Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Commun., 31 August 2022
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic The Next Phase in Heritage Language Studies: Methodological Considerations and Advancements View all 23 articles

Predictors of language proficiency and cultural identification in heritage bilinguals

  • 1Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK, United States

According to the 2020 U.S. Census Bureau, more than 66 million residents over the age of 5 in the United States speak a language other than English at home. Some bilinguals become dominant in the majority language that is spoken in the community as opposed to their native “heritage” language acquired at home. The objective of the current study was to uncover the predictors of language proficiency and cultural identification in different groups of heritage speakers. In our sample, heritage speakers acquired their heritage language first and English second and rated their proficiency in their heritage language lower than in English. We found that English proficiency was most reliably predicted by the duration of heritage language immersion, while heritage language proficiency was most reliably predicted by contexts of acquisition and exposure to both languages. Higher heritage language proficiency was associated with greater heritage language experience through friends and reading, less English experience through family, and later age of English acquisition. The trade-off between heritage language and English language experience was more pronounced for non-Spanish than Spanish heritage speakers. Finally, despite higher proficiency in English, cultural identification was higher with the heritage language, and was predicted by heritage language receptive proficiency and heritage language experience through family and reading. We conclude that self-reported proficiency and cultural identification differ depending on heritage speakers' native languages, as well as how the heritage language and majority language are acquired and used. Our findings highlight the importance of taking individual language history into consideration when combining different groups of heritage speakers.

Introduction

A growing percentage of the U.S. population speaks a language other than English at home. From 23.06 million in 1980 (Zeigler and Camarota, 2019) to 66.09 million in 2020 (U.S. Census Bureau, 2020), the number of people over the age of 5 who speak a non-English language at home has nearly tripled. These non-English home languages are often referred to as heritage languages and carry familial, cultural, and historical significance. Heritage bilinguals tend to feel strong personal connections to their heritage culture. However, as a result of acquiring the majority language at an early age and being formally educated in the majority language, heritage bilinguals generally prefer using the language of the community as opposed to their home language(s) (Valdés, 2000; Scontras et al., 2015).

Heritage bilinguals vary greatly in the age of second language acquisition and heritage language proficiency. While some heritage bilinguals immigrate to the host country with their parents and acquire the majority language in early childhood at school, others are born in the host country to foreign-born parents and acquire both languages simultaneously. Furthermore, while some heritage bilinguals have native-like proficiency in both languages, others show better linguistic command in the majority language than home language. Some can communicate fluently in both languages but are unable to read and write in the heritage language, and others have some understanding of the heritage language but have limited expressive skills (Montrul, 2005). Thus, heritage bilinguals are qualitatively distinct from second-language learners and native monolingual speakers (see Montrul, 2011 for review). Given that heritage speakers exist along a continuum of linguistic abilities and experiences, the present study aims to capture the linguistic predictors associated with self-reported measures of proficiency and cultural identification in different groups of heritage bilinguals.

Language proficiency

There are several factors impacting heritage language proficiency, including language exposure (Gathercole and Thomas, 2009; Hoff et al., 2012; Thomas et al., 2014; Gollan et al., 2015; Jia and Paradis, 2015; Unsworth, 2016; Hovsepian, 2018; Makarova et al., 2019; Giguere and Hoff, 2020; Tao et al., 2021; Vorobyeva and Bel, 2021) and frequency of use (Hakuta and D'Andrea, 1992; Bedore et al., 2012; Albirini, 2014; Chen et al., 2018; Schmid and Yilmaz, 2018; Daskalaki et al., 2019; Otwinowska et al., 2021) in and outside of the home. Access to a heritage language community that extends beyond the home context positively predicts heritage language vocabulary and lexical retrieval (Albirini, 2014; Gollan et al., 2015; Schmid and Yilmaz, 2018; Tao et al., 2021), morphosyntax (Kupisch and Rothman, 2018; Rodina et al., 2020; Torregrossa et al., 2022), and pronunciation (Au and Romo, 1997; de Leeuw et al., 2010; Stoehr et al., 2017; Karayayla and Schmid, 2019; McCarthy and de Leeuw, 2022). Being surrounded by native speakers of the heritage language affords opportunities to listen and practice the language in various settings and discuss a wide variety of topics. Furthermore, heritage bilinguals exist along a continuum of linguistic abilities. In terms of reading and writing, heritage bilinguals are more likely to be literate in the majority language by virtue of being educated in that language. If heritage bilinguals do become literate in the heritage language, their reading skills tend to be better than their writing skills (Polinsky, 2015). Therefore, home and socio-linguistic contexts play important roles in the development of heritage language proficiency, and heritage bilinguals often exhibit variable degrees of fluency depending on the type of linguistic ability under examination (e.g., listening, speaking, reading, and writing).

In addition to the frequency of heritage language use, the age of second language acquisition and duration of immersion have been found to predict heritage language proficiency. The later a child becomes exposed to the majority language, the more likely they are to attain and retain competency in their heritage language (Polinsky and Kagan, 2007; Albirini, 2014; Jia and Paradis, 2015; Montrul, 2016; Gharibi and Boers, 2017; Meir et al., 2017; Armon-Lotem et al., 2021; Meir and Janssen, 2021). Studies have shown that sequential bilinguals often have greater proficiency in their heritage language than simultaneous bilinguals (e.g., Jia and Aaronson, 2003; Carreira and Kagan, 2011). For instance, children who acquire the majority language simultaneously or soon after the heritage language (e.g., before the age of 3) often score lower on tests of HL vocabulary (Gharibi and Boers, 2017; Armon-Lotem et al., 2021) and morphosyntax (Albirini, 2014; Jia and Paradis, 2015; Meir et al., 2017; Meir and Janssen, 2021) relative to children who spent more time learning the heritage language before acquiring the majority language. Age of acquisition predicts language aptitude and preference even among bilinguals who acquire the majority language later in adolescence. In a longitudinal study, Jia and Aaronson (2003) evaluated the changes in language preferences and Chinese proficiency among native Chinese-speaking children and adolescents who immigrated to the United States. Participants who immigrated to the United States at an early age (before the age of 9) switched their language preference from Chinese to English and became more proficient in English than Chinese within the first year. Those who immigrated to the United States at a later age (between 10 and 16 years of age) maintained their preference for Chinese across all 3 years and continued to use Chinese with their parents and siblings. Altogether, such findings demonstrate that both age of second language acquisition and duration of immersion influence heritage language proficiency.

To determine which factors promote heritage language proficiency, Gollan et al. (2015) tested Chinese-English and Spanish-English heritage bilingual adults on the Multilingual Naming Test (MINT; Gollan et al., 2012), which is an objective measure of language proficiency. For Chinese-English bilinguals, higher heritage language proficiency was associated with exposure to a greater number of heritage speakers during childhood. For Spanish-English bilinguals, higher heritage language proficiency was instead associated with less English use. The authors proposed that the differences between groups may stem from cross-cultural variations in the interpretation of the questionnaire items. Across all participants, proficiency in the heritage language was uniquely predicted by the number of heritage language speakers encountered during childhood, the primary caregiver's level of English proficiency, and the participants' age of English acquisition. For Persian-English bilingual children, parents' attitude toward the heritage language was the strongest predictor of heritage language proficiency (as measured by a verbal fluency task and auditory picture-word matching test) in simultaneous bilinguals, whereas the age at emigration was the strongest predictor of heritage language proficiency in sequential heritage bilinguals (Gharibi and Boers, 2017). These findings demonstrate that individual variation within and across different groups of heritage bilinguals influences heritage language proficiency. The present study thus compares Spanish-English heritage speakers to other groups of heritage speakers (i.e., non-Spanish) living in the United States on heritage language and English proficiency ratings.

