schatzberg AF. On the Interactions of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) Axis and Sleep: Normal HPA Axis Activity and Circadian Rhythm, Exemplary Sleep Disorders. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2005) 90(5):3106–14. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-1056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Chorous G, Vgontzas AN, Kritikou I. HPA Axis and Sleep. Endotext [Internet]. South Dartmouth (MA) (2016).

Google Scholar

30. van Dalfsen JH, Markus CR. The Influence of Sleep on Human Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) Axis Reactivity: A Systematic Review. Sleep Med Rev (2017) 39:187–94. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Dijk D-J, Lockley SW. Invited Review: Integration of Human Sleep-Wake Regulation and Circadian Rhythmicity. J Appl Physiol (2002) 92(2):852–62. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00924.2001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Skeldon AC, Derks G, Dijk D-J. Modelling Changes in Sleep Timing and Duration Across the Lifespan: Changes in Circadian Rhythmicity or Sleep Homeostasis? Sleep Med Rev (2016) 28:96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.05.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Steiger A. Sleep and the Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenocortical System. Sleep Med Rev (2002) 6(2):125–38. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Balbo M, Leproult R, Van Cauter E. Impact of Sleep and Its Disturbances on Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Activity. Int J Endocrinol (2010) 2010. doi: 10.1155/2010/759234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Born J, Späth-Schwalbe E, Schwakenhofer H, Kern W, Fehm H. Influences of Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone, Adrenocorticotropin, and Cortisol on Sleep in Normal Man. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1989) 68(5):904–11. doi: 10.1210/jcem-68-5-904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Follenius M, Brandenberger G, Bandesapt J, Libert J, Ehrhart J. Nocturnal Cortisol Release in Relation to Sleep Structure. Sleep (1992) 15(1):21–7. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.1.21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Neylan TC, Lenoci M, Maglione ML, Rosenlicht NZ, Metzler TJ, Otte C, et al. Delta Sleep Response to Metyrapone in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology (2003) 28(9):1666. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Born J, Schenk U, Späth-Schwalbe E, Fehm H. Influences of Partial REM Sleep Deprivation and Awakenings on Nocturnal Cortisol Release. Biol Psychiatry (1988) 24(7):801–11. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90256-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Fehm H, Späth-Schwalbe E, Pietrowsky R, Kern W, Born J. Entrainment of Nocturnal Pituitary-Adrenocortical Activity to Sleep Processes in Man—a Hypothesis. Exp Clin Endocrinol Diabetes (1993) 101(05):267–76. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1211243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Chang F-C, Opp MR. Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone (CRH) as a Regulator of Waking. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2001) 25(5):445–53. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00024-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Papanicolaou DA, Kales A, Stratakis CA, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Correlates With the Overall Activities of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis and Sympathetic System in Healthy Humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1997) 82(10):3278–80. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.10.4307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Steiger A, Pawlowski M, Kimura M. Sleep Electroencephalography as a Biomarker in Depression. ChronoPhysiol Ther (2015) 5:15–25. doi: 10.2147/CPT.S41760

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Brzezinski A, Vangel MG, Wurtman RJ, Norrie G, Zhdanova I, Ben-Shushan A, et al. Effects of Exogenous Melatonin on Sleep: A Meta-Analysis. Sleep Med Rev (2005) 9(1):41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2004.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cajochen C, Kräuchi K, Wirz-Justice A. Role of Melatonin in the Regulation of Human Circadian Rhythms and Sleep. J Neuroendocrinol (2003) 15(4):432–7. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00989.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Van Cauter E, Refetoff S. Multifactorial Control of the 24-Hour Secretory Profiles of Pituitary Hormones. J Endocrinol Invest (1985) 8(4):381–91. doi: 10.1007/BF03348519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Späth-Schwalbe E, Gofferje M, Kern W, Born J, Fehm H. Sleep Disruption Alters Nocturnal ACTH and Cortisol Secretory Patterns. Biol Psychiatry (1991) 29(6):575–84. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(91)90093-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary Cortisol. Encycl Stress (2000) 3:379–83. doi: 10.1159/000118611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Von Auer K, Jobst S, et al. Free Cortisol Levels After Awakening: A Reliable Biological Marker for the Assessment of Adrenocortical Activity. Life Sci (1997) 61(26):2539–49. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(97)01008-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Smyth N, Thorn L, Hucklebridge F, Evans P, Clow A. Detailed Time Course of the Cortisol Awakening Response in Healthy Participants. Psychoneuroendocrino (2015) 62:200–3. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Steptoe A, Serwinski B. Cortisol Awakening Response. In: Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier (2016). p. 277–83.

Google Scholar

51. Born J, Hansen K, Marshall L, Mölle M, Fehm HL. Timing the End of Nocturnal Sleep. Nature (1999) 397(6714):29. doi: 10.1038/16166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Clow A, Hucklebridge F, Stalder T, Evans P, Thorn L. The Cortisol Awakening Response: More Than a Measure of HPA Axis Function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 35(1):97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Fries E, Dettenborn L, Kirschbaum C. The Cortisol Awakening Response (CAR): Facts and Future Directions. Int J Psychophysiol (2009) 72(1):67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Charles LE, Fekedulegn D, Burchfiel CM, Hartley TA, Andrew ME, Violanti JM, et al. Shiftwork and Diurnal Salivary Cortisol Patterns Among Police Officers. J Occup Environ Med/Am Coll Occup Environ Med (2016) 58(6):542. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Goichot B, Weibel L, Chapotot F, Gronfier C, Piquard F, Brandenberger G. Effect of the Shift of the Sleep-Wake Cycle on Three Robust Endocrine Markers of the Circadian Clock. Am J Physiol-Endocrinol Metab (1998) 275(2):E243–E8. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1998.275.2.E243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Hung EWM, Aronson KJ, Leung M, Day A, Tranmer J. Shift Work Parameters and Disruption of Diurnal Cortisol Production in Female Hospital Employees. Chronobiol Int (2016) 33(8):1045–55. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2016.1196695

