- 1LOGIN Research Group, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain
- 2School of Speech-Language Pathology, Austral University of Chile, Puerto Montt, Chile
Williams syndrome (WS) is a genetic neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by language skills above what is expected considering non-verbal intelligence. Research on phonological development is scarce, with many studies focusing on grammar in children and adolescents. In one of our previous studies transversally explored the profiles of late phonological development in Spanish-speaking WS children, adolescents, and adults, while our objective is to longitudinally determine these profiles for WS children based on present error indexes in spontaneous speech. Participants were seven WS children (aged 3;7–8;2), engaging in two spontaneous conversations within a 6-month interval. They were compared cross-sectionally with 240 typically developing (TD) children aged 3–6 years, divided into six groups. All speech samples were transcribed and analyzed with the CLAN software package of the CHILDES Project. Phonological profiles were established on the basis of phonological error indexes obtained dividing absolute frequency of errors by the total number of words produced. WS children showed a mean reduction of more than 25% in the absolute frequency of phonological errors after 6 months. As for the comparison with the normative groups, their error index was consistent with the stage of expansion in TD, however, after 6 months, this was consistent with the stage of stabilization. This atypical acceleration in phonological development could be related to lexical growth in the context of relative preservation of phonological memory. Furthermore, the trajectories of late phonological development in WS children might not be linear, as postulated by neuroconstructivist models, suggesting the need for intervention approaches specifically adapted to the phonological profiles of WS children.
1 Introduction
Williams Syndrome (WS) is a multisystem neurodevelopmental disorder caused by a heterozygous deletion on chromosome 7q11.23 (Pérez-Jurado, 2003), whose prevalence according to the most cited epidemiological study, is estimated at 1 in 7,500 births (Strømme et al., 2002), with no sex difference, racial or ethnic predilection (Morris et al., 2020). The WS physical phenotype includes distinctive and easily recognizable facial features, cardiovascular anomalies, endocrine-metabolic alterations, hoarse voice, and sound sensitivities (hyperacusis, odynacusis, auditory allodynia, and auditory fascinations) (Kozel et al., 2021). Individuals with WS show a specific neurocognitive profile characterized by a complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses (Karmiloff-Smith et al., 2003b; Vicari et al., 2004; Mervis and John, 2010; Hocking et al., 2015; Wuang and Tsai, 2017; Miezah et al., 2021) and they may show mild-to-moderate intellectual disability (Bellugi et al., 2000; Mervis et al., 2000). In general, deficits in visuospatial construction skills constitute a specific weakness (Mervis et al., 2000; Brown et al., 2003; Farran and Jarrold, 2003; Van der Geest et al., 2005; Porter and Coltheart, 2006), whereas auditory processing and face recognition are strengths in the WS profile (D'Souza et al., 2015; Miezah et al., 2021).
Earlier studies described language as selectively preserved and dissociated from other cognitive functions (Bellugi et al., 1988, 1994, 2000). Further research highlighted that superior verbal skills reported in individuals with WS may be explained in terms of asynchronous trajectories of development, with verbal abilities increasing more rapidly than non-verbal abilities (Jarrold et al., 2001). This asymmetric profile of WS would not be explained as the product of a cluster of damaged or preserved static modules, but as the emergent result of the dynamic processes of development where genes, the brain, cognition, behavior, and the environment interact multidirectionally throughout the life span (Karmiloff-Smith et al., 2003a; Karmiloff-Smith, 2009).
In general, pragmatic abilities have been described as an area of relative weakness in individuals with WS, arising both in narrative and conversational settings (Stojanovik, 2006; Diez-Itza et al., 2018, 2022). In contrast, structural components of language, i.e., phonology, morphosyntax, and vocabulary, have been considered relative strengths. From a preservation perspective, the results of different studies on morphological skills in WS were interpreted in terms of a typically functioning system with some impaired components (Clahsen and Almazan, 1998; Clahsen et al., 2004; Penke and Krause, 2004). However, more recent studies suggest a certain degree of atypical morphological processing (Benítez-Burraco et al., 2017; Diez-Itza et al., 2017, 2019). Several studies have pointed out that grammatical ability is strongly correlated with expressive vocabulary size (Vicari et al., 2002; Volterra et al., 2003).
Regarding lexical acquisition, several studies have emphasized that young children with WS follow an atypical pattern. Unlike typically developing (TD), children with WS produce their first words before understanding or producing protodeclarative gestures (Mervis and Bertrand, 1997) or produce them in smaller quantities even while producing referential language (Laing et al., 2002). It is also known that adolescents and adults have a vocabulary which is extensive and sometimes unusual for their age with an atypical pattern of semantic categorization (Purser et al., 2010). However, the initial stages of development are not clearly indicative of these results.
The idea of good phonological skills in individuals with WS has been mainly consolidated from studies on phonological short-term memory by using word repetition and pseudoword tasks, suggesting that they may depend more on phonology than semantic information (Grant et al., 1997; Majerus et al., 2003), probably because of a semantic-phonological mismatch (Thomas and Karmiloff-Smith, 2003). Nonetheless, very few studies have focused on assessing this level. In this sense, the results of direct studies of production by using articulation test show that these skills are not fully preserved and that difficulties persist into adolescence and adulthood (Hidalgo, 2019; Hidalgo and Garayzábal, 2019; Huffman, 2019).
