Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Cell. Infect. Microbiol., 29 October 2014
Sec. Microbiome in Health and Disease
This article is part of the Research Topic New perspectives in evolution: The superorganisms, symbiopoiesis and integration of systems. View all 7 articles

Friends with social benefits: host-microbe interactions as a driver of brain evolution and development?

  • 1Alimentary Pharmabiotic Centre, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
  • 2Department Anatomy and Neuroscience, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
  • 3Departments of Biological Sciences and Pathology, Microbiology, and Immunology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland

The tight association of the human body with trillions of colonizing microbes that we observe today is the result of a long evolutionary history. Only very recently have we started to understand how this symbiosis also affects brain function and behavior. In this hypothesis and theory article, we propose how host-microbe associations potentially influenced mammalian brain evolution and development. In particular, we explore the integration of human brain development with evolution, symbiosis, and RNA biology, which together represent a “social triangle” that drives human social behavior and cognition. We argue that, in order to understand how inter-kingdom communication can affect brain adaptation and plasticity, it is inevitable to consider epigenetic mechanisms as important mediators of genome-microbiome interactions on an individual as well as a transgenerational time scale. Finally, we unite these interpretations with the hologenome theory of evolution. Taken together, we propose a tighter integration of neuroscience fields with host-associated microbiology by taking an evolutionary perspective.

Introduction

“NOTHING IN BIOLOGY MAKES SENSE EXCEPT IN THE LIGHT OF EVOLUTION”

(Dobzhansky, 1973)

It is now over 40 years since Dobzhansky published his famous essay aimed at defending evolutionary thinking against the increasing influence of creationist belief (Dobzhansky, 1973). Today the foundation of the neo-Darwinian synthesis that Dobzhansky helped engineer has grown into one of the central pillars of modern biology and underlines the essentiality of considering biological processes from an evolutionary viewpoint.

On inspection of the evolutionary and genomic trajectories of macroscopic species, biologists increasingly find that they are not only determined by changes in gene frequencies in the nuclear and cytoplasmic genomes, but also strongly by genetic variation in the single-celled symbionts that can be understood as part of the total genetics of the macroscopic organism. In this light, intergenomic interactions between the nucleus and microbiome are similar to the intergenomic interactions between the nucleus and the mitochondria, or even between chromosomes of the nucleus. Indeed since their origins, eukaryotes and their microbial symbionts have been and are being united in diverse associations, ranging from obligate intracellular to extracellular microbes that forge mutualistic, commensal and parasitic interactions (Dale and Moran, 2006; Dethlefsen et al., 2007; McFall-Ngai et al., 2013; Douglas, 2014). These diverse associations serve as raw genomic variation for natural selection to operate on. Just as a gene-gene interaction (e.g., epistasis) emerges from mutational events in the genome and can be selected upon, so too are symbiont-host associations formed and forged over time.

With the advent of multicellularity and mobile animals, energy demands rose and opened the door for microbial symbioses to intensify their role in host nutrition and metabolism. Still today, microbial influences continue to shape eukaryotic and animal evolution. The microbiota affects nearly every aspect of animal fitness as they colonize animal organs including the mouth, skin, reproductive organs or specialized organs, such as female reproductive tissues (Funkhouser and Bordenstein, 2013), the light organ of the Hawaiian bobtail squid Euprymna scolopes, and other surface organs exposed to the environment, with the gastrointestinal tract reaching the highest densities of bacterial cells in mammals (Turnbaugh et al., 2007; Dave et al., 2012; Schloissnig et al., 2013). Taken together, it is important to note that the universality of symbiosis in eukaryotic evolution does not obviate canonical mechanisms of evolutionary biology such as natural selection or even other levels of selection such as selfish genes. Rather, natural selection at multiple levels and symbiosis are operating together in underappreciated ways that are borne out as the microbiome sciences mature.

The host-associated microbiota is not only comprised of bacteria, but also archaea and eukaryotes such as protozoa, fungi and nematodes. Furthermore, viruses of all three cellular domains, collectively termed the virome, can be found in the microbiota (Virgin, 2014). Large-scale microbial sequencing projects like the Human Microbiome Project (HMP) (Turnbaugh et al., 2007; Human Microbiome Project Consortium, 2012), the European MetaHIT (Qin et al., 2010), and the Eldermet project (defining the microbial composition associated with aging, Claesson et al., 2012) have contributed to identify the human-associated microbiota, consisting of at least 40,000 bacterial strains in 1800 genera (Luckey, 1972; Frank and Pace, 2008; Forsythe and Kunze, 2013), which collectively harbor at least 9.9 million non-human genes (Li et al., 2014). Carrying approximately 500 times the human protein-coding genes currently annotated (http://www.ensembl.org), the ~100 trillion non-human associated cells make up 1–2 kg in an adult body (Forsythe and Kunze, 2013), which is comparable to the weight of the adult human brain (ca. 1.5 kg, Parent and Carpenter, 1996).