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Klumpers UM, Veltman DJ, van Tol M-J, Kloet RW, Boellaard R, Lammertsma AA, et al. Neurophysiological Effects of Sleep Deprivation in Healthy Adults, a Pilot Study. PloS One (2015) 10(1):e0116906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Späth-Schwalbe E, Schöller T, Kern W, Fehm H, Born J. Nocturnal Adrenocorticotropin and Cortisol Secretion Depends on Sleep Duration and Decreases in Association With Spontaneous Awakening in the Morning. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1992) 75(6):1431–5. doi: 10.1210/jc.75.6.1431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Wright KP Jr., Drake AL, Frey DJ, Fleshner M, Desouza CA, Gronfier C, et al. Influence of Sleep Deprivation and Circadian Misalignment on Cortisol, Inflammatory Markers, and Cytokine Balance. Brain Behav Immun (2015) 47:24–34. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Chrousos GP, Gold PW. The Concepts of Stress and Stress System Disorders: Overview of Physical and Behavioral Homeostasis. JAMA (1992) 267(9):1244–52. doi: 10.1001/jama.267.9.1244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Vgontzas AN, Zoumakis M, Bixler EO, Lin H-M, Prolo P, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Impaired Nighttime Sleep in Healthy Old Versus Young Adults Is Associated With Elevated Plasma Interleukin-6 and Cortisol Levels: Physiologic and Therapeutic Implications. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2003) 88(5):2087–95. doi: 10.1210/jc.2002-021176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Chapotot F, Buguet A, Gronfier C, Brandenberger G. Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Activity Is Related to the Level of Central Arousal: Effect of Sleep Deprivation on the Association of High-Frequency Waking Electroencephalogram With Cortisol Release. Neuroendocrinology (2001) 73(5):312–21. doi: 10.1159/000054648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Leproult R, Copinschi G, Buxton O, Van Cauter E. Sleep Loss Results in an Elevation of Cortisol Levels the Next Evening. Sleep (1997) 20(10):865–70. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.10.865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Meerlo P, Koehl M, van der Borght K, Turek F. Sleep Restriction Alters the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Response to Stress. J Neuroendocrinol (2002) 14(5):397–402. doi: 10.1046/j.0007-1331.2002.00790.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Minkel J, Moreta M, Muto J, Htaik O, Jones C, Basner M, et al. Sleep Deprivation Potentiates HPA Axis Stress Reactivity in Healthy Adults. Health Psychol (2014) 33(11):1430. doi: 10.1037/a0034219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Treuer K, Norman TR, Armstrong SM. Overnight Human Plasma Melatonin, Cortisol, Prolactin, TSH, Under Conditions of Normal Sleep, Sleep Deprivation, and Sleep Recovery. J Pineal Res (1996) 20(1):7–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-079X.1996.tb00232.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Guzman-Marin R, McGinty D. Sleep Deprivation Suppresses Adult Neurogenesis: Clues to the Role of Sleep in Brain Plasticity. Sleep Biol Rhythms (2006) 4(1):27–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2006.00203.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Bennion KA, Mickley Steinmetz KR, Kensinger EA, Payne JD. Sleep and Cortisol Interact to Support Memory Consolidation. Cereb Cortex (2015) 25(3):646–57. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Payne JD, Kensinger EA. Sleep’s Role in the Consolidation of Emotional Episodic Memories. Curr Dir psychol Sci (2010) 19(5):290–5. doi: 10.1177/0963721410383978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Scullin MK, Bliwise DL. Sleep, Cognition, and Normal Aging: Integrating a Half Century of Multidisciplinary Research. Perspect psychol Sci (2015) 10(1):97–137. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Spencer RM, Walker MP, Stickgold R. Sleep and Memory Consolidation. In: Sleep Disorders Medicine. New York: Springer (2017). p. 205–23.

Google Scholar

72. Stickgold R. Sleep-Dependent Memory Consolidation. Nature (2005) 437(7063):1272. doi: 10.1038/nature04286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Payne JD, Nadel L. Sleep, Dreams, and Memory Consolidation: The Role of the Stress Hormone Cortisol. Learn Mem (2004) 11(6):671–8. doi: 10.1101/lm.77104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Maquet P. The Role of Sleep in Learning and Memory. Science (2001) 294(5544):1048–52. doi: 10.1126/science.1062856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Born J, Wilhelm I. System Consolidation of Memory During Sleep. psychol Res (2012) 76(2):192–203. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Walker MP. Sleep-Dependent Memory Processing. Harvard Rev Psychiatry (2008) 16(5):287–98. doi: 10.1080/10673220802432517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Giuditta A. Sleep Memory Processing: The Sequential Hypothesis. Front Syst Neurosci (2014) 8:219. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Stickgold R, Walker MP. Sleep-Dependent Memory Triage: Evolving Generalization Through Selective Processing. Nat Neurosci (2013) 16(2):139. doi: 10.1038/nn.3303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Walker MP. The Role of Sleep in Cognition and Emotion. Ann New York Acad Sci (2009) 1156(1):168–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Dang-Vu TT, Desseilles M, Peigneux P, Maquet P. A Role for Sleep in Brain Plasticity. Pediatr Rehabil (2006) 9(2):98–118. doi: 10.1080/13638490500138702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Gais S, Born J. Declarative Memory Consolidation: Mechanisms Acting During Human Sleep. Learn Memory (2004) 11(6):679–85. doi: 10.1101/lm.80504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Marshall L, Born J. The Contribution of Sleep to Hippocampus-Dependent Memory Consolidation. Trends Cognit Sci (2007) 11(10):442–50. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Nishida M, Pearsall J, Buckner RL, Walker MP. REM Sleep, Prefrontal Theta, and the Consolidation of Human Emotional Memory. Cereb Cortex (2008) 19(5):1158–66. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC. Current Concepts in Procedural Consolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci (2004) 5(7):576. doi: 10.1038/nrn1426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Lahl O, Wispel C, Willigens B, Pietrowsky R. An Ultra Short Episode of Sleep Is Sufficient to Promote Declarative Memory Performance. J Sleep Res (2008) 17(1):3–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00622.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Tucker MA, Hirota Y, Wamsley EJ, Lau H, Chaklader A, Fishbein W. A Daytime Nap Containing Solely Non-REM Sleep Enhances Declarative But Not Procedural Memory. Neurobiol Learn Memory (2006) 86(2):241–7. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Fischer S, Hallschmid M, Elsner AL, Born J. Sleep Forms Memory for Finger Skills. P Natl Acad Sci USA (2002) 99(18):11987–91. doi: 10.1073/pnas.182178199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Stickgold R, Walker MP. Sleep-Dependent Memory Consolidation and Reconsolidation. Sleep Med (2007) 8(4):331–43. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice With Sleep Makes Perfect: Sleep-Dependent Motor Skill Learning. Neuron (2002) 35(1):205–11. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00746-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Ellenbogen JM, Hulbert JC, Jiang Y, Stickgold R. The Sleeping Brain’s Influence on Verbal Memory: Boosting Resistance to Interference. PloS One (2009) 4(1):e4117. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Griessenberger H, Hoedlmoser K, Heib D, Lechinger J, Klimesch W, Schabus M. Consolidation of Temporal Order in Episodic Memories. Biol Psychol (2012) 91(1):150–5. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Wilhelm I, Diekelmann S, Molzow I, Ayoub A, Mölle M, Born J. Sleep Selectively Enhances Memory Expected to be of Future Relevance. J Neurosci (2011) 31(5):1563–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-10.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joëls M. Functional Implications of Brain Corticosteroid Receptor Diversity. Cell Mol Neurobiol (1993) 13(4):433–55. doi: 10.1007/BF00711582