Cultural identification

Language serves as a bridge for creating a sense of belonging to an ethnic group in children (Yu, 2015; Arredondo et al., 2016), adolescents (Phinney et al., 2001; Oh and Fuligni, 2010), and adults (Noels et al., 1996; Cho, 2000; Chen et al., 2008; Gatbonton and Trofimovich, 2008; Yu, 2015). Across all ages, greater proficiency in a heritage language is associated with stronger ethnic identity and affiliation with the ethnic group. However, heritage speakers vary in their cultural affiliation toward their heritage language and majority language. For example, individuals from minority groups sometimes report conflicting identities, in which they want to preserve the cultural values associated with their heritage language, but also want to fit in with the culture associated with the majority language (Phinney, 1990). On open-ended questions from the Multigroup Ethic Identity Measure and Ethnic Identity Scale, Arredondo et al. (2016) found that Spanish-English heritage bilingual children reported feeling a sense of pride for being able to speak Spanish, showed an appreciation for cultural diversity, enjoyed communicating exclusively with friends and family in a “secret” language, and expressed positivity toward helping their parents learn English and in turn, learning Spanish from their parents. In the same study, some of the children described Spanish as confusing or too difficult at times. Furthermore, heritage bilinguals are more likely to assimilate to the customs and practices of the host culture with each successive generation compared to the last (Felix-Ortiz et al., 1994). Hence, among heritage bilinguals, factors related to heritage language proficiency and migration, such as age of second language acquisition and duration of immersion, may predict cultural identification.

In a large heterogeneous sample of adult bilinguals varying in language and cultural backgrounds, Schroeder et al. (2017) identified the linguistic factors that predict cultural identification. Increased first language (L1) exposure through media, higher L1 proficiency, fewer years immersed in a second language (L2) family context, but more years immersed in an L2 school/work context led to increased first-language cultural affiliation. In contrast, increased immersion in an L2 school/work context, lower L2 perceived accent, and earlier L2 age of acquisition was associated with increased cultural identification with the second-language culture. These findings demonstrate that factors related to the second language influence both first-language and second-language cultural affiliation, whereas factors associated with the first language only influence first-language cultural identification. These effects also differed by age of L2 acquisition and whether the language was learned in a formal or informal context. Schroeder et al. argued that through language, bilinguals can access their culture by interacting with members of the same cultural group, actively participating in various cultural activities, and engaging in media from that culture (through TV, radio, and books). To our knowledge, no study to date has taken a similar approach in identifying linguistic predictors of cultural identification among different groups of heritage bilinguals.

With over 40.5 million people over the age of 5 speaking Spanish at home, Spanish is by far the most spoken non-English language in the United States (U.S. Census Bureau, 2020). Therefore, Spanish heritage bilinguals may have more opportunities to use and practice with native speakers and engage in cultural activities with members of the same cultural group than other non-English heritage bilinguals. For example, in the city of Chicago, Latinos are the second largest ethnic group at 29.7%, whereas Asians and other cultural groups make up around 12.7% of the city's population (The Economist, 2017). Second, the one-to-one mapping between the Spanish language and Latino culture is less clear, as multiple cultural groups from various countries speak Spanish. In contrast, the mapping between language and culture for other languages is more consistent (e.g., Korean with Korea). Hence, there is the possibility that the linguistic and cultural experiences of Spanish heritage bilinguals are more diverse and less homogeneous compared to non-Spanish heritage bilinguals. Third, studies have shown that the motivation for maintaining the heritage language differs between Spanish-English heritage learners and non-Spanish heritage learners. Hur et al. (2021) examined the expectations and attitudes toward heritage language courses. While Spanish-English heritage learners perceived their classes as a necessary tool for professional success, Korean-English heritage learners used their classes as a way to reconnect with their Korean culture and other members of their heritage language community. For these reasons, we distinguish between Spanish and non-Spanish heritage bilinguals to examine how predictors of proficiency and cultural identification are moderated by native language background.

The present study

The goal of the present study was to identify the predictors of self-reported language proficiency and cultural identification in different groups of heritage speakers. Specifically, we examined how age of acquisition, duration of immersion, and contexts of acquisition and exposure (i.e., through friends, family, media, reading, and language tapes and self-instruction) influenced self-reported measures of proficiency and cultural identification in the heritage language and in English among Spanish heritage bilinguals and non-Spanish heritage bilinguals. Considering heritage bilinguals typically have better comprehension than oral skills in their heritage language (Polinsky, 2015), we separated expressive (speaking) from receptive (understanding and reading) proficiency in our analyses. Based on past research, we hypothesized that heritage language proficiency and cultural identification will be predicted by heritage language usage in informal contexts, such as in the home through family and in the community through friends (Gollan et al., 2015; Jia and Paradis, 2015; Montrul, 2016), as well as the age of English acquisition and length of immersion in an English-speaking country (Montrul, 2008; Gathercole and Thomas, 2009; Vorobyeva and Bel, 2021). In addition, we hypothesized that English proficiency and cultural identification will be predicted by both heritage language and English usage in informal contexts (i.e., home, friends) and more formal individual contexts (i.e., language tapes, language labs, and self-instruction). Altogether, the present study provides a deeper understanding of the interactivity between language and culture in heritage bilinguals.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants included 133 heritage speakers who acquired a non-English native language first and English second, and who rated English as more proficient than their native language on the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q; Marian et al., 2007). Data were compiled from previous studies conducted in our lab between 2011 and 2022 (i.e., secondary data analysis; Bartolotti et al., 2011; Chabal et al., 2015, 2022; Freeman et al., 2016, 2022; Shook and Marian, 2016; Chen et al., 2017; Marian et al., 2018, 2021; Hayakawa et al., 2020). Participants' mean age at the time of testing was 23.97 (SD = 6.24), and 67% were female. Seventy-nine participants had Spanish as their heritage language, while the remaining 54 participants had a non-Spanish language as their heritage language. The non-Spanish languages all utilized a different script than English and included Korean (n = 31), Chinese (n = 16), Thai (n = 4), Hebrew, Russian, and Tamil (n = 1 each). Spanish and non-Spanish heritage speakers did not significantly differ in age (M = 24.68 and 22.93, respectively), gender (67.1 and 67.6% female), or years of education (M = 14.66 and 15.21), ps > 0.137. Non-Spanish bilinguals knew marginally more languages (M = 2.43) than Spanish bilinguals (M = 2.22), p = 0.067. Refer to Table 1 for the linguistic profiles of each group of heritage bilinguals (Spanish and non-Spanish bilinguals), including self-reported heritage language (HL) and English proficiency, ages of HL and English acquisition, and contexts of HL and English acquisition and exposure. Participants had no history of a language or learning disability and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Linguistic profiles of Spanish and non-Spanish heritage speakers.

