Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Commun., 14 April 2022
Sec. Culture and Communication
This article is part of the Research Topic Examining Bias-Based Cyberaggression and Cybervictimization from a Cross-Cultural Perspective View all 5 articles

Cyber-Bystander Behavior Among Canadian and Iranian Youth: The Influence of Bystander Type and Relationship to the Perpetrator on Moral Responsibility

\nPooja Megha Nagar
Pooja Megha Nagar*Karissa LeducKarissa LeducNadia KhaliliNadia KhaliliVictoria TalwarVictoria Talwar
  • The Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada

The current study examines how social determinants influence the way youth from Canadian and Iranian contexts evaluate and morally disengage as bystanders of cyberbullying. While Iranian culture differs from other individualistic and collectivist cultures, Iranian youth have become just as technologically acculturated as their global peers. Despite this, less is understood about how Iranian youth respond to cyberbullying in comparison to youth from individualistic societies. Participants from Canada (N = 60) and Iran (N = 59) who were between the ages of 8-to-15 years old (N = 119, M = 11.33 years, SD = 1.63 years) read 6 cyberbullying scenarios that varied according to Bystander Relationship to Perpetrator (Acquaintance or Friend) and Bystander Response (Assists Cyberbully, Does Nothing, Defends Victim). After reading each scenario, participants were asked to evaluate the bystander's behavior. They were also asked how they would feel if they were the bystander. Similar to past research, these responses were coded on a continuous scale ranging from morally disengaged to morally responsible. Overall, Canadians were more critical of passive bystander behaviors and more supportive toward defending behaviors compared to Iranians. Iranians were more supportive of the behaviors of bystanders who were friends of perpetrators than Canadians were, and Iranians were more critical toward acquaintances of perpetrators. Significant interactions were also found between participants' country of origin, the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator and the bystander's behavior. Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of differentiating between negative judgments and moral attributions of bystander responses.

Introduction

Cyberbullying is an issue that is affecting teens world-wide. Similar to traditional bullying, cyberbullying can be characterized by a willful intent to harm another (Patchin and Hinduja, 2006; Nocentini et al., 2010; Hutson, 2016). However, cyberbullying is distinct from traditional bullying because hateful content can be created and shared anonymously across different online contexts (Menesini et al., 2011; Bryce and Fraser, 2013; Sticca and Perren, 2013; Hutson, 2016; Leduc et al., 2022). Thus, the current generation of youth are continuously exposed to cyberbullying, and they are becoming increasingly desensitized to it (Pabian et al., 2016).

Desensitization to online bullying is problematic given the key role that bystanders play in reducing the severity of bullying (e.g., Salmivalli, 2010; DeSmet et al., 2019; Ng et al., 2021). Positive bystander action is a prosocial construct that involves defending the victim (Pronk et al., 2019) while simultaneously diminishing the perpetrators' power (Salmivalli, 2010; Anderson et al., 2014). Nonetheless, past research (Quirk and Campbell, 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2017; Song and Oh, 2018) has demonstrated that bystanders in both elementary school and high school stand by and remain passive when they witness cyberbullying, which highlights the prominence of bystander inaction across childhood and adolescence. Additionally, social media engagement is becoming increasingly popular with younger children (Gomez-Garibello et al., 2012; Talwar et al., 2014; Conway et al., 2016). As such, the current study aims to examine the factors that facilitate positive bystander action in these age groups.

Since cyberbullying is a global phenomenon that can occur cross-culturally, it is also important to understand how positive bystander behavior can be facilitated across diverse cultural contexts. Nonetheless, most research that has investigated the determinants of positive bystander action has been limited to Western cultures (e.g., DeSmet et al., 2014; Machackova and Pfetsch, 2016; Domínguez-Hernández et al., 2018), and less is known about how these determinants vary in Eastern contexts, particularly in the Middle East. For instance, the Middle East holds values that are more collectivist (e.g., focusing on the collective good) than individualistic (e.g., focusing on individual well-being; Buda and Elsayed-Elkhouly, 1998; Minkov et al., 2017). Previous research suggests that collectivist societies who are more likely to conform with group norms are less likely to defend a single victim (Kogut et al., 2015; Liu and Tung, 2018). To illustrate, Liu and Tung (2018) found that Taiwanese junior high students with high levels of conformity were less likely to defend cyberbullying victims. The researchers also found that those who had positive peer relationships were just as likely to defend as they were to stay quiet about the cyberbullying. Thus, in collectivist societies, inaction may be perceived as an attempt to maintain group harmony (Huang and Chou, 2010), which may allow one to morally disengage (Roccas et al., 2006). However, it remains unclear whether the same pattern of results is found among Middle Eastern youth.

Other research suggests that the computer-mediated world is known to be a distinct community within itself, and the technological affordances of social media have resulted in disinhibited, individualistic behavior among Middle Eastern users (Ghanem et al., 2013). Nonetheless, it remains unclear how youth from collectivist societies perceive theory-driven determinants of positive cyber-bystander behavior. As such, the current study aims to better understand the dynamic relationship between the determinants of bystander action by investigating how behavioral determinants (e.g., the relationship between bystander and perpetrator) affects the moral disengagement process for cyber-bystander behavior among individuals from both Canadian and Iranian contexts.

Theories of Bystander Intervention

While bystanders can take on several roles in the online world, there is a consensus that bystanders can be categorized into at least 3 distinct roles: outsiders (e.g., doing nothing), assistants (e.g., joining in on the cyberbullying), and defenders (e.g., actively intervening; Salmivalli, 1999; Quirk and Campbell, 2015; Song and Oh, 2018; Pepler et al., 2021). Nonetheless, research continues to demonstrate that most cyber-bystanders maintain their role as outsiders and remain inactive in intervening (Quirk and Campbell, 2015; Allison and Bussey, 2016; DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2017; Song and Oh, 2018).

To better understand the factors that facilitate positive bystander intervention in cyberbullying, DeSmet et al. (2018) merged behavior change theories (e.g., the Theory of Reasoned Action and the Theory of Planned Behavior) with the Social Cognitive Theory to examine how cyber-bystander behavior changes across moral, social, behavioral, and cognitive domains. This multi-faceted analysis of bystander intervention has allowed us to better understand the complex interplay between the various determinants of bystander intervention. The behavior change theories highlight how background variables (e.g., past behavior, stereotypes, stigma, personality, culture) and environmental factors influence attitudes, beliefs, behavioral intentions, and actual behavior (DeSmet et al., 2018). The Social Cognitive Theory adds the influence of moral disengagement, which is a set of psychological mechanisms that are used to justify immoral behavior to avoid negative emotions and social disapproval (Bandura, 1998; Bandura et al., 2002). The cognitive mechanisms associated with moral disengagement involve re-framing negative behavior in a positive way, minimizing one's responsibility, distorting consequences, and blaming the victim (Bandura et al., 2002; Gini et al., 2020). These mechanisms overlap with the mechanisms that are associated with passive bystander behavior, such as diffusion of responsibility, distortion of consequences, and attribution of blame (Lo Cricchio et al., 2021). As such, the role of moral reasoning in cyber-bystander intervention has received increased attention.

Moral Reasoning in Cyber-Bystander Behavior

Several studies have investigated the relationship between being a cyber-bystander and the cognitive mechanisms of moral disengagement (Bussey et al., 2015, 2020; Allison and Bussey, 2016; Conway et al., 2016; DeSmet et al., 2016; Leduc et al., 2018; Song and Oh, 2018; Luo and Bussey, 2019). These studies found that low levels of moral disengagement (i.e., high moral responsibility) were positively associated with cyber-bystander defending behavior, and this was particularly true when youth also had: high self-efficacy for defending (Bussey et al., 2015, 2020), high individual morality (Allison and Bussey, 2016), awareness of contextual cues (Luo and Bussey, 2019), lower anti-social conformity, and a bad relationship with the bully (Song and Oh, 2018). These findings highlight the additive influence of the various determinants of cyber-bystander behavior in combination with low moral disengagement in facilitating positive bystander behavior.

In addition to cognitive mechanisms, moral reasoning can also involve emotional processes (Decety et al., 2012; Malti and Ongley, 2014; Conway et al., 2016). Morally responsible emotions, such as guilt (i.e., self-attributions to one's behavior) and shame (self-attributions to one's self; Eisenberg, 2000), can cause individuals to experience negative self-evaluations to the point that they feel a sense of moral responsibility toward another, which then predicts engagement in prosocial behavior (Eisenberg et al., 2010). In the context of traditional bullying, youth who score higher in morally responsible emotions are more likely to defend bullied victims (e.g., Gini et al., 2007; Caravita et al., 2009; Barchia and Bussey, 2011). In contrast, morally disengaged emotions, such as hubristic pride (i.e., self-attributions to one's ability) and authentic pride (i.e., self-attributions to one's effort; Ttofi and Farrington, 2008), are negatively associated with defending behavior among bystanders (e.g., Menesini et al., 2015; Thornberg et al., 2015; Romera et al., 2019). However, few studies have examined moral emotions in relation to cyber-bystander behavior. The technological features of online contexts minimize context-specific emotional cues (e.g., text-based communication, anonymous users), which can distort one's interpretation of cyber-aggression and change the moral emotions that are activated (Pornari and Wood, 2010; Runions and Bak, 2015).