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Kirschbaum C, Wolf O, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer D. Stress-And Treatment-Induced Elevations of Cortisol Levels Associated With Impaired Declarative Memory in Healthy Adults. Life Sci (1996) 58(17):1475–83. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(96)00118-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Sex Differences in HPA Axis Responses to Stress: A Review. Biol Psychol (2005) 69(1):113–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Lupien S, Gillin CJ, Hauger RL. Working Memory Is More Sensitive Than Declarative Memory to the Acute Effects of Corticosteroids: A Dose–Response Study in Humans. Behav Neurosci (1999) 113(3):420. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.3.420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Wolf O, Atsak P, De Quervain D, Roozendaal B, Wingenfeld K. Stress and Memory: A Selective Review on Recent Developments in the Understanding of Stress Hormone Effects on Memory and Their Clinical Relevance. J Neuroendocrinol (2016) 28(8). doi: 10.1111/jne.12353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Bird CM, Burgess N. The Hippocampus and Memory: Insights From Spatial Processing. Nat Rev Neurosci (2008) 9(3):nrn2335. doi: 10.1038/nrn2335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Davachi L, DuBrow S. How the Hippocampus Preserves Order: The Role of Prediction and Context. Trends Cognit Sci (2015) 19(2):92–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Eldridge LL, Knowlton BJ, Furmanski CS, Bookheimer SY, Engel SA. Remembering Episodes: A Selective Role for the Hippocampus During Retrieval. Nat Neurosci (2000) 3(11):1149. doi: 10.1038/80671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical Role of the Hippocampus in Memory for Sequences of Events. Nat Neurosci (2002) 5(5):458. doi: 10.1038/nn834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Maguire EA, Intraub H, Mullally SL. Scenes, Spaces, and Memory Traces: What Does the Hippocampus do? Neurosci (2016) 22(5):432–9. doi: 10.1177/1073858415600389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Postle BR. The Hippocampus, Memory, and Consciousness. Neurol Conciousness (Second Edition): Elsevier (2016) p:349–63. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800948-2.00021-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and Declarative Memory: Role of the Hippocampus. Hippocampus (1998) 8(3):198–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The Stressed Hippocampus, Synaptic Plasticity and Lost Memories. Nat Rev Neurosci (2002) 3(6):453–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Domenech P, Koechlin E. Executive Control and Decision-Making in the Prefrontal Cortex. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2015) 1:101–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Donoso M, Collins AG, Koechlin E. Foundations of Human Reasoning in the Prefrontal Cortex. Science (2014) 344(6191):1481–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1252254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Euston DR, Gruber AJ, McNaughton BL. The Role of Medial Prefrontal Cortex in Memory and Decision Making. Neuron (2012) 76(6):1057–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Kane MJ, Engle RW. The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Working-Memory Capacity, Executive Attention, and General Fluid Intelligence: An Individual-Differences Perspective. Psychon Bull Rev (2002) 9(4):637–71. doi: 10.3758/BF03196323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Stokes MG. ‘Activity-Silent’working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex: A Dynamic Coding Framework. Trends Cognit Sci (2015) 19(7):394–405. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Alderson AL, Novack TA. Neurophysiological and Clinical Aspects of Glucocorticoids and Memory: A Review. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2002) 24(3):335–55. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.3.335.987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Atsak P, Guenzel FM, Kantar-Gok D, Zalachoras I, Yargicoglu P, Meijer OC, et al. Glucocorticoids Mediate Stress-Induced Impairment of Retrieval of Stimulus-Response Memory. Psychoneuroendocrino (2016) 67:207–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Lupien S, Lecours AR, Lussier I, Schwartz G, Nair N, Meaney MJ. Basal Cortisol Levels and Cognitive Deficits in Human Aging. J Neurosci (1994) 14(5):2893–903. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02893.1994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Lupien S, McEwen BS. The Acute Effects of Corticosteroids on Cognition: Integration of Animal and Human Model Studies. Brain Res Rev (1997) 24:1–27. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00004-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Lupien S, Wilkinson CW, Brière S, Ménard C, Kin NNY, Nair N. The Modulatory Effects of Corticosteroids on Cognition: Studies in Young Human Populations. Psychoneuroendocrino (2002) 27(3):401–16. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00061-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Maheu FS, Collicutt P, Kornik R, Moszkowski R, Lupien SJ. The Perfect Time to be Stressed: A Differential Modulation of Human Memory by Stress Applied in the Morning or in the Afternoon. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2005) 29(8):1281–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Newcomer JW, Craft S, Te H, Askins K, Bardgett M. Glucocorticoid-Induced Impairment in Declarative Memory Performance in Adult Humans. J Neurosci (1994) 14(4):2047–53. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-04-02047.1994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Schönfeld P, Ackermann K, Schwabe L. Remembering Under Stress: Different Roles of Autonomic Arousal and Glucocorticoids in Memory Retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrino (2014) 39:249–56. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Seeman TE, McEwen BS, Singer BH, Albert MS, Rowe JW. Increase in Urinary Cortisol Excretion and Memory Declines: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1997) 82(8):2458–65. doi: 10.1210/jc.82.8.2458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Young A, Sahakian B, Robbins T, Cowen P. The Effects of Chronic Administration of Hydrocortisone on Cognitive Function in Normal Male Volunteers. Psychopharmacology (1999) 145(3):260–6. doi: 10.1007/s002130051057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Arnsten AFT. Stress Signalling Pathways That Impair Prefrontal Cortex Structure and Function. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10(6):410–22. doi: 10.1038/nrn2648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Arnsten AFT, Raskind MA, Taylor FB, Connor DF. The Effects of Stress Exposure on Prefrontal Cortex: Translating Basic Research Into Successful Treatments for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Neurobiol Stress (2015) 1:89–99. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2014.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Luethi M, Meier B, Sandi C. Stress Effects on Working Memory, Explicit Memory, and Implicit Memory for Neutral and Emotional Stimuli in Healthy Men. Front Behav Neurosci (2009) 2:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.005.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. McCormick C, Lewis E, Somley B, Kahan T. Individual Differences in Cortisol Levels and Performance on a Test of Executive Function in Men and Women. Physiol Behav (2007) 91(1):87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Schoofs D, Wolf OT, Smeets T. Cold Pressor Stress Impairs Performance on Working Memory Tasks Requiring Executive Functions in Healthy Young Men. Behav Neurosci (2009) 123(5):1066. doi: 10.1037/a0016980

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Cornelisse S, Joëls M, Smeets T. A Randomized Trial on Mineralocorticoid Receptor Blockade in Men: Effects on Stress Responses, Selective Attention, and Memory. Neuropsychopharmacology (2011) 36(13):2720. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Elzinga BM, Roelofs K. Cortisol-Induced Impairments of Working Memory Require Acute Sympathetic Activation. Behav Neurosci (2005) 119(1):98. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.98

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Wolf O, Convit A, McHugh P, Kandil E, Thorn E, De Santi S, et al. Cortisol Differentially Affects Memory in Young and Elderly Men. Behav Neurosci (2001) 115(5):1002. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.5.1002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Yuen EY, Liu W, Karatsoreos IN, Feng J, McEwen BS, Yan Z. Acute Stress Enhances Glutamatergic Transmission in Prefrontal Cortex and Facilitates Working Memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2009) 106(33):14075–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906791106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Belanoff JK, Kalehzan M, Sund B, Fleming Ficek SK, Schatzberg AF. Cortisol Activity and Cognitive Changes in Psychotic Major Depression. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158(10):1612–6. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Gold PW, Drevets WC, Charney DS. New Insights Into the Role of Cortisol and the Glucocorticoid Receptor in Severe Depression. Bilogical Psychiatry (2002) 52:381–5. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01480-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Judd LL, Schettler PJ, Brown ES, Wolkowitz OM, Sternberg EM, Bender BG, et al. Adverse Consequences of Glucocorticoid Medication: Psychological, Cognitive, and Behavioral Effects. Am J Psychiatry (2014) 171(10):1045–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13091264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. McEwen BS, Sapolsky RM. Stress and Cognitive Function. Curr Opin Neurobiol (1995) 5(2):205–16. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Wolf O. HPA Axis and Memory. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab (2003) 17(2):287–99. doi: 10.1016/S1521-690X(02)00101-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joëls M. Stress and Cognition: Are Corticosteroids Good or Bad Guys? Trends Neurosci (1999) 22(10):422–6. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01438-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Abercrombie HC, Kalin NH, Thurow ME, Rosenkranz MA, Davidson RJ. Cortisol Variation in Humans Affects Memory for Emotionally Laden and Neutral Information. Behav Neurosci (2003) 117(3):505. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.505

