Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Endocrinol., 05 May 2021
Sec. Obesity
This article is part of the Research Topic Beyond Bariatric Surgery: Expected and Unexpected Long-Term Evolution View all 9 articles

Neurocognitive and Psychopathological Predictors of Weight Loss After Bariatric Surgery: A 4-Year Follow-Up Study

  • 1Psychiatric Chair, Department of Systems Medicine, University of Rome “Tor Vergata”, Rome, Italy
  • 2Cognitive and Clinical Psychology Laboratory, Department of Human Science, European University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 3Obesity Unit, Department of Surgery, University of Rome “Tor Vergata”, Rome, Italy

Twenty to thirty percent of patients experience weight regain at mid and long-term follow-up. Impaired cognitive functions are prevalent in people suffering from obesity and in those with binge eating disorder, thereby, affecting the weight-loss outcomes. The aim of our study was to investigate neurocognitive and psychopathological predictors of surgical efficacy in terms of percentage of excess weight loss (%EWL) at follow-up intervals of one year and 4-year. Psychosocial evaluation was completed in a sample of 78 bariatric surgery candidates and included psychometric instruments and a cognitive battery of neuropsychological tests. A schedule of 1-year and 4-year follow-ups was implemented. Wisconsin Sorting Card Test total correct responses, scores on the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test, and age predicted %EWL at, both, early and long-term periods after surgery while the severity of pre-operative binge eating (BED) symptoms were associated with lower %EWL only four years after the operation. Due to the role of pre-operative BED in weight loss maintenance, the affected patients are at risk of suboptimal response requiring ongoing clinical monitoring, and psychological and pharmacological interventions when needed. As a result of our findings and in keeping with the latest guidelines we encourage neuropsychological assessment of bariatric surgery candidates. This data substantiated the rationale of providing rehabilitative interventions tailored to cognitive domains and time specific to the goal of supporting patients in their post-surgical course.

Introduction

Individuals suffering from obesity reported mild and specific cognitive deficiencies compared to the general population (1, 2). Decreased executive functions, such as problem solving and planning, attention, memory, and inhibitory control were the most cited dysfunctions (3, 4). According to the theory of obesity occurring when the energy homeostasis is “out of balance”, difficulties in these neurocognitive abilities combined with the obesogenic environment may contribute to its pathogenesis (5). In fact, due to the growing abundance of readily available, highly palatable, calorie dense foods, it was advanced that a neurocognitive effort may be required to avoid maladaptive eating behaviors and weight gain (6).

Bariatric surgery is the leading treatment that contributes to the success of battling the obesity epidemic (7, 8). However, in terms of weight loss, the efficacy of surgical interventions may vary significantly between individuals, with 20% to 30% of patients experiencing insufficient weight loss and weight regain at mid and long-term after the operation (911). To date, there are only limited studies exploring the role of neurocognition on weight loss after surgery, with the most long-term findings being the association between cognitive dysfunction and suboptimal weight loss at 36-month follow-up (12).

Literature among psychopathological predictors of bariatric surgery efficacy yielded inconsistent results with the exception of some evidence regarding the negative role of binge eating behaviors and depressive symptoms on long-term outcomes (13). In fact, it was highlighted that by the end of the honeymoon period (i.e. the period of rapid and drastic weight loss that begins immediately after surgery and continues for six to 12 months), differences in patients’ weight loss trajectories became apparent (14, 15). More precisely, it was reported that either binge eating disorders (BED) or major depression consistently exhibited their negative effect on body mass index (BMI) evolution at least two years after surgery (1619).

Yet, these disorders did not represent surgical contraindications since findings were mixed and pathogenic mechanisms were not proven (20). Notably, patients with obesity and BED exhibited impaired executive functioning and inhibitory control compared to those with obesity alone (21). Moreover, psychiatric disorders such as depression were found to affect cognition in bariatric surgery candidates (22).

In view of this, a clear picture of the possible and independent effect of neurocognition, eating disorder, and psychopathology in the long-term period is warranted. Our study aimed to investigate neurocognitive and psychopathological predictors of weight loss after bariatric surgery, both, before and after the honeymoon period.

Materials and Methods

This research came from a larger prospective study investigating psychiatric aspects of bariatric surgery at the University of Rome, “Tor Vergata”, Italy (23, 24). The present study was performed in accordance with the Helsinki declaration standards and was approved by the Institutional Ethics Review Committee of the University of Rome “Tor Vergata”, with all the participants providing written informed consent.

Our sample consisted of 78 bariatric surgery candidates (59 women and 19 men; mean age: 44.88 ± 11.33 years) referred to the Obesity Unit at the University of Rome “Tor Vergata” for a preoperative psychosocial evaluation. A psychiatrist with training relevant to obesity performed the preoperative psychosocial–behavioral evaluation. Following the national guidelines for bariatric surgery (25), patients with a binge eating disorder and a psychiatric diagnosis were treated at either our center or at their local center. Some patients were prescribed medication, while others, depending on the severity of their symptoms, were submitted to psychotherapy. The clearance to surgery is made of a written report which is given to the patient at the end of the evaluation/treatment pathway (26). All patients were administered psychometric instruments and a cognitive battery of neuropsychological tests. Postoperatively, patients attended follow-ups with the surgical staff at 12 (mean; min-max: 13.8; 12-24) and 48-month (mean; min-max: 55.2; 48-71) periods after surgery. At the second follow-up period, 17 participants were lost (total sample=61).