The above comparison ending in the analogy to the weight of the brain is not an arbitrary exercise in numbers. It is a window into the connections between neuroscience and microbiology. During human evolution, the primate brain underwent structural reconstructions of fast and dramatic increases in relative volume, leading to the brain as the most energy-demanding organ in the body (Khatri and Man, 2013). Interestingly, it has been observed that at the same time, the gastrointestinal tract shrunk accordingly, which led to the “expensive-tissue hypothesis” (Aiello and Wheeler, 1995), proposing compensation of growth of one metabolically expensive organ by reduction of another. While this latter hypothesis has been challenged (Navarrete et al., 2011; Warren and Iglesias, 2012), recent evidence suggests that the presence and types of microorganisms in a given host individual not only has multiple, critical consequences for host physiological processes such as postnatal development and immunomodulation, but also surprisingly affects neurodevelopment, host behavior and cognition (Cryan and Dinan, 2012).

In light of this new evidence and data from cognitive neurogastroenterology studies (e.g., Cryan and O'Mahony, 2011), we will explore the possibility that host-microbe associations critically affect mammalian brain evolution and development. We will further argue that to understand transgenerational inter-kingdom communication and their effects on brain adaptation and plasticity, it is inevitable to consider epigenetic mechanisms as important mediators of these host-microbe interactions.

Genome-Microbiome Interactions and Animal Evolution

“THERE IS A FUNDAMENTAL ERROR IN SEPARATING THE PARTS FROM THE WHOLE, THE MISTAKE OF ATOMIZING WHAT SHOULD NOT BE ATOMIZED. UNITY AND COMPLEMENTARITY CONSTITUTE REALITY”

(Werner Heisenberg, 1930)

According to long-standing dogma in biology, a mammal's first contact with bacteria occurs during delivery through the birth canal. However, there is an increasing body of evidence that demonstrates maternal transmission of certain microbes occuring in utero, and thus the sterile-womb paradigm is out-dated (Funkhouser and Bordenstein, 2013). Moreover, the mother's gut microbiota changes dramatically during pregnancy (Koren et al., 2012). After delivery through the birth canal, the microbiota becomes more complex and abundant, and these community-level changes continue via breast-feeding and uptake of new microbes from the environment (Koenig et al., 2011). It is therefore not surprising that the microbiota critically influences pre-, peri- and postnatal development, and changes during early life stages will result in phenotypic alterations in adulthood (Borre et al., 2014). Moreover, microbial successions during animal development are well-established (Koenig et al., 2011; Brucker and Bordenstein, 2012b; Pantoja-Feliciano et al., 2013) and can be influenced by various environmental factors such as diet, lifestyle or habitat (Marques et al., 2010).

Microbiota colonization can depend on the genetics of the host, and there is an intensifying interest today in resolving the relative contributions of the environment and host genes on the assembly of host-associated microbial communities. In particular, the host genome may filter environmental microbes into host tissues as a form of symbiont domestication each generation, and reciprocally, environmental microbes may prefer to occupy specific lineages of hosts (Brucker and Bordenstein, 2012a). Several studies have explored genome-microbiome associations (Dethlefsen et al., 2007; Arumugam et al., 2011; Moeller et al., 2012), and laboratory studies in model systems are beginning to be utilized to control the influences of the macro- and microenvironment on gut microbiota assembly, thereby leading to an excavation of the intrinsic host-genetic influence on the assembly of the microbiota across species. Differential microbial compositions occur between closely related species when maintained on the same diet and under identical rearing conditions (Brucker and Bordenstein, 2012b; Franzenburg et al., 2013), and the community relationships of each species' microbiome parallels the phylogenetic relationships of the host genome, a pattern termed “phylosymbiosis” in the insect model Nasonia (Brucker and Bordenstein, 2013b). Moreover, in the early branching metazoan Hydra (a cnidarian that reproduces asexually), specialized anti-microbial peptides partly regulate phylosymbiosis across related species (Fraune and Bosch, 2007; Franzenburg et al., 2013). It has also been demonstrated that interspecific host-microbe specificity is required for proper immune system development in mice (Chung et al., 2012).

Within species, there are differences in the microbiome that can be attributed to single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) or copy-number variations (CNVs) (Rausch et al., 2011; Tong et al., 2014; Wacklin et al., 2014). As such, genetic variation between mouse strains is responsible for variations in gut microbiota (Benson et al., 2010; Kovacs et al., 2011). In humans, there is evidence that microbiota composition is more similar in closely related individuals such as monozygotic twins (Zoetendal et al., 2001) and correlates with ethnic affiliation (Ravel et al., 2011; Mason et al., 2013). Understanding the interplay between colonization dynamics of microbes, human genetics, and complex diseases, including neurodevelopmental and psychiatric diseases (i.e., autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), schizophrenia and depression), is an important endeavor to ultimatley define genetic risk factors for a potentially fatal microbial composition (Spor et al., 2011). In general, genotype-enterotype interactions may be a key determinant of microbial variation between individuals. With the progressing trend toward fecal transplantation treatments in mind (Borody and Khoruts, 2012), which is now even feasible from frozen fecal suspensions that can be deposited in specific fecal bio-banks (Youngster et al., 2014), incompatible or otherwise detrimental host genome-microbiome combinations should also be considered and donor-recipient pairs screened accordingly prior to fecal transplantation.