A recent cross-sectional study explored phonological development profiles across late stages in Spanish-speaking children, adolescents, and adults with WS based on the analysis of phonological processes in spontaneous speech samples when compared to two TD groups. The results showed atypical and complex trajectories, from the expansion of the system (around 3 years of age) for the children group to its resolution (around 5 years of age) for the adolescent and adult group, which cannot be described as simply delayed or protracted (Pérez et al., 2022). Nevertheless, in late phonological development, individuals with WS reach more advanced stages than other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as Williams duplication syndrome, Smith-Magenis syndrome, Down syndrome, and Fragile X syndrome (Mervis et al., 2015; Huelmo et al., 2017; Hidalgo and Garayzábal, 2019; Diez-Itza et al., 2021).
Regarding phonological development in TD, three late stages have been described using a methodological approach based on the analysis of spontaneous speech corpora in Spanish-speaking children aged 3–5 (Diez-Itza et al., 2001; Diez-Itza and Martínez, 2004; Martínez, 2010). The results showed a reduction of the frequency of errors and changes in their relative distribution as age increased, suggesting a first stage of expansion (age 3), an intermediate stage of stabilization (age 4), and a final stage of resolution (age 5). Within the same theoretical framework, the present study aimed to further advance in a detailed description of longitudinal phonological development in children with WS.
1.1 Objectives
The main goal of this study was to determine the longitudinal profile of phonological development in a group of Spanish-speaking children with WS in order to find out changes across developmental stages and discover whether specific features would be exhibited. The profiles were based on the analysis of five error types (Syllable Structure, Segmental Substitution, Segmental Omission, Assimilation, Segmental Addition) in spontaneous speech. The frequency and percentage distribution of phonological error index were calculated for each of the two assessments. It was hypothesized that children with WS presented a lower frequency of errors from the first to the second assessment times, this reduction affecting differently in quantitative and qualitative terms. A second hypothesis was that phonological development in WS follows the stages of typical development (i.e., expansion, stabilization, and resolution) and that phonological patterns not only show quantitative differences (interpretable as “delayed”) but also, taking into account the error types, atypical characteristics (interpretable as “disordered”).
2 Method
2.1 Participants
The participants were seven individuals with WS, previously diagnosed by the molecular genetic test fluorescence in situ hybridization system and presenting the characteristic physical phenotype. In addition, all participants had associated intellectual disability. The group with WS consisted of three boys and four girls (chronological age: M = 5.9; range: 3;07–8;02; verbal age: M = 3.6; range: 2;05–5;02). All participants were monolingual Spanish speakers, belonging to urban middle-class families, and attending mainstream schools (n = 7), and whose families provided informed consent to participate in the study. Verbal age was obtained from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn et al., 2010).
The normative group consisted of 240 TD Spanish-speaking preschoolers [part of Martínez, 2010 study] aged 3–6 years divided into six groups based on chronological age and with 40 children in each group (20 girls and 20 boys): 3;0 TD: chronological age: M = 3.2; range: 3;00−3;05, 3;6 TD: chronological age: M = 3.9; range: 3;06–3;11), 4;0 TD: chronological age: M = 4.2; range: 4;00–4;05, 4;6 TD: chronological age: M = 4.9; range: 4;06–4;11, 5;0 TD: chronological age: M = 5.2; range: 5;00–5;05), 5;6 TD: chronological age: M = 5.8; range: 5;06–5;11. These children had no history of language disorder and were enrolled in regular schools distributed in the central area of Asturias (Spain).
2.2 Instruments and procedure
The RETAMHE methodology, -short for Recording, Transcription, and Analysis of Spontaneous Speech Samples (Diez-Itza, 1992; Diez-Itza et al., 1999) was used to obtain the spontaneous speech samples, which were collected via audio-visual recordings of dyadic conversations between each participant and a researcher, with an estimated duration of 45 min in natural settings, and which are part of larger corpora within the Syndroling Project (Diez-Itza et al., 2014). Individuals from the WS group were recorded in two sessions spaced 6 months apart. These conversations were transcribed in CHAT (Codes for the Human Analysis of Transcripts) format and analyzed with the FREQ program, one of the CLAN (Computerized Language Analysis) software programs, both provided by the CHILDES Project (MacWhinney, 2000). Each transcription was completed by a trained researcher and reviewed by two other researchers independently. Difficulties detected were analyzed jointly by the three investigators and discrepancies were resolved by the principal investigator.
The phonological errors were analyzed and classified into one of the following types: Syllable Structure (SYS), Segmental Substitution (SBT), Segmental Omission (OMI), Assimilation (ASM), and Segmental Addition (ADD). The following example illustrates the transcription and coding procedure according to the minCHAT format of the CHILDES Project:
*CHI: nombe [*] [: name].