Materials

Language experience and proficiency questionnaire

The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q; Marian et al., 2007) was used to acquire each participant's linguistic profile. Participants were asked to list the languages they know in order of dominance as well as acquisition. Information about each language's (1) acquisition, (2) proficiency, and (3) exposure were obtained. For age of acquisition, participants provided the ages at which they began acquiring, became fluent, began reading, and became fluent at reading each language. For proficiency, participants rated their proficiency in each language in terms of speaking, understanding, and reading on a scale from 0 (None) to 10 (Perfect). For manner of acquisition, participants rated the extent to which various factors contributed to learning each language on a scale from 0 (Not a Contributor) to 10 (Most Important Contributor). These factors included friends, family, reading, language tapes/self-instruction, watching TV, and listening to radio/music. For language exposure, participants rated the extent to which they were currently exposed to each language in various contexts, including friends, family, watching TV, listening to radio/music, reading, and language lab/self-instruction on a scale from 0 (Never) to 10 (Always).

Cultural identification information was obtained by asking participants to list the cultures they identified with and rate the extent to which they identified with each culture on a scale from 0 (No Identification) to 10 (Complete Identification). Cultural identification with the HL and English was determined based on ratings given to cultures associated with each language (e.g., “Korea” for cultural identification with Korean as a HL, “USA” for cultural identification with English). If more than one culture associated with a language was listed (e.g., Culture 1: “Latino” and Culture 2: “Mexican” for Spanish), we selected the rating for the culture that was ranked highest. In addition to linguistic and cultural information, demographic information such as age, gender, years of formal education, highest level of education, year of migration to the United States (if applicable), and any history of vision, hearing, language, or learning disabilities were provided by each participant. Although some participants were fluent in a third language, we did not analyze the third language information due to the small number of participants who were fluent in a third language.

Procedure

All studies included in the secondary analysis were reviewed and approved by Northwestern University's Institutional Review Board. In all studies, the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (Marian et al., 2007) was administered toward the end of the testing session. Participants provided informed consent prior to the start of the experiment and were debriefed at the end of the experiment.

Data analysis

Two sets of analyses were conducted to examine predictors of heritage language (HL) and English proficiency and cultural identification among Spanish and non-Spanish heritage speakers. To address issues of multicollinearity, we began by examining the correlational structure of LEAP-Q measures and created 7 composite measures for each language, which included Age of Acquisition (AoA), Duration of Immersion (average number of years immersed in a country, school, or workplace in which each language was spoken), and five composite measures which each represented an aggregated measure of manner of acquisition and current exposure in different contexts. The included contexts were Family Acquisition and Exposure (averaged across ratings of how much family contributed to the acquisition of each language and how much participants are currently exposed to each language through family), Friends Acquisition and Exposure, Media Acquisition and Exposure (e.g., through TV, radio), Reading Acquisition and Exposure, and Individual Acquisition and Exposure (e.g., through language tapes/language lab/self-instruction). In order to assess the impact of relative language experience, we additionally calculated a dominance score for each composite measure by subtracting the HL score from the English score. All fixed effects had VIF scores < 5, indicating minimal multicollinearity.

Effects of HL, English, and relative language experience measures within each set of analyses were examined with separate linear mixed-effects models, with variable numbers of participants depending on the availability of relevant proficiency or cultural identification measures for individual subjects. Models therefore included effects of (1) HL experience on HL and English receptive (averaged across understanding and reading) and expressive (speaking) proficiency (n = 126), (2) English experience on HL and English receptive and expressive proficiency (n = 127), (3) relative language experience on HL and English receptive and expressive proficiency (n = 126), (4) HL experience on HL and English cultural identification (n = 79), (5) English experience on HL and English cultural identification (n = 79), and (6) relative language experience on HL and English cultural identification (n = 79).

Fixed effects for proficiency models included the 7 HL, English, or relative language experience composite measures plus all two-, three-, and four-way interactions with Heritage Group (Spanish vs. non-Spanish), Language (HL vs. English proficiency), and Measure (receptive vs. expressive proficiency). Cultural identification models included the 7 composite measures, receptive and expressive proficiency, plus all two- and three-way interactions with Heritage Group (Spanish vs. non-Spanish) and Language (HL vs. English cultural identification). All models included a random intercept for participant. Contrasts for Heritage Group (Spanish: −0.57 vs. Non-Spanish: +0.43), Language (HL: −0.5 vs. English: +0.5), and Measure (Expressive: −0.5 vs. Receptive: +0.5) were centered and weighted by the number of responses. Continuous fixed effects were mean-centered and scaled via z-score transformation.

Parameter estimates and significance of fixed effects were assessed with the Satterwhite method using the lme4 (Bates et al., 2014) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017) R packages. Tukey-adjusted follow-up tests of simple effects were conducted using the emmeans and emtrends functions of the emmeans R package (Lenth et al., 2018).

Results

Predictors of heritage language and English proficiency

Effects of heritage language (HL) experience

Self-reported proficiency was significantly higher in English (M = 9.55, 95% CI [9.38, 9.73]) than in the heritage language (M = 7.93, 95% CI [7.76, 8.10]), p < 0.001. See Table 2 for full output. A two-way interaction between Language and Heritage Group (p = 0.014) indicated that Spanish bilinguals had significantly higher HL proficiency than non-Spanish bilinguals [Estimate = 0.64, SE = 0.17, t(163.94) = 3.70, p < 0.001], whereas the two groups did not differ in English proficiency [Estimate = 0.27, SE = 0.17, t(163.94) = 1.53, p = 0.128].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effects of heritage language experience on self-reported heritage language and English proficiency.

Across both groups, the composite measures of HL Acquisition and Exposure through friends (Language x Friends: p = 0.023; Figure 1A) and reading (Language x Reading: p < 0.001) predicted higher self-reported HL proficiency [Friends: Estimate = 0.16, SE = 0.09, t(163.94) = 1.81, p = 0.070; Reading: Estimate = 0.68, SE = 0.09, t(163.94) = 7.33, p < 0.001], but not English proficiency (ps > 0.202). The effect of reading experience was greater for receptive HL proficiency [Estimate = 0.89, SE = 0.11, t(274) = 8.18, p < 0.001] compared to expressive proficiency [Estimate = 0.47, SE = 0.11, t(274) = 4.27, p < 0.001; Language x Measure x Reading: p = 0.017].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effects of heritage language (HL) experience on self-reported HL (dark gray) and English (light gray) proficiency. Across both groups, HL proficiency increased with greater HL acquisition and exposure through friends (A), while English proficiency decreased with greater HL immersion duration (B). HL proficiency increased with greater HL reading acquisition and exposure for non-Spanish, but not Spanish bilinguals (C). Overall proficiency decreased with greater HL individual acquisition and exposure, which was particularly the case for HL proficiency among non-Spanish bilinguals (D). Dot sizes reflect the number of participants contributing to each aggregated value (max = 32, median = 3, min = 1).

In contrast, a two-way interaction between Language and Immersion (p = 0.002) revealed that a longer duration of HL immersion was associated with significantly lower English proficiency [Estimate = −0.17, SE = 0.08, t(163.94) = −2.03, p = 0.044], but not HL proficiency [Estimate = 0.05, SE = 0.08, t(163.94) = 0.53, p = 0.595; Figure 1B]. Greater HL acquisition and exposure through individual contexts (e.g., self-instruction, language labs, and language tapes) was associated with lower proficiency overall (p < 0.001), which was particularly the case for HL proficiency [Estimate = −0.63, SE = 0.10, t(164) = −6.25, p < 0.001] compared to English proficiency [Estimate = −0.30, SE = 0.10, t(164) = −2.91, p = 0.004; Language x Individual: p = 0.001].