Preoperatively and at the two follow-up visits the surgical staff calculated body mass index (BMI) and the percentage of excess weight loss (%EWL) measuring participants’ height and weight in their street clothes.

Psychometric Instruments

The Italian version of the Symptom Checklist 90-Revised (SCL-90-R) was used (27). SCL-90-R is a 90-item self-report on a 5-point Likert scale (0-4) assessing general psychopathology and emotional distress that has been widely used in bariatric surgery candidates (28, 29). It provides a global severity index (GSI-90) proposed as an index of overall psychological distress, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of psychopathological distress as well as greater severity of self-reported symptoms. Cronbach’s alpha in the present sample was 0.97.

The Italian version of the Binge Eating Scale (BES) was administered (30). The BES is a 16-item self-report questionnaire assessing the severity of binge eating behavior. Total scores range from 0 to 46 with cut-off scores of possible (18- 26) and probable (≥ 27) BED (31). Cronbach’s alpha in the present sample was 0.91.

Neuropsychological Tests

The computerized version of the Wisconsin Sorting Card Test (WCST) was used to measure executive domains (32). Four stimulus cards with different symbols (in terms of color, number, and shape) are displayed on the computer screen. Participants are requested to match a response card with one of the four stimulus cards according to a criterion (which can be color, number, or form). After 10 consecutive correct pairings, the criterion for matching the cards changed. The task continues until all 128 response cards have been displayed. Different indices can be calculated: “total categories completed’’, ‘‘perseverative errors’’, ‘‘total non-perseverative errors’’, ‘‘total correct responses’’, ‘‘number of perseverative responses’’, and ‘‘the total number of errors’’.

The Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test (SPM) is one of the most widely used instruments measuring general intelligence (33) and was also adopted in the pre-surgical psychosocial evaluation (34). SPM is relatively independent of education or cultural influence. It takes almost 40 minutes to complete. The task consists of several visual analogy problems. Each item consists of a matrix of geometric designs representing the problem with one design removed from the sequence. Participants are invited to glean the kind of relationship between designs and choose the missing figure from among the alternative set. SPM includes five sets (A through E) progressing in difficulty.

The Continuous Performance Test II (CPT) (35) is a computer-administered task that measures sustained attention and impulsivity. The subject is asked to respond every time a specific letter appears on the screen (target stimulus) and not to respond when different stimuli appear on the screen (distractors). Errors can be rated as responses to non-target stimuli (commission errors) and as failure to respond to target stimuli (omission errors). Six scores are computed: omission errors, commission errors (response to non-target stimulus), reaction times (average response time in milliseconds to stimuli), standard deviation in reaction time indicating variability of the performance, and warning (ability to discriminate between target and non-target).

The verbal fluency test includes two tasks (namely semantic and phonemic fluency tasks) (36): Letter (LF) (37) and Category (CF) verbal Fluency (38) which were used to assess both verbal ability and executive control. LF consists of producing as many words as possible in response to a specific letter. CF consists of producing as many words as possible in response to a specific semantic category. These tasks permit an evaluation of the ability to generate words and investigate the ability of the responder to form a strategy in response to a required task. Verbal fluency scores are computed as the number of correct words produced in 1 minute.

Digit Span (39) was used to assess the short-term memory ability. The examiner reads a sequence of numbers to the participants, who are required to repeat the same sequence. If subjects correctly repeat the sequence of numbers, the examiner moves on to the next sequence increasing the span of numbers that the participants have to repeat.

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (40) assessed patients’ verbal memory. The RAVLT is widely used to evaluate the nature and severity of memory dysfunction. The task is divided into two sections. In the first section, the examiner reads a list of 15 words to the participant who has to immediately recall as many words as possible. This procedure is repeated five times. In the second section, the examiner reads the list, this time there is a delay of 15 minutes before the participant is requested to recall the words. The test gives two quantitative measures: the immediate recall (Rey-T0) and delayed recall of the list (Rey-T15).

The Trail Making Test (TMT) was performed to assess visual attention, mental flexibility, and executive functioning (41). It consists of two parts (A-B). Part A is a good measure of rote memory. Part B is more sensitive to executive functioning.

The Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test was performed in one session, with other tests being performed in a second session.

Statistical Analysis

All the analyses were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 25.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). Relationships between variables were computed through Pearson’s indices of associations (r). Pearson’s r coefficients < 0.10 are considered a negligible or null effect, r between 0.10 and 0.30 a small effect, r between 0.30 and 0.50 a medium effect, and r ≥ 0.50 a large effect (42). We conducted both zero order and first order correlations, controlling for sex, given that 75.6% of the total sample was composed of women (43). Hierarchical linear regression analyses were performed to investigate whether clinical and cognitive variables were significant predictors of percentage of excess weight loss at the first (%EWL I) and second (%EWL II) follow up, while controlling sociodemographic variables (i.e. age, sex, school attainment). The independent variables were entered into the regression model according to the following blocks: (1) cognitive variables; and (2) sociodemographic and clinical variables. The associations were reported as standardized beta coefficients (β) and their p-values.