%err: nombe = nombre $PHO:SYS:CCR;
2.3 Data analysis
Once the transcriptions were coded, the frequency of lexical variables was obtained using the FREQ program, that is, the total number of words produced (“tokens”) by each participant, as well as the count of different words (“types”) in each transcription. Next, the frequency of the classes of phonological errors encoded was obtained with the same program. In order to control for variability in the size of the spontaneous speech samples, a Phonological Error Index (PEI) was calculated to indicate the frequency of errors. This index is obtained dividing the absolute frequency of errors by the total number of words produced (tokens) per 100. In addition, the Relative Frequency (RF) was calculated, i.e., the percentage distribution of phonological errors by classes. To calculate the RF, participants in each group who did not present phonological errors in the classes or subclasses analyzed were eliminated.
Intra-group differences in PEI and RF regarding both the total number of errors and error types between the two assessment times were analyzed using the Wilcoxon-signed-rank test for dependent samples.
Additionally, the effect size was calculated by Cohen's d using G*Power 3.1 statistical software. The d values are typically quantified as small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8) (Cohen, 1988). In turn, the differences between groups by chronological age groups in total PEI and by error types, and RF were analyzed using the Kruskal-Wallis nonparametric test adjusted with the Bonferroni correction (expressed with the H value) for independent samples, given that the distributions did not always approach normality according to the Shapiro-Wilk test. Spearman correlation was used to analyze the bivariate relationships between chronological age, verbal age, and PEI.
Statistical analysis of the data was performed using SPSS software (Statistical Product and Service Solutions IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0).
3 Results
3.1 Intra-group differences in phonological error index and relative frequency
A strong positive correlation was found between chronological age and verbal age (rs = 0.94; p = 0.002) in the WS group. The PEI was negatively correlated with chronological age in the first assessment (rs = −0.74; p = 0.058) and in the second assessment (rs = −0.72; p = 0.068). Furthermore, PEI correlated negatively with verbal age at the first assessment (rs = −0.71; p = 0.071) and at the second assessment (rs = −0.64; p = 0.012). However, a strong positive correlation was found between the PEI at both assessments (rs = 0.857; p = 0.01).
Table 1 reports the PEI for the WS group in the first and the second assessment, including means for total errors and each class of errors. WS children showed a mean reduction of more than 25% in the absolute frequency of phonological errors after 6 months, although this difference failed to be statistically significant. Wilcoxon comparisons showed statistically significant differences between both assessments only for SYS errors (p = 0.018), with a large effect size and with this type of error leading the decrease up to almost 40%. In the OMI and ASM error types there was also a decrease of 28 and 44% respectively, although no statistical differences were observed, with a medium effect size. An increase in segmental SBT and ADD errors was observed in the second assessment although these differences were not statistical either, with a small and medium effect size, respectively.
Table 1. Phonological error index (total and error types) means and standard deviations for WS group in the first and the second assessment times, Wilcoxon test, and effect size.
The compared profiles of RF, i.e., the percentage distribution, for error types are shown for the WS group between the first and the second assessment times (Figure 1). At both times, the most frequent error types were those affecting SYS and segmental SBT. Nevertheless, the profile was different, since in the case of SYS a tendency to a reduction in the percentage from the first to the second assessments was observed, whereas in the case of SBT an increase from 16 to 25% was observed, also this difference being statistically significant (Z = −2.197; p = 0.02; d = 1.15). There was also a trend toward a reduction in the percentage of OMI errors and an increase in ASM and ADD errors. However, the Wilcoxon test did not yield statistically significant differences: SYS (Z = −1.690; p = 0.09; d = 0.86); OMI (Z = −0.845; p = 0.39; d = 0.22); ASM (Z = −0.734; p = 0.46; d = 0.37); ADD (Z = −1.690; p = 0.09; d = 0.82).
Figure 1. Profile of relative frequency of error types for WS group in the first and the second assessment. SYS, syllable structure; SBT, substitution; OMI, omission; ASM, assimilation; ADD, addition.
3.2 Inter-group differences in phonological error index and relative frequency in the first assessment
The Kruskal-Wallis test was applied to analyze whether there were differences between the WS and TD age subgroups in the Total phonological error index and by error types in the first assessment.
Significant differences were observed for all variables: PEI (H = 80.17; p < 0.001); SYS (H = 78.80; p < 0.01); SBT (H = 67.43; p < 0.001); OMI (H = 36.21; p < 0.001); ASM (H = 34.08; p < 0.001); ADD (H = 34.08; p = 0.001). Taking into account age group and after applying the Bonferroni correction, the test specifically showed that there were statistically significant differences between the WS and TD 3;6 years (H = 59.52; p = 0.042), 4;0 years (H = 77.05; p = 0.008), 4;6 years (H = 90.42; p = 0.002), 5; years (H = 119.15; p = 0.001), and 5;6 years (H = 142.67; p < 0.001) in the total PEI. In the case of error types, it was observed that in SYS there were also statistically significant differences between WS and TD 3;6 years (H = 69.09; p = 0.018), 4;0 years (H = 87.82; p = 0.003), 4;6 years (H = 97.47; p = 0.001), 5;0 years (H = 128.29; p < 0.001) and 5;6 years (H = 146.04; p < 0.001). For SBT these differences were observed at 4;6 years (H = 70.80; p = 0.015), at 5;0 years (H = 101.55; p = 0.001), and at 5;6 years (H = 108.07; p = 0.001). Regarding OMI, differences were observed between the WS with the group of 3;6 years (H = 60.57; p = 0.034), 4;0 years (H = 88.60; p = 0.002), 4;6 years (H = 88.72; p = 0.002), 5;0 years (H = 78.37; p = 0.006), and 5;6 years (H = 124.42; p < 0.001). Concerning ASM, these differences were found at 5;0 years (H = 63.08; p = 0.028) and 5;6 years (H = 67.65; p = 0.018), and for ADI also at 5;0 years (H = 66.93; p = 0.048) and 5;6 years (H = 67.93; p = 0.019).