Finally, three-way interactions with Language and Heritage Group revealed that ratings of HL acquisition and exposure through reading (p < 0.001) and individual contexts (p = 0.002) were more predictive of HL proficiency for non-Spanish bilinguals [Reading: Estimate = 1.25, SE = 0.15, t(164) = 8.57, p < 0.001; Individual: Estimate = −1.06, SE = 0.17, t(164) = −6.36, p < 0.001] than Spanish bilinguals [Reading: Estimate = 0.25, SE = 0.15, t(164) = 1.68, p = 0.095; Individual: Estimate = −0.21, SE = 0.12, t(164) = −1.78, p = 0.077; see Figures 1C,D].

Effects of English experience

Earlier ages of English acquisition (Language x AoA: p < 0.001) and higher ratings of English acquisition and exposure through family (Language x Family: p = 0.012) predicted lower HL proficiency [AoA: Estimate = 0.36, SE = 0.10, t(163) = 3.50, p < 0.001; Family: Estimate = −0.34, SE = 0.11, t(163) = −3.04, p < 0.001], but not English proficiency (ps > 0.48). Refer to Table 3 for full output. Although simple effects did not reach significance, an interaction between Language and Reading (p = 0.016) indicated that higher ratings of English acquisition and exposure through reading were associated with lower HL proficiency [Estimate = −0.08, SE = 0.12, t(163) = −0.62, p = 0.536], but higher English proficiency [Estimate = 0.18, SE = 0.12, t(163) = 1.52, p = 0.131].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Effects of English experience on self-reported heritage language and English proficiency.

Three-way interactions with Language and Heritage Group revealed that the effects of age of English acquisition (p = 0.046) and family (p = 0.029) on HL proficiency were greater for non-Spanish bilinguals [AoA: Estimate = 0.65, SE = 0.15, t(163) = 4.30, p < 0.001; Family: Estimate = −0.55, SE = 0.19, t(163) = −2.97, p = 0.003] than Spanish bilinguals [AoA: Estimate = 0.07, SE = 0.13, t(163) = 0.50, p = 0.62; Family: Estimate = −0.13, SE = 0.12, t(163) = −1.02, p = 0.311; see Figures 2A,B, respectively].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effects of English experience on self-reported HL (dark gray) and English (light gray) proficiency. Among non-Spanish bilinguals, HL proficiency increased with later ages of English acquisition (A) and decreased with greater English acquisition and exposure through family (B). Dot sizes reflect the number of participants contributing to each aggregated value (max = 18, median = 3, min = 1).

Effects of relative language experience (English—HL)

A significant three-way interaction between Language, Heritage Group, and relative immersion (p = 0.016) revealed that among non-Spanish bilinguals, relatively longer English (vs. HL) immersion predicted higher self-reported proficiency in both the HL [Estimate = 0.53, SE = 0.17, t(151) = 3.16, p = 0.002] and in English [Estimate = 0.43, SE = 0.17, t(151) = 2.58, p = 0.011]. Among Spanish bilinguals, relatively longer English immersion was associated with marginally lower HL proficiency [Estimate = −0.22, SE = 0.12, t(151) = −1.90, p = 0.059], with no effect on English proficiency (p = 0.549; see Figure 3A and Table 4 for full output).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effects of relative language experience (English—HL) on self-reported HL (dark gray) and English (light gray) proficiency. Among non-Spanish bilinguals, both HL and English proficiency increased with relatively greater English (vs. HL) immersion, while for Spanish bilinguals, HL proficiency decreased with relatively greater English immersion (A). Among non-Spanish bilinguals, lower HL proficiency was predicted by relatively earlier ages of English (vs. HL) acquisition (B), as well as relatively higher ratings of English (vs. HL) acquisition and exposure through family (C), media (D), and reading (E). Higher ratings of English (vs. HL) acquisition and exposure in individual contexts was non-significantly associated with higher English proficiency and lower HL proficiency (F). Dot sizes reflect the number of participants contributing to each aggregated value (max = 46, median = 10, min = 1).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of relative language experience (English—HL) on self-reported heritage language and English proficiency.

A series of three-way interactions additionally emerged for relative age of acquisition (p < 0.001) and the composite measures for relative acquisition and exposure through family (p = 0.007), media (p = 0.008), and reading (p < 0.001). Among non-Spanish bilinguals, HL proficiency was negatively predicted by more similar ages of HL and English acquisition [Estimate = 0.60, SE = 0.14, t(151) = 4.41, p < 0.001] and relatively higher ratings of English (vs. HL) acquisition and exposure experience through family [Estimate = −0.50, SE = 0.13, t(151) = −3.85, p < 0.002], media [Estimate = −0.52, SE = 0.14, t(151) = −3.76, p < 0.002], and reading [Estimate = −0.63, SE = 0.13, t(151) = −4.82, p < 0.001]. Relative AoA, manner of acquisition, and exposure did not predict HL proficiency for Spanish bilinguals (ps > 0.242) or English proficiency for either group (ps > 0.118). Finally, a significant two-way interaction between Language and relative individual experience [Estimate = 0.17, SE = 0.07, t(327) = 2.34, p = 0.020] indicated that greater English (vs. HL) acquisition and exposure in individual contexts was (non-significantly) associated with lower HL proficiency [Estimate = −0.11, SE = 0.10, t(151) = −1.15, p = 0.252] and higher English proficiency [Estimate = 0.08, SE = 0.10, t(151) = 0.79, p = 0.438; see Figures 3B–F].

In sum, higher HL proficiency was predicted by greater HL experience through reading and friends, later absolute and relative ages of English acquisition, less absolute and relative English experience through family, and less relative English experience through reading and media. HL reading experience had a greater impact on HL receptive proficiency (understanding/reading) compared to expressive proficiency (speaking). English proficiency declined with longer durations of HL immersion, and proficiency in both languages increased with longer relative durations of English (vs. HL) language immersion. Proficiency in both languages declined with greater HL experience in individual contexts (see Figure 4). Notably, effects of both HL and English experience were generally more robust among non-Spanish compared to Spanish bilinguals.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Overview of heritage language (HL) and English experience effects on self-reported HL and English proficiency.

Predictors of heritage language and English cultural identification

Effects of heritage language experience

Cultural identification was significantly higher with the heritage language (M = 7.71, 95% CI [7.07, 8.35]) than English (M = 6.74, 95% CI [6.09, 7.38]), p = 0.035. See Table 5 for full output.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Effects of heritage language experience on heritage language and English cultural identification.

A three-way interaction between Language, Heritage Group, and self-reported Receptive Proficiency (p = 0.020) revealed that among Spanish bilinguals, cultural identification with English (but not the HL) declined with higher receptive proficiency in the HL [Estimate = −2.16, SE = 0.89, t(116) = −2.43, p = 0.017; Figure 5A]. A three-way interaction with Reading (p = 0.042) revealed that among Spanish bilinguals, higher ratings of HL acquisition and exposure through reading [Estimate = 0.79, SE = 0.43, t(116) = 1.83, p = 0.062] were associated with greater cultural identification with English, but lower cultural identification with the HL [Estimate = −0.82, SE = 0.43, t(118) = −1.89, p = 0.071; Figure 5B]. Cultural identification with English was not moderated by receptive HL proficiency (p = 0.834) or reading experience (p = 0.669) among non-Spanish bilinguals.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effects of heritage language (HL) experience on cultural identification with the HL (dark gray) and English (light gray). Among Spanish bilinguals, cultural identification with English increased with lower HL receptive proficiency (A), as well as greater HL acquisition and exposure through reading (B) and family (C). Dot sizes reflect the number of participants contributing to each aggregated value (max = 27, median = 2, min = 1).