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Smeets T, Giesbrecht T, Jelicic M, Merckelbach H. Context-Dependent Enhancement of Declarative Memory Performance Following Acute Psychosocial Stress. Biol Psychol (2007) 76(1-2):116–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Het S, Ramlow G, Wolf O. A Meta-Analytic Review of the Effects of Acute Cortisol Administration on Human Memory. Psychoneuroendocrino (2005) 30(8):771–84. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Kuhlmann S, Piel M, Wolf OT. Impaired Memory Retrieval After Psychosocial Stress in Healthy Young Men. J Neurosci (2005) 25(11):2977–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5139-04.2005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Shields GS, Sazma MA, McCullough AM, Yonelinas AP. The Effects of Acute Stress on Episodic Memory: A Meta-Analysis and Integrative Review. Psychol Bull (2017) 143(6):636. doi: 10.1037/bul0000100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Payne JD, Jackson ED, Hoscheidt S, Ryan L, Jacobs WJ, Nadel L. Stress Administered Prior to Encoding Impairs Neutral But Enhances Emotional Long-Term Episodic Memories. Learn Memory (2007) 14(12):861–8. doi: 10.1101/lm.743507

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Smeets T. Acute Stress Impairs Memory Retrieval Independent of Time of Day. Psychoneuroendocrino (2011) 36:495–501. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Wolf O, Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, McEwen BS, Kirschbaum C. The Relationship Between Stress Induced Cortisol Levels and Memory Differs Between Men and Women. Psychoneuroendocrino (2001) 26(7):711–20. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00025-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Buss C, Wolf OT, Witt J, Hellhammer DH. Autobiographic Memory Impairment Following Acute Cortisol Administration. Psychoneuroendocrino (2004) 29(8):1093–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Monk CS, Nelson CA. The Effects of Hydrocortisone on Cognitive and Neural Function: A Behavioral and Event-Related Potential Investigation. Neuropsychopharmacology (2002) 26(4):505–19. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00384-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Newcomer JW, Selke G, Melson AK, Hershey T, Craft S, Richards K, et al. Decreased Memory Performance in Healthy Humans Induced by Stress-Level Cortisol Treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56:527–33. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.6.527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Rimmele U, Domes G, Mathiak K, Hautzinger M. Cortisol has Different Effects on Human Memory for Emotional and Neutral Stimuli. Neuroreport (2003) 14(18):2485–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200312190-00038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Tops M, Pompe GVD, Baas D, Mulder LJ, Den Boer JA, Meijman TF, et al. Acute Cortisol Effects on Immediate Free Recall and Recognition of Nouns Depend on Stimulus Valence. Psychophysiology (2003) 40(2):167–73. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. De Quervain DJ, Henke K, Aerni A, Treyer V, McGaugh JL, Berthold T, et al. Glucocorticoid-Induced Impairment of Declarative Memory Retrieval Is Associated With Reduced Blood Flow in the Medial Temporal Lobe. Eur J Neurosci (2003) 17(6):1296–302. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02542.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. De Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM, McGaugh JL, Hock C. Acute Cortisone Administration Impairs Retrieval of Long-Term Declarative Memory in Humans. Nat Neurosci (2000) 3(4):313. doi: 10.1038/73873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Duncko R, Cornwell B, Cui L, Merikangas KR, Grillon C. Acute Exposure to Stress Improves Performance in Trace Eyeblink Conditioning and Spatial Learning Tasks in Healthy Men. Learn Memory (2007) 14(5):329–35. doi: 10.1101/lm.483807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Guenzel FM, Wolf OT, Schwabe L. Sex Differences in Stress Effects on Response and Spatial Memory Formation. Neurobiol Learn Memory (2014) 109:46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Luine V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen BS. Repeated Stress Causes Reversible Impairments of Spatial Memory Performance. Brain Res (1994) 639(1):167–70. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91778-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Lupien S, De Leon M, De Santi S, Convit A, Tarshish C, Nair NPV, et al. Cortisol Levels During Human Aging Predict Hippocampal Atrophy and Memory Deficits. Nat Neurosci (1998) 1(1):69. doi: 10.1038/271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Lupien S, Fiocco A, Wan N, Maheu F, Lord C, Schramek T, et al. Stress Hormones and Human Memory Function Across the Lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrino (2005) 30(3):225–42. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Schwabe L, Oitzl MS, Philippsen C, Richter S, Bohringer A, Wippich W, et al. Stress Modulates the Use of Spatial Versus Stimulus-Response Learning Strategies in Humans. Learn Memory (2007) 14(1-2):109–16. doi: 10.1101/lm.435807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Taverniers J, Van Ruysseveldt J, Smeets T, von Grumbkow J. High-Intensity Stress Elicits Robust Cortisol Increases, and Impairs Working Memory and Visuo-Spatial Declarative Memory in Special Forces Candidates: A Field Experiment. Stress (2010) 13(4):324–34. doi: 10.3109/10253891003642394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Thomas KG, Laurance HE, Nadel L, Jacobs WJ. Stress-Induced Impairment of Spatial Navigation in Females. South Afr J Psychol (2010) 40(1):44–53. doi: 10.1177/008124631004000104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Sutherland RJ. Virtual Navigation in Humans: The Impact of Age, Sex, and Hormones on Place Learning. Hormones Behav (2005) 47(3):326–35. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Klopp C, Garcia C, Schulman AH, Ward CP, Tartar JL. Acute Social Stress Increases Biochemical and Self Report Markers of Stress Without Altering Spatial Learning in Humans. Neuro Endocrinol Lett (2012) 33(4):425–30.

Google Scholar

161. McCormick C, Teillon SM. Menstrual Cycle Variation in Spatial Ability: Relation to Salivary Cortisol Levels. Hormones Behav (2001) 39(1):29–38. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. van Gerven DJ, Ferguson T, Skelton RW. Acute Stress Switches Spatial Navigation Strategy From Egocentric to Allocentric in a Virtual Morris Water Maze. Neurobiol Learn Memory (2016) 132:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Schwabe L, Römer S, Richter S, Dockendorf S, Bilak B, Schächinger H. Stress Effects on Declarative Memory Retrieval Are Blocked by a β-Adrenoceptor Antagonist in Humans. Psychoneuroendocrino (2009) 34(3):446–54. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress Modulates the Engagement of Multiple Memory Systems in Classification Learning. J Neurosci (2012) 32(32):11042–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1484-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Lupien S, Gaudreau S, Tchiteya B, Maheu F, Sharma S, Nair N, et al. Stress-Induced Declarative Memory Impairment in Healthy Elderly Subjects: Relationship to Cortisol Reactivity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1997) 82(7):2070–5. doi: 10.1210/jc.82.7.2070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Lupien S, Nair NP, Briere S, Maheu F, Tu MT, Lemay M, et al. Increased Cortisol Levels and Impaired Cognition in Human Aging: Implication for Depression and Dementia in Later Life. Rev Neurosci (1999) 10(2):117–39. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.1999.10.2.117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Nyberg L, Persson J, Habib R, Tulving E, McIntosh AR, Cabeza R, et al. Large Scale Neurocognitive Networks Underlying Episodic Memory. J Cogn Neurosci (2000) 12(1):163–73. doi: 10.1162/089892900561805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Burgess PW. Confabulation and the Control of Recollection. Memory (1996) 4(4):359–412. doi: 10.1080/096582196388906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Eichenbaum H. Prefrontal–hippocampal Interactions in Episodic Memory. Nat Rev Neurosci (2017) 18(9):547. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.74