Since different surgery procedures may lead to different weight loss outcomes (44) and 82% of our total sample (n=64) being composed of patients undergoing sleeve gastrectomy, we conducted additional analyses on those participants undergoing this surgery. Among the subsample of sleeve gastrectomy patients, we conducted both zero order and first order correlations, controlling for sex. Hierarchical linear regression analyses were performed to investigate whether sociodemographic (i.e. age, sex, school attainment), clinical and cognitive variables were significant predictors of a percentage of excess weight loss at, both, the first (%EWL I) and second (%EWL II) follow up.

According to the last observation carried forward (LOCF) approach, missing values for % EWL at the second follow-up were replaced with the last available observation (45).

Results

Characteristics of the Sample

Average BMI of the patients undergoing bariatric surgery was 43.15 Kg/m2 (SD = 6.01; Range= 33.26-55.83). According to the BMI cut-off of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) 8 (10.3%) patients were included in class I obesity (i.e., 30 Kg/m2 ≤ BMI ≤ 34.99 Kg/m2), 20 (25.6%) patients in class II obesity (i.e., 35 Kg/m2 ≤ BMI ≤ 39.99 Kg/m2), and 50 (64.1%) in class III obesity (i.e., BMI ≥ 40 Kg/m2). Eleven patients (14.3%) met the criteria for moderate levels of binge eating (18 ≤ BES ≤ 26), and 8 (10.1%) had severe levels of binge eating (BES ≥ 27) (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the sample (N=78).

Descriptive statistics for the subsample of patients undergoing sleeve gastrectomy procedures were included in the Supplementary material (see Table S1). Average BMI of the patients undergoing sleeve gastrectomy procedures was 43 Kg/m2 (SD = 5.83; Range= 33.26-55.83). According to the BMI cut-off of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) 4 (6.3%) patients were included in class I obesity (i.e., 30 Kg/m2 ≤ BMI ≤ 34.99 Kg/m2), 17 (26.5%) patients in class II obesity (i.e., 35 Kg/m2 ≤ BMI ≤ 39.99 Kg/m2), and 43 (67.2%) in class III obesity (i.e., BMI ≥ 40 Kg/m2). Eight patients (12.5%) met the criteria for moderate levels of binge eating (18 ≤ BES ≤ 26), and 5 (7.8%) had severe levels of binge eating (BES ≥ 27).

Variables Associated With the Percentage of Excess Weight Loss (%EWL) at 1-Year and 4-Year Follow-Up

Correlations among %EWL I, %EWL II and sociodemographic and clinical variables are reported in Table 2, and correlations among %EWL I, %EWL II and cognitive variables are reported in Table 3. Both the %EWL I (r = -0.27, p < 0.05) and %EWL II (r = -0.31, p < 0.01) were negatively and significantly correlated with age. The associations with BES scores and the SCL GSI were nonsignificant for both %EWL I and %EWL II. In partial correlations, when controlling for sex, both the %EWL I (r = -0.26, p < 0.05) and %EWL II (r = -0.32, p < 0.01) were still negatively and significantly correlated with age, and the associations with BES scores and the SCL GSI for both %EWL I and %EWL II were still nonsignificant.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Associations between %EWL at I and II follow-up, socio-demographic and clinical variables (N=78).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Associations between %EWL at I and II follow-up and cognitive variables (N=78).

The %EWL I correlated significantly and positively with SPM-E scores (r = 0.22, p < 0.05) and WCST total correct responses (r = 0.28, p < 0.05), and negatively with the CTP-SD (r = -0.23, p < 0.05). When controlling for sex, %EWL I remained significantly and positively correlated with SPM-E scores (r = 0.24, p < 0.05) and WCST total correct responses (r = 0.26, p < 0.05), but the correlation with the CTP-SD (r = -0.21, p = 0.06) became nonsignificant. Furthermore, at the zero-order correlations, %EWL II did not correlate with any of the cognitive variables, and only when controlling for sex, %EWL II significantly and positively correlated with SPM-E scores (r = 0.25, p < 0.05). However, all these correlations were relatively weak (r < 0.40).

Additional analyses were performed on patients who underwent sleeve gastrectomy procedures (reported in Tables S2 and S3). %EWL II was negatively and significantly associated only with age (r = -0.29, p < 0.05). All the remaining associations with sociodemographic and clinical variables were nonsignificant. %EWL II (r = -0.26, p < 0.05) was still negatively and significantly associated with age even when controlling for sex. %EWL I was associated significantly and positively with SPM-E scores (r = 0.25, p < 0.05) and WCST total correct responses (r = 0.32, p < 0.01), even when controlling for sex (SPM-E: r = 0.29, p < 0.05; WCST total correct responses r = 0.34, p < 0.01).