Finally, the composite measure of family HL acquisition and exposure was unexpectedly associated with increased cultural identification with English [Estimate = 0.69, SE = 0.33, t(116) = 2.08, p = 0.040], but not the HL (p = 0.195; Language × Family: p = 0.023). Although the three-way interaction with Heritage Group was not significant (p = 0.123), simple effects revealed that the effect of HL family experience on English identification was driven by Spanish [Estimate = 1.52, SE = 0.57, t(116) = 2.65, p = 0.009] rather than non-Spanish bilinguals [Estimate = −0.15, SE = 0.46, t(116) = −0.45, p = 0.65; see Figure 5C]. No significant effects of English experience were observed for cultural identification (see Supplementary Table 1 for full output).

Effects of relative language experience (English—HL)

A significant main effect of relative immersion indicated that relatively longer durations of English (vs. HL) immersion predicted greater cultural identification with both languages (p = 0.019; see Table 6 for full output). Consistent with the effect of self-reported HL receptive proficiency, a three-way interaction between Language, Heritage Group, and relative self-reported receptive proficiency (p = 0.041) indicated that among Spanish bilinguals, cultural identification with English (but not the HL) increased with greater relative English (vs. HL) receptive proficiency [Estimate = 2.28, SE = 1.04, t(116) = 2.18, p = 0.031; see Figure 6A]. Relative proficiency did not moderate cultural identification with either language for non-Spanish bilinguals (ps > 0.42). A significant interaction between Heritage Group and relative family acquisition and exposure (p = 0.021) indicated that relative English (vs. HL) family experience was a (non-significant) negative predictor of overall cultural identification among Spanish bilinguals [Estimate = −0.64, SE = 0.42, t(58) = −1.52, p = 0.135] and a marginally positive predictor of overall identification among non-Spanish bilinguals [Estimate = 0.56, SE = 0.29, t(58) = 1.93, p = 0.058]. Although the three-way interaction with Language did not approach significance (p = 0.435), relatively greater English (vs. HL) family experience was associated with significantly lower cultural identification with English among Spanish bilinguals [Estimate = −1.21, SE = 0.6, t(116) = −2.03, p = 0.045] and marginally greater cultural identification with the HL among non-Spanish bilinguals [Estimate = 0.73, SE = 0.41, t(116) = 1.78, p = 0.08; see Figure 6B].

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Effects of relative language experience (English—HL) on heritage language and English cultural identification.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Effects of relative language experience (English—HL) on cultural identification with the HL (dark gray) and English (light gray). Among Spanish bilinguals, cultural identification with English increased with higher relative English (vs. HL) receptive proficiency (A). Relatively higher ratings of English (vs. HL) acquisition and exposure through family was associated with lower English identification among Spanish bilinguals and marginally higher HL identification among non-Spanish bilinguals (B). Dot sizes reflect the number of participants contributing to each aggregated value (max = 13, median = 3, min = 1).

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to uncover linguistic predictors of self-reported language proficiency and cultural identification among different groups of adult heritage bilinguals. Self-reported proficiency in the majority language (English) was best predicted by the duration of immersion in the heritage language (HL). As expected, a longer cumulative duration of immersion in a country or school and/or work environment in which the HL was spoken was associated with lower reported English proficiency. Higher reported HL proficiency was predicted by higher ratings of HL acquisition and use through reading and friends, lower ratings of English acquisition and use through family, and later ages of English acquisition. Proficiency in both languages declined with greater HL experience in individual contexts (e.g., acquisition and exposure through self-instruction, language tapes, and language labs). Finally, despite higher self-reported English proficiency, cultural identification was higher with the HL, and this was especially true for Spanish heritage bilinguals. English cultural identification was negatively associated with subjective HL receptive proficiency, and to a lesser extent, positively associated with greater reliance on reading and family for HL acquisition and use. In addition to characterizing the factors that promote language proficiency and cultural identification, a critical finding from the present investigation is that the impact of heritage language and English language experience varied depending on heritage speakers' native languages.

Heritage group and self-reported language proficiency

First, we found that greater reliance on reading for HL acquisition and exposure predicted higher self-reported HL receptive proficiency among non-Spanish, but not Spanish bilinguals. One probable explanation for this finding is that the two groups differed in how much they could rely on English reading skills to support literacy in the HL. Unlike Spanish-English bilinguals, the non-Spanish bilinguals' heritage languages (Chinese, Hebrew, Korean, Russian, Tamil, and Thai) all utilized a different script from English, which may have reduced the amount of cross-linguistic transfer in literacy (Huang and Hanley, 1995; Durgunoglu, 2002; Lindsey et al., 2003; Bialystok et al., 2005a,b; Luk, 2005) and other academic skills (Van der Slik, 2010; Zhang, 2013; Kostelecká et al., 2015; Siu and Ho, 2015; see Koda, 2005; Genesee et al., 2006 for reviews). For instance, Bialystok et al. (2005a) observed that same-script bilinguals transferred literacy skills across languages, while different-script bilinguals did not. Because Spanish and English utilize the same script, the ability to comprehend written text in Spanish may be supported by English reading skills even without extensive exposure to Spanish text. In contrast, for different-script bilinguals, the ability to comprehend written text in the heritage language may be more contingent on dedicated exposure to HL text through reading. Consistent with this interpretation, non-Spanish bilinguals with minimal HL reading experience had significantly lower reading proficiency than matched Spanish bilinguals (-1 SD; Ms = 4.33 and 7.30, respectively; p < 0.001). This gap closed among non-Spanish and Spanish bilinguals with greater HL reading experience (+1 SD; Ms = 8.67 and 7.80, respectively; p = 0.070). This finding suggests that HL experience through reading may be particularly important for different-script bilinguals.

We additionally found that earlier ages of English acquisition and a more substantial role of family for English acquisition and use predicted lower self-reported HL proficiency among non-Spanish, but not Spanish bilinguals. Because bilinguals need to split their time between their two languages, time spent using one language leads to decreased use of the other language (Meir and Janssen, 2021). HL proficiency often declines with greater majority language use and less HL use (Jia and Aaronson, 2003; Gollan et al., 2015; Montrul, 2016; Vorobyeva and Bel, 2021). Our findings suggest that the negative impact of reduced HL use on HL proficiency may be minimized for speakers of more typologically similar languages, potentially because reading and conversational skills acquired from the majority language can transfer to the HL. In addition to orthographic similarities, the degree of lexical and grammatical overlap between English and Spanish (two Indo-European languages) is likely greater than between English and non-Spanish languages (primarily non-Indo-European) included in the present study. Consequently, even if time spent using English detracts from time spent using the HL, Spanish-English bilinguals may be better able to benefit from positive linguistic transfer between languages (Odlin, 1989; Bialystok et al., 2003, 2005a; Melby-Lervåg and Lervåg, 2011).

Due to the high number of Spanish speakers in the United States, Spanish-English bilinguals may also be able to benefit from greater HL experience outside of the home even if English is used more frequently with family. Indeed, Spanish bilinguals in the present study did report significantly greater HL exposure through music/radio, reading, and individual instruction, as well as numerically greater HL exposure through family and TV relative to non-Spanish bilinguals (see Table 1). Supplementary analyses provide preliminary support for such a compensatory mechanism, as the negative effects of both English AoA and family use on HL proficiency declined with greater overall HL experience (aggregated across contexts of acquisition and exposure; see Supplementary Table 2 for details). Together, these findings suggest that the extent to which majority language experience helps vs. hinders HL acquisition and maintenance is subject to variability in linguistic similarity across languages, as well as the amount of HL use across different contexts.