Despite these differences between traditional and online bullying, only a handful of studies have examined moral emotions in relation to cyber-bystander behavior. Overall, these studies found that youth experienced morally responsible emotions when adopting the perpetrator's perspective, but morally disengaged emotions when considering the perspective of a bystander (Shultz et al., 2014; Conway et al., 2016; Leduc et al., 2018; Shohoudi Mojdehi et al., 2019; Tong and Talwar, 2020). While some researchers have found developmental trends for the moral attributions of cyber-bystander behavior (e.g., Conway et al., 2016; Leduc et al., 2018), Tong and Talwar (2020) found that age does not predict moral judgments of cyber-bystander behavior (Tong and Talwar, 2020), and concluded that bystander behavior may be better explained by factors other than age.

However, fewer studies have examined how moral emotions change in relation to the other determinants of cyber-bystander behavior. To illustrate, Pabian et al. (2016) found that continued exposure to cyberbullying lowers levels of morally responsible emotions, for bystanders. However, Shultz et al. (2014) found that continued exposure did not affect cyber-bystander responses if the bystander socially identified with the victim, which resulted in increased levels of empathy. In contrast, Tong and Talwar (2020) found that youth who had been bully-victims in the past experienced greater morally disengaged emotions when taking on the perspective of the bystander in comparison to victims, which suggests that bully-victims can identify with both parties and feel positively that they are in a position of power as they are not the ones being victimized. Taken together, these studies suggest that group membership plays a role in the moral emotions experienced by cyber-bystanders. However, more research is needed to better understand the role of in-group membership in the emotional processes that influence moral engagement with cyber-bystander behavior.

The Role of In-group Membership in Cyber-Bystander Behavior

Relational Processes

A bystanders' willingness to intervene in cyberbullying can be influenced by their relationship with those involved (Jones et al., 2011; Desmet et al., 2012; DeSmet et al., 2014, 2016; Machackova et al., 2013; Bastiaensens et al., 2014; Price et al., 2014; Coyne et al., 2019). When an individual has a relationship with another person, they are considered as being part of the same “in-group”. Typically, individuals who share group membership tend to converge on their values, attitudes, beliefs, and even personality variables. Adhering to group norms and maintaining close in-group relations is especially critical in adolescence, as peer acceptance is a significant indicator of healthy development and overall well-being (Bukowski et al., 1993).

In-group relations may also influence teenagers' perceptions of cyberbullying and determine their cyber-bystander behavior (Jones et al., 2011). Teen bystanders are more likely to join in on the cyberbullying when other bystanders were their friends (Bastiaensens et al., 2014; Coyne et al., 2019). Similarly, DeSmet et al. (2014) found that 12- to-16-year-old participants expected bystanders who were both a friend of the victim and high in popularity to take on a defending role. To our knowledge, only Machackova et al. (2013) has looked at how the bystander's relationship to the perpetrator influences defending behavior among adolescents. Specifically, they found that bystanders who were friends with the bully were less supportive of victims, regardless of their emotional response to the cyberbullying (Machackova et al., 2013). However, it remains unclear how one's relationship to the perpetrator affects the mechanisms of moral disengagement. The present study will further examine this relationship in a cultural context.

Cultural Context

The research examining the relationship with involved others provides insight into how the norms of a specific sub-culture (i.e., the peer-group) influence cyber-bystander behavior. However, online behavior is influenced by several sub-cultures, many of which have conflicting norms (Brody, 2021). One sub-culture that is often neglected in the literature is ethnicity, with only a few studies considering the norms and values of specific ethnic groups. There has been some investigation of cyber-bystander behavior in East Asian and South Asian cultural contexts (Huang and Chou, 2010; Park et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2014; Bhat et al., 2017; Ma and Chen, 2019; Leung, 2021), South American cultural contexts (Ferreira et al., 2016; Souza et al., 2018), European cultural contexts (DeSmet et al., 2014, 2016, 2018; Bastiaensens et al., 2016; Machackova and Pfetsch, 2016; Piccoli et al., 2020; Thornberg et al., 2021), and North American cultural context (e.g., Taylor et al., 2019; Pepler et al., 2021). This research demonstrates the differences in norm expectancies of bystander inaction across cultures. For example, Ma and Chen (2019) found that “doing nothing” (i.e., being an outsider as a bystander) aligned with the perceived collectivistic values of Asian cultures; whereas, bystander intervention in individualistic contexts relies on personal factors, such as individual attitudes and social factors, such as one's relationship to the victim (e.g., Bastiaensens et al., 2014). Only a handful of studies have conducted cross-cultural analyses (Lapidot-Lefler and Barak, 2012; Ma and Bellmore, 2016; Wright et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2017; Wachs et al., 2019; Cook et al., 2021); however, none were in relation to how the socio-cultural context (e.g., culture and relationship between bystander and perpetrator) specifically influences the processes involved with moral disengagement.

Cyber-Bystander Behavior in Middle Eastern Cultures

Middle Eastern cultures have been neglected in these investigations, which is problematic given the increased usage of technology in Middle Eastern countries (Shadmanfaat et al., 2019). In contrast to communist countries in East Asia, such as China, Middle Eastern countries' political and cultural construction have been influenced by Islam (Rabiei, 2013; Rezapour et al., 2019). Language diversities and political conflicts in the Middle East also have shaped socio-cultural differences within this region (Rabiei, 2013). Therefore, collectivistic roots and values in the Middle East can be different from other parts of Asia.

Only a handful of studies have examined cyber-bystander behavior in Middle Eastern communities. For instance found, Lapidot-Lefler and Barak (2012) that adolescent girls who were Israeli-Arab were more likely to bully others online and less likely to intervene as a bystander than adolescent girls who were Israeli-Jewish. The authors attributed this finding to the lack of supervision and control in online contexts, which allows Israeli-Arab girls to remove themselves from their collectivist identity and exercise their freedom of speech. Similarily, Shohoudi Mojdehi et al. (2019) found that Iranian youth attributed less morally responsible emotions (shame and guilt) in comparison to Canadian and Chinese youth. These findings highlight how Iranian youths' persectives may be shaped by different cultural influences than other Eastern cultures like China. However, more research is required to fully understand how the determinants of cyber-bystander behavior dynamically interact to influence the moral attributions assigned by Middle Eastern Youth.

Since Middle Eastern communities are high in collectivism, they tend to be emotionally interdependent and prioritize the welfare of the community, societal structure, and power differences (Schwartz, 1994). Many Middle Eastern countries overlap in collectivist values such as: (a) their high-power dynamics where one does not question authority figures (Samovar et al., 1981; Singhapakdi et al., 1999); (b) their emphasis on tradition and preservation of the past (Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck, 1961); (c) and their conformity to cultural norms and Islamic law (e.g., well-defined societal roles and group harmony; Ghanem et al., 2013). Previous research (Nesdale and Naito, 2005) has found that similar collectivist values have contributed to less bystander action in traditional bullying, especially when the bystander had a relationship with the perpetrator.

However, as technology evolves, these cultural characterizations are less pronounced in computer-mediated environments. The low-context communication style of online environments (e.g., instant messaging, clicking like) are a mismatch with high-context, collectivist cultures, which typically rely on non-verbal communication and contextual cues such as background variables, status, and hierarchy (Hall, 1989; Croucher et al., 2012). With the rise of the technological era, youth from high-context collectivist cultures are now engaging online in more depersonalized and uninhibited ways (Ghanem et al., 2013). Accordingly, that there has been a shift in communication style among online communities in the Middle East (Ghanem et al., 2013). Accordingly, researchers have been challenged with the task of re-examining how culture plays a role in online contexts, especially in relation to other contextual cues, such as one's relationship to those involved.

While there are several Middle Eastern communities, the current study will focus on Iranian culture. As one of the world's oldest civilizations in this area (Barrington, 2012), Iran has several religions, languages, and revolutions in its history (Abrahamian, 2021). Thus, research from this area has a specific context (secular-religious) and could add new pieces of literature to cross-cultural studies. In addition, both cyberbullying rates and passive bystander behavior are steadily increasing in Iran (Razjouyan et al., 2018). Understanding how one's context influences perceptions of bystander intervention can ensure that cyberbullying intervention programs are both culturally sensitive and inclusive.

Current Study

The current study examined how Iranian youth evaluate cyber-bystander behavior in comparison to Canadian youth. This study also investigated how the interaction between background variables (e.g., cultural group) and contextual variables (e.g., relationship to the perpetrator) influences the moral disengagement process in cyber-bystander behavior. Consistent with previous research (Salmivalli, 1999; Quirk and Campbell, 2015; Song and Oh, 2018; Pepler et al., 2021), the types of bystander action that are presented in this study will be: (a) positively defending the victim; (b) assisting the cyberbully; and (c) being an outsider (i.e., doing nothing). A 3 (Bystander Type: defend, assist, outsider) x 2 (Relationship: acquaintance, friend) within subjects design was used among youth in both Canadian and Iranian cultures. The current study controlled for previous experiences of victimization and aggression because of previous research (e.g., Cao and Lin, 2015) that suggests previous experiences are a determinant of bystander behavior.

Hypotheses were made for moral evaluations (hypotheses 1a, 1b, and 1c) and the moral disengagement process (hypotheses 2a, 2b, and 2c) in Canadian and Iranian youth according to the three types of bystander action (i.e., assistants, outsiders, defenders; e.g., Pepler et al., 2021), and the relationship of the bystander to the perpetrator of cyberbullying (i.e., acquaintance, friend).