Predictors of the Percentage of Excess Weight Loss at I Follow-Up

Significant variables at the bivariate analyses were included as predictors in a hierarchical linear regression model, with %EWL I as the criterion. Although nonsignificant at the bivariate analyses, we also included in the regression model, sex, school attainment, and BMI pre-surgery. The model fitted the data well (Block 1: F = 4.186; p < 0.01. Block 2: F = 3.652; p < 0.01; see Table 4) and explained between 11% and 23% of the variance of the data (Block 1: R2 Change = 0.15; p < 0.01. Block 2: R2 Change = 0.16; p < 0.05). WCST total correct responses (β = 0.31; p = 0.007) were independently associated with %EWL I even when controlling for the presence of sociodemographic and clinical variables. SPM-E (β = 0.33; p = 0.004) was independently associated with %EWL I only when controlling for the presence of sociodemographic and clinical variables. Among the other variables included in the model, only age (β = -0.32; p = 0.01) was independently associated with %EWL I.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Hierarchical linear regression analysis predicting the percentage of excess weight loss at the first follow-up (N=78).

Additional analyses were performed separately on patients undergoing sleeve gastrectomy procedures (reported in Table S4). The multivariate model fitted the data well (Block 1: F = 7.949; p < 0.01. Block 2: F = 2.454; p < 0.05). However, the inclusion of sociodemographic variables did not increase the variance explained by the model (F Change = 0.553, p = 0.73). Conversely, the variance explained decreased nonsignificantly from 17% to 14%. This result could indicate a specification error in the model related to one or more of the sociodemographic variables included. In fact, the standard error (SE) associated with the variation in sex was 6.04. WCST total correct responses (β = 0.34; p = 0.01) and SPM-E (β = 0.31; p = 0.02) were independently associated with %EWL I even when controlling for the presence of sociodemographic and clinical variables.

Predictors of the Percentage of Excess Weight Loss at II Follow-Up

Although only age was significant at the bivariate analyses, other sociodemographic, clinical and cognitive variables were included as predictors in a hierarchical linear regression model, with %EWL II as criterion. The model fitted the data well (Block 1: F = 2.925; p < 0.05. Block 2: F = 4.866; p < 0.001) and explained between 7% and 30% of the variance of the data (Block 1: R2 Change = 0.11; p < 0.05. Block 2: R2 Change = 0.26; p < 0.001. See Table 5). In the first block none of the cognitive variables were significantly associated with %EWL II. In the second block, when controlling for socio-demographic and clinical variables, SPM-E scores (β = 0.36; p = 0.001) and WCST total correct responses (β = 0.23; p = 0.03) were independently associated with %EWL II. Among other variables included in the model, age (β = -0.41; p = 0.001) and BES (β = -0.30; p = 0.006) were independently associated with %EWL II.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Hierarchical linear regression analysis predicting the percentage of excess weight loss at the second follow up (N=78).

The same analysis was performed on patients undergoing sleeve gastrectomy procedures (reported in Table S5). Model 2 fitted the data well (Block 2: F = 2.195; p < 0.05) while model 1 did not (Block 1: F = 3.062; p = 0.054). However, the increase in the variance explained by model 2 (R2 Change = 0.12, F Change = 1767, p = 0.13) was nonsignificant when compared to model 1. As reported for the previous model with %EWL I as the dependent variable, the standard error associated with gender was high (SE = 6.71). WCST total correct responses (β = 0.25; p = 0.04) was the only variable independently associated with %EWL II.

Discussion

We documented that cognitive impairment predicted weight-loss at the early (i.e., one year) and long-term (i.e., >four years) periods after surgery, while the severity of pre-operative binge eating behavior did not emerge as a risk factor for decrease in weight-loss until four years after the operation. To the best of our knowledge, among studies investigating the neurocognitive predictors of bariatric surgery, at 55 months, we have provided the longest follow-up. Apart from binge eating symptoms, general psychopathology was not correlated with weight-loss outcomes. Accordingly, bariatric surgery is a viable treatment for those suffering from a psychiatric disorder in terms of weight-loss outcomes. Despite this, it is recommended to have a close and ongoing monitoring of these vulnerable patients, due to the increased risk of depression and self-harm after surgery (46). Moreover, we reinforced the observation that bariatric surgery is less effective for older patients (47), which was previously attributed to the longer duration of comorbidities and impaired metabolic functioning compared to young people (48, 49). Our findings differed from and added to past results which reported an association between cognitive impairment and poorer bariatric surgery outcomes.

We highlighted various strengths of this study. First of all, participants underwent an exhaustive battery of cognitive tests assessing different areas of cognitive function: mental flexibility, working memory, attention, concentration, verbal learning and memory, verbal fluency, impulse control as well as intellectual capacity which were separately investigated in previous studies (50, 51).

In exploring the possible influence of all the above cognitive domains, we were able to achieve the significant prediction of executive functions and basic cognitive level as measured by WCST and SPM tests on short and long-term weight loss. Executive functions mainly carried out by the prefrontal cortex can be defined as “high level” activities that modulate “lower-level” subcortical responses (52). Executive functions such as decision making, response inhibition, and cognitive flexibility, consist of mental capacities necessary to engage in the planning for and achieving of goal-directed activities. If executive functions are intact, people can have the capacity to complete a life-plan and be productive. On the other hand, the impairment or loss of these functions restricts the ability to maintain a proper self-improving, and productive life (53). Although we did not provide data among post-operative eating habits of patients (54), it was proven that brain circuits involved in executive control might be a distinctive determinant of successful surgical outcome (55). Accordingly, since executive domains are pivotal to the adjustment in novel situations, the anatomical change which was imposed by surgery may force a new eating regimen representing an impenetrable target for these patients. The lack of patients adaptation to the post-surgery dietary and behavioral recommendations may result in suboptimal outcomes in terms of both weight loss and surgical complications (5658).