To assess differences in relative frequency of phonological error index by types (Figure 2), the Kruskal-Wallis test was also applied. Statistically significant differences were only observed in terms of relative frequency for segmental OM between the WS and TD 4;0 years (H = 66.21; p = 0.020), 4;6 years (H = 63.65; p = 0.026), and 5;6 years (H = 72.80; p = 0.005).
Figure 2. Profiles of relative frequency of phonological error index by types for WS group and TD age groups in first assessment. SYS, syllable structure; SBT, substitution; OMI, omission; ASM, assimilation; ADD, addition. (A) Profiles of WS and 3;0 TD group. (B) Profiles of WS and 3;6 TD group. (C) Profiles of WS and 4;0 TD group. (D) Profiles of WS and 4;6 TD group. (E) Profiles of WS and 5;0 TD group. (F) Profiles of WS and 5;6 TD group.
3.3 Inter-group differences in phonological error index and relative frequency in the second assessment
The Kruskal-Wallis test was applied to analyze whether there were differences between the WS and TD age subgroups for the Total phonological error index and by error types in the second assessment. Significant differences were observed in the variables: PEI (H = 73.91; p < 0.001); SYS (H = 71.63; p < 0.001); SBT (H = 65.07; p < 0.001); OMI (H = 31.78; p < 0.001); ASM (H = 35.96; p < 0.001); ADD (H = 23.57; p = 0.001). Focusing on age group and after applying the Bonferroni correction, the test specifically showed that there were only statistically significant differences between WS and TD 5;0 years (H = 88.42; p = 0.005) and 5;6 years (H = 106.21; p < 0.001) in the total PEI. As for error types, statistically significant differences were observed for SYS and TD 5;0 years (H = 85.86; p = 0.003), and 5;6 years (H = 103.96; p < 0.001), for SBT and TD 4;6 years (H = 57.27; p = 0.05), 5;0 years (H = 88.25; p = 0.003), and 5;6 years (H = 104.60; p < 0.001), in OMI and TD 4;0 years (H = 65.68; p = 0.021), 4;6 years (H = 65.79; p = 0.021), and 5;6 years (H = 101.52; p < 0.001), in ASM and TD 5;0 years (H = 73.39; p = 0.011) and 5;6 years (H = 78.02; p = 0.007), and in ADD and TD 4;0 years (H = 56.92; p = 0.049), 4;6 years (H = 61.85; p = 0.032), 5;0 years (H = 69.92; p = 0.015), and 5;6 years (H = 80.77; p = 0.005).
To assess differences in relative frequency of phonological error index by types (Figure 3), the Kruskal-Wallis test was also applied. Statistically significant differences were only observed in terms of relative frequency in segmental Omissions between the WS group and the 5;6 TD group (H = 66.45; p = 0.020).
Figure 3. Profiles of relative frequency of phonological error index by types for WS group and TD age groups in second assessment. SYS, syllable structure; SBT, substitution; OMI, omission; ASM, assimilation; ADD, addition. (A) Profiles of WS and 3;0 TD group. (B) Profiles of WS and 3;6 TD group. (C) Profiles of WS and 4;0 TD group. (D) Profiles of WS and 4;6 TD group. (E) Profiles of WS and 5;0 TD group. (F) Profiles of WS and 5;6 TD group.
4 Discussion
The purpose of this study was to determine the longitudinal profile of phonological development in a group of Spanish-speaking WS children in order to find out changes across developmental stages and whether specific features would be exhibited. Profiles were based on five error types (Syllable Structure, Segmental Substitution, Segmental Omission, Assimilation, Segmental Addition) in spontaneous speech, calculating their PEI (frequency of errors/100 tokens) and their RF (percentage distribution) for each of both assessments within a 6-month interval. To determine if phonological development in WS followed the stages of typical development (i.e., expansion, stabilization, and resolution) and if they presented specific characteristics, not only quantitative differences (interpretable as delayed) but also atypical characteristics (interpretable as disordered), they were also compared with the profiles of TD preschool children of similar verbal age.
Our results showed that, although as chronological and verbal ages of WS children increased, the PEI decreased, but this reduction was not statistically significant for neither assessment. Taking into account that phonological development in TD children culminates at the age of 7 years (Bosch-Galcerán, 2004), this lack of significance between chronological age and PEI could be explained by age differences, as there were two children aged 3 and 4 years and other two over 7 years of age.