In conclusion, host selection of the microbiota exhibits features of an extended phenotype encoded by the core genome. The “extended phenotype” concept was introduced by Richard Dawkins to include modulatory effects on the environment as part of a gene's phenotype that extends beyond modulation of the cells in which it is expressed (Dawkins, 1983). As the phenotypes that we are discussing have genomes themselves, the analogy is more appropriately extended to an extended genome that encodes the essential features missing in the host genome. According to this perspective, symbiosis in general and microbial endosymbiosis in particular can be viewed as the essential complement of the missing activity of an organism's core genome, a view that is compatible with the hologenome concept, first introduced by Rosenberg et al. (2007). This concept considers the host's genome and it's associated microbiome as an organism's total genome, in which the summed genetics of all members can affect fitness and is thus a newly appreciated unit of selection that affects adaptation and speciation (Rosenberg et al., 2007; Brucker and Bordenstein, 2013a). Therefore, the hologenome concept embraces the contemporary gene-centric view of life, but upgrades it to include the microbiome as a central facet of an organism's genetics. By all accounts, this viewpoint blurs the differences between the genome and environment. It embraces a vibrant and more satisfying view of the nature of biology, namely that the microbiome is as essential as the genome in defining what an animal or plant is and is not.

The Social Network: Microbiota, RNA and the Evolution of the Social Brain

“LIFE DID NOT TAKE OVER THE GLOBE BY COMBAT, BUT BY NETWORKING”

(Margulis and Sagan, 1986)

Minds That Think Alike: Advantages of Social Brains

Many mammalian species have evolved to give up solitary life and form social groups of cooperative individuals. Group living offers a variety of advantages ranging from mutual protection to cooperative foraging for food and finding a mating partner. Also support during rearing of offspring is likely to exhibit an evolutionary advantage in family-like structured groups as predicted by kin selection theory, a mathematical framework providing an explanation for the apparently paradox altruistic investment of resources to help nurturing offspring of close relatives (Hamilton, 1964). However, group living also poses certain challenges on the physiology and behavior of individuals within a social group. As such, group members need to recognize each other, which demands interaction of visual and memory systems in the brain. In order to plan and organize cooperative undertakings such as hunting, as observed in group-living mammals like wolfs, lions and some primates, also involves an acknowledgement of what other individuals know, see or feel. This form of empathy or “Theory of Mind” is a feature that is highly developed in humans and commonly disturbed in neurodevelopmental disorders of the autistic spectrum (Baron-Cohen, 2009). Moreover, neurobiological mechanisms for behaviors such as those related to affection need to be implemented in the brains of social individuals in order to secure social bonding within a group.

Therefore, the brains of social species exhibit a set of common features that need to work together for group living to become advantageous. Brain areas such as the prefrontal cortex or the amygdala have undergone pronounced changes in the evolution of social mammals such as humans (Hrvoj-Mihic et al., 2013). Also neuroendocrine systems, such as vasopressin and oxytocin, the latter of which is sometimes referred to as the “social hormone,” is important for group living to support affection and empathy among group members (Insel, 2010; Meyer-Lindenberg and Tost, 2012; Lukas et al., 2013; Skuse et al., 2014). Furthermore, it is increasingly appreciated that the social environment is tightly associated with susceptibility to mental health problems, with city-living being more detrimental than rural close-knit communities (Lederbogen et al., 2011).

Relationships to other individuals of the same species as well as behavior toward other species are also a key environmental factor influencing the specific microbiota of an individual. Social isolation or maternal neglect can lead to severe stress-related disturbances of the gut microbiota, which potentially mediate further adverse physiological reactions associated with stressful situations (Bailey and Coe, 1999; O'Mahony et al., 2009; Bailey et al., 2011). In fact, also from the reverse perspective, microbiota and social life may be more intimately connected than generally appreciated.

Expanding the Gene Pool: The Role of Microbes in the Evolution of the Social Group Living

Unlike our core genome, our microbiome is contagious. Microbial cells can actively or passively leave the body and spread to new habitats and hosts, and from an evolutionary perspective it can be assumed that natural selection has favored those microbes that increase their own transmission. On the other hand, exchange of microbes by intimate contact with conspecifics may offer benefits for the host as well. These features of the microbiome have motivated some authors to hypothesize that social behavior has, at least in part, evolved to enhance transmission of microbes (Troyer, 1984; Lombardo, 2008; Ezenwa et al., 2012; Montiel-Castro et al., 2013). As such, it can be an advantage to transfer beneficial symbionts, which are used by recipients either to increase resistance against infectious agents and toxins or increase abilities to digest a wider range of foods (Figure 1, left side). Lombardo has referred to this “access to mutualistic endosymbiotic microbes” as a driving force in the evolution of sociality in animals (Lombardo, 2008). While the microbe-dependent ability to process plant-based diets has been especially important for herbivorous species (Troyer, 1984), protection from parasites and pathogens was suggested to be driving social behavior in non-herbivores (Lombardo, 2008). The latter is likely achieved by exchange of microbes that contribute to the secondary metabolism by producing toxins or antibiotics that help to provide a defense against parasites or degrade toxic xenobiotics and create a healthy homeostatic microenvironment (Douglas, 2014) (Figure 1, right side). Furthermore, some microbial components such as bacterial cell wall material or other microbe-associated molecular patterns (MAMPs), (Ausubel, 2005) are recognized by the immune system and thereby prime it for encounters with pathogenic or unwanted microbes (Chung et al., 2012; Lee and Mazmanian, 2014). Montiel-Castro et al. further discussed the evolutionary role of specific social behaviors such as kissing, grooming and sexual intercourse for enhancing the transfer of microbes for selective colonization (Montiel-Castro et al., 2013) and just recently focused on the role of microbes in social and socioeconomic decision making (Montiel-Castro et al., 2014). Although the social-behavior-driven mechanisms of exchange of symbionts will also always be subject to hijacking by parasites, we must assume that the exchange of beneficial microbes prevails, considering the fact that there was never an option for evolution to select against all symbionts.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Friends with benefits: Social group living and transmission of microbes. Advantages of living in groups may occur through horizontal transfer of beneficial symbionts that increase abilities to digest a wider range of foods (left) or confer resistance against infectious agents (right). Protection from parasites was suggested to be especially important for non-herbivores, where beneficial microbes create a healthy homeostatic microenvironment and train the immune system of the host. The acquired microbes can further be transmitted vertically to the next generation, strengthening the symbiotic association between host and microbe and conferring increased biological fitness.