Moreover, compared to expedient studies on cognitive impairment, the second merit of our research was the longer follow-up time and the two-step assessment. This strategy allowed us to go largely beyond and to distinguish the ante from the post-honeymoon period which was typically fixed around 12 to 18 months after surgery (59). This period can be selected as the first “critical” window during which weight regain might appear (60).

Another strong point of our study was the contrast between the cognitive and the psychopathological impairment effect on weight loss trajectory. The following results deepen the insight on how and when different clusters of predictors may exhibit their influence on weight loss outcomes. Then, a long time after surgery, old eating attitudes, such as binge behaviors, may generate their harmful effect. Whereas cognitive impairment was found to be relevant to the nature of BED (61, 62), differentiating their effects also reduced possible confounding issues.

Beyond the worth of our study, we must recognize its limitations. Due to the high prevalence (82% of the total sample) of sleeve gastrectomy procedures, we were unable to analyze the effect of potential interaction between type of operation and cognitive deficits on the evolution of BMI over the two follow-up periods (57, 63, 64). Moreover, we did not perform the cognitive battery test after surgery, neglecting the possibility of measuring changes in cognitive functioning associated with weight-loss (65). Furthermore, the sample was quite small when considering the number of neurocognitive and clinical variables investigated. Finally, data regarding patients’ adherence to nutritional follow-ups were not available.

Nonetheless, as a result of the above findings and in keeping with the latest guideline (66), we should encourage neuropsychological assessment of bariatric surgery candidates and, consequently, appropriate treatments, such as cognitive remediation therapy, should be arranged post-operatively. Cognition demonstrated a strong and bidirectional relationship with obesity (67), thus, its role in the bariatric surgery field may substantiate the rationale of providing rehabilitative interventions. Such treatments might be tailored to cognitive domains and time specific to the goal of supporting patients in their post-surgical course (68).

Conclusion

In conclusion, our findings answered the need to analyze the interactions among all factors involved in predicting pathways of suboptimal weight loss after surgery, such as demographics, psychopathology, eating disorders, and cognitive functioning. Additionally, we limited the possible overlap between cognitive impairment and eating disorders which were independently and at different times associated with poorer weight loss outcomes.

Neither psychopathology nor BED should prevent surgery; however, due to the crucial implications of BED for the prognosis, it is worth noting that this disorder is manageable through psychological and pharmacological interventions. Cognitive functioning needs to be addressed preoperatively and may be improved by appropriate rehabilitative techniques.

Finally, with the two-step follow-up, our findings provided the challenge to better understand how cognitive and psychopathological domains of patients might interact with weight loss and weight loss maintenance.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Rome Tor Vergata. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

EB and PG designed the study, wrote the manuscript, and performed psychiatric and surgical assessments. GR and TS performed the statistics, wrote the results, and contributed to the interpretation of data and to the discussion section. MI supervised statistical analyses and contributed to the first draft of the paper. LP and LC collected psychiatric and cognitive tests and completed the database. MI, MF, and CI revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fendo.2021.662252/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Rochette AD, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, et al. Mild cognitive impairment is prevalent in persons with severe obesity. Obesity (2016) 24:1427–9. doi: 10.1002/oby.21514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Lokken KL, Boeka AG, Austin HM, Gunstad J, Harmon CM. Evidence of executive dysfunction in extremely obese adolescents: a pilot study. Surg Obes Related Diseases (2009) 5:547–52. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2009.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lin Z, Qu S. Legend of Weight Loss: a Crosstalk Between the Bariatric Surgery and the Brain. Obes Surgery (2020) 30:1988–2002. doi: 10.1007/s11695-020-04474-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hogenkamp PS, Sundbom M, Nilsson VC, Benedict C, Schiöth HB. Patients lacking sustainable long-term weight loss after gastric bypass surgery show signs of decreased inhibitory control of prepotent responses. PloS One (2015) 10:e0119896. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Cope EC, Gould E. New Evidence Linking Obesity and Food Addiction. Biol Psychiatry Elsevier Inc (2017) 81:734–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kenny PJ. Reward Mechanisms in Obesity: New Insights and Future Directions. Neuron (2011) 69:664–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Chang SH, Stoll CRT, Song J, Varela JE, Eagon CJ, Colditz GA. The effectiveness and risks of bariatric surgery an updated systematic review and meta-analysis, 2003-2012. JAMA Surgery (2014) 149:275–87. doi: 10.1001/jamasurg.2013.3654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gentileschi P. Laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy as a primary operation for morbid obesity: Experience with 200 PATIENTS. Gastroenterol Res Pract (2012) 2012:801325. doi: 10.1155/2012/801325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Voorwinde V, Steenhuis IHM, Janssen IMC, Monpellier VM, van Stralen MM. Definitions of Long-Term Weight Regain and Their Associations with Clinical Outcomes. Obes Surgery (2020) 30:527–36. doi: 10.1007/s11695-019-04210-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Elhag W, El Ansari W. Weight Regain and Insufficient Weight Loss after Bariatric Surgery: A Call for Action. In: Bariatric Surgery - From the Non-surgical Approach to the Post-surgery Individual Care [Working Title] (2020).