WS children showing a high frequency of phonological errors in terms of PEI in the first assessment were those who continued to present greater PEI in the second one. However, the PEI was reduced by 25% within a 6-month interval, indicating that late phonological development was in progress. The tendency for phonological errors to markedly decrease over chronological age in WS children suggests that accelerated phonological development occurs, which is consistent with findings previously reported by Martínez et al. (2014) in two WS children. This accelerated rate of phonological development over a 6-month interval would compensate for the delay in language onset, which has been linked to delayed babbling (Masataka, 2001) and auditory-visual integration difficulties observed in young WS children and in other neurodevelopmental syndromes (D'Souza et al., 2015). Despite the PEI reduction in WS children within a 6-month interval, phonological development does not seem to culminate at these ages since WS adolescents and adults, as occurs in other neurodevelopmental disorders such as Down syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, or Smith-Magenis syndrome, continue to manifest phonological difficulties (Huelmo et al., 2017; Hidalgo and Garayzábal, 2019; Diez-Itza et al., 2021; Pérez et al., 2022).
Taking into account the error types, it was observed that WS children showed a higher frequency in SYS followed by SBT in both assessments, which is consistent with previous research in English-speaking WS children and adolescents (Huffman, 2019) and in Spanish-speaking WS children, adolescents, and adults (Hidalgo and Garayzábal, 2019; Pérez et al., 2022). This tendency has also been observed for TD (Bosch-Galcerán, 2004; Martínez, 2010) and in other neurodevelopmental genetic disorders (Barnes et al., 2009; Huelmo et al., 2017; Hidalgo and Garayzábal, 2019; Diez-Itza et al., 2021). Nevertheless, only for errors affecting SYS was a significant reduction observed after 6 months, since SBT segmental errors increased in frequency in the second assessment, a pattern also observed in TD at around 4 years of age (Diez-Itza and Martínez, 2004). It was also observed that the frequency of OMI and ASM segmental errors decreased in the second assessment. However, it was found that OMI errors continued to present a high frequency in WS adolescents and adults (Pérez et al., 2022). In the case of ASM errors, it has been observed that they were still present at ages 6 and 7 in WS (Hidalgo and Garayzábal, 2019), although with a lower incidence. The same occurs in TD (Martínez and Diez-Itza, 2012) where ASM errors have been considered representative of the late phonology of Spanish with a significant percentage at 7 years old (Bosch-Galcerán, 2004).
As for the profile of relative frequency of error types in the first and second assessment, it was observed that, as in absolute terms, the most frequent errors were those of SYS, STB, OMI, ASM, and ADD and this was similar to that observed in TD children of similar verbal age (Martínez, 2010) and DS children and adolescents in spontaneous speech (Diez-Itza et al., 2021). However, the intersections between the relative frequency profiles in WS children might suggest that the trajectories from the first to the second assessment was toward reducing the proportion of SYS and OMI and increasing the proportion of SBT, ASM and ADD errors, although only in the case of SBT this increase was statistically significant. This would suggest that in the second assessment there was a reconversion of the phonological system in relation to SBT segmental errors similar to that observed at the age of 4;6 years for TD (Diez-Itza and Martínez, 2004; Martínez, 2010).
When comparing WS children in the first assessment with the age groups of the normative group, it was observed that WS presented higher PEI than every normative group, except for the 3;0 TD group. These results would indicate that initially their phonological error index was analogous to that of the expansion stage, corresponding to ages 3;0–3;6 in TD. However, 6 months later, the frequency of the error index was significantly reduced and could be equated with the 4;6 year-old group, thus consistent with the stage of stabilization (ages 4;0–4;6 in TD), therefore showing an accelerated phonological development as previous studies had suggested (Martínez et al., 2014). There appeared to be dynamic development over the 6-month interval as WS children moved from one stage to another. Such atypical acceleration might be related to lexical growth, given the close relationship between lexical and phonological development, and their relatively preserved phonological memory (Majerus et al., 2003; Mervis et al., 2004; Stoel-Gammon, 2011), which would show the interdependence of the processes as well as the dynamic nature of linguistic development (Mareschal et al., 2007).
Concerning error types, the developmental pattern was different in both assessments. Thus, while WS children moved from the expansion stage to stabilization for SYS errors, showing a strongly accelerated growth rate of the phonological system, in the case of SBT, ASM, and ADD errors these children would be in the stabilization stage whereas for OMI segmental errors they would be in the expansion stage, although a reduction in their phonological index was observed in the 6-month interval. This contrast in evolution would suggest a slowdown in the growth rate of the phonological system of WS children, which could be interpreted in terms of delayed phonological acquisition (Pérez et al., 2022).