First, based on previous research (Shohoudi Mojdehi et al., 2019), we hypothesized that both Canadians and Iranians will morally evaluate assisting behavior as negative, regardless of whether the bystander is a friend or acquaintance to the perpetrator (hypothesis 1a). Based on Shohoudi Mojdehi et al. (2019), we also predict that Canadians will evaluate outsider behavior more negatively than Iranians, regardless of their relationship to the perpetrator (hypothesis 1b). Finally, we predict that both cultural groups will evaluate defending behavior positively when the bystander is friends with the perpetrator, since both cultures strive toward reducing in-group conflict (e.g., Nesdale and Naito, 2005; Ghanem et al., 2013; Machackova et al., 2013; Bastiaensens et al., 2014; hypothesis 1c).

With regard to moral disengagement, we predict that Canadians will feel a stronger sense of moral responsibility for assisting behavior, as previous research suggests Canadians tend to be more confrontational than Iranians (Hall, 1989; Ma and Bellmore, 2016; Hamelin et al., 2018; hypothesis 2a). Moreover, since both Eastern and Western cultures have shown to be morally disengaged in response to outsider behavior (e.g., see Zhao and Yu, 2021 for a review), we expect less moral responsibility for outsider behavior among both cultural groups (hypothesis 2b). Finally, we expect Iranians to be morally disengaged toward defending behavior when the bystander is an acquaintance to the perpetrator since collectivist cultures tend to be conflict avoiding (e.g., Huang and Chou, 2010; Ghanem et al., 2013; Ma and Chen, 2019; hypothesis 2c).

Method

Participants

One hundred and nineteen participants from Canada (N = 60) and Iran (N = 59) completed the study. Participants were between the ages of 8-to-15 years old (M = 11.33 years, SD = 1.63 years; 63.3% female). Participants were matched for age and gender across country of origin. For both Iranian and Canadian participants, 63.3% of the sample was female.

Selection of participants was done through convenience sampling. In Iran, participants were recruited from schools in [city name removed for confidentiality purposes]. In Canada, participants were recruited from university research recruitment database of families with children and adolescents in the [city name removed for confidentiality purposes] area of Quebec, Canada.

Materials

Vignettes

Participants read a total of six gender-matched vignettes (Appendix) depicting cyberbullying scenarios that involved a perpetrator, a victim and a bystander. Scenarios varied according to two independent variables. First, the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator was manipulated as either being acquaintances (3 scenarios) or friends (3 scenarios). Second, the bystander's response to the cyberbullying situation varied according to whether they assisted the cyberbully (2 scenarios), acted as an outsider (e.g., doing nothing) in response to the cyberbullying (2 scenarios), or defended the victim (2 scenarios). As in previous cyberbullying research (e.g., Conway et al., 2016; Leduc et al., 2018), vignette methodology was chosen to allow better control and consistency over the independent variables and scenarios that participants are asked to evaluate (Aguinis and Bradley, 2014).

Evaluations of Bystander Behaviors

After reading each scenario, participants responded to the question “What do you think about [bystander's] behavior?” They answered on a scale from −2 (very bad) to 2 (very good) to indicate how negatively or positively they perceived the bystander's behavior in the vignette.

Moral Emotions Attributed to Bystander Behaviors

Participants also responded to the question “How would you feel if you were [bystander]?” Participants answered on an ordinal scale of 0 (bad), 1 (neither good or bad) to 2 (good). Similar to past research (e.g., Menesini et al., 2015; Conway et al., 2016), participants were provided with example emotions for feeling “bad” of feeling sad, angry, or guilty, and for feeling “good” as feeling happy, or proud. For vignettes in which bystanders who assisted the cyberbully or who were passive (i.e., did not get involved), feeling bad was coded as morally responsible and feeling good was coded as morally disengaged. Attributed emotions were coded on the same ordinal scale of 0 (morally disengaged) to 2 (morally responsible), with 1 (feeling neither good or bad) indicating moral indifference or neutrality. Coding was reversed for vignettes where the bystander engaged in positive bystander behavior (i.e., supported the victim).

Cyber-Aggression and Cyber-Victimization Scale

This scale was used to measure participants previous experiences as perpetrators of cyberbullying (Cyber-Aggression subscale) and victims of cyberbullying (Cyber-Victimization subscale). Each subscale included 9 items for which participants were asked to reflect on their experiences in the last 2 months. Example items include “I sent insults on social media” (Cyber-Aggression) and “I received rude comments on a website” (Cyber-Victimization). Responses were scored on a scale of 0 (never) to 4 (several times a week) for a possible total score of 36 per subscale. All values were anchored, and the subscale showed good reliability (α = 0.80).

Procedure

This study was approved by [name removed for confidentiality purposes] University's Research Ethics Board (Tier III). The study was conducted in-person. After their parents provided informed consent, participants provided assent prior to completing the Cyber-Victimization and –Aggression questionnaire, and the vignettes. The order of measures and vignettes was completely counterbalanced across subjects, and the questions asked for the vignettes followed the fix order presented above.

Results

Country differences in moral evaluations of bystander responses were examined through a repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), while controlling for previous experiences of cyber-aggression and cyber-victimization. Friedman's test was used to examine participants' attributions of moral responsibility and disengagement as a function of their country of origin (Canada or Iran). Friedman's test is a non-parametric alternative to a repeated-measures ANOVA that was chosen because our dependant variable (attributions of moral responsibility and moral disengagement) is measured on an ordinal scale (Gignac, 2019). Both analyses are described in detail below. Descriptive statistics for moral evaluations and moral disengagement scores are in Table 1. Since developmental differences were not significant, the subsequent analyses controlled for age.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means (SDs) for moral evaluation and moral disengagement scores according to country of origin, bystander response and bystander's relationship to the perpetrator.

The results are presented according to the hypotheses presented above. First, findings from the repeated measures ANCOVA for moral evaluations (hypotheses 1a, 1b, and 1c) of different types of cyber-bystander action according to country of origin and the bystander's relationship to the perpetrator are presented. Second, findings from Friedman's non-parametric repeated measures test for participants' attributions of moral responsiblity and disengagement (hypotheses 2a, 2b, and 2c) toward different types of bystanders according to country of origin and the bystander's relationship to the perpetrator are presented.

Moral Evaluations of Bystander Behaviors

A three-way mixed repeated measures ANCOVA examined participants' moral evaluations of bystander behaviors in cyberbullying (dependent variable) with bystander response (assist, defend, be an outsider) and the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator (friend, acquaintances) as within-subject variables (independent variables), and the participants' country of origin (Canada or Iran) as a between-subject variable (independent variable). To control for the possible bias of participants' lived experiences, past involvement in cyberbullying as victims and/or perpetrators was entered as a covariate. Mauchly's test of sphericity, χ(2)2 = 8.85, p = 0.01 was violated and the more conservative Greenhouse-Geisser was used when assessing tests of within-subject effects. All other statistical assumptions were met, including normality and Levene's test for equality of variances (p < 0.05). Bonferroni corrections were applied to alpha levels for all follow-up tests.

There was a statistically significant three-way interaction between the bystander's response, the bystander's relationship to the perpetrator and country of origin on participants' moral evaluations of bystander behaviors in the scenarios, F(1.86, 206.07) = 3.41, p = 0.04, partial eta2 = 0.03, regardless of participants' previous experiences with cyber-aggression and cyber-victimization. There were no statistically significant differences between Iranians and Canadians for bystanders that assisted the perpetrator, regardless of their relationship. In support of hypothesis 1a, they were both more negative in their evaluations. Moreover, hypothesis 1b was partially supported. Canadians evaluated the bystander's behavior more negatively than Iranians in their evaluations of bystanders who remained passive in response to the cyberbullying situation (i.e., they were an outsider), p < 0.01, but this was when the bystander was a friend to the perpetrator (M = −0.90; SD = 0.82). Conversely, Iranians were more neutral or indifferent in their evaluations of the same bystanders (M = −0.07; SD = 1.30). Finally, with regard to bystanders who defended targets of cyberbullying, hypothesis 1c was also partially supported. The relationship with the perpetrator did not have a significant effect on the evaluations of Canadians, p = 0.132, but Iranians provided more positive moral evaluations, p < 0.01, when the bystander was friends with the perpetrator (M = 1.47; SD = 1.02) than when they were acquaintances (M = 0.45; SD = 1.15).

Findings from the repeated measures ANCOVA revealed additional findings. There were statistically significant two-way interactions between the country of origin and the bystander's response, F(1.89, 209.24) = 4.53, p = 0.01, partial eta2 = 0.04. Specifically, Canadians evaluated bystander behaviors more negatively than Iranians when the bystander did nothing in response to the cyberbullying. However, when the bystander defended the victim, Canadians provided more positive evaluations than Iranians, p < 0.01. There was also a two-way interaction between the participants' country of origin and the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator, F(1, 111) = 30.67, p < 0.01, partial eta2 = 0.22. While there were no significant differences in Canadians' moral evaluations, p = 0.132, Iranians gave more positive evaluations to the behaviors of bystanders who were friends with the perpetrator than those that were acquaintances, p < 0.01. Iranians were more negative toward bystanders who were acquaintances to the perpetrator.