Google Scholar

11. Petrescu O, Fan X, Gentileschi P, Hossain S, Bradbury M, Gagner M, et al. Long-chain fatty acid uptake is upregulated in omental adipocytes from patients undergoing bariatric surgery for obesity. Int J Obes (2005) 29:196–203. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Spitznagel MB, Alosco M, Galioto R, Strain G, Devlin M, Sysko R, et al. The role of cognitive function in postoperative weight loss outcomes: 36-Month follow-up. Obes Surgery (2014) 24:1078–84. doi: 10.1007/s11695-014-1205-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bianciardi E, Gentileschi P, Niolu C, Innamorati M, Fabbricatore M, Contini LM, et al. Assessing psychopathology in bariatric surgery candidates: discriminant validity of the SCL-90-R and SCL-K-9 in a large sample of patients. Eating Weight Disord (2020). doi: 10.1007/s40519-020-01068-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Lynch A. When the honeymoon is over, the real work begins:” Gastric bypass patients’ weight loss trajectories and dietary change experiences. Soc Sci Med (2016) 151:241–9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lombardo M, Bellia A, Mattiuzzo F, Franchi A, Ferri C, Padua E, et al. Frequent Follow-Up Visits Reduce Weight Regain in Long-Term Management after Bariatric Surgery. Bariatric Surg Pract Patient Care (2015) 10:119–25. doi: 10.1089/bari.2015.0021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Smith CE, Hawkins MAW, Williams-Kerver GA, Duncan J. Depression subtypes, binge eating, and weight loss in bariatric surgery candidates. Surgery for Obesity and Related Diseases. Am Soc Bariatric Surgery (2020) 16:690–7. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2019.12.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Smith KE, Orcutt M, Steffen KJ, Crosby RD, Cao L, Garcia L, et al. Loss of Control Eating and Binge Eating in the 7 Years Following Bariatric Surgery. Obes Surgery (2019) 29:1773–80. doi: 10.1007/s11695-019-03791-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sarwer DB, Allison KC, Wadden TA, Ashare R, Spitzer JC, McCuen-Wurst C, et al. Psychopathology, disordered eating, and impulsivity as predictors of outcomes of bariatric surgery. Surg Obes Related Diseases (2019) 15:650–5. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2019.01.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Pedro J, Neves JS, Ferreira MJ, Guerreiro V, Salazar D, Viana S, et al. Impact of Depression on Weight Variation after Bariatric Surgery: A Three-Year Observational Study. Obes Facts (2020) 13:213–20. doi: 10.1159/000506404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Dawes AJ, Maggard-gibbons M, Maher AR, Booth MJ, Miake-lye I, Beroes JM, et al. Mental Health Conditions Among Patients Seeking and Undergoing Bariatric Surgery: A Meta-analysis. JAMA (2016) 90095:150–63. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.18118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lavender JM, Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Cohen R, et al. Association between binge eating disorder and changes in cognitive functioning following bariatric surgery. J Psychiatr Res (2014) 59:148–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Moscovici M, Wnuk S, Okrainec A, Hawa R, Sockalingam S. Psychosocial Predictors of Cognition in Bariatric Surgery. Psychosomatics (2019) 60:164–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2018.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bianciardi E, Di Lorenzo G, Niolu C, Betrò S, Zerbin F, Gentileschi P, et al. Body image dissatisfaction in individuals with obesity seeking bariatric surgery: Exploring the burden of new mediating factors. Rivista di Psichiatria (2019) 54:8–17. doi: 10.1708/3104.30935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gentileschi P, Di Paola M, Catarci M, Santoro E, Montemurro L, Carlini M, et al. Bile duct injuries during laparoscopic cholecystectomy: A 1994-2001 audit on 13,718 operations in the area of Rome. Surg Endoscopy Other IntervenT Techniques (2004) 18:232-6. doi: 10.1007/s00464-003-8815-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Grossi G. Società Italiana della Chirurgia dell'OBesità e delle malattie metaboliche. Linee guida. (2016). pp. 1–100. Available at: https://www.sicob.org/00_materiali/linee_guida_2016.pdf.

Google Scholar

26. Bianciardi E, Orsaria P, Niolu C, Siracusano A, Gentileschi P. Laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy for morbid obesity and Klinefelter syndrome: clinical report on two patients, with long-term follow-up. Eating Weight Disord (2020). doi: 10.1007/s40519-020-00951-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Prunas A, Sarno I, Preti E, Madeddu F, Perugini M. Psychometric properties of the Italian version of the SCL-90-R: A study on a large community sample. Eur Psychiatry (2012) 27:591–7. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Derogatis LR. SCL-90-R. In: Administration, scoring, and procedures manual I for the revised version od the SCL-90. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press (1977).