These notions are particularly intriguing in the light of British anthropologist Robin Dunbar's social brain hypothesis. This assumes a causal positive relationship between neocortex size and social behavior within primates as a factor critically contributing to evolution of human intelligence (Dunbar, 1998). It is therefore tempting to speculate that enhanced transmission of microbes through group living may have contributed to the gradual increase in cortical size and function. Studies investigating divergence of the microbiome along evolutionary speciation trajectories would need to show matching host and symbiont phylogenies to help inform such a hypothesis (Dale and Moran, 2006; Zaneveld et al., 2008; Fitzpatrick, 2014). A recent study comparing the chimpanzee and human microbiomes is a first step in this direction (Moeller et al., 2012) and makes it particularly intriguing for comparative biology to embrace microbiota differences that facilitate phylosymbiosis with regard to social complexity.

Regulatory RNA Networks

Tightly associated with the accelerated expansion of the neocortex during primate evolution, the human genome has seen accelerated evolution, especially in certain non-protein-coding regions. With the advent of whole-transcriptome sequencing technologies, we learned that a lot of these regions actually do contain valuable information. Interestingly, most of these regions are transcribed into RNA, albeit with often unknown or unassigned function. Yet, there is a steadily increasing amount of evidence that these non-coding RNAs (ncRNAs) have a potent regulatory impact on the cell's transcriptional landscape. They are grouped as small (<200 bp) and long-non-coding RNAs (>200 bp). Small RNAs have been established early in evolution (also prokaryotes are known to use them; Liu et al., 2012; Mika and Hengge, 2013) and are further divided into different subgroups, most importantly microRNAs (miRNAs), small-interfering RNAs (siRNAs) and piwi-interacting RNA (piRNAs), which together function in post-transcriptional regulation of gene expression by interfering with primary transcripts. Long non-coding RNAs (lncRNA), however, appear much later in evolution and are only found in plants and animals. In fact, about one third of the known lncRNAs seem to be primate-specific (Derrien et al., 2012; Barry, 2014). It is important to note that the brain is not only the main site of expression of lncRNAs but also of other RNA-based regulatory mechanisms, including alternative splicing, RNA editing and RNA methylation (Paul and Bass, 1998; Meyer et al., 2012; Li et al., 2013; Niu et al., 2013), most of which have greater prominence in the human genome, are particularly prevalent in the human brain, and are therefore promising candidates for a key role in the evolution of neurodevelopmental processes and complex human social behavior and cognition (Blow et al., 2004; Xing and Lee, 2006; Lin et al., 2010; Barry and Mattick, 2012; Qureshi and Mehler, 2012).

Together, both enhanced microbial transmission through group living and expansion of non-coding RNA regulation, likely have contributed to advanced social behavior in primates and ultimately human intelligence (Figure 2). However, it will be intriguing to understand whether these two systems developed in parallel and independently, or whether host-symbiont co-evolution had an impact on the development of more complex RNA regulation and vice versa (Figure 2). Based on this, we predict that dysbiosis during development or the complete absence of microbes during germ-free life leads, among other effects and through mechanisms that remain to be identified, to a deregulation of key RNA-based processes necessary for normal brain maturation and function.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Microbes, RNA networks and brain development: A social triangle? An integrated model is proposed for the evolution of human social behavior. Recent human evolution was accompanied by accelerated extension of the neocortex along with an increase in the importance of ncRNAs and RNA diversity, processing and plasticity in the brain. At the same time host-microbe co-evolution contributed to enhance sociability by providing endosymbiotic developmental signals through the microbiota-gut-brain axis. In addition, social behavior affects the composition of the microbiota and vice-versa and differential expression of ncRNAs has been observed in cognitive disorders that are associated with altered social behavior (black arrow heads). Whether the microbiota and the brain's transcriptome interact, especially on the level of non-coding RNAs, is currently under investigation.

Microbiota, RNA Networks and Brain Development: A Social Triangle?

In order to interrogate a potential interaction between RNA networks and the microbiota in neurodevelopment and evolution of social behavior, it will be helpful to look at two kinds of experimental contexts. Firstly, scenarios where disturbances of social capabilities of a host need to be investigated with respect to changes in the microbiota and RNA-based gene regulation. Secondly, effects of the absence of microbes or of a disturbed microbiota composition (dysbiosis) on RNA-based mechanisms and social behavior will be crucial in informing the genetic basis of this social triangle (Figure 2).