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Stomby A, Boraxbekk C-J, Lundquist A, Nordin A, Nilsson L-G, Adolfsson R, et al. Higher Diurnal Salivary Cortisol Levels Are Related to Smaller Prefrontal Cortex Surface Area in Elderly Men and Women. Eur J Endocrinol/Eur Fed Endocr Soc (2016) 175(2):117–26. doi: 10.1530/EJE-16-0352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Liston C, Miller MM, Goldwater DS, Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Hof PR, et al. Stress-Induced Alterations in Prefrontal Cortical Dendritic Morphology Predict Selective Impairments in Perceptual Attentional Set-Shifting. J Neurosci (2006) 26(30):7870–4. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1184-06.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Moghaddam B. Stress Activation of Glutamate Neurotransmission in the Prefrontal Cortex: Implications for Dopamine-Associated Psychiatric Disorders. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 51(10):775–87. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01362-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular Basis of Working Memory. Neuron (1995) 14(3):477–85. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Lisman JE, Fellous J-M, Wang X-J. A Role for NMDA-Receptor Channels in Working Memory. Nat Neurosci (1998) 1(4):273. doi: 10.1038/1086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Wingenfeld K, Wolf OT. Effects of Cortisol on Cognition in Major Depressive Disorder, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Borderline Personality Disorder-2014 Curt Richter Award Winner. Psychoneuroendocrino (2015) 51:282–95. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. de Kloet ER. From Receptor Balance to Rational Glucocorticoid Therapy. Endocrinology (2014) 155(8):2754–69. doi: 10.1210/en.2014-1048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Vogel S, Fernández G, Joëls M, Schwabe L. Cognitive Adaptation Under Stress: A Case for the Mineralocorticoid Receptor. Trends Cognit Sci (2016) 20(3):192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. de Kloet ER, Vreugdenhil E, Oitzl MS, Joels M. Brain Corticosteroid Receptor Balance in Health and Disease. Endocr Rev (1998) 19(3):269–301. doi: 10.1210/edrv.19.3.0331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Roozendaal B. Systems Mediating Acute Glucocorticoid Effects on Memory Consolidation and Retrieval. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2003) 27(8):1213–23. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. de Kloet ER. Functional Profile of the Binary Brain Corticosteroid Receptor System: Mediating, Multitasking, Coordinating, Integrating. Eur J Pharmacol (2013) 719(1-3):53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2013.04.053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Hinkelmann K, Moritz S, Botzenhardt J, Riedesel K, Wiedemann K, Kellner M, et al. Cognitive Impairment in Major Depression: Association With Salivary Cortisol. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66(9):879–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Joëls M, Karst H, DeRijk R, de Kloet ER. The Coming Out of the Brain Mineralocorticoid Receptor. Trends Neurosci (2008) 31(1):1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Otte C, Wingenfeld K, Kuehl LK, Kaczmarczyk M, Richter S, Quante A, et al. Mineralocorticoid Receptor Stimulation Improves Cognitive Function and Decreases Cortisol Secretion in Depressed Patients and Healthy Individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology (2015) 40(2):386. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Rimmele U, Besedovsky L, Lange T, Born J. Blocking Mineralocorticoid Receptors Impairs, Blocking Glucocorticoid Receptors Enhances Memory Retrieval in Humans. Neuropsychopharmacology (2013) 38(5):884. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Schultebraucks K, Wingenfeld K, Heimes J, Quinkler M, Otte C. Cognitive Function in Patients With Primary Adrenal Insufficiency (Addison’s Disease). Psychoneuroendocrino (2015) 55:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Carpenter JW, Gruen PH. Cortisol’s Effects on Human Mental Functioning. J Clin Psychopharmacol (1982) 2(2):91–101. doi: 10.1097/00004714-198204000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Forget H, Lacroix A, Somma M, Cohen H. Cognitive Decline in Patients With Cushing’s Syndrome. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2000) 6(1):20–9. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700611037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Martignoni E, Costa A, Sinforiani E, Liuzzi A, Chiodini P, Mauri M, et al. The Brain as a Target for Adrenocortical Steroids: Cognitive Implications. Psychoneuroendocrino (1992) 17(4):343–54. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90040-E

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Mauri M, Sinforiani E, Bono G, Vignati F, Berselli M, Attanasio R, et al. Memory Impairment in Cushing’s Disease. Acta Neurol Scand (1993) 87(1):52–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1993.tb04075.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Mitchell AJ, Dening TR. Depression-Related Cognitive Impairment: Possibilities for Its Pharmacological Treatment. J Affect Disord (1996) 36(3-4):79–87. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00070-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Wolkowitz OM, Reus VI, Weingartner H, Thompson K, Breier A. Cognitive Effects of Corticosteroids. Am J Psychiatry (1990) 147(10):1297. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.10.1297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Conrad CD, Lupien SJ, McEwen BS. Support for a Bimodal Role for Type II Adrenal Steroid Receptors in Spatial Memory. Neurobiol Learn Memory (1999) 72(1):39–46. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Joëls M. Corticosteroid Effects in the Brain: U-Shape it. Trends Pharmacol Sci (2006) 27(5):244–50. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2006.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. McEwen BS, Magarinos A. Stress Effects on Morphology and Function of the Hippocampusa. Ann New York Acad Sci (1997) 821(1):271–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48286.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. McEwen BS, Nasca C, Gray JD. Stress Effects on Neuronal Structure: Hippocampus, Amygdala, and Prefrontal Cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41(1):3. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Luine V, Martinez C, Villegas M, Magariños AM, McEwen BS. Restraint Stress Reversibly Enhances Spatial Memory Performance. Physiol Behav (1996) 59(1):27–32. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02016-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Lupien S, Wilkinson C, Briere S, Ng Ying Kin N, Meaney M, Nair N. Acute Modulation of Aged Human Memory by Pharmacological Manipulation of Glucocorticoids. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2002) 87(8):3798–807. doi: 10.1210/jcem.87.8.8760

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Huang C-W, Lui C-C, Chang W-N, Lu C-H, Wang Y-L, Chang C-C. Elevated Basal Cortisol Level Predicts Lower Hippocampal Volume and Cognitive Decline in Alzheimer’s Disease. J Clin Neurosci (2009) 16(10):1283–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2008.12.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. McNally RJ. Cognitive Abnormalities in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Trends Cognit Sci (2006) 10(6):271–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Staufenbiel SM, Penninx BW, Spijker AT, Elzinga BM, van Rossum EF. Hair Cortisol, Stress Exposure, and Mental Health in Humans: A Systematic Review. Psychoneuroendocrino (2013) 38(8):1220–35. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Wang LY, Raskind MA, Wilkinson CW, Shofer JB, Sikkema C, Szot P, et al. Associations Between CSF Cortisol and CSF Norepinephrine in Cognitively Normal Controls and Patients With Amnestic MCI and AD Dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2018) 33(5):763–8. doi: 10.1002/gps.4856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

