Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Commun., 09 January 2023
Sec. Health Communication
This article is part of the Research Topic Existential Narratives: Increasing Psychological Wellbeing Through Story View all 12 articles

Editorial: Existential narratives: Increasing psychological wellbeing through story

\nEnny Das
Enny Das1*Kobie van KriekenKobie van Krieken1Anja KalchAnja Kalch2Guan Soon KhooGuan Soon Khoo3
  • 1Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 2Department for Media, Knowledge and Communication, University of Augsburg, Augsburg, Germany
  • 3Moody College of Communication, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States

Humans are story-telling animals; they understand reality through story, they learn and make sense of the world through story (Schank and Berman, 2003; Mar and Oatley, 2008; Gottschall, 2012). From Homer's Odyssey to the movie Titanic, and from Little Red Riding Hood to the Netflix series Stranger Things: fictional story worlds confront the audience with complex social and interpersonal situations that often involve tragic love affairs, violence, loss, and death. The fact that humans voluntarily seek out tragic stories is striking, because they go to great lengths to avoid tragedy in real life. What, if anything, do people gain from watching or reading tragic stories? While extensive research on the so-called sad film paradox (Oliver, 2006) has been conducted to understand why we appreciate tragic movies or books, research on psychological, health-related effects is scarce. Could existential stories help viewers or readers face fundamental fears, overcome psychological hurdles in life, and increase their psychological wellbeing? If so, which story aspects and processes play a role in producing these effects? The relatively little attention that has been paid to the relation between narratives and psychological wellbeing is surprising because mental health problems are highly prevalent in modern society and became even more urgent during the COVID-19 crisis in the last couple of years (OECD, 2021). Indeed, the reframing of narrative research to focus on its psychological and health impact is timely and potentially valuable.

The aim of this Research Topic is to fill this gap by examining how narratives, and in particular existential narratives, affect mental processes, and psychological outcomes. Existential narratives are stories in which questions about the meaning of life and death are explored through the individual experiences of characters who find themselves at a turning point in their life. Drawing from existential psychology, a turning point is understood as moments when characters' basic beliefs about their world are disrupted, such as moments of death or loss (Martin et al., 2004). For example, the films Life Is Beautiful (1997) and American Beauty (1999) raise these existential questions by appealing to a wide range of emotions, from despair to delight. In line with the idea that particularly the combination of eudaimonic (i.e., meaningful) and hedonic (i.e., pleasurable) experiences fosters wellbeing (Huta, 2016), existential narratives transcend the eudaimonic-hedonic distinction and encompass aspects of meaningfulness as well as on aspects of enjoyment. Similarly, existential narratives are not bound to a particular genre or medium: they can be audiovisual (television or Netflix series, movies), textual (poetry, novels), digital (interactive stories, games), fictional or non-fictional, entertaining or persuasive or informative, et cetera.

The broad variety of existential narratives is represented in this Research Topic, with articles exploring the relation between various types of existential narratives and psychological wellbeing as well as underlying processes from theoretical and empirical perspectives. Based on that, three structuring lines emerge: (1) along the types of existential narratives addressed, (2) in terms of wellbeing related effects, and (3) in terms of underlying narrative and reflective processes.

1) Types of existential narratives

Seven of the articles in this Research Topic focus on experiences related to individual (Bálint et al., study 2; (Daalmans et al.; Fitzgerald et al.; Rieger and Schneider; Sopcak et al.) or collective death (Bilandzic and Blessing), confirming the dominant theme of mortality in existential narratives. For example, Bálint et al. (study 2) used an animated short film about dealing with grief throughout life, Daalmans et al. investigated a popular Netflix series about suicide, and Bilandzic and Blessing included an apocalyptic science-fiction parody of modern man's inability to face terrifying facts. Human suffering is another regular theme in the articles, such as the hardships of Canadian Indigenous people in Residential Schools (Sopcak et al.), giving an eulogy at a funeral of a good friend (Fitzgerald et al.), or suffering from depression (Scholl et al.), pulmonary embolism (Kalch et al.), and burnout (Bálint et al., study 1).