With respect to the former, among the most pronounced disturbances of social and emotional behavior are disorders within the autism spectrum. On the one hand, ASDs and other neurodevelopmental disorders have recently been associated with impaired function of ncRNAs (Mellios and Sur, 2012; Van De Vondervoort et al., 2013; Ziats and Rennert, 2013). Moreover, on the other hand, recent research suggests that neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASDs, are tightly interleaved with the gut microbiota. Though it is unknown at the moment what is cause and what consequence, the most common symptom of ASDs—a lack of pro-social behavior such as sociability—is often accompanied by gastrointestinal (GI) symptoms and alterations in microbiota composition and function (Ming et al., 2012; Cao et al., 2013). Interestingly, both, CNS and GI symptoms are also commonly co-occurring in animal models for ASDs and seem to be dependent on host-microbe interactions (Hsiao et al., 2013; Desbonnet et al., 2014).

Some correlative evidence for an interaction of the microbiota and RNA editing comes from a study in genetically obese leptin-deficient (ob/ob) mice, which are known to harbor a different microbiota compared to lean mice (Turnbaugh et al., 2006), and show altered editing of the serotonin receptor Htr2c mRNA in the hippocampus and hypothalamus (Schellekens et al., 2012). Interestingly, these mRNA-editing sites are precisely conserved in humans and alterations in editing frequency has been shown to be associated with schizophrenia (Burns et al., 1997; Dracheva et al., 2003), although regulation in humans happens to be more complex due to alternative splicing of the editing cassette. Also RNA methylation might be connected to microbiota composition through metabolism, since one of two known RNA demethylases, the Fat mass and obesity associated (Fto) gene, was found to be strongly associated with body mass index in humans (Speakman, 2013; Zheng et al., 2013).

In order to take a closer look at the second experimental context, i.e., the effects of the absence of microbes on social capabilities, we have recently shown that germ-free mice largely lack pro-social behavior and social cognition (Desbonnet et al., 2014). Germ-free mice spent significantly less time with a conspecific and did not show the typical preference for a novel mouse when given a choice between a familiar and a novel interaction partner. While sociability could be rescued by introduction of a normal microbiota post-weaning, recognition memory was not amenable by microbiota replenishment, suggesting a critical developmental time window for microbiota-dependent cues to act upon the central nervous system and the establishment of social abilities (Desbonnet et al., 2014). Decreased sociability, albeit less pronounced compared to the mouse model, was also demonstrated in germ-free rats (Crumeyrolle-Arias et al., 2014). Future experiments should also focus on dynamic regulation of gene expression and especially the role of regulatory RNA in these animals, with a focus on brain regions involved in social behavior.

Taken together these concepts support a model in which the evolution of human sociability, which was accompanied by accelerated extension of the neocortex, is a key example of host-microbe co-evolution, and is dependent on endosymbiotic developmental signals through the microbiota-gut-brain axis (Figure 2). The development of the forebrain, esp. the neocortex, in social mammals and ultimately primates and humans depends on correct and timely signals from microbial symbionts—which is disturbed, when the microbiota is absent or disturbed, as seen in artificial germ-free models or in more natural settings such as cesarean section or pre-, peri- or early life stress (Gilbert et al., 2010; Borre et al., 2014).

But what are these environmental cues that the microbiota provide for host brain development? And how can they interfere with RNA-based mechanisms? In the following sections we will review evidence that, in addition to modulation of neurotransmitter systems, epigenetic mechanisms of gene regulation provide a suitable interface for host-microbe interactions. To do so, we will first explore how epigenetic mechanisms shape Darwinian evolution and then focus on the molecular epigenetic machinery that provides an interface for interaction with such mechanisms for the microbiota to affect evolution and development of the social brain.

Epigenetic Mechanisms in Evolution: Nature and Nurture

“EPIGENETICS IS A USEFUL WORD IF YOU DON'T KNOW WHAT'S GOING ON – IF YOU DO, YOU USE SOMETHING ELSE.”

(Adrian Bird, 1995)

In addition to microbe-induced genetic variation such as lateral gene-transfer (LGT, see Box 1), symbiont-dependent epigenetic mechanisms can generate heritable variety within a few generations, which may reduce the amount of germline LGT needed to make symbiont-induced changes to host development on short time scales.

Box 1. Lateral gene transfer (LGT) as a source of selectable variability.

Interdependencies between species do not only occur between host and microbes but also among the individual microbes, namely through metabolic chains and genomic complementarity, in which metabolites and gene products are exchanged to fulfill those missing from the interacting partner(s). In addition to chemical or peptide exchanges, genetic material can also be swapped between microbe and host. From the perspective of the hologenome theory, host-associated microbes represent a continuum of symbiotic interactions that scale from bacteria-derived organelles to endosymbionts and extracellular microbes. Across the developmental trajectory of early eukaryotes, the bulk of chloroplast and mitochondrial genes have been relocated to the nuclear genome. While the interactions between eukaryotic nucleus and extracellular microbes are certainly less intimate, it has to be noted that interspecies lateral gene transfer (LGT, also known as horizontal gene transfer) is continuing to occur also between eukaryotic and viral as well as prokaryotic genomes (Salzberg et al., 2001; Robinson et al., 2013; Overballe-Petersen and Willerslev, 2014). It is clear that viral genes strongly contributed to animal genome evolution and fitness. Up to 8% of the human genome is estimated to be of viral origin (Belshaw et al., 2004), followed by another 37% sharing homology with bacterial genes (McFall-Ngai et al., 2013), though it is still unclear if the majority of these bacterial genes have been transferred to animal genomes by LGT or stem from early eukaryotic evolution (Domazet-Loso and Tautz, 2008).