In addition, there were significant main effects of the bystander's response, F(1, 209.24) = 294.79, p < 0.01, partial eta2 = 0.73, and of the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator, F(1, 111) = 14.39, p < 0.01, partial eta2 = 0.12, on participants' moral evaluations. For bystander responses, regardless of country of origin and relationship with the perpetrator, participants provided more negative evaluations of bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and those who did nothing and more positive evaluations of bystanders who defended victims of cyberbullying, p < 0.01. With regard to the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator, regardless of the bystander's response to the cyberbullying event and country of origin, participants provided more negative evaluations to acquaintances depicted in the cyberbullying scenarios, than they did to friends, p < 0.01.

No statistically significant interaction between the bystander's response and their relationship with the perpetrator, F(1.86, 206.07) = 2.33, p = 0.10, partial eta2 = 0.02.

Moral Emotions Toward Bystander Behaviors

A series of Friedman's non-parametric repeated measures ANOVA tests were run to examine differences in moral emotion attributions (dependent variable) across bystander responses and the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator (independent variables). Given the nature of our variables, data was split between country of origin (Canada or Iran) (independent variable). For both sets of analyses, Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons, and post-hoc tests were ran when appropriate. Data was also tested for normality and the assumption was met. The following results are presented by country of origin of the sample (Canada and Iran) given that the file was split in this way for analyses. Table 2 summarizes the 9 comparisons per country of origin and median ranks.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of comparisons (and median ranks) of moral emotion attributions to bystander behavior according to country and the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator.

Canadians' Moral Emotion Attributions

When the bystander was an acquaintance of the perpetrator, distributions of moral emotion scores were significantly different between bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and those who did nothing, χ(1)2 = 5.00, p = 0.03. Specifically, in partial support of hypotheses 2a and 2b, Canadians' moral emotions scores ranked higher in moral responsibility for bystanders who assisted the perpetrator (Mdn = 1.54) than those who did nothing (Mdn = 1.46). There were also significant differences when comparing bystanders who did nothing and those who defended victims. Specifically, in partial support of hypotheses 2b and 2c, Canadians' attributed moral emotion scores ranked higher in moral responsibility toward bystanders who defended victims (Mdn = 1.55), than those who did nothing (Mdn = 1.45), χ(1)2 = 6.00, p < 0.01. In additional analyses comparing distributions of moral emotion scores between bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and those who defended the victim, there were no significant differences among Canadians, χ(1)2 = 1.00, p = 0.32.

Regarding the influence of relationship with the perpetrator (hypothesis 2c), when bystanders were friends of the perpetrator, there were no significant differences among Canadians in their moral emotion attributions between bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and those who did nothing, χ(1)2 = 3.00, p = 0.08, those who assisted the perpetrator and those who defended the victim, χ(1)2 = 2.67, p = 0.10, and between those who did nothing and those who defended the victim, χ(1)2 = 0.33, p = 0.56.

In additional analyses comparing distributions of moral responsibility scores across bystander responses (assisting the perpetrator, doing nothing, or defending the victim), and the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator (acquaintance or friend), no significant differences were found for bystanders who assisted the perpetrator among Canadians' moral emotion scores, χ(1)2 = 0.67, p = 0.41, or for bystanders who defended victims, χ(1)2 = 0.33, p = 0.56. However, significant differences were found for bystanders who did nothing. Specifically, Canadians' moral emotion scores ranked higher in moral responsibility when bystanders who were friends with the perpetrator and did nothing (Mdn = 1.55), than when bystanders were acquaintances with the perpetrator and did nothing (Mdn = 1.45), χ(1)2 = 4.50, p = 0.03.

Iranians' Moral Emotion Attributions

For Iranian participants, hypotheses 2a and 2b were not supported. When bystanders were acquaintances of the perpetrator, there were no differences among the moral emotion scores for bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and bystanders who did nothing, χ(1)2 = 0.43, p = 0.51. Moreover, as in the Canadian sample, there were significant differences when comparing bystanders who did nothing and those who defended victims. However, hypothesis 2c was not fully supported for Iranians, χ(1)2 = 4.26, p = 0.04, who also ranked higher moral responsibility scores toward bystanders who defended victims (Mdn = 1.58), than bystanders who did nothing (Mdn = 1.42). In additional analyses, distributions of moral emotion scores between bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and those who defended the victim, were not significant among Iranians, χ(1)2 = 1.92, p = 0.17.

Regarding the influence of relationship with the perpetrator (hypothesis 2c), when bystanders were friends with the perpetrator, significant differences were found between distributions of moral emotion scores toward bystanders who assisted the perpetrator and bystanders who did nothing, χ(1)2 = 6.40, p = 0.01. Iranians' moral emotion scores ranked higher in moral responsibility for bystanders who did nothing (Mdn = 1.58), than for bystanders who assisted the perpetrator (Mdn = 1.42). There were also significant differences between distributions of moral emotion scores toward bystanders who assisted the perpetrator, and those who defended the victim, χ(1)2 = 7.36, p = 0.01. Iranians' moral emotion scores ranked higher in moral responsibility toward bystanders who defended the victim (Mdn = 1.58), than those who assisted the perpetrator (Mdn = 1.42). However, there were no differences between distributions of Iranians' moral emotion scores for bystanders who did nothing and bystanders who defended the victim, χ(1)2 = 0.33, p = 0.56.

Finally, in additional analyses, when comparing distributions of moral responsibility scores across bystander responses (assisting, defending, outsider) and the bystander's relationship with the perpetrator (acquaintance, friend), no significant differences were found for bystanders who assisted the perpetrator among Iranians' moral emotion scores, χ(1)2 = 0.20, p = 0.66. However, as among Canadians, significant differences were found for bystanders who did nothing. Specifically, Iranians' moral emotion scores, χ(1)2 = 13.25, p < 0.01, ranked higher in moral responsibility for bystanders who were friends with the perpetrator and did nothing (Mdn = 1.64), than those who were acquaintances with the perpetrator and did nothing (Mdn = 1.36). Finally, for bystanders who defended victims, while no significant differences were found for Canadians, there were significant differences for Iranians, χ(1)2 = 5.44, p = 0.02. Iranians' moral emotion scores ranked higher in moral responsibility for friends who defended victims (Mdn = 1.56) than acquaintances who defended victims (Mdn = 1.44).

Discussion

The current study aimed to examine cross-cultural differences in the way Canadian and Iranian youth morally evaluate cyber-bystander behavior depending on the bystander's behavior (assist, defend, be an outsider) and the bystander's relationship to the perpetrator (acquaintance, friend). In addition, participants were asked to reflect on the emotions they experienced in relation to the bystander's behavior, and then these emotional attributions were coded as morally engaged, neutral, or morally disengaged.

Moral Evaluations

We predicted that both Canadians and Iranians would negatively evaluate assisting behavior (e.g., helping the cyberbully), regardless of whether the bystander is a friend or acquaintance to the perpetrator (hypothesis 1a). Our findings were consistent with this predicition, as youth in both cultures do not support a peers' decision to join in on the cyberbullying, even if the bystander is friends with the perpetrator. Although past research (Desmet et al., 2012; DeSmet et al., 2014, 2016) has found that adolescents will stick up for a friend even if the circumstances around the interpersonal conflict are unclear, the current findings suggest that friends of the perpetrator may be doing so against their moral convictions. Since this pattern of results was demonstrated cross-culturally, educational bystander intervention programs could standardize a component that is aimed at acknowledging this discrepancy between adolescents' moral beliefs and their behaviors in order to facilitate actions that are consistent with one's beliefs.

Based on previous findings (e.g., Shohoudi Mojdehi et al., 2019), we predicted that Canadians would evaluate outsider behavior (i.e., doing nothing in response to the cyberbullying) more negatively than Iranians, regardless of their relationship to the perpetrator (hypothesis 1b). The results were slightly more nuanced than expected as we found that Canadians evaluated outsider behavior negatively when the bystander was a friend, which is consistent with previous research (DeSmet et al., 2016), and further suggests there are higher expectations from friends to intervene (Price et al., 2014). In contrast, Iranians were more neutral or indifferent toward the same bystanders (friend who acted as an outsider). These findings are consistent with the findings of Huang and Chou (2010), who found a similar effect among Chinese youth, and further supports the idea that bystander inaction may be more socially acceptable in collectivist societies than individualistic societies (Samovar et al., 1981; Singhapakdi et al., 1999; Ghanem et al., 2013). Again, given the collectivistic values of exercising forgiveness and tolerance (Shohoudi Mojdehi et al., 2019), outsider behavior may be another instance of groupthink, as passive responses can be perceived as a way of maintaining group harmony and avoiding further conflict.

We also predicted that both cultural groups would evaluate defending behavior (e.g., helping the victim) positively when the bystander is friends with the perpetrator (hypothesis 1c), since most cultures value the importance of reducing in-group conflict (e.g., Cuhadar and Dayton, 2011). While our prediction was true for Iranians, there was no difference found in evaluations of defending behavior for friends and acquaintances of the bystander among Canadians. These findings may further support other research (e.g., Palomera et al., 2021) that suggests a youth's decision to engage in confrontational cyber-bystander behavior (e.g., defending) is shaped by their social values. Perhaps there was no difference in defending behavior among friends and acquintances for Canadians since individualist cultures value confrontational behavior moreso than Iranians (Hall, 1989; Ma and Bellmore, 2016; Hamelin et al., 2018). Whereas, Iranian youth may only engage in confrontational behavior when it is in the best interest of their in-group.