Heritage group and cultural identification

Among Spanish bilinguals, identification with English-speaking cultures (e.g., American) increased with lower self-reported HL receptive proficiency, as well as with greater HL experience through reading and family. Prior work has demonstrated a robust relationship between cultural identification and language proficiency, most often showing a positive association between cultural identification and proficiency within a given language (e.g., between HL proficiency and HL ethnic identity; Bankston and Zhou, 1995; Cho, 2000; Pease-Alvarez, 2002; Oh and Fuligni, 2010; Yu, 2015; Arredondo et al., 2016; Schroeder et al., 2017). Our findings indicate that proficiency in one language can be inversely related to cultural identification with the other, and that the relationship between language experience and cultural identification varies across different groups of heritage speakers.

More unexpected was our finding that cultural identification with English increased with higher ratings of Spanish acquisition and exposure through reading and family. Effects of relative proficiency further indicated that while identification with the two languages was comparably high among Spanish bilinguals with substantially greater Spanish (vs. English) family experience, identification with English became progressively lower with more balanced use of the two languages at home. Although speculative, some heritage speakers may develop stronger or weaker identification with each culture to compensate for imbalances in language use and immersion at home. Cheryan and Monin (2005) found that Asian Americans expressed greater American cultural identification when their American identities were threatened. Additionally, while bilinguals primed with a particular language or culture often exhibit culturally-congruent behaviors and judgments (i.e., assimilation), there are also cases in which bilinguals instead respond in culturally-incongruent ways, particularly if they perceive their cultural identities to be threatened or in conflict with one another (Benet-Martínez et al., 2002; Cheng et al., 2006; Zou et al., 2008). The fact that a positive association between English identification and HL experience was found for Spanish bilinguals, but not for the non-Spanish bilinguals may potentially stem from differences in the extent to which the two groups perceive their cultural identities to be compatible vs. in conflict. Indeed, an exploratory examination of the relationship between English and HL identification within the two groups provides tentative support for this interpretation. Specifically, while there was a non-significant negative correlation between English and HL cultural identification among Spanish bilinguals (r = −0.22, p = 0.206), there was a marginal positive correlation between identification with the two languages among non-Spanish bilinguals (r = 0.29, p = 0.059). Similar group differences were observed by Gong (2007) who found that while there was no correlation between ethnic (minority culture) identity and national (majority culture) identity among African Americans, ethnic and national identity were positively correlated among American-born Chinese Americans. A possible avenue for future research may therefore be to examine whether different groups of heritage speakers vary in the perceived compatibility of their two cultures, and whether such differences moderate the impact of language experience on cultural identification within and across languages.

Future research may additionally examine the extent to which the observed effects and predictors of self-reported language proficiency are replicated using objective measures of language ability. The inclusion of objective measures assessing a variety of linguistic domains (e.g., lexicon, syntax, pronunciation) will contribute to determining the generalizability of the present findings and for characterizing the impact of heritage and majority language experience on different aspects of language proficiency. Second, our understanding of systematic variability across different heritage speakers would benefit from the inclusion of a greater number of participants from a more diverse range of language backgrounds. In particular, the roles of script and cross-linguistic transfer could be more fully elucidated through the inclusion of same- and different-script bilinguals within (e.g., German and Italian vs. German and Greek) and across (e.g., German and Vietnamese vs. German and Mandarin) language families. Likewise, interactions between language experience and culture could be examined more fully by crossing linguistic and cultural similarity.

In conclusion, the present findings reveal that the relationships between language experience, self-reported language proficiency, and cultural identification systematically vary as a function of heritage speakers' native languages. We additionally provide preliminary evidence to suggest that such differences may partly stem from variability in the degree of linguistic (e.g., orthographic overlap) and cultural (e.g., cultural compatibility) similarity across languages, as well as in opportunities for HL exposure outside of the home. Together, our results demonstrate the complex interplay between heritage and majority language experience, and highlight the need to consider individual measures within the broader context of bilinguals' linguistic environments and history. Greater sensitivity to the needs and abilities of different types of bilinguals can promote the development of more effective heritage bilingual curricula, and provide a more nuanced understanding of heritage bilinguals' language acquisition and identity.

Data availability statement

The dataset analyzed for this study is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Northwestern University's Institutional Review Board. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SH, AC-F-Y, and VM contributed to the conception of the study, revised the manuscript, and approved the final version. SH and AC-F-Y organized the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SH performed the analyses and visualization. VM supervised the study and provided funding. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported in part by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01HD059858 to VM.

Acknowledgments

We thank current and past members of the Bilingualism and Psycholinguistics Research Group, whose studies were used to identify heritage bilinguals for inclusion in the present dataset.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fcomm.2022.994709/full#supplementary-material

References

Albirini, A. (2014). Toward understanding the variability in the language proficiencies of Arabic heritage speakers. Int. J. Bilingual. 18, 730–765. doi: 10.1177/1367006912472404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armon-Lotem, S., Rose, K., and Altman, C. (2021). The development of English as a heritage language: the role of chronological age and age of onset of bilingualism. First Lang. 41, 67–89. doi: 10.1177/0142723720929810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arredondo, M. M., Rosado, M., and Satterfield, T. (2016). Understanding the impact of heritage language on ethnic identity formation and literacy for U.S. Latino children. J. Cogn. Cult. 16, 245–266. doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au, T. K., and Romo, L. F. (1997). “Does childhood language experience help adult learners?,” in The Cognitive Processing of Chinese and Related Asian Languages, Ed H.-C. Chen (Hong Kong: Chinese University Press), 417–441.

Google Scholar

Bankston, C. L. III., and Zhou, M. (1995). Effects of minority-language literacy on the academic achievement of Vietnamese youths in New Orleans. Sociol. Educ. 68, 1–17. doi: 10.2307/2112760

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartolotti, J., Marian, V., Schroeder, S. R., and Shook, A. (2011). Statistical learning of a Morse code language is improved by bilingualism and inhibitory control. Proc. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 33, 885–890. Available online at: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6rt4964b

Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Summers, C. L., Boerger, K. M., Resendiz, M. D., Greene, K., et al. (2012). The measure matters: language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish-English bilingual children. Bilingualism 15, 616–629. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benet-Martínez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., and Morris, M. W. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 33, 492–516. doi: 10.1177/0022022102033005005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Luk, G., and Kwan, E. (2005a). Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: interactions among languages and writing systems. Sci. Stud. Read. 9, 43–61. doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0901_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., and Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: is there a bilingual advantage? Appl. Psycholinguist. 24, 27–44. doi: 10.1017/S014271640300002X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., and Luk, G. (2005b). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. J. Educ. Psychol. 97, 580–590. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreira, M., and Kagan, O. (2011). The results of the National Heritage Language Survey: implications for teaching, curriculum design, and professional development. Foreign Lang. Ann. 44, 40–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1944-9720.2010.01118.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chabal, S., Hayakawa, S., and Marian, V. (2022). Language is activated by visual input regardless of memory demands or capacity. Cognition 222:104994. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chabal, S., Schroeder, S. R., and Marian, V. (2015). Audio-visual object search is changed by bilingual experience. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 77, 2684–2693. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0973-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P., Bobb, S. C., Hoshino, N., and Marian, V. (2017). Neural signatures of language co-activation and control in bilingual spoken word comprehension. Brain Res. 1665, 50–64. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. H., Zhou, Q., and Uchikoshi, Y. (2018). Heritage language socialization in Chinese American immigrant families: prospective links to children's heritage language proficiency. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 24, 1193–1209. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2018.1547680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. X., Benet-Martínez, V., and Bond, M. (2008). Bicultural identity, bilingualism, and psychological adjustment in multicultural societies: immigration-based and globalization-based acculturation. J. Pers. 76, 803–838. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00505.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, C. Y., Lee, F., and Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). Assimilation and contrast effects in cultural frame switching: bicultural identity integration and valence of cultural cues. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 37, 742–760. doi: 10.1177/0022022106292081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheryan, S., and Monin, B. (2005). Where are you really from?: Asian Americans and identity denial. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 717–730. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, G. (2000). The role of heritage language in social interactions and relationships: reflections from a language minority group. Biling. Res. J. 24, 369–384. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2000.10162773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daskalaki, E., Chondrogianni, V., Blom, E., Argyri, F., and Paradis, J. (2019). Input effects across domains: the case of Greek subjects in child heritage language. Sec. Lang. Res. 35, 421–445. doi: 10.1177/0267658318787231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Leeuw, E., Schmid, M. S., and Mennen, I. (2010). The effects of contact on native language pronunciation in an L2 migrant setting. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 13, 33–40. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durgunoglu, A. Y. (2002). Cross-linguistic transfer in literacy development and implications for language learners. Ann. Dyslexia 52, 189–204. doi: 10.1007/s11881-002-0012-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix-Ortiz, M., Newcomb, M. D., and Myers, H. (1994). A multidimensional measure of cultural identity for Latino and Latina adolescents. Hispanic J. Behav. Sci. 16, 99–115. doi: 10.1177/07399863940162001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., Carlson, M. T., and Marian, V. (2022). First-language influence on second language speech perception depends on task demands. Lang. Speech 65, 28–51. doi: 10.1177/0023830920983368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., and Marian, V. (2016). Phonotactic constraints are activated across languages in bilinguals. Front. Psychol. 7:702. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatbonton, E., and Trofimovich, P. (2008). The ethnic group affiliation and L2 proficiency link: empirical evidence. Lang. Aware 17, 229–248. doi: 10.1080/09658410802146867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, V., and Thomas, E. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 12, 213–237. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909004015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genesee, F., Geva, E., Dressler, C., and Kamil, M. (2006). “Synthesis: cross-linguistic relationships,” in Developing Literacy in Second-Language Learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth, Eds D. August and T. Shanahan (New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 153–174.

Google Scholar

Gharibi, K., and Boers, F. (2017). Influential factors in incomplete acquisition and attrition of young heritage speakers' vocabulary knowledge. Lang. Acquis. 24, 52–69. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2016.1187613

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giguere, D., and Hoff, E. (2020). Home language and societal language skills in second-generation bilingual adults. Int. J. Bilingual. 24, 1071–1087. doi: 10.1177/1367006920932221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollan, T. H., Starr, J., and Ferreira, V. S. (2015). More than use it or lose it: the number-of-speakers effect on heritage language proficiency. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 147–155. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0649-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., and Cera, C. M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: a multi-lingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 15, 594–615. doi: 10.1017/S1366728911000332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, L. (2007). Ethnic identity and identification with the majority group: relations with national identity and self-esteem. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 31, 503–523. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2007.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hakuta, K., and D'Andrea, D. (1992). Some properties of bilingual maintenance and loss in Mexican background high-school students. Appl. Linguist. 13, 72–99. doi: 10.1093/applin/13.1.72

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayakawa, S., Shook, A., and Marian, V. (2020). When it's harder to ignorar than to ignore: evidence of greater attentional capture from a non-dominant language. Int. J. Bilingual. 24, 999–1016. doi: 10.1177/1367006920915277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., and Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. J. Child Lang. 39, 1–27. doi: 10.1017/S0305000910000759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hovsepian, A. (2018). Vocabulary growth in Armenian–English bilingual kindergarteners. First Lang. 38, 47–71. doi: 10.1177/0142723717715985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, H. S., and Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition 54, 73–98. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00641-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hur, E., Lopez Otero, J. C., and Lee, E. (2021). Attitudes and expectations towards heritage language instruction: evidence from Korean and Spanish in the US. Languages 6:14. doi: 10.3390/languages6010014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, G., and Aaronson, D. (2003). A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the United States. Appl. Psycholinguist. 24, 131–161. doi: 10.1017/S0142716403000079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, R., and Paradis, J. (2015). The use of referring expressions in narratives by Mandarin heritage language children and the role of language environment factors in predicting individual differences. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 18, 737–752. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karayayla, T., and Schmid, M. S. (2019). First language attrition as a function of age at onset of bilingualism: first language attainment of Turkish–English bilinguals in the United Kingdom. Lang. Learn. 69, 106–142. doi: 10.1111/lang.12316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koda, K. (2005). “Learning to read across writing systems: transfer, metalinguistic awareness, and second language reading development,” in Second Language Writing Systems, Eds V. Cook and B. Bassetti (Tonawanda, NY: Multilingual Matters), 311–314.

Google Scholar

Kostelecká, Y., Kostelecký, T., Vodičková, K., and Jančarík, A. (2015). Linguistic integration of middle school immigrant children in Czechia. AUC Geogr. 50, 181–192. doi: 10.14712/23361980.2015.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupisch, T., and Rothman, J. (2018). Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. Int. J. Bilingual. 22, 564–582. doi: 10.1177/1367006916654355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenth, R., Singmann, H., Love, J., Buerkner, P., and Herve, M. (2018). Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version. 1:3. Available online at: https://cran.rproject.org/web/packages/emmeans/emmeans.pdf

Google Scholar

Lindsey, K. A., Manis, F. R., and Bailey, C. E. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 482–494. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luk, G. (2005). Exploring the latent factors behind inter-language correlations in reading and phonological awareness [Doctoral dissertation] Retrieval from ProQuest Dissertation (Accession No. 0612993523). University: York University, Toronto, Canada.

Google Scholar

Makarova, V., Terekhova, N., and Mousavi, A. (2019). Children's language exposure and parental language attitudes in Russian-as-a-heritage-language acquisition by bilingual and multilingual children in Canada. Int. J. Bilingual. 23, 457–485. doi: 10.1177/1367006917740058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., and Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 940–967. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marian, V., Hayakawa, S., Lam, T. Q., and Schroeder, S. R. (2018). Language experience changes audiovisual perception. Brain Sci. 8:85. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8050085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marian, V., Hayakawa, S., and Schroeder, S. R. (2021). Memory after visual search: overlapping phonology, shared meaning, and bilingual experience influence what we remember. Brain Lang. 222:105012. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, K. M., and de Leeuw, E. (2022). Prosodic patterns in Sylheti-English bilinguals. Stud. Sec. Lang. Acquisit. 44, 562–579. doi: 10.1017/S027226312100036X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meir, N., and Janssen, B. (2021). Child heritage language development: an interplay between cross-linguistic influence and language-external factors. Front. Psychol. 12:651730. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meir, N., Walters, J., and Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Bi-directional cross-linguistic influence in bilingual Russian-Hebrew children. Linguist. Approach. Bilingual. 7, 514–553. doi: 10.1075/lab.15007.mei