Inter-microbial LGT is common and particularly (in)famous for the quick spread of antimicrobial resistance. Interestingly, it can also serve as a relatively quick response toward a changing gut environment, e.g., during gut inflammation (Stecher et al., 2012). Compared to bacteria-to-bacteria transfer, LGT between animal hosts and microbes is less common (Blaxter, 2007). This is mostly due to the fact that LGT has to occur, at least in sexually reproducing animals, in the germ cells to be transmitted to the next generation and be stabilized in the population (Robinson et al., 2013). However, recently these events are identified more frequently, especially in invertebrates (Boto, 2012), some of which are tightly associated with the germline-transmitted bacterial endosymbiont Wolbachia (Dunning Hotopp et al., 2007). By modifying an organisms DNA, LGT provides a source of selectable genetic variation over time in addition to base pair mutations, recombination, insertions, deletions, etc., and may therefore act as an effective driver of co-evolution, especially on longer evolutionary time scales.

Initially the term “epigenetics” was used to describe developmental programming (Waddington, 1953), and was only later defined to refer to heritable changes in gene expression that do not originate from mutations of the DNA sequence (Holliday, 1987). More recently, the definition of the term is discussed frequently and controversially (as outlined by the initial quote found in Ledford, 2008; Ptashne, 2013a,b) and now often used in a much broader sense, with specific connotations depending on the field of study. In contrast to the former strict definition, some researchers focus on the aspect of sequence-independent transgenerational germline inheritance of a phenotypic trait, especially neuroscientists or biological psychiatrists, who highlight the impact of early-life experiences on development and behavior during later life (e.g., Meaney and Szyf, 2005). Yet another branch of developmental biology interprets transgenerational epigenetic traits in the context of cellular differentiation during multicellular organismal growth (somatic mitotis; Müller and Leutz, 2001; Steffen and Ringrose, 2014). Most of these definitions have in common one or the other aspect of (transcriptional) memory in the sense that the effect of a stimulus is perpetuated even if the initial signal or event disappears (Ptashne, 2007). Inspired by an apparent overlap of this with memories in the brain, the term epigenetics in molecular neurobiology is often used rather loosely to point to a common set of molecular signaling cascades affecting dynamic regulation of gene expression due to neuronal activity, also referred to as neuroepigenetics or chromatin plasticity (Dulac, 2010; Sweatt, 2013; Fischer, 2014a).

Some recent studies have pointed to the intriguing possibility that life experience and other environmental insults acquired in the parental generation may result in altered brain function and behavioral changes in subsequent generations (Weaver et al., 2004; Arai et al., 2009; Franklin et al., 2010; Bohacek et al., 2013; Dias and Ressler, 2014; Gapp et al., 2014). While only few studies showed that supportive influences such as enhanced memory function through environmental enrichment (Arai et al., 2009) or avoidance of a potentially harmful odor (Dias and Ressler, 2014) can be passed on to the next generation, this epigenetic transgenerational inheritance is best documented for rather stressful events such as exposure to trauma. This bias might be explained by easier access to experimental models that induce robust and functional epigenetic changes that have negative outcomes such as increased depressive or anxiety-like behaviors than appetitive-based environmental situations. Additionally, epigenetic transgenerational inheritance might have evolved to provide offspring with protection against rapid, adverse changes in the environment of the parental generation and serve as means to quickly adapt to current environmental situations. They can thus be interpreted as mechanisms that offer evolutionary “short-cuts” to circumvent the long process of sustained natural selection, which is needed to represent environmental information in the DNA sequence of the genome, and be able to respond to unpredictable, rapid changes in the environment that occur within the life-time of a generation. Interestingly, in experimental models, the behavioral effects of these epigenetic adaptations quickly wear off if not reinforced, which underlines their plasticity and reversibility. This impermanence also highlights that there is little need for some epigenetic traits to be stabilized in a population and become part of hard-wired Mendelian mechanisms of inheritance, since the environmental factors leading to these traits are dynamic and unpredictable.

However, if the effects of traumatic or stressful experiences can be inherited to prepare and protect offspring from a harmful environment, it raises the question whether the observed behavioral phenotype in experimental models, including depressive- and anxiety-like behavior, are in fact protective. If these changes are not just pleiotropic by-products of the epigenetic machinery regulating gene expression in the parent, then there must be a beneficial effect of passing them on to the next generation and the behaviors expressed by the offspring may actually be protective. While we have little reservation in accepting that useful information about current conditions should be (epigenetically) transmitted to the next generation, there is some difficulty in understanding the benefit when adverse conditions trigger negative effects such as increased risk for psychiatric disorders in subsequent generations. However, there could be scenarios in which it would be beneficial to inherit information about environmental challenges that negatively affected the parental generation and will continue to affect offspring.