202. Yates KF, Sweat V, Yau PL, Turchiano MM, Convit A. Impact of Metabolic Syndrome on Cognition and Brain: A Selected Review of the Literature. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol (2012) 32(9):2060–7. doi: 10.1161/ATVBAHA.112.252759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

203. Ellenbogen JM, Payne JD, Stickgold R. The Role of Sleep in Declarative Memory Consolidation: Passive, Permissive, Active or None? Curr Opin Neurobiol (2006) 16(6):716–22. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Anglin R, Rosebush P, Mazurek M. The Neuropsychiatric Profile of Addison’s Disease: Revisiting a Forgotten Phenomenon. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2006) 18(4):450–9. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2006.18.4.450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

205. Arlt W, Allolio B. Adrenal Insufficiency. Lancet (2003) 361(9372):1881–93. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13492-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Ten S, New M, Maclaren N. Addison’s Disease 2001. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2001) 86(7):2909–22. doi: 10.1210/jcem.86.7.7636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Tytherleigh MY, Vedhara K, Lightman SL. Mineralocorticoid and Glucocorticoid Receptors and Their Differential Effects on Memory Performance in People With Addison’s Disease. Psychoneuroendocrino (2004) 29(6):712–23. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00103-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

208. Johannsson G, Falorni A, Skrtic S, Lennernäs H, Quinkler M, Monson JP, et al. Adrenal Insufficiency: Review of Clinical Outcomes With Current Glucocorticoid Replacement Therapy. Clin Endocrinol (2015) 82(1):2–11. doi: 10.1111/cen.12603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

209. Peacey SR, Guo CY, Robinson AM, Price A, Giles MA, Eastell R, et al. Glucocorticoid Replacement Therapy: Are Patients Over Treated and Does it Matter? Clin Endocrinol (1997) 46(3):255–61. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.1997.780907.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

210. Suliman AM, Freaney R, Smith TP, McBrinn Y, Murray B, McKenna TJ. The Impact of Different Glucocorticoid Replacement Schedules on Bone Turnover and Insulin Sensitivity in Patients With Adrenal Insufficiency. Clin Endocrinol (2003) 59(3):380–7. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.2003.01860.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

211. Mah PM, Jenkins RC, Rostami-Hodjegan A, Newell-Price J, Doane A, Ibbotson V, et al. Weight-Related Dosing, Timing and Monitoring Hydrocortisone Replacement Therapy in Patients With Adrenal Insufficiency. Clin Endocrinol (2004) 61(3):367–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2004.02106.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

212. Groves R, Toms G, Houghton B, Monson J. Corticosteroid Replacement Therapy: Twice or Thrice Daily? J R Soc Med (1988) 81(9):514. doi: 10.1177/014107688808100906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

213. Howlett TA. An Assessment of Optimal Hydrocortisone Replacement Therapy. Clin Endocrinol (1997) 46(3):263–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.1997.1340955.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

214. Blomgren J, Ekman B, Andersson PO, Arnqvist H. Non-Physiological Levels of Circulating Cortisol in Growth Hormone-Treated Hypopituitary Adults After Conventional Cortisone Substitution. Scand J Clin Lab Invest (2004) 64(2):132–9. doi: 10.1080/00365510410004867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

215. Chan S, Debono M. Replication of Cortisol Circadian Rhythm: New Advances in Hydrocortisone Replacement Therapy. Ther Adv Endocrinol Metab (2010) 1(3):129–38. doi: 10.1177/2042018810380214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

216. Debono M, Price JN, Ross RJ. Novel Strategies for Hydrocortisone Replacement. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab (2009) 23(2):221–32. doi: 10.1016/j.beem.2008.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

217. Derendorf H, Möllmann H, Barth J, Möllmann C, Tunn S, Krieg M. Pharmacokinetics and Oral Bioavailability of Hydrocortisone. J Clin Pharmacol (1991) 31(5):473–6. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.1991.tb01906.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

218. Johannsson G, Nilsson A, Bergthorsdottir R, Burman P, Dahlqvist P, Ekman B, et al. Improved Cortisol Exposure-Time Profile and Outcome in Patients With Adrenal Insufficiency: A Prospective Randomized Trial of a Novel Hydrocortisone Dual-Release Formulation. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2012) 97(2):473–81. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-1926

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

219. Simon N, Castinetti F, Ouliac F, Lesavre N, Brue T, Oliver C. Pharmacokinetic Evidence for Suboptimal Treatment of Adrenal Insufficiency With Currently Available Hydrocortisone Tablets. Clin Pharmacokinet (2010) 49(7):455–63. doi: 10.2165/11531290-000000000-00000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

220. Elder C, Dimitri P. Hydrocortisone for Adrenal Insufficiency. Arch Dis Childhood-Educ Pract (2015) 100(5):272–6. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-307325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

221. Arlt W. The Approach to the Adult With Newly Diagnosed Adrenal Insufficiency. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2009) 94(4):1059–67. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-0032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

222. Lennernäs H, Skrtic S, Johannsson G. Replacement Therapy of Oral Hydrocortisone in Adrenal Insufficiency: The Influence of Gastrointestinal Factors. Expert Opin Drug Metab Toxicol (2008) 4(6):749–58. doi: 10.1517/17425255.4.6.749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

223. Harbeck B, Kropp P, Mönig H. Effects of Short-Term Nocturnal Cortisol Replacement on Cognitive Function and Quality of Life in Patients With Primary or Secondary Adrenal Insufficiency: A Pilot Study. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2009) 34(2):113–9. doi: 10.1007/s10484-009-9082-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

224. Clow A, Thorn L, Evans P, Hucklebridge F. The Awakening Cortisol Response: Methodological Issues and Significance. Stress: Int J Biol Stress (2004) 7(1):29–37. doi: 10.1080/10253890410001667205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

225. Arlt W, Rosenthal C, Hahner S, Allolio B. Quality of Glucocorticoid Replacement in Adrenal Insufficiency: Clinical Assessment vs. Timed Serum Cortisol Measurements. Clin Endocrinol (2006) 64(4):384–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2006.02473.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

226. Løvås K, Husebye ES. Continuous Subcutaneous Hydrocortisone Infusion in Addison’s Disease. Eur J Endocrinol/Eur Fed Endocr Soc (2007) 157(1):109–12. doi: 10.1530/EJE-07-0052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

227. Merza Z, Rostami-Hodjegan A, Memmott A, Doane A, Ibbotson V, Newell-Price J, et al. Circadian Hydrocortisone Infusions in Patients With Adrenal Insufficiency and Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia. Clin Endocrinol (2006) 65(1):45–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2006.02544.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

228. Øksnes M, Björnsdottir S, Isaksson M, Methlie P, Carlsen S, Nilsen RM, et al. Continuous Subcutaneous Hydrocortisone Infusion Versus Oral Hydrocortisone Replacement for Treatment of Addison’s Disease: A Randomized Clinical Trial. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2014) 99(5):1665–74. doi: 10.1210/jc.2013-4253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

229. Gagliardi L, Nenke MA, Thynne TR, von der Borch J, Rankin WA, Henley DE, et al. Continuous Subcutaneous Hydrocortisone Infusion Therapy in Addison’s Disease: A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trial. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2014) 99(11):4149–57. doi: 10.1210/jc.2014-2433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