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M., and Lervåg, A. (2011). Cross-linguistic transfer of oral language, decoding, phonological awareness and reading comprehension: a meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. J. Res. Read. 34, 114–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01477.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montrul, S. (2005). Second language acquisition and first language loss in adult early bilinguals: exploring some differences and similarities. Sec. Lang. Res. 21, 199–249. doi: 10.1191/0267658305sr247oa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete Acquisition in Bilingualism: Re-examining the Age Factor. Studies in Bilingualism, Vol. 39. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Montrul, S. (2011). Interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua Spec. Issue Interfaces Lang. Acquisit. 212, 591–604. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montrul, S. (2016). The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Noels, K. A., Pon, G., and Clément, R. (1996). Language, identity, and adjustment: the role of linguistic self-confidence in the acculturation process. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 15, 246–264. doi: 10.1177/0261927X960153003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odlin, T. (1989). Language Transfer, Vol. 27. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Oh, J. S., and Fuligni, A. J. (2010). The role of heritage language development in the ethnic identity and family relationships of adolescents from immigrant backgrounds. Soc. Dev. 19, 202–220. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00530.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otwinowska, A., Meir, N., Ringblom, N., Karpava, S., and La Morgia, F. (2021). Language and literacy transmission in heritage language: evidence from Russian-speaking families in Cyprus, Ireland, Israel and Sweden. J. Multiling. Multicult. Dev. 42, 357–382. doi: 10.1080/01434632.2019.1695807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pease-Alvarez, L. (2002). Moving beyond linear trajectories of language shift and bilingual language socialization. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 24, 114–137. doi: 10.1177/0739986302024002002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phinney, J. S. (1990). Ethnic identity in adolescents and adults: review of research. Psychol. Bull. 108, 499–514.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Phinney, J. S., Horenczyk, G., Liebkind, K., and Vedder, P. (2001). Ethnic identity, immigration, and well-being: an interactional perspective. J. Soc. Issues 57, 493–510. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polinsky, M. (2015). Heritage languages and their speakers: State of the field, challenges, perspectives for future work, and methodologies. Zeitschr. Fremdsprachwissenschaft 26, 7–27. Available online at: http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:37108756

Google Scholar

Polinsky, M., and Kagan, O. (2007). Heritage languages: in the 'wild' and in the classroom. Lang. Linguist. Compass. 1, 368–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00022.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodina, Y., Kupisch, T., Meir, N., Mitrofanova, N., Urek, O., and Westergaard, M. (2020). Internal and external factors in heritage language acquisition: evidence from heritage Russian in Israel, Germany, Norway, Latvia and the United Kingdom. Front. Educ. 5:20. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2020.00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, M. S., and Yilmaz, G. (2018). Predictors of language dominance: an integrated analysis of first language attrition and second language acquisition in late bilinguals. Front. Psychol. 9:1306. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, S. R., Lam, T. Q., and Marian, V. (2017). Linguistic predictors of cultural identification in bilinguals. Appl. Linguist. 38, 463–488. doi: 10.1093/applin/amv049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scontras, G., Fuchs, Z., and Polinsky, M. (2015). Heritage language and linguistic theory. Front. Psychol. 6:1545. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shook, A., and Marian, V. (2016). The influence of native-language tones on lexical access in the second language. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 139, 3102–3109. doi: 10.1121/1.4953692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siu, C. T. S., and Ho, C. S. H. (2015). Cross-language transfer of syntactic skills and reading comprehension among young Cantonese–English bilingual students. Read. Res. Q. 50, 313–336. doi: 10.1002/rrq.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoehr, A., Benders, T., Van Hell, J. G., and Fikkert, P. (2017). Second language attainment and first language attrition: the case of VOT in immersed Dutch–German late bilinguals. Sec. Lang. Res. 33, 483–518. doi: 10.1177/0267658317704261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tao, L., Cai, Q., and Gollan, T. H. (2021). Effects of cumulative language exposure on heritage and majority language skills: Spanish and Mandarin heritage speakers in the USA. Linguist. Approach. Bilingual. 11, 168–191. doi: 10.1075/lab.18044.tao

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Economist (2017). Latinos Have Become Chicago's Second-Largest Ethnic Group. Available online at: https://www.economist.com/democracy-in-america/2017/10/05/latinos-have-become-chicagos-second-largest-ethnic-group (accessed June 23, 2022).

Google Scholar

Thomas, E. M., Williams, N., Jones, L. A., Davies, S., and Binks, H. (2014). Acquiring complex structures under minority language conditions: bilingual acquisition of plural morphology in Welsh. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 17, 478–494. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torregrossa, J., Flores, C., and Rinke, E. (2022). What modulates the acquisition of difficult structures in a heritage language? A study on Portuguese in contact with French, German and Italian. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1–14. doi: 10.1017/S1366728922000438

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

U.S. Census Bureau (2020). Language Spoken at Home (S1601). Available online at: https://data.census.gov/cedsci/table?q=languageandtid=ACSST5Y2020.S1601

Google Scholar

Unsworth, S. (2016). “Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development,” in Lifespan Perspectives on Bilingualism, Eds E. Nicoladis and S. Montanari (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, APA), 136–196.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Valdés, G. (2000). “The teaching of heritage languages: An introduction for Slavic-teaching professionals,” in The Learning and Teaching of Slavic Languages and Cultures, Eds O. Kagan and B. Rifkin (Bloomington, IN: Slavica), 375–403.

Google Scholar

Van der Slik, F. W. (2010). Acquisition of Dutch as a second language: the explanative power of cognate and genetic linguistic distance measures for 11 West European first languages. Stud. Sec. Lang. Acquisit. 32, 401–432. doi: 10.1017/S0272263110000021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vorobyeva, T., and Bel, A. (2021). Factors affecting language proficiency in heritage language: the case of young Russian heritage speakers in Spain. J. Lang. Contact. 14, 304–330. doi: 10.1163/19552629-14020003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, S.-C. (2015). The relationships among heritage language proficiency, ethnic identity, and self-esteem. Forum Int. Res. Educ. 2, 57–71. doi: 10.18275/fire201502021039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeigler, K., and Camarota, S. A. (2019). 67.3 Million in the United States Spoke a Foreign Language at Home in 2018. Center for Immigration Studies. Available online at: https://cis.org/sites/default/files/2019-10/camarota-language-19_0.pdf

Google Scholar

Zhang, D. (2013). Linguistic distance effect on cross-linguistic transfer of morphological awareness. Appl. Psycholinguist. 34, 917–942. doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, X., Morris, M. W., and Benet-Martínez, V. (2008). Identity motives and cultural priming: cultural (dis)identification in assimilative and contrastive responses. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1151–1159. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: vocabulary knowledge, cultural identification, proficiency, native language, heritage speakers, bilingualism

Citation: Hayakawa S, Chung-Fat-Yim A and Marian V (2022) Predictors of language proficiency and cultural identification in heritage bilinguals. Front. Commun. 7:994709. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2022.994709

Received: 15 July 2022; Accepted: 11 August 2022;
Published: 31 August 2022.

Edited by:

Sergio Miguel Pereira Soares, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Cristina Maria Flores, University of Minho, Portugal
Katrin Monika Schmitz, University of Wuppertal, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Hayakawa, Chung-Fat-Yim and Marian. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sayuri Hayakawa, sayuri.hayakawa@northwestern.edu

ORCID: Sayuri Hayakawa orcid.org/0000-0001-9863-1406
Ashley Chung-Fat-Yim orcid.org/0000-0002-6905-8302
Viorica Marian orcid.org/0000-0002-8335-1433

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.