One such situation proposes that negative effects, such as the physiological and behavioral responses to stressors that are observed in experimental setups, are not negative to begin with. Consequently, increased stress-responsiveness, decreased resilience or depressive-like behavior may actually be protective traits under the conditions experienced by the parental generation. This reasoning is in agreement with the interpretation of the physiological stress response that Hans Selye advocated when he first published the stress theory (Selye, 1978; Dubrovsky, 2002) and that was later reused by Munck et al. (1984) to underline the protective function of elevated glucocorticoid levels in response to stress. Interestingly, this interpretation also offers an explanation of the apparent “corticosterone-paradox” observed in germ-free mice: While corticosterone levels are generally higher in germ-free mice at baseline and even more exaggerated under stress conditions, they actually show decreased anxiety-like behavior. Accordingly, evolutionary psychiatric researchers such as Nesse (2000) or Stevens and Price (2000) argued for the hypothesis that depression and other psychiatric conditions may actually be evolutionary adaptations with a net increase in human fitness. However, this latter hypothesis has been heavily criticized with respect to the interpretation of evolutionary mechanisms (Dubrovsky, 2002; McLoughlin, 2002) and their relevance to clinical practice.

Alternatively, epigenetic inheritance of stressful events could be pleiotropic, i.e., they are negative side effects of otherwise beneficial mechanisms that provide flexibility and versatility in response to environmental challenges and usually confer an overall net advantage. Similarly, researchers such as Crow and Baron-Cohen advocate a role for pleiotropic effects in mental disorders with Crow suggesting that schizophrenia and psychosis may be atypical patterns of brain hemisphere lateralization and therefore a side-effect of genetic variation that was necessary for the evolution of human language (Crow, 1995, 1997) and Baron-Cohen proposing a theory of autism that explains ASDs as adverse effects of the development of higher cognitive function (“systemizing”) on the cost of empathy-supporting functions (“empathizing”) (Baron-Cohen, 2009). It is tempting to speculate whether also negative side effects of epigenetic inheritance are involved in these conditions.

While it is difficult to empirically test whether either or both of these scenarios are actually at the root of increasing risk for such disorders, these events likely reflect the costs of maintaining a beneficial epigenetic inheritance system that can sometimes go awry.

The Molecular Epigenetic Machinery: An Interface for Microbe-Brain Interactions in Evolution and Development

“THESE IDEAS ARE LIKELY TO HAVE PROFOUND CONSEQUENCES WHEN YOU START TO TALK ABOUT HOW THE STRUCTURE OF SOCIETY INFLUENCES COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT. WE'RE BEGINNING TO DRAW CAUSE-AND-EFFECT ARROWS BETWEEN SOCIAL AND ECONOMIC MACROVARIABLES DOWN TO THE LEVEL OF THE CHILD'S BRAIN”

(Michael Meaney, 2006)

Chromatin Plasticity and Non-Coding RNAs in Neuronal Function and Development

The gut microbiota produces many neuroactive compounds, which can directly affect how neurons communicate with each other. Among these are amino acids, (e.g., GABA and tryptophan), as well as monoamines, such as serotonin, histamine and dopamine, used as neurotransmitters in the brain or precursors of such (Lyte and Freestone, 2010; Lyte, 2011; Thomas et al., 2012a; Wall et al., 2014). In germ-free mice, dopamine and glutamate receptor expression (Sudo et al., 2004; Heijtz et al., 2011; Neufeld et al., 2011) as well as tryptophan metabolism and serotonin levels are significantly altered in the circulation but also in the brain during development (Clarke et al., 2013), suggesting that neurotransmitters and their precursors may be some of the cues that are provided by the microbiota to establish the gut-brain axis as an important regulator of neurodevelopment. However, there is accumulating evidence that also molecular epigenetic mechanisms are involved in shaping brain formation and functioning and that can be influenced by microbial symbionts.

The question of which molecules and molecular mechanisms make up the epigenetic machinery follows the debate about its definition. Even if final proof about cause or consequence with respect to gene regulation is scarce, the majority of the literature now includes mainly three molecular mechanisms to constitute the molecular epigenetic machinery, mediating plastic changes in regulation of nuclear architecture, chromatin structure, and gene expression. Namely, these are histone modifications, such as acetylation or methylation, DNA modifications, such as CpG-methylation or –hydroxymethylation, and regulatory RNAs (Figure 3). These key regulators of gene expression integrate environmental signals and other stimuli at the transcriptional or translational level and can thereby lead to switching the amount of expression of a gene, which is an important factor exposing the effect of polymorphisms to the current environment. This makes epigenetic mechanisms important mediators of gene-environment and genome-microbiome interactions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The epigenetic trio: RNA, DNA and Chromatin modifications. The molecular epigenetic machinery is comprised of DNA, chromatin modifications and several RNA-based mechanisms, most importantly various species of non-coding RNA. Together these mechanisms orchestrate developmental and gene expression patterns. They form a regulatory network, with significant interaction and mutual influences between the different domains (black arrows).

Importantly, all of these processes have also been shown to play an important role in cognitive function during health and disease (for recent reviews see Day and Sweatt, 2011; Kosik et al., 2012; Fischer, 2014a; Woldemichael et al., 2014). As such, long-term memory consolidation and synaptic plasticity greatly depend on dynamic regulation of gene expression in the hippocampus (Igaz et al., 2002, 2004; Da Silva et al., 2008) and roles and interactions of all three mechanisms of the epigenetic machinery have been identified. Histone acetylation is best known for its adjuvant role in learning-induced gene regulation. The catalyzing enzymes (histone acetyltransferases, HATs, and histone deacetylases, HDACs) are well-studied and can be targeted pharmacologically, which makes them promising targets for the treatment of neurodegenerative diseases and cognitive decline (Stilling and Fischer, 2011; Gräff and Tsai, 2013; Fischer, 2014b). The mechanism of action for these treatments supposedly involves the enhancement of the neuron's inherent response toward activation or facilitation of the normal regulatory program where it got out of balance (Peleg et al., 2010; McQuown and Wood, 2011; Stilling and Fischer, 2011).