Google Scholar

29. Imperatori C, Bianciardi E, Niolu C, Fabbricatore M, Gentileschi P, Di Lorenzo G, et al. The symptom-checklist-K-9 (SCL-K-9) discriminates between overweight/obese patients with and without significant binge eating pathology: Psychometric properties of an Italian version. Nutrients (2020) 12:674. doi: 10.3390/nu12030674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Imperatori C, Innamorati M, Lamis DA, Contardi A, Continisio M, Castelnuovo G, et al. Factor Structure of the Binge Eating Scale in a Large Sample of Obese and Overweight Patients Attending Low Energy Diet Therapy. Eur Eating Disord Rev (2016) 24:174–8. doi: 10.1002/erv.2384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gormally J, Black S, Daston S, Rardin D. The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addictive Behav (1982) 47–55. doi: 10.1037/t08303-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Berg EA. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J Gen Psychol (1948) 15–22. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Raven JC. The Progressive Matrices and Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale in Western Societies. In: Irvine SH, Berry JW, editors. Human Assessment and Cultural Factors. NATO Conference Series (III Human Factors) Vol. 21. Boston, MA: Springer (1983). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-2151-2_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Fabricatore AN, Crerand CE, Wadden TA, Sarwer DB, Krasucki JL. How do mental health professionals evaluate candidates for bariatric surgery? Survey results. Obes Surgery (2006) 567–73. doi: 10.1381/096089206776944986

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Conners CK, Staff MHS, Connelly V, Campbell S, MacLean M, Barnes J. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II V. 5) Vol. 29. Multi-Health Systems Inc (2000) p. 175–96.

Google Scholar

36. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment, 5th Edition. (2012) Oxford University Press: New York.

Google Scholar

37. Newcombe FG. Missile Wounds of the Brain: a study of psychological deficits. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (1970). 145 p.

Google Scholar

38. Benton AL. Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia (1968) 53–60. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(68)90038-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Blackburn HL, Benton AL. Revised administration and scoring of the Digit Span Test. J Consult Psychol (1957) 139–43. doi: 10.1037/h0047235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Rey A. L"Examen Clinique en Psychologie [Clinical Examination in Psychology]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France (1964).

Google Scholar

41. Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage. Perceptual Motor Skills (1958) 393–4. doi: 10.2466/PMS.8.7.271-276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Rutledge (2013) 567 p.

Google Scholar

43. Perrone F, Bianciardi E, Benavoli D, Tognoni V, Niolu C, Siracusano A, et al. Gender Influence on Long-Term Weight Loss and Comorbidities After Laparoscopic Sleeve Gastrectomy and Roux-en-Y Gastric Bypass: a Prospective Study With a 5-Year Follow-up. Obes Surgery (2016) 276–87. doi: 10.1007/s11695-015-1746-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. D’Alessio R, Watanabe M, Gallo IF, Manfrini S, Tuccinardi D, Bruni V. The gastro-jejunal anastomosis site influences dumping syndrome and weight regain in patients with obesity undergoing Laparoscopic Roux-en-Y Gastric Bypass. Eating Weight Disord (2020). doi: 10.1007/s40519-020-01030-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Abraham WT, Russell DW. Missing data: A review of current methods and applications in epidemiological research. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2004) 315–21. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000133836.34543.7e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Sockalingam S. Incidence and determinants of mental health service use after bariatric surgery. Nat Rev Endocrinol (2020) 16:12–3. doi: 10.1038/s41574-019-0292-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Courcoulas AP, King WC, Belle SH, Berk P, Flum DR, Garcia L, et al. Seven-year weight trajectories and health outcomes in the Longitudinal Assessment of Bariatric Surgery (LABS) study. JAMA Surgery (2018) 427–34. doi: 10.1001/jamasurg.2017.5025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Contreras JE, Santander C, Court I, Bravo J. Correlation between age and weight loss after bariatric surgery. Obes Surgery (2013) 1289–9. doi: 10.1007/s11695-013-0905-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bellia A, Marinoni G, D’Adamo M, Guglielmi V, Lombardo M, Donadel G, et al. Parathyroid hormone and insulin resistance in distinct phenotypes of severe obesity: A cross- sectional analysis in middle-age1d men and premenopausal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2012) 4724–32. doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-2513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Smith KR, Moran TH, Papantoni A, Speck C, Bakker A, Kamath V, et al. Short-term improvements in cognitive function following vertical sleeve gastrectomy and Roux-en Y gastric bypass: a direct comparison study. Surg Endoscopy (2020) 2248–57. doi: 10.1007/s00464-019-07015-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Votruba K, Marshall D, Finks J, Giordani B. Neuropsychological factors and bariatric surgery: A review. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2014) 448. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0448-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Weiss F, Barbuti M, Carignani G, Calderone A, Santini F, Maremmani I, et al. Psychiatric Aspects of Obesity: A Narrative Review of Pathophysiology and Psychopathology. J Clin Med (2020) 9:2344. doi: 10.3390/jcm9082344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lezak MD. The problem of assessing executive functions. Int J Psychol (1982) 17:281–97. doi: 10.1080/00207598208247445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Applegate KL. Comment on: Cognitive function predicts 24 month weight loss success following bariatric surgery. Surg Obes Related Diseases (2013) 771–2. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2013.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Goldman RL, Canterberry M, Borckardt JJ, Madan A, Byrne TK, George MS, et al. Executive control circuitry differentiates degree of success in weight loss following gastric-bypass surgery. Obesity (2013) 2189–96. doi: 10.1002/oby.20575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Montuori M, Benavoli D, D’ugo S, Di Benedetto L, Bianciardi E, Gaspari AL, et al. Integrated approaches for the management of staple line leaks following sleeve gastrectomy. J Obes (2017) 2017. doi: 10.1155/2017/4703236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Tognoni V, Benavoli D, Bianciardi E, Perrone F, Ippoliti S, Gaspari A, et al. Laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy versus laparoscopic banded sleeve gastrectomy: First prospective pilot randomized study. Gastroenterol Res Pract (2016) 2016. doi: 10.1155/2016/6419603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Gentileschi P, Kini S, Gagner M. Palliative laparoscopic hepatico- and gastrojejunostomy for advanced pancreatic cancer. iJSLS: J Soc Laparoendoscopic Surgeons (2002) 6:331–8. Society of Laparoendoscopic Surgeons.