Moral Emotions

Given the more confrontational nature of Canadians, we predicted that Canadian youth would feel a stronger sense of moral responsibility for assisting behavior, due to urges to intervene, irrespective of the bystander's relationship to the perpetrator (hypothesis 2a). However, our results showed that relationship to the bystander mattered for both Canadians and Iranians. For Canadians, youth experienced increased moral responsibility when the bystander was an acquaintance and assisted the perpetrator compared to when they acted as an outsider. Since North American cultures tend to be “information-based” (Urschler, 2016), it is possible that Canadians remain passive and become morally disengaged in situations where they do not fully understand the context and when they do not have group membership. For this reason, it is possible that Canadians experience cognitive dissonance when they witness a person from an “out-group” join in on a potentially “in-group” conflict, as previous research has found that disagreements within social groups can be a source of cognitive dissonance (Matz and Wood, 2005). Thus, feelings of moral responsibility toward intervening on acquaintances who help perpetrators may reduce this experienced cognitive dissonance.

In contrast, Iranians felt less moral responsibility when the bystander was a friend and assisted in comparison to when a friend acted as both an outsider and defender. Since Iranians value group membership, it is possible they are more tolerant of assisting behavior if it is considered a norm or expectation of the group to maintain group harmony. According to Janis (1991), highly cohesive groups can engage in “groupthink” which is when people strive toward unanimity and solidarity to the point that it encourages self-censorship; groupthink could be an important barrier of bystander intervention in collectivist communities.

We expected cross-cultural similarities when it came to the moral emotions associated with outsider behavior (hypothesis 2b). Since both Eastern and Western cultures have shown to be morally disengaged in response to outsider behavior (Allison and Bussey, 2016; Brody and Vangelisti, 2016; Razjouyan et al., 2018; Lo Cricchio et al., 2021; Shariatpanahi et al., 2021), we expected less moral responsibility for outsider behavior among both cultural groups. The results somewhat support this prediction. Both cultural groups felt less moral responsibility when the bystander was an acquaintance to the perpetrator and did nothing in comparison to when the acquaintance defended the victim. However, both cultural groups felt increased moral responsibility when the bystander was friends with the perpetrator and acted as an outsider. These findings suggest that the peer group as a “sub-culture” may better facilitate youths' moral responsibility to intervene as a cyber-bystander in comparison to the larger cultural context. Consistent with previous research (e.g., Machackova et al., 2013; Shultz et al., 2014; Brody and Vangelisti, 2016; DeSmet et al., 2016), youth in both cultures felt more responsible for the behavior of their peer in-group members than those who were in their peer out-group. Since adolescence is a time where friendships play a key role in psychosocial and behavioral development (Vitaro et al., 2009), as well as in identity formation (Jones et al., 2014), these findings highlight the importance of educational intervention programs that include components that strengthen the behavioral styles and interactions between same-aged peers. While previous researchers have recommended targeting friendship to strengthen ties within the community to reduce vulnerability online (Patterson et al., 2017), our findings add that targeting friendship characteristics can also facilitate a sense of moral responsibility for prosocial bystander intervention.

In addition, even though Iranians attributed lower moral evaluations to outsider behavior among friends of the perpetrator, they still experienced emotions that elicited a sense of moral responsibility. Emotions are typically expected to be controlled in collectivist cultures; however, the experience of strong emotions among collectivist group members can be regarded as an interactive experience that mirrors the social context (Mesquita, 2001). Thus, when the Iranian participants exhibited emotions associated with moral responsibility, it may have provided them with a “situation specific clue” (p.5, Keshtiari and Kuhlmann, 2016) that oriented them to the contextual variable (i.e., relationship with perpetrator), which consequently influenced the way they responded. It is possible that the role of peer relationships as a contextual variable also cues empathy, which is an important determinant of prosocial bystander intervention.

For defending behavior (hypothesis 2c), we accurately predicted that Iranians felt increased moral responsibility for defending behavior when the bystander was a friend to the perpetrator in comparison to when they were acquaintances. Whereas, the same differences were not found among Canadians. Thus, despite the fact that Iranians felt less moral responsibility (and more tolerance) for assisting the cyberbully behavior within one's in-group, they simultaneously feel moral responsibility to intervene as a defender. This pattern of results further suggests that Iranians are willing to compromise group harmony within their in-group if there is a greater purpose, whereas this effect was not found among Canadian youth. Furthermore, the tolerance and forgiveness that is valued by Iranian culture could apply toward both the assistor and the defender, which would allow group members to feel comfortable enough to intervene. In contrast, Canadian youth who take on the perspective of a “friend of the perpetrator” may not agree with their friend's decision to cyberbully (as indicated by their moral evaluation), but they do not feel as morally responsible to address the cyberbullying as Iranians. It is possible that Canadians who are friends of perpetrators morally disengage when the cyberbullying occurs to allow the perpetrator to have autonomy over their choices, which is a value of individualistic cultures.

Limitations and Future Directions

The current study is not without its limitations. First, we did not examine the various cultural constructs that define ethnic groups. Future research could conduct cross-cultural comparisons in relation to cultural constructs that are on an ecological level (e.g., egalitarian commitment vs. conservatism; utilitarian involvement vs. loyal involvement; societal cynicism) and an individual level (e.g., uncertainty avoidance; power distance; time orientation; Matsumoto and Yoo, 2006). By investigating these constructs, we would achieve a richer understanding of the cultural determinants of bystander intervention. Researchers could also investigate how these cultural constructs relate to the mechanisms involved with moral disengagement to understand how the two processes interact to influence cyber-bystander behavior. Furthermore, the current study did not further examine the justifications behind youths' emotional attributions. A qualitative analysis of youths' moral justifications could allow for a better understanding of the social norms around bystander intervention in specific circumstances (e.g., when there is a relationship to those involved). Additionally, the current study relied on youths' evaluations of a hypothetical other; whereas future research could benefit from measuring youth's actual cyber-bystander behavior to these incidents either retrospectively (recording natural behavior online) or spontaneously (in-vivo/experimentally) and if their behavior varies among in-group and out-group members. Future research could also benefit from examining both constructive and aggressive forms of bystander intervention (e.g., Moxey and Bussey, 2020), as well as contradictory bystander roles (e.g., frenemies who change their allegiance according to peer context; Wójcik and Flak, 2021) across a wider range of cultural and gender groups. Finally, the current study was also limited in sample size, and future research could benefit from a sample with more statistical power.

Implications

The current study demonstrates that cultural values can be maintained in online interactions where the involved parties are known by one another. For this reason, it is critical that educational cyberbullying intervention programs that target bystander action are aware of these cross-cultural nuances and introduce strategies that are culturally sensitive. For example, educational curriculum designed as part of bystander intervention programs in collectivist societies could facilitate conversations about groupthink and the discrepancy between moral beliefs and moral actions to bring awareness to obstacles that prevent bystander action. Furthermore, since youth tend to interact with their same-aged classmates on social media, existing bystander intervention programs in educational settings could add content that strengthens the behavioral styles and interactions between same-aged peers in culturally appropriate ways. Many existing intervention programs fail to consider how cultural norms and peer norms influence the targeted outcomes (e.g., prosocial cyber-bystander action), and this study aims to encourage further research in this area.

Conclusion

Overall, the current study provides further insight into how peer-group norms and cultural norms dynamically interact to influence perceptions of cyber-bystander behavior in Middle Eastern and Canadian youth. Iranians morally evaluated assisting behavior similarly to Canadians; however, they felt less moral responsibility when the bystander was a friend to the perpetrator and assisted. On the other hand, Canadians felt moral responsibility when the bystander was an acquaintance and assisted. This pattern of results shows that the two cultural groups have different ways of interpreting negative cyber-bystander behavior, which may be largely influenced by their cultural values (e.g., information-based values in individualistic societies and solidarity-based values in collectivist societies). Furthermore, when the bystander was friends with the perpetrator and then acted as an outsider, Canadians assigned negative moral evaluations; whereas, Iranian youth are indifferent. Despite this difference in moral evaluations, both Canadians and Iranians felt morally responsible for the bystander's behavior, which suggest that the peer group as a “sub-culture” may better facilitate youths' moral responsibility in relation to passive bystander behavior, especially in comparison to the larger cultural context. Furthermore, being part of an “in group” among same-aged peers (e.g., friends with the perpetrator) may matter more for Iranians when it comes to defending behavior, whereas the relationship to the perpetrator mattered less for Canadians in defending behavior. This pattern of results suggests that Iranians may be more willing to compromise group harmony by defending the victim if it is among members of their own in-group. However, they will not facilitate inter-group conflict by defending against an out-group (i.e., acquaintance of the perpetrator). This study has important implications for the construction of culturally-appropriate bystander intervention programs for youth.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by McGill Research Ethics Board-3 (REB-3). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

VT and KL contributed to the conception and design of the study. KL and NK collected the data for this study and organized the database. PN and KL performed the statistical analysis. PN wrote the first draft of the manuscript. KL, NK, and VT wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^Vignettes were gender-matched. Characters in the vignettes read to male participants included male characters.