230. Debono M, Ghobadi C, Rostami-Hodjegan A, Huatan H, Campbell MJ, Newell-Price J, et al. Modified-Release Hydrocortisone to Provide Circadian Cortisol Profiles. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2009) 94(5):1548–54. doi: 10.1210/jc.2008-2380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

231. Johannsson G, Bergthorsdottir R, Nilsson AG, Lennernas H, Hedner T, Skrtic S. Improving Glucocorticoid Replacement Therapy Using a Novel Modified-Release Hydrocortisone Tablet: A Pharmacokinetic Study. Eur J Endocrinol/Eur Fed Endocr Soc (2009) 161(1):119–30. doi: 10.1530/EJE-09-0170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

232. Giordano R, Guaraldi F, Marinazzo E, Fumarola F, Rampino A, Berardelli R, et al. Improvement of Anthropometric and Metabolic Parameters, and Quality of Life Following Treatment With Dual-Release Hydrocortisone in Patients With Addison’s Disease. Endocrine (2016) 51(2):360–8. doi: 10.1007/s12020-015-0681-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

233. Mongioì LM, Condorelli RA, La Vignera S, Calogero AE. Dual-Release Hydrocortisone Treatment: Glycometabolic Profile and Health-Related Quality of Life. Endocr Connections (2018) 7(1):211–9. doi: 10.1530/EC-17-0368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

234. Quinkler M, Miodini Nilsen R, Zopf K, Ventz M, Øksnes M. Modified-Release Hydrocortisone Decreases BMI and HbA1c in Patients With Primary and Secondary Adrenal Insufficiency. Eur J Endocrinol (2015) 172(5):619–26. doi: 10.1530/EJE-14-1114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

235. Krekeler C, Kropp P, Blacha AK, Rahvar A-H, Harbeck B. Dual-Release Hydrocortisone and its Benefits on Cognitive Function and Quality of Sleep. Endocrine (2021) 72(1):223–33. doi: 10.1007/s12020-020-02552-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

236. Løvås K, Loge JH, Husebye ES. Subjective Health Status in Norwegian Patients With Addison’s Disease. Clin Endocrinol (2002) 56(5):581–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.2002.01466.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

237. van der Valk ES, Smans LC, Hofstetter H, Stubbe JH, Vries M, Backx FJ, et al. Decreased Physical Activity, Reduced QoL and Presence of Debilitating Fatigue in Patients With Addison’s Disease. Clin Endocrinol (2016) 85(3):354–60. doi: 10.1111/cen.13059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

238. García-Borreguero D, Wehr TA, Larrosa O, Granizo JJ, Hardwick D, Chrousos GP, et al. Glucocorticoid Replacement Is Permissive for Rapid Eye Movement Sleep and Sleep Consolidation in Patients With Adrenal Insufficiency 1. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2000) 85(11):4201–6. doi: 10.1210/jcem.85.11.6965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

239. Gillin J, Jacobs L, Snyder F, Henkin R. Effects of Decreased Adrenal Corticosteroids: Changes in Sleep in Normal Subjects and Patients With Adrenal Cortical Insufficiency. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1974) 36:283–9. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90170-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

240. Bliwise DL. Sleep in Normal Aging and Dementia. Sleep: J Sleep Res Sleep Med (1993) 160(1):40–81. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.1.40

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

241. Armitage R. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms in Mood Disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2007) 115(s433):104–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00968.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

242. Armitage R, Emslie GJ, Hoffmann RF, Rintelmann J, Rush AJ. Delta Sleep EEG in Depressed Adolescent Females and Healthy Controls. J Affect Disord (2001) 63(1):139–48. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00194-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

243. Koffel E, Khawaja IS, Germain A. Sleep Disturbances in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Updated Review and Implications for Treatment. Psychiatr Ann (2016) 46(3):173–6. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20160125-01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

244. Shipley JE, Schteingart DE, Tandon R, Starkman MN. Sleep Architecture and Sleep Apnea in Patients With Cushing’s Disease. Sleep (1992) 15(6):514–8. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.6.514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

245. Shipley JE, Schteingart DE, Tandon R, Pande AC, Grunhaus L, Haskett RF, et al. EEG Sleep in Cushing’s Disease and Cushing’s Syndrome: Comparison With Patients With Major Depressive Disorder. Biol Psychiatry (1992) 32(2):146–55. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90017-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

246. van den Bos R, Harteveld M, Stoop H. Stress and Decision-Making in Humans: Performance Is Related to Cortisol Reactivity, Albeit Differently in Men and Women. Psychoneuroendocrino (2009) 34:1449–58. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

247. Klement J, Hubold C, Cords H, Oltmanns KM, Hallschmid M, Born J, et al. High-Calorie Glucose-Rich Food Attenuates Neuroglycopenic Symptoms in Patients With Addison’s Disease. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2010) 95(2):522–8. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-1752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

248. Tiemensma J, Andela CD, Biermasz NR, Romijn JA, Pereira AM. Mild Cognitive Deficits in Patients With Primary Adrenal Insufficiency. Psychoneuroendocrino (2016) 63:170–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

249. Blacha AK, Rahvar AH, Flitsch J, van de Loo I, Kropp P, Harbeck B. Impaired Attention in Patients With Adrenal Insufficiency–Impact of Unphysiological Therapy. Steroids (2021) 167:108788. doi: 10.1016/j.steroids.2020.108788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

250. Andela CD, van Haalen FM, Ragnarsson O, Papakokkinou E, Johannsson G, Santos A, et al. Mechanisms in Endocrinology: Cushing’s Syndrome Causes Irreversible Effects on the Human Brain: A Systematic Review of Structural and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies. Eur J Endocrinol/Eur Fed Endocr Soc (2015) 173(1):R1–14. doi: 10.1530/EJE-14-1101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

251. Shansky RM, Lipps J. Stress-Induced Cognitive Dysfunction: Hormone-Neurotransmitter Interactions in the Prefrontal Cortex. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:123. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

252. Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. The Adrenocortical Axis in the Aged Rat: Impaired Sensitivity to Both Fast and Delayed Feedback Inhibition. Neurobiol Aging (1986) 7(5):331–5. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(86)90159-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

253. Arnsten AFT, Pliszka SR. Catecholamine Influences on Prefrontal Cortical Function: Relevance to Treatment of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Related Disorders. Pharmacol Biochem Be (2011) 99(2):211–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.01.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

254. Payne JD, Elie CJ, Blackwell JM, Neuschatz JS. Memory Illusions: Recalling, Recognizing, and Recollecting Events That Never Occurred. J Memory Lang (1996) 35(2):261–85. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

255. De Leon M, McRae T, Rusinek H, Convit A, De Santi S, Tarshish C, et al. Cortisol Reduces Hippocampal Glucose Metabolism in Normal Elderly, But Not in Alzheimer’s Disease. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1997) 82(10):3251–9. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.10.4305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

256. Geerlings MI, Sigurdsson S, Eiriksdottir G, Garcia ME, Harris TB, Gudnason V, et al. Salivary Cortisol, Brain Volumes, and Cognition in Community-Dwelling Elderly Without Dementia. Neurology (2015) p.976–83. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

257. Travis SG, Coupland NJ, Hegadoren K, Silverstone PH, Huang Y, Carter R, et al. Effects of Cortisol on Hippocampal Subfields Volumes and Memory Performance in Healthy Control Subjects and Patients With Major Depressive Disorder. J Affect Disord (2016) 201:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