The study of the relative frequencies of error types showed that in the first assessment the profile was not comparable to that of children aged 3;0 because its frequency is higher for all types. However, in the second assessment the profile overlaps with that of children aged 3;6 years. In relative terms, these WS children would be in the expansion stage at both times although there would be certain progress in their phonological development. On the other hand, the high relative frequency of OMI at both assessment times may be considered atypical and specific to WS since TD children aged 4;0 years no longer produce this type of error with only between 20 and 30% of children showing absence of multiple vibrating/r/(Bosch-Galcerán, 2004; Diez-Itza et al., 2005). This was confirmed in our previous study where children, adolescents and adults showed a high frequency of vowel omission and liquid consonant omissions compared to the 5-year-old TD normative group therefore suggesting a deviant developmental trajectory (Pérez et al., 2022).
In conclusion, the results of the present study seem to confirm that the frequency of phonological errors in WS children decreases over a 6-month interval, showing an atypical acceleration. Moreover, the trajectories of late phonological development in WS children may not be linear, but dynamic as postulated by neuroconstructivist models since in a short period of time they move from the expansion stage (age 3) to the stabilization stage (age 4), perhaps favored by its interrelation with other components at different levels such as the lexicon (Mervis et al., 2004; Stoel-Gammon, 2011). Although the results are not conclusive on delayed vs. disordered phonological profiles, highly increased frequency of errors at the two time points assessed asynchronous with verbal age, suggests atypical developmental trajectories of phonological development in the WS children. The description of the detailed longitudinal phonological profile results in a better understanding of the syndrome as well as improved effectiveness of assessments and speech therapy intervention.
The shortcomings of this study stem mainly from the absence of controlled individual differences that could explain significant percentages of the variance observed in WS children. A larger number of participants would have been necessary to minimize these differences and make comparisons by age groups in WS. However, this is a small-scale exploratory study and confidence in the conclusions drawn from the results is reinforced by the large effect size for total errors and for errors affecting SYS. Further study would be necessary to assess the specific features and errors for each of the five types studied.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Oviedo for studies involving humans (Approval code 6_RRI_2022 and Approval date 05/10/2022). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the minor(s)' legal guardian/next of kin for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author contributions
VM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Methodology. VP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation. MAA: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MM: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. PV: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by grant FFI2012-39325-C03-03 from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO) to the SYNDROLING Project and Asociación por el estudio de la adquisición del lenguaje (AEAL).
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank the families who generously agreed to participate in this study and the collaborators of the LOGIN Research Group at the University of Oviedo.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Barnes, E., Roberts, J., Long, S. H., Martin, G. E., Berni, M. C., Mandulak, K. C., et al. (2009). Phonological accuracy and intelligibility in connected speech of boys with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52, 1048–1061. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0001)
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., and St. George, M. (2000). I. The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12(Suppl. 1), 7–29. doi: 10.1162/089892900561959
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A., and Sabo, H. (1988). “Dissociation between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndrome,” in Language Development in Exceptional Circumstances, eds D. Bishop, and K. Mogford (London: Churchill Livingstone), 177–189
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P., and Jernigan, T. L. (1994). “Williams syndrome: an unusual neuropsychological profile,” in (Atypical Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain Function, eds S. Broman, and K. Grafman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 23–56.
Benítez-Burraco, A., Garayzábal, E., and Cuetos, F. (2017). Morphology in Spanish-speaking children with Williams syndrome. Lang. Cogn. 9, 728–740. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2017.6
Brown, J. H., Johnson, M. H., Paterson, S. J., Gilmore, R., Longhi, E., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Spatial representation and attention in toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia 41, 1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00299-3
Clahsen, H., and Almazan, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition 68, 167–198. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00049-3
Clahsen, H., Ring, M., and Temple, C. (2004). “Lexical and morphological skills in English-speaking children with Williams syndrome,” in Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, Vol. 36, eds S. Bartke, and J. Siegmüller (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 221–244.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Diez-Itza, E., and Martínez, V. (2004). Las etapas tardías de la adquisición fonológica: Procesos de reducción de grupos consonánticos UB J. Psychol. 35, 177–202.
Diez-Itza, E., Martínez, V., Cantora, R., Justicia, F., and Bosch, L. (2001). “Late phonological processes in the acquisition of Spanish,” in Research on Child Language Acquisition, eds M. Almgren, A. Barreña, M. J. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazábal, and B. MacWhinney (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press), 790–799.
Diez-Itza, E., Martínez, V., Fernández-Urquiza, M., and Antón, A. (2017). “Morphological profile of Williams syndrome: typical or atypical?,” in Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children, Vol. 14, eds A. Auza Benavides, and R. G. Schwartz (Amsterdam: Springer International Publishing), 311–327.
Diez-Itza, E., Martínez, V., Miranda, M., Antón, A., Ojea, A. I., Fernández-Urquiza, M., et al. (2014). “The Syndroling Project: a comparative linguistic analysis of typical development profiles and neurodevelopmental genetic syndromes (Down, Williams and fragile X syndromes),” in Proceedings of the IASCL-XII International Congress for the Study of Child Language (Amsterdam).
Diez-Itza, E., Martínez, V., and Miranda, V. (2005). “Análisis y codificación fonológicos de corpus de habla infantil con el proyecto CHILDES: Errores de omisión en el desarrollo normal y en el Síndrome de Down,” in La enseñanza de las lenguas en una Europa multicultural, eds J. M. En Oro Cabanas, J. Anderson, and J. Varela Zapata (Santiago de Compostela: Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Servicio de Publicaciones), 272–285.