References

Abrahamian, E. (2021). Iran Between Two Revolutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Aguinis, H., and Bradley, K. J. (2014). Best practice recommendations for designing and implementing experimental vignette methodology studies. Organ. Res. Methods 17, 351–371. doi: 10.1177/1094428114547952

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, K. R., and Bussey, K. (2016). Cyber-bystanding in context: a review of the literature on witnesses' responses to cyberbullying. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 65, 183–194. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.03.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J., Bresnahan, M., and Musatics, C. (2014). Combating weight-based cyberbullying on Facebook with the dissenter effect. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 17, 281–286. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1998). Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychol. Health 13, 623–649. doi: 10.1080/08870449808407422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., and Pastorelli, C. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. J. Moral Educ. 31, 101–119. doi: 10.1080/0305724022014322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barchia, K., and Bussey, K. (2011). Predictors of student defenders of peer aggression victims: empathy and social cognitive factors. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 35, 289–297. doi: 10.1177/0165025410396746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrington, L. (2012). Comparative Politics: Structures and Choices. Cengage Learning.

Bastiaensens, S., Pabian, S., Vandebosch, H., Poels, K., Van Cleemput, K., DeSmet, A., et al. (2016). From normative influence to social pressure: how relevant others affect whether bystanders join in cyberbullying. Soc. Dev. 25, 193–211. doi: 10.1111/sode.12134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaensens, S., Vandebosch, H., Poels, K., Van Cleemput, K., DeSmet, A., and De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2014). Cyberbullying on social network sites. an experimental study into bystanders' behavioural intentions to help the victim or reinforce the bully. Comput. Human Behav. 31, 259–271. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhat, C. S., Ragan, M. A., Selvaraj, P. R., and Shultz, B. J. (2017). Online bullying among high-school students in India. Int. J. Adv. Counsell. 39, 112–124. doi: 10.1007/s10447-017-9286-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, N. (2021). Bystander intervention in cyberbullying and online harassment: the role of expectancy violations. Int. J. Commun. 15, 647–667.

Brody, N., and Vangelisti, A. L. (2016). Bystander intervention in cyberbullying. Commun. Monogr. 83, 94–119. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2015.1044256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryce, J., and Fraser, J. (2013). “It's common sense that it's wrong”: young people's perceptions and experiences of cyberbullying. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 16, 783–787. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buda, R., and Elsayed-Elkhouly, S. M. (1998). Cultural differences between Arabs and Americans: individualism-collectivism revisited. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 29, 487–492. doi: 10.1177/0022022198293006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukowski, W. M., Hoza, B., and Boivin, M. (1993). Popularity, friendship, and emotional adjustment during early adolescence. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1993, 23–37. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219936004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bussey, K., Fitzpatrick, S., and Raman, A. (2015). The role of moral disengagement and self-efficacy in cyberbullying. J. Sch. Violence 14, 30–46. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2014.954045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bussey, K., Luo, A., Fitzpatrick, S., and Allison, K. (2020). Defending victims of cyberbullying: the role of self-efficacy and moral disengagement. J. Sch. Psychol. 78, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2019.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, B., and Lin, W.-Y. (2015). How do victims react to cyberbullying on social networking sites? The influence of previous cyberbullying victimization experiences. Comput. Hum. Behav. 52, 458–465. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., and Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Soc. Dev. 18, 140–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00465.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, L., Gomez-Garibello, C., Talwar, V., and Shariff, S. (2016). Face-to-face and online: an investigation of childrens' and adolescents' bullying behavior through the lens of moral emotions and judgments. J. Sch. Violence 15, 503–522. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2015.1112805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, C. L., Schaafsma, J., Antheunis, M. L., Shahid, S., Lin, J.-H. T., and Nijtmans, H. W. (2021). Trolls without borders: A cross-cultural examination of victim reactions to verbal and silent aggression online. Front. Psychol. 12, 549955. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.549955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyne, I., Gopaul, A.-M., Campbell, M., Pankász, A., Garland, R., and Cousans, F. (2019). Bystander responses to bullying at work: The role of mode, type and relationship to target. J. Bus. Ethics 157, 813–827. doi: 10.1007/s10551-017-3692-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croucher, S. M., Bruno, A., McGrath, P., Adams, C., McGahan, C., Suits, A., et al. (2012). Conflict styles and high–low context cultures: a cross-cultural extension. Commun. Res. Rep. 29, 64–73. doi: 10.1080/08824096.2011.640093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuhadar, E., and Dayton, B. (2011). The social psychology of identity and inter-group conflict: From theory to practice. Int. Stud. Perspect. 12, 273–293. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-3585.2011.00433.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., and Kinzler, K. D. (2012). The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: a neurodevelopmental study. Cereb. Cortex 22, 209–220. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmet, A., Bastiaensens, S., Cleemput, K. V., Vandebosch, H., and Bourdeaudhuij, I. D. (2012). Mobilizing bystanders of cyberbullying: an exploratory study into behavioural determinants of defending the victim. Ann. Rev. Cyberther. Telemed. 10, 58–63. doi: 10.3233/978-1-61499-121-2-58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeSmet, A., Bastiaensens, S., Van Cleemput, K., Poels, K., Vandebosch, H., Cardon, G., et al. (2016). Deciding whether to look after them, to like it, or leave it: a multidimensional analysis of predictors of positive and negative bystander behavior in cyberbullying among adolescents. Comput. Human Behav. 57, 398–415. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeSmet, A., Bastiaensens, S., Van Cleemput, K., Poels, K., Vandebosch, H., Deboutte, G., et al. (2018). The efficacy of the Friendly Attac serious digital game to promote prosocial bystander behavior in cyberbullying among young adolescents: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Comput. Human Behav. 78, 336–347. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeSmet, A., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., Walrave, M., and Vandebosch, H. (2019). Associations between bystander reactions to cyberbullying and victims' emotional experiences and mental health. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 22, 648–656. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeSmet, A., Veldeman, C., Poels, K., Bastiaensens, S., Van Cleemput, K., Vandebosch, H., et al. (2014). Determinants of self-reported bystander behavior in cyberbullying incidents amongst adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 17, 207–215. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-Hernández, F., Bonell, L., and Martínez-González, A. (2018). A systematic literature review of factors that moderate bystanders' actions in cyberbullying. Cyberpsychology 12, 4. doi: 10.5817/CP2018-4-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51, 665–697. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., and Edwards, A. (2010). “Empathy-related responding and moral development,” in Emotions, Aggression, and Morality in Children: Bridging Development and Psychopathology, eds W.F. Arsenio and E.A. Lemerise (American Psychological Association), 115–135.

Erreygers, S., Pabian, S., Vandebosch, H., and Baillien, E. (2016). Helping behavior among adolescent bystanders of cyberbullying: the role of impulsivity. Learn. Individ. Differ. 48, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, P. C., Simão, A. M. V., Ferreira, A., Souza, S., and Francisco, S. (2016). Student bystander behavior and cultural issues in cyberbullying: when actions speak louder than words. Comput. Human Behav. 60, 301–311. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghanem, S., Kalliny, M., and Elgoul, S. (2013). The impact of technology on the Arab communication style and culture: Implications for marketing. J. Market. Commun. 19, 324–340. doi: 10.1080/13527266.2011.649775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gignac, G. (2019). How2statsbook (Online Edition 1). Available online at: http://www.how2statsbook.com/p/chapters.html

Google Scholar

Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., and Alto,è, G. (2007). Does empathy predict adolescents' bullying and defending behavior? Aggress. Behav. 33, 467–476. doi: 10.1002/ab.20204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gini, G., Thornberg, R., and Pozzoli, T. (2020). Individual moral disengagement and bystander behavior in bullying: the role of moral distress and collective moral disengagement. Psychol. Violence 10, 38–47. doi: 10.1037/vio0000223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Garibello, C., Shariff, S., McConnell, M., and Talwar, V. (2012). Adolescents' evaluation of cyberbullying events. Alberta J. Educ. Res. 58, 474–477. doi: 10.11575/ajer.v58i3.55634

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, E. T. (1989). Beyond Culture. Anchor Books.

Hamelin, N., Nasiri, N., Rezaei, S., and El Haddou-Yousfi, Y. (2018). Comparing work-related values of US, Canadian, Chinese, Iranian, and Moroccan business students: multi-theory perspective. Asian J. Bus. Res. 8, 1–17. doi: 10.14707/ajbr.180040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., and Chou, C. (2010). An analysis of multiple factors of cyberbullying among junior high school students in Taiwan. Comput. Human Behav. 26, 1581–1590. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutson, E. (2016). Cyberbullying in adolescence. Adv. Nurs. Sci. 39, 60–70. doi: 10.1097/ANS.0000000000000104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janis, I. (1991). “Groupthink,” in A First Look at Communication Theory, ed E. Griffin (McGraw Hill), p. 235–246. doi: 10.1109/EMR.2008.4490137 Janis, I.L. (2008). Groupthink. IEEE Eng. Manag. Rev. 36, 36.

CrossRef Full Text

Jones, R. M., Vaterlaus, J. M., Jackson, M. A., and Morrill, T. B. (2014). Friendship characteristics, psychosocial development, and adolescent identity formation. Pers. Relat. 21, 51–67. doi: 10.1111/pere.12017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. E., Manstead, A. S. R., and Livingstone, A. G. (2011). Ganging up or sticking together? Group processes and children's responses to text-message bullying. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 71–96. doi: 10.1348/000712610X502826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keshtiari, N., and Kuhlmann, M. (2016). The effect of culture and gender on the recognition of emotional speech: Evidence from Persian speakers living in a collectivist society. Int. J. Soc. Culture Lang. 4, 71–86. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.1.1159.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluckhohn, F. R., and Strodtbeck, F. L. (1961). Variations in Value Orientations. Row, Peterson.