2) Wellbeing related aspects

Two studies do not focus on specific types of existential narratives but instead follow an overarching approach by looking at the use and effects of entertainment following experiences of a natural disaster as a life changing event (Raney et al.) as well as longitudinal effects of eudaimonic and hedonic entertainment (Reinecke and Kreling). Results of the study by Raney et al. indicate that self-transcendent media use is positively related to post-traumatic growth. By contrast, results of the study by Reinecke and Kreling showed no effect of eudaimonic and hedonic entertainment on the wellbeing aspects of hope, meaning in life, or resilience. Together, these findings suggest that media stories may affect certain aspects of wellbeing but not others, and perhaps only in times of existential need, such as periods when people are grappling with challenging life events. A range of additional wellbeing correlates is studied in the various other studies, such as loneliness, emotional distress, connectedness, emotional clarity, death acceptance, and coping with self-threats. For example, Kalch et al. discuss how narratives may function to promote self-reflection and inspire mindful mastery while reducing feelings of loneliness and emotional distress among patients suffering from a cardiovascular disease. Hanauer furthermore shows that narrative writing could enhance wellbeing in terms of heightened levels of insight and emotional clarity. This finding demonstrates that beneficial wellbeing effects can be achieved not only by consuming narratives, but also by actively producing narratives.

3) Underlying narrative and reflective processes

How does the narrative impact on wellbeing come about? Eight of the articles aim to answer this question. Rieger and Schneider demonstrate how narratives can serve as a coping tool via heightened narrative experiences of transportation, enjoyment, and appreciation. In a similar context, Fitzgerald et al. compared the impact of a poignant-focused eulogy vs. a humor-focused eulogy on death acceptance through narrative processing. Scholl et al. demonstrate how interactive narratives positively affect readers' transformative learning about depression through identification with the narrative character. Several other studies focus on how self-reflection on emotional experiences could promote wellbeing, specifically through people's manner of reflection. Daalmans et al. reported that existential narratives can provoke moral rumination among young adults, whereas Bilandzic and Blessing introduce the idea of critical thinking as a mind-set effect, which implies that media narratives can stimulate viewers and readers to think critically within and outside of the situation that originally triggered this mindset. Sopcak et al. investigate how reading literature promotes empathy and moral outcomes via different forms of reading engagement, cognitive perspective taking, and empathy. In addition to these reading processes, Hanauer investigates self-effects of writing about significant, life-changing moments in a poetic style vs. free writing about daily events.

Together, the findings point to promising avenues for future research about existential narratives and the intricate relation between their form and content, the processes they generate and their impact on wellbeing. First, we observe that in existential narratives, the themes of death and human suffering are put center stage, suggesting that such narratives simulate experiences that are too terrifying or dangerous to try out in real life (Gottschall, 2012). Second, several studies suggest a thin line between complex emotional narrative content on the one hand and moral reasoning and critical reflection on the other hand. For example, mixed affect can stimulate or decrease learning from a narrative, depending on the topic. Finally, we conclude that reading and writing existential narratives can contribute to wellbeing by giving meaning to life and by providing tools to cope with existential threats.

Author contributions

ED and KK drafted a concept article. ED, KK, AK, and GK provided edits and comments. All authors approved of the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Netherlands Science Foundation NWO (grant number 406.18.SW.039).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Gottschall, J. (2012). The Storytelling Animal: How Stories Make us Human. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Google Scholar

Huta, V. (2016). “An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts,” in The Routledge Handbook of Media Use and Well-being, eds L. Reinecke and M. B. Oliver (Routledge), 14–33.

Google Scholar

Mar, R. A., and Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 173–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00073.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L. L., Campbell, W. K., and Henry, C. D. (2004). “The roar of awakening: Morality acknowledgement as a call to authentic living,” in Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology, eds J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, and T. A. Pyszczynski (Guilford Press) 431–448.

Google Scholar

OECD (2021). COVID-19 and Well-being: Life in the Pandemic. OECD Publishing.

Google Scholar

Oliver, M. B. (2006). Exploring the paradox of the enjoyment of sad films. Hum. Commun. Res. 19, 315–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1993.tb00304.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schank, R. C., and Berman, T. R. (2003). “The pervasive role of stories in knowledge and action,” in Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations, eds T. C. Brock, J. J. Strange and M. C. Green (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 287–313.

Google Scholar

Keywords: narratives, psychological wellbeing, existential motives, mental processes, books, movies

Citation: Das E, van Krieken K, Kalch A and Khoo GS (2023) Editorial: Existential narratives: Increasing psychological wellbeing through story. Front. Commun. 7:1113441. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2022.1113441

Received: 01 December 2022; Accepted: 28 December 2022;
Published: 09 January 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Rukhsana Ahmed, University at Albany, United States

Copyright © 2023 Das, van Krieken, Kalch and Khoo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Enny Das, yes enny.das@ru.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.