258. Fleischer J, Metz S, Düsenberg M, Grimm S, Golde S, Roepke S, et al. Neural Correlates of Glucocorticoids Effects on Autobiographical Memory Retrieval in Healthy Women. Behav Brain Res (2018) 359:895–902. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.06.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

259. O’Brien JT, Schweitzer I, Ames D, Tuckwell V, Mastwyk M. Cortisol Suppression by Dexamethasone in the Healthy Elderly: Effects of Age, Dexamethasone Levels, and Cognitive Function. Biol Psychiatry (1994) 36(6):389–94. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91214-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

260. Schmidt LA, Fox NA, Goldberg MC, Smith CC, Schulkin J. Effects of Acute Prednisone Administration on Memory, Attention and Emotion in Healthy Human Adults. Psychoneuroendocrino (1999) 24(4):461–83. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00007-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

261. Joëls M. Corticosteroid Actions in the Hippocampus. J Neuroendocrinol (2001) 13(8):657–69. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2001.00688.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

262. Diamond DM, Bennett MC, Fleshner M, Rose GM. Inverted-U Relationship Between the Level of Peripheral Corticosterone and the Magnitude of Hippocampal Primed Burst Potentiation. Hippocampus (1992) 2(4):421–30. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

263. Pavlides C, Ogawa S, Kimura A, McEwen BS. Role of Adrenal Steroid Mineralocorticoid and Glucocorticoid Receptors in Long-Term Potentiation in the CA1 Field of Hippocampal Slices. Brain Res (1996) 738(2):229–35. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(96)00776-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

264. Shors TJ, Chua C, Falduto J. Sex Differences and Opposite Effects of Stress on Dendritic Spine Density in the Male Versus Female Hippocampus. J Neurosci (2001) 21(16):6292–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06292.2001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

265. Groch S, Wilhelm I, Lange T, Born J. Differential Contribution of Mineralocorticoid and Glucocorticoid Receptors to Memory Formation During Sleep. Psychoneuroendocrino (2013) 38(12):2962–72. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

266. Hinkelmann K, Wingenfeld K, Kuehl LK, Fleischer J, Heuser I, Wiedemann K, et al. Stimulation of the Mineralocorticoid Receptor Improves Memory in Young and Elderly Healthy Individuals. Neurobiol Aging (2015) 36(2):919–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

267. Lupien S, Maheu F, Tu M, Fiocco A, Schramek TE. The Effects of Stress and Stress Hormones on Human Cognition: Implications for the Field of Brain and Cognition. Brain Cogn (2007) 65(3):209–37. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

268. Born J, Rasch B, Gais S. Sleep to Remember. Neuroscientist (2006) 12(5):410–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858406292647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

269. Walker MP, Stickgold R. Overnight Alchemy: Sleep-Dependent Memory Evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci (2010) 11(3):218. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762-c1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

270. Dudai Y, Karni A, Born J. The Consolidation and Transformation of Memory. Neuron (2015) 88(1):20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

271. Palmer CA, Alfano CA. Sleep and Emotion Regulation: An Organizing, Integrative Review. Sleep Med Rev (2017) 31:6–16. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

272. Dagys N, McGlinchey EL, Talbot LS, Kaplan KA, Dahl RE, Harvey AG. Double Trouble? The Effects of Sleep Deprivation and Chronotype on Adolescent Affect. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2012) 53(6):660–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02502.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

273. Norlander T, Johansson Å, Bood SÅ. The Affective Personality: Its Relation to Quality of Sleep, Well-Being and Stress. Soc Behav Pers: Int J (2005) 33(7):709–22. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2005.33.7.709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

274. Cluydts R. Comparing the Effects of Sleep Loss After Experimental Sleep Deprivation and in Clinical Patients. Sleep Med Rev (2003) 7(4):293–5. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

275. Zohar D, Tzischinsky O, Epstein R, Lavie P. The Effects of Sleep Loss on Medical Residents’ Emotional Reactions to Work Events: A Cognitive-Energy Model. Sleep (2005) 28(1):47–54. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.1.47

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

276. Zhai L, Zhang H, Zhang D. Sleep Duration and Depression Among Adults: A Meta-Analysis of Prospective Studies. Depression Anxiety (2015) 32(9):664–70. doi: 10.1002/da.22386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

277. Wagner U, Hallschmid M, Rasch B, Born J. Brief Sleep After Learning Keeps Emotional Memories Alive for Years. Biol Psychiatry (2006) 60(7):788–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

278. Cartwright R, Baehr E, Kirkby J, Pandi-Perumal SR, Kabat J. REM Sleep Reduction, Mood Regulation and Remission in Untreated Depression. Psychiatry Res (2003) 121(2):159–67. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00236-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

279. Sonino N, Fava GA, Raffi AR, Boscaro M, Fallo F. Clinical Correlates of Major Depression in Cushing’s Disease. Psychopathology (1998) 31(6):302–6. doi: 10.1159/000029054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

280. Walker MP, van der Helm E. Overnight Therapy? The Role of Sleep in Emotional Brain Processing. psychol Bull (2009) 135(5):731. doi: 10.1037/a0016570

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

281. Thomsen AF, Kvist TK, Andersen PK, Kessing LV. The Risk of Affective Disorders in Patients With Adrenocortical Insufficiency. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2006) 31(5):614–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

282. Almeida OP, Pfaff JJ. Sleep Complaints Among Older General Practice Patients: Association With Depression. Br J Gen Pract (2005) 55(520):864–6.

Google Scholar

283. Schwarz N. Emotion, Cognition, and Decision Making. Cogn Emotion (2000) 14(4):433–40. doi: 10.1080/026999300402745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

284. Storbeck J, Clore GL. On the Interdependence of Cognition and Emotion. Cogn Emotion (2007) 21(6):1212–37. doi: 10.1080/02699930701438020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

285. Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD. Cognitive Impairment in Depression: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol Med (2014) 44(10):2029–40. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

286. Weber MT, Maki PM, McDermott MP. Cognition and Mood in Perimenopause: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol (2014) 142:90–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2013.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

287. King SL, Hegadoren KM. Stress Hormones: How do They Measure Up? Biol Res Nurs (2002) 4(2):92–103. doi: 10.1177/1099800402238334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

288. Meyer G, Hackemann A, Reusch J, Badenhoop K. Nocturnal Hypoglycemia Identified by a Continuous Glucose Monitoring System in Patients With Primary Adrenal Insufficiency (Addison’s Disease). Diabetes Technol Ther (2012) 14(5):386–8. doi: 10.1089/dia.2011.0158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Addison’s disease, cortisol, sleep, cognition, circadian rhythm

Citation: Henry M, Thomas KGF and Ross IL (2021) Sleep, Cognition and Cortisol in Addison’s Disease: A Mechanistic Relationship. Front. Endocrinol. 12:694046. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2021.694046

Received: 13 April 2021; Accepted: 02 August 2021;
Published: 27 August 2021.

Edited by:

Alberto Falorni, University of Perugia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Andrew Coogan, Maynooth University, Ireland
Christopher S. Colwell, University of California, Los Angeles, United States

Copyright © 2021 Henry, Thomas and Ross. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michelle Henry, bS5oZW5yeUB1Y3QuYWMuemE=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.