Kogut, T., Slovic, P., and Västfjäll, D. (2015). Scope insensitivity in helping decisions: Is it a matter of culture and values? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144, 1042–1052. doi: 10.1037/a0039708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapidot-Lefler, N., and Barak, A. (2012). Effects of anonymity, invisibility, and lack of eye-contact on toxic online disinhibition. Comput. Human Behav. 28, 434–443. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leduc, K., Conway, L., Gomez-Garibello, C., and Talwar, V. (2018). The influence of participant role, gender, and age in elementary and high-school children's moral justifications of cyberbullying behaviors. Comput. Human Behav. 83, 215–220. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.01.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leduc, K., Nagar, P. M., Caivano, O., and Talwar, V. (2022). “The thing is, it follows you everywhere”: child and adolescent conceptions of cyberbullying. Comput. Human Behav. 130, 107180. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. Y., Kwon, Y., Yang, S., Park, S., Kim, E.-M., and Na, E.-Y. (2017). Differences in friendship networks and experiences of cyberbullying among Korean and Australian adolescents. J. Genet. Psychol. 178, 44–57. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2016.1242475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, A. N. M. (2021). To help or not to help: Intervening in cyberbullying among Chinese cyber-bystanders. Front. Psychol. 12, 483250. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.483250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z., and Tung, Y. (2018). The impact of peer relationships, conformity, and cognitive empathy of bystander behavioral intentions for cyberbullying. Chin. J. Psychol. 60, 101–124. doi: 10.6129/CJP.201806_60(2)0.0002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo Cricchio, M. G., García-Poole, C., te Brinke, L. W., Bianchi, D., and Menesini, E. (2021). Moral disengagement and cyberbullying involvement: a systematic review. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 18, 271–311. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2020.1782186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, A., and Bussey, K. (2019). The selectivity of moral disengagement in defenders of cyberbullying: contextual moral disengagement. Comput. Human Behav. 93, 318–325. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.12.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, T.-L., and Bellmore, A. (2016). Early adolescents' responses upon witnessing peer victimization: a cross-culture comparison between students in Taiwan and the United States. Int. J. Dev. Sci. 10, 33–42. doi: 10.3233/DEV-150176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, T.-L., and Chen, W.-T. (2019). The benefits of being defended: perceived bystander participant roles and victims' emotional and psychosocial adjustment. J. Sch. Violence 18, 77–91. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2017.1387132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machackova, H., Cerna, A., Sevcikova, A., Dedkova, L., and Daneback, K. (2013). Effectiveness of coping strategies for victims of cyberbullying. Cyberpsychology 7, 4294. doi: 10.5817/CP2013-3-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machackova, H., and Pfetsch, J. (2016). Bystanders' responses to offline bullying and cyberbullying: The role of empathy and normative beliefs about aggression. Scand. J. Psychol. 57, 169–176. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malti, T., and Ongley, S. F. (2014). “The development of moral emotions and moral reasoning,” in Handbook of Moral Development, eds M. Killen and J. G. Smetana (Psychology Press), 163–183.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, D., and Yoo, S. H. (2006). Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 234–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00014.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matz, D. C., and Wood, W. (2005). Cognitive dissonance in groups: the consequences of disagreement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 22–37. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menesini, E., Nocentini, A., and Calussi, P. (2011). The measurement of cyberbullying: dimensional structure and relative item severity and discrimination. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 14, 267–274. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menesini, E., Palladino, B. E., and Nocentini, A. (2015). Emotions of moral disengagement, class norms, and bullying in adolescence: a multilevel approach. Merrill Palmer Q. 61, 124–143. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesquita, B. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minkov, M., Dutt, P., Schachner, M., Morales, O., Sanchez, C., Jandosova, J., et al. (2017). A revision of Hofstede's individualism-collectivism dimension: a new national index from a 56-country study. Cross Cultural Strategic Manag. 24, 386–404. doi: 10.1108/CCSM-11-2016-0197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moxey, N., and Bussey, K. (2020). Styles of bystander intervention in cyberbullying incidents. Int. J. Bullying Prev. 2, 6–15. doi: 10.1007/s42380-019-00039-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesdale, D., and Naito, M. (2005). Individualism-collectivism and the attitudes to school bullying of Japanese and Australian students. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 36, 537–556. doi: 10.1177/0022022105278541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, K., Niven, K., and Notelaers, G. (2021). Does bystander behavior make a difference? How passive and active bystanders in the group moderate the effects of bullying exposure. J. Occupat. Health Psychol. 27, 119–135. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nocentini, A., Calmaestra, J., Schultze-Krumbholz, A., Scheithauer, H., Ortega, R., and Menesini, E. (2010). Cyberbullying: Labels, behaviours and definition in three European countries. J. Psychol. Counsell. Sch. 20, 129–142. doi: 10.1375/ajgc.20.2.129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olenik-Shemesh, D., Heiman, T., and Eden, S. (2017). Bystanders' behavior in cyberbullying episodes: active and passive patterns in the context of personal–socio-emotional factors. J. Interpers. Violence 32, 23–48. doi: 10.1177/0886260515585531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pabian, S., Vandebosch, H., Poels, K., Van Cleemput, K., and Bastiaensens, S. (2016). Exposure to cyberbullying as a bystander: an investigation of desensitization effects among early adolescents. Comput. Human Behav. 62, 480–487. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palomera, R., Fernández-Rouco, N., Briones, E., and González-Yubero, S. (2021). Adolescent bystanders' responses and the influence of social values on applying bullying and cyberbullying strategies. J. Sch. Violence 20, 523–535. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2021.1979017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S., Na, E.-Y., and Kim, E. (2014). The relationship between online activities, netiquette and cyberbullying. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 42, 74–81. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patchin, J. W., and Hinduja, S. (2006). Bullies move beyond the schoolyard: a preliminary look at cyberbullying. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 4, 148–169. doi: 10.1177/1541204006286288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, L. J., Allan, A., and Cross, D. (2017). Adolescent bystander behavior in the school and online environments and the implications for interventions targeting cyberbullying. J. Sch. Violence 16, 361–375. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2016.1143835

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pepler, D., Mishna, F., Doucet, J., and Lameiro, M. (2021). Witnesses in cyberbullying: roles and dilemmas. Children Sch. 43, 45–53. doi: 10.1093/cs/cdaa027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccoli, V., Carnaghi, A., Grassi, M., Strag,à, M., and Bianchi, M. (2020). Cyberbullying through the lens of social influence: Predicting cyberbullying perpetration from perceived peer-norm, cyberspace regulations and ingroup processes. Comput. Human Behav. 102, 260–273. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pornari, C. D., and Wood, J. (2010). Peer and cyber aggression in secondary school students: the role of moral disengagement, hostile attribution bias, and outcome expectancies. Aggress. Behav. 36, 81–94. doi: 10.1002/ab.20336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, D., Green, D., Spears, B., Scrimgeour, M., Barnes, A., Geer, R., et al. (2014). A qualitative exploration of cyber-bystanders and moral engagement. J. Psychol. Counsell. Sch. 24, 1–17. doi: 10.1017/jgc.2013.18

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pronk, J., Olthof, T., Goossens, F. A., and Krabbendam, L. (2019). Differences in adolescents' motivations for indirect, direct, and hybrid peer defending. Soc. Dev. 28, 414–429. doi: 10.1111/sode.12348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quirk, R., and Campbell, M. (2015). On standby? A comparison of online and offline witnesses to bullying and their bystander behaviour. Educ. Psychol. 35, 430–448. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2014.893556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabiei, K. (2013). The development of sociology in the Middle East: Studying the obstacles since the 20th century. J. Iranian Soc. Stud. 6, 77–101. Available online at: http://en.isa.org.ir/publication/iranian-journal-of-sociology/100-journal-of-iranian-social-studies-et-al/285-the-development-of-sociologyin-the-middle-east-studying-the-obstacles-since-the-20th-century

Google Scholar

Razjouyan, K., Hirbod Mobarake, A., Sadr, S. S., Samimi Ardestani, S. M., and Yaseri, M. (2018). The relationship between emotional intelligence and the different roles in cyberbullying among high school students in Tehran. Iranian J. Psychiatry Behav. Sci. 12, 3. doi: 10.5812/ijpbs.11560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezapour, M., Khanjani, N., and Mirzai, M. (2019). Exploring associations between school environment and bullying in Iran: Multilevel contextual effects modeling. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 99, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.01.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roccas, S., Klar, Y., and Liviatan, I. (2006). The paradox of group-based guilt: modes of national identification, conflict vehemence, and reactions to the in-group's moral violations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 698. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romera, E. M., Ortega-Ruiz, R., Rodríguez-Barbero, S., and Falla, D. (2019). How do you think the victims of bullying feel? A study of moral emotions in primary school. Front. Psychol. 10, 1753. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runions, K. C., and Bak, M. (2015). Online moral disengagement, cyberbullying, and cyber-aggression. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 18, 400–405. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salmivalli, C. (1999). Participant role approach to school bullying: Implications for interventions. J. Adolesc. 22, 453–459. doi: 10.1006/jado.1999.0239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: a review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 15, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samovar, L. A., Porter, R. E., and Jain, N. C. (1981). Understanding Intercultural Communication. Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Schwartz, S. H. (1994). “Beyond individualism/collectivism: new cultural dimensions of values,” in Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications, eds U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç. Kâgitçibaşi, S. C. Choi, and G. Yoon (Publications, Inc.), 85–119.