Google Scholar

59. Biron S, Biertho L, Marceau S, Lacasse Y. Long-term follow-up of disease-specific quality of life after bariatric surgery. Surgery for Obesity and Related Diseases. Elsevier Inc (2018) 14:658–64. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2018.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Le Foll D, Lechaux D, Rascle O, Cabagno G. Weight loss and quality of life after bariatric surgery: a 2-year longitudinal study. Surg Obes Related Diseases (2020) 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2019.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lee Y, Carmona NE, Shekotikhina M, Subramaniapillai M, Mansur RB, Cha DS, et al. Is binge eating a cognitive disorder? Results from the International Mood Disorders Collaborative Project. Ann Clin psychiatry: Off J Am Acad Clin Psychiatrists (2018) 25–31.

Google Scholar

62. Bianciardi E, Fabbricatore M, Lorenzo GDI, Innamorati M, Tomassini L, Gentileschi P, et al. Prevalence of Food Addiction and Binge Eating in an Italian sample of bariatric surgery candidates and overweight/obese patients seeking low-energy-diet therapy. Rivista di Psichiatria (2019) 54:127–30 doi: 10.1708/3181.31602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Gentileschi P, Bianciardi E, Siragusa L, Tognoni V, Benavoli D, D’Ugo S. Banded Sleeve Gastrectomy Improves Weight Loss Compared to Nonbanded Sleeve: Midterm Results from a Prospective Randomized Study. Kuk JL, editor. J Obes [Internet] Hindawi; (2020) 2020:9792518. doi: 10.1155/2020/9792518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Perrone F, Bianciardi E, Ippoliti S, Nardella J, Fabi F, Gentileschi P. Long-term effects of laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy versus Roux-en-Y gastric bypass for the treatment of morbid obesity: a monocentric prospective study with minimum follow-up of 5 years. Updates Surgery (2017) 69:101–7. doi: 10.1007/s13304-017-0426-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Thiara G, Cigliobianco M, Muravsky A, Paoli RA, Mansur R, Hawa R, et al. Evidence for Neurocognitive Improvement After Bariatric Surgery: A Systematic Review. Psychosomatics (2017) 217–27. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2017.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Mechanick JI, Apovian C, Brethauer S, Garvey WT, Joffe AM, Kim J, et al. Clinical practice guidelines for the perioperative nutrition, metabolic, and nonsurgical support of patients undergoing bariatric procedures – 2019 update: cosponsored by American Association of Clinical Endocrinologists/ American College of Endocrino. Surg Obes Related Dis (2020) 16:175–247. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2019.10.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Hartanto A, Yong JC, Toh WX. Bidirectional associations between obesity and cognitive function in midlife adults: A longitudinal study. Nutrients (2019) 2343. doi: 10.3390/nu11102343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Galioto R, Gunstad J, Heinberg LJ, Spitznagel MB. Adherence and weight loss outcomes in bariatric surgery: Does cognitive function play a role? Obes Surgery (2013) 1703–10. doi: 10.1007/s11695-013-1060-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: binge eating disorder, cognitive impairment, executive function, obesity, bariatric surgery, psychopathology, bariatric surgery psychosocial evaluation

Citation: Bianciardi E, Raimondi G, Samela T, Innamorati M, Contini LM, Procenesi L, Fabbricatore M, Imperatori C and Gentileschi P (2021) Neurocognitive and Psychopathological Predictors of Weight Loss After Bariatric Surgery: A 4-Year Follow-Up Study. Front. Endocrinol. 12:662252. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2021.662252

Received: 31 January 2021; Accepted: 30 March 2021;
Published: 05 May 2021.

Edited by:

Zoltan Pataky, Geneva University Hospitals (HUG), Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Francesca Abbatini, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Mikiko Watanabe, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Bianciardi, Raimondi, Samela, Innamorati, Contini, Procenesi, Fabbricatore, Imperatori and Gentileschi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emanuela Bianciardi, bianciardi@med.uniroma2.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.