Diez-Itza, E., Martínez, V., Pérez, V., and Fernández-Urquiza, M. (2018). Explicit oral narrative intervention for students with Williams syndrome. Front. Psychol. 8:2337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02337
Diez-Itza, E., Miranda, M., Pérez, V., and Martínez, V. (2019). “Profiles of grammatical morphology in Spanish-speaking adolescents with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome,” in Atypical Language Development in Romance Languages, eds E. Aguilar-Mediavilla, L. Buil-Legaz, R. López-Penadés, V. A. Sanchez-Azanza, and D. Adrover-Roig (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 219–234.
Diez-Itza, E., Snow, C., and MacWhinney, B. (1999). La metodología RETAMHE y el proyecto CHILDES: Breviario para la codificación y análisis de lenguaje infantil. Psicothema 11, 517–530.
Diez-Itza, E., Vergara, P., Barros, M., Miranda, M., and Martínez, V. (2021). Assessing phonological profiles in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: the effect of elicitation methods. Front. Psychol. 12:662257. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.662257
Diez-Itza, E., Viejo, A., and Fernández-Urquiza, M. (2022). Pragmatic profiles of adults with Fragile X syndrome and Williams syndrome. Brain Sci. 12:385. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12030385
D'Souza, D., Cole, V., Farran, E. K., Brown, J. H., Humphreys, K., Howard, J., et al. (2015). Face processing in Williams syndrome is already atypical in infancy. Front. Psychol. 6:760. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00760
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., and Arribas, D. (2010). PPVT-III Peabody. Test de vocabulario en imágenes. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Farran, E. K., and Jarrold, C. (2003). Visuospatial cognition in Williams syndrome: reviewing and accounting for the strengths and weaknesses in performance. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 173–200. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN231&2_8
Grant, J., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Gathercole, S. A., Paterson, S., Howlin, P., Davies, M., et al. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and its relationship to language in Williams syndrome. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 2, 81–99. doi: 10.1080/135468097396342
Hidalgo, I. (2019). El nivel fónico de la población con síndrome de Smith Magenis: particularidades fonatorias y fonético-fonológicas. Comparativa con síndrome de Williams, síndrome de Down y desarrollo típico (Doctoral Dissertation). Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain.
Hidalgo, I., and Garayzábal, E. (2019). Diferencias fonológicas entre síndromes del neurodesarrollo: Evidencias a partir de los procesos de simplificación fonológica más frecuentes. Rev. Investig. Logopedia 9, 81–106. doi: 10.5209/rlog.62942
Hocking, D. R., Reeve, J., and Porter, M. A. (2015). Characterising the profile of everyday executive functioning and relation to IQ in adults with Williams Syndrome: Is the BRIEF adult version a valid rating scale? PLoS ONE 10:e0137628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137628
Huelmo, J., Martínez, V., and Diez-Itza, E. (2017). Evaluación de perfiles fonológicos en el síndrome X-Frágil mediante índices de error. Int. J. Dev. Educ. Psychol. 4, 67–76. doi: 10.17060/ijodaep.2017.n1.v4.1028
Huffman, M. J. (2019). Speech articulation in children with Williams syndrome or 7q11.23 duplication syndrome (Doctoral Dissertation). University of Louisville, Louisville, KY.
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Hewes, A. K., and Phillips, C. (2001). A Longitudinal assessment of diverging verbal and non-verbal abilities in the Williams syndrome phenotype. Cortex 37, 423–431. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70583-5
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders. Dev. Psychol. 45, 56–63. doi: 10.1037/a0014506
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Brown, J. H., Grice, S., and Paterson, S. (2003a). Dethroning the myth: cognitive dissociations and innate modularity in Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 227–242. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2003.9651893
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Scerif, G., and Ansari, D. (2003b). Double dissociations in developmental disorders? Theoretically misconceived, empirically dubious. Cortex 39, 161–163. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70091-1
Kozel, B. A., Barak, B., Kim, C. A., Mervis, C. B., Osborne, L. R., Porter, M., et al. (2021). Williams syndrome. Nat. Rev Dis. Prim. 7:42. doi: 10.1038/s41572-021-00276-z
Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsödl, M., Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., et al. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Dev. Sci. 5, 233–246. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00225
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Volume I: Transcription Format and Programs. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Majerus, S., Barisnikov, K., Vuillemin, I., Poncelet, M., and Linden, M. (2003). An investigation of verbal short-term memory and phonological processing in four children with Williams syndrome. Neurocase 9, 390–401. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.5.390.16558
Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M., and Westermann, G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism, Vol. I: How the Brain Constructs Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Martínez, V. (2010). Etapas tardías del desarrollo fonológico infantil: Procesos y límites del trastorno (Doctoral Dissertation). Universidad de Oviedo, Oviedo.