Shadmanfaat, S. M., Shamila, Cochran, J. K., Muniz, C. N., and Kabiri, S. (2019). Soccer hooliganism in Iran: A test of Agnew's General Strain Theory. Deviant Behav. 40, 544–558. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2018.1431136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shariatpanahi, G., Tahouri, K., Asadabadi, M., Moienafshar, A., Nazari, M., and Sayarifard, A. (2021). Cyberbullying and its contributing factors among Iranian adolescents. Int. J. High Risk Behav. Addict. 10, 3. doi: 10.5812/ijhrba.112178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shohoudi Mojdehi, A., Leduc, K., Shohoudi Mojdehi, A., and Talwar, V. (2019). Examining cross-cultural differences in youth's moral perceptions of cyberbullying. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 22, 243–248. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shultz, E., Heilman, R., and Hart, K. J. (2014). Cyber-bullying: an exploration of bystander behavior and motivation. Cyberpsychology 8, 4. doi: 10.5817/CP2014-4-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singhapakdi, A., Rawwas, M. Y. A., Marta, J. K., and Ismail Ahmed, M. (1999). A cross-cultural study of consumer perceptions about marketing ethics. J. Consum. Market. 16, 257–272. doi: 10.1108/07363769910271496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, J., and Oh, I. (2018). Factors influencing bystanders' behavioral reactions in cyberbullying situations. Comput. Human Behav. 78, 273–282. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souza, S. B., Veiga Simão, A. M., Ferreira, A. I., and Ferreira, P. C. (2018). University students' perceptions of campus climate, cyberbullying and cultural issues: implications for theory and practice. Stud. Higher Educ. 43, 2072–2087. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2017.1307818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sticca, F., and Perren, S. (2013). Is cyberbullying worse than traditional bullying? Examining the differential roles of medium, publicity, and anonymity for the perceived severity of bullying. J. Youth Adolesc. 42, 739–750. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9867-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talwar, V., Gomez-Garibello, C., and Shariff, S. (2014). Adolescents' moral evaluations and ratings of cyberbullying: The effect of veracity and intentionality behind the event. Comput. Human Behav. 36, 122–128. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. H., DiFranzo, D., Choi, Y. H., Sannon, S., and Bazarova, N. N. (2019). Accountability and empathy by design: encouraging bystander Intervention to cyberbullying on social media. Proc. ACM Human Comput. Interact. 118, 1–118:26. doi: 10.1145/3359220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornberg, R., Pozzoli, T., and Gini, G. (2021). Defending or remaining passive as a bystander of school bullying in sweden: The role of moral disengagement and antibullying class norms. J. Interpers. Violence 10, 08862605211037427. doi: 10.1177/08862605211037427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornberg, R., Pozzoli, T., Gini, G., and Jungert, T. (2015). Unique and interactive effects of moral emotions and moral disengagement on bullying and defending among school children. Elementary Sch. J. 116, 322–337. doi: 10.1086/683985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, D., and Talwar, V. (2020). Students' moral emotions and judgments of cyberbullying: the influence of previous cyberbullying experiences. Curr. Psychol. 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00916-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ttofi, M. M., and Farrington, D. P. (2008). Reintegrative shaming theory, moral emotions and bullying. Aggress. Behav. 34, 352–368. doi: 10.1002/ab.20257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urschler, D. F. (2016). To help, or not to help, that is not the only question: an investigation of the interplay of different factors to predict helping behavior in an accurate and effective way (Doctoral dissertation der Universität Regensburg). Available online at : https://epub.uni-regensburg.de/33937/.

Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., and Bukowski, W. M. (2009). “The role of friendship in child and adolescent psychosocial development,” in Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups, eds K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, and B. Laursen (The Guilford Press), 568–585.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wachs, S., Wright, M. F., Sittichai, R., Singh, R., Biswal, R., Kim, E., et al. (2019). Associations between witnessing and perpetrating online hate in eight countries: the buffering effects of problem-focused coping. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16, 3992. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16203992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wójcik, M., and Flak, W. (2021). Frenemy: A new addition to the bullying circle. J. Interpers. Violence 36, 19–20. doi: 10.1177/0886260519880168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, M. F., Yanagida, T., Ševčíkov,á, A., Aoyama, I., Dědkov,á, L., Macháčkov,á, H., et al. (2016). Differences in coping strategies for public and private face-to-face and cyber victimization among adolescents in six countries. Int. J. Dev. Sci. 10, 43–53. doi: 10.3233/DEV-150179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S. C., Lin, C.-Y., and Chen, A.-S. (2014). A study of Taiwanese teens' traditional and cyberbullying behaviors. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 50, 525–552. doi: 10.2190/EC.50.4.e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, L., and Yu, J. (2021). A meta-analytic review of moral disengagement and cyberbullying. Front. Psychol. 12, 681299. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.681299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

English versions of vignettes1

Vignette 1

• Bystander type : Assistant

• Relationship: Acquaintance to perpetrator

Rebecca, Nancy and Aisha are in the same class. None of them are close friends. They just received their grade for their math test. Nancy did not understand the material very well and got a very low grade. While Nancy is away from her desk, Aisha sees Nancy's exam and takes a picture of it to show her low grade. Aisha posts the picture on Facebook to show other students how poorly Nancy did on the exam. Aisha posts the picture every day for a week. Both Nancy and Rebecca see the picture. Rebecca laughs when she sees it and shares Aisha's post so her friends can see it too.

Vignette 2

• Bystander type: Outsider

• Relationship: Acquaintance to perpetrator

Catherina, Page and Audrey are in the same computer class. None of them are close friends. They all play online games on Friv. Catherina won great scores and found herself at the top of the leaderboard. Page, who used to be at the top of the leaderboard, is upset that Catherina won and writes a comment on the message board that says “Catherina is a cheater! She does not deserve first place!” Page posts that message everyday for a week. Both Catherina and Audrey see the mean comments. Audrey does nothing after seeing the mean comments.

Vignette 3

• Bystander type: Defender

• Relationship: Acquaintance to perpetrator

Emily, Olivia and Carmen go to the same school. None of them are close friends. They all play online games on Friv. Carmen is not very good at the games so her score is always at the bottom of the leaderboard, but she keeps playing to get better. Olivia sees Carmen's low scores everyday and writes a comment on the message board saying “Carmen is the worst player I ever saw! These games are obviously too hard for you, why are you even trying?”. Olivia posts that comment everyday for a week. Both Carmen and Olivia see the mean comments. When Emily sees all the comments on the message board, she reports Olivia to the school principal.

Vignette 4

• Bystander type: Assistant

• Relationship: Friend to perpetrator

Taylor, Janet and Rosa go to the same school. Taylor and Janet are friends. Rosa plays guitar and made a YouTube channel to post videos of her music. Taylor and Janet like to watch YouTube videos together. When Taylor sees Rosa's videos, she laughs at her and posts a mean comment that says: “Rosa, you are a bad guitar player. Stop posting videos!”. Taylor posts a comment everyday for a week. Both Rosa and Janet see the comments. When Janet sees Taylor's comments, she laughs and writes “Taylor is right, Rosa. You should stop!”

Vignette 5

• Bystander type: Outsider

• Relationship: Friend to perpetrator

Emma, Abigail and Mia are in the same class. All three girls are assigned to work together on a project. Emma and Abigail are friends, but Mia is a new student. Mia has trouble with English so when she writes her part of the project for Emma and Abigail to review, there are a lot of mistakes. Abigail laughs at Mia and writes on Facebook “Mia is the worst writer! She can't even spell correctly!” Abigail posts a mean comment about Mia everyday for a week. Both Mia and Emma see the comments. Emma does nothing after seeing the mean comment.

Vignette 6

• Bystander type: Defender

• Relationship: Friend to perpetrator

Sophia, Mary and Alicia are in the same computer class. Mary and Alicia are friends. Sophia really loves everything to do with technology and made a YouTube channel to teach other students how to do things on computers like build graphs and figures and create websites. Mary and Alicia, like other students in the class, watch the videos. Alicia posts mean messages on Sophia's videos. She tells Sophia “You are such a geek and no one likes your videos. Stop posting them!” Alicia posts this message everyday for a week. Both Sophia and Mary see the mean comments. When Mary sees Alicia's mean posts, she tells Alicia that she should stop because Sophia works hard on her videos and other students find them useful.

Keywords: cyberbullying, bystander, cross-culture, moral disengagement, moral evaluation

Citation: Nagar PM, Leduc K, Khalili N and Talwar V (2022) Cyber-Bystander Behavior Among Canadian and Iranian Youth: The Influence of Bystander Type and Relationship to the Perpetrator on Moral Responsibility. Front. Commun. 7:796146. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2022.796146

Received: 16 October 2021; Accepted: 22 March 2022;
Published: 14 April 2022.

Edited by:

Leslie Ramos Salazar, Texas A&M University System, United States

Reviewed by:

Andreia de Bem Machado, Federal University of Santa Catarina, Brazil
Vysakh Kani Kolil, Amrita Vishwa Vidyapeetham (Amritapuri Campus), India
Saima Eman, The University of Sheffield, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Nagar, Leduc, Khalili and Talwar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pooja Megha Nagar, megha.nagar@mail.mcgill.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.