Martínez, V., Antón, A., Miranda, M., Pérez, V., Fernández-Toral, J., and Diez-Itza, E. (2014). “Accelerated phonological development in Williams syndrome: A two case corpus-based study of late phonological processes,” in Proceedings of the IASCL-XII International Congress for the Study of Child Language (Amsterdam).
Martínez, V., and Diez-Itza, E. (2012). Procesos de asimilación en las etapas tardías del desarrollo. Psicothema 24, 193–198.
Masataka, N. (2001). Why early linguistic milestones are delayed in children with Williams syndrome: late onset of hand banging as a possible rate–limiting constraint on the emergence of canonical babbling. Dev. Sci. 4, 158–164. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00161
Mervis, C. B., and Bertrand, J. (1997). “Developmental relations between cognition and language: evidence from Williams syndrome,” in Communication and Language Acquisition: Discoveries From Atypical Development, eds L. B. Adamson, and M. A. Romsky (Baltimore, MA: Paul Brookes Publishing), 75–160.
Mervis, C. B., and John, A. E. (2010). Cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with Williams syndrome: implications for intervention approaches. Am. J. Med. Genet. 154C, 229–248. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.30263
Mervis, C. B., Klein-Tasman, B. P., Huffman, M. J., Velleman, S. L., Pitts, C. H., Henderson, D. R., et al. (2015). Children with 7q11.23 duplication syndrome: psychological characteristics. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part A 167, 1436–1450. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.37071
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Bertrand, J., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B. P., and Armstrong, S. C. (2000). The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain Cogn. 44, 604–628. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2000.1232
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Rowe, M. L., Becerra, A. M., and Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2004). “Relations between language and cognition in Williams syndrome,” in Language Acquisition and Language Disorders Vol. 36, eds S. Bartke and J. Siegmüller (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 63–92. doi: 10.1075/lald.36.08mer
Miezah, D., Porter, M., Rossi, A., Kazzi, C., Batchelor, J., and Reeve, J. (2021). Cognitive profile of young children with Williams syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 65, 784–794. doi: 10.1111/jir.12860
Morris, C. A., Braddock, S. R., and Council On Genetics (2020). Health care supervision for children with Williams Syndrome. Pediatrics 145:e20193761. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-3761
Penke, M., and Krause, M. (2004). “Regular and irregular inflectional morphology in German Williams syndrome,” in Williams Syndrome Across Languages, eds S. Bartke and J. Siegmüller (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 245–270.
Pérez, V., Martínez, V., and Diez-Itza, E. (2022). Late phonological development in Williams syndrome. Front. Psychol. 13:992512. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.992512
Pérez-Jurado, L. A. (2003). Williams-Beuren syndrome: a model of recurrent genomic mutation. Horm. Res. Paediatr. 59, 106–113. doi: 10.1159/000067836
Porter, M. A., and Coltheart, M. (2006). Global and local processing in Williams syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome: perception, attention, and construction. Dev. Neuropsychol. 30, 771–789. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3003_1
Purser, H. R. M., Thomas, M. S. C., Snoxall, S., Mareschal, D., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). Definitions versus categorization: assessing the development of lexico-semantic knowledge in Williams syndrome. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 46, 361–373. doi: 10.3109/13682822.2010.497531
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. J. Child Lang. 38, 1–34. doi: 10.1017/S0305000910000425
Stojanovik, V. (2006). Social interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndrome. J. Neurolinguist. 19, 157–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.11.005
Strømme, P., Bjørnstad, P. G., and Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. J. Child Neurol. 17, 269–271. doi: 10.1177/088307380201700406
Thomas, M. S. C., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Modeling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. Psychol. Rev. 110, 647–682. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.647
Van der Geest, J. N., Lagers-van Haselen, G. C., van Hagen, J. M., Brenner, E., Govaerts, L. C., de Coo, I. F., et al. (2005). Visual depth processing in Williams-Beuren syndrome. Exp. Brain Res. 166, 200–209. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2355-1
Vicari, S., Bates, E., Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: an example of a dissociation between language and cognition? J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 862–876. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704106073
Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F., and Volterra, V. (2002). Language acquisition in special populations: a comparison between Down and Williams syndromes. Neuropsychologia 40, 2461–2470. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00083-0
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., Tonucci, F., and Vicari, S. (2003). Early linguistic abilities of Italian children with Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 33–58. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN231&2_3
Keywords: Williams syndrome, phonological profiles, spontaneous speech, atypical language development, neurodevelopmental disorder
Citation: Martínez V, Pérez V, Antón MA, Miranda M and Vergara P (2024) Longitudinal profiles of late phonological development in children with Williams syndrome. Front. Commun. 9:1386899. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2024.1386899
Received: 16 February 2024; Accepted: 02 April 2024;
Published: 16 April 2024.
Edited by:
Victoria Marrero-Aguiar, National University of Distance Education (UNED), SpainReviewed by:
Marie Thérèse Le Normand, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), FranceMaria Fernanda Lara-Diaz, National University of Colombia, Colombia
Copyright © 2024 Martínez, Pérez, Antón, Miranda and Vergara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Verónica Martínez, martinezveronica@uniovi.es; Vanesa Pérez, UO6965@uniovi.es