More recently, also RNA-based plasticity is beginning to emerge as a crucial regulator of neuronal function, brain development, cognition and psychiatric disease (Barry and Mattick, 2012; Qureshi and Mehler, 2012). These include small RNAs such as miRNAs (O'Connor et al., 2012; Saab and Mansuy, 2014) and piRNAs (Landry et al., 2013) but also lncRNAs (Ng et al., 2013; Schaukowitch and Kim, 2014) as well as RNA editing, which primarily affects mRNAs expressed in the brain and has dramatically increased in humans compared to other species, and stimulus-dependent alternative splicing (Schor et al., 2009, 2013). Also mRNA methylation was recently reported to be enriched in brain tissue and considerably increased in the adult brain (Meyer et al., 2012). Importantly, many of these RNA-based processes are not only interacting with each other (Barry and Mattick, 2012) but are also intimately linked to other partners of the epigenetic machinery (Figure 3). For example, lncRNAs can act as sequence-specific guide molecules for histone-modifying enzymes, such as histone methyltransferases (Sanchez-Elsner et al., 2006; Rinn et al., 2007), and chromatin modifications help to determine splicing site selection (Luco et al., 2011; Kornblihtt et al., 2013), to name just a few.

How can the Microbiota Act on Development and Behavior Through Epigenetic Mechanisms?

The idea that epigenetic mechanisms could be key mediators of interactions between hosts and pathogenic or parasitic microorganisms was previously put forward (Minárovits, 2009; Paschos and Allday, 2010; Al Akeel, 2013; Silmon de Monerri and Kim, 2014; Stilling et al., 2014). However, it is clear that similar constructs apply to the interaction between host and non-pathogenic microbiota (Shenderov, 2012; Shenderov and Midtvedt, 2014) and that this may have strong implications for the regulation of brain evolution. In the first instance, it is important to detail some of the potential players in this context.

Short-chain fatty acids

In addition to synthesizing neurotransmitters or precursors, the gut microbiota produces other chemicals with neuro-modulatory potential. As such, fermentation of fiber by gut bacteria is the prime source for short chain fatty acids (SCFAs) such as butyric acid, propionic acid and acetic acid. SCFAs are not neuroactive substances per se but may act on neuronal function more subtly. For example, butyrate is best known for its potent inhibition of HDACs (Candido et al., 1978; Davie, 2003). While we recently reviewed details of the interactions of SCFAs with the epigenetic machinery elsewhere (Stilling et al., 2014), there is now solid new evidence that microbes do have a significant impact on epigenetic regulation in the host's gut epithelium and immune system. The effects were reported to be largely mediated by butyrate and related to altered HDAC activity (Alenghat et al., 2013; Arpaia et al., 2013; Furusawa et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2013; Chang et al., 2014).

In addition, acetic acid will affect the availability of HAT substrate (Acetyl-Coenzyme A) and thereby lead to higher levels of dynamic histone acetylation in neurons, which may be impaired when the normal microbiota is disturbed or completely absent. Together, both mechanisms will lead to increased histone acetylation, which facilitates memory consolidation, neurogenesis and neuroprotection (Fischer et al., 2007, 2010; Kilgore et al., 2010; Peleg et al., 2010; Govindarajan et al., 2011). Notably, intracerebroventricular infusions of propionic acid was shown to induce autistic-like behaviors in rats (MacFabe et al., 2011; Thomas et al., 2012b), suggesting SCFAs are also implicated in modulating social behaviors. Though the effects of SCFAs that cross the blood-brain barrier under normal conditions may be marginal, persistent secretion of SCFAs by the gut microbiota may result in cumulative, long-lasting effects on gene expression patterns that are necessary for appropriate neuronal development and function.

In conclusion, microbial SCFAs are important contributors to host metabolism and thus form a key part of the holometabolome acting as an energy source or through balancing host gene expression throughout brain development and, more dynamically, in adulthood (Selkrig et al., 2014).

Microbial mimicry of the host epigenetic machinery

Today, it is well-documented that microbes can directly target the host's transcriptional regulatory machinery. Especially viruses are known to harness infected cells for their own benefit, which comes to no surprise since they critically depend on their host's molecular machinery for replication and propagation. As such, certain influenza viruses use the host's epigenetic machinery to stimulate their own replication or hide within silenced regions of the hosts genome (Minárovits, 2009). Another mechanism to evade the host cell's antiviral response utilizes the virus-encoded histone-mimicking protein NS1 that mediates transcriptional repression (Marazzi et al., 2012). But also several bacteria can secrete proteins that mimic components of the host epigenetic machinery (Bhavsar et al., 2007; Murata et al., 2007; Hamon and Cossart, 2008; Pennini et al., 2010; Bierne and Cossart, 2012; Bierne et al., 2012; Rennoll-Bankert and Dumler, 2012; Bierne, 2013; Eskandarian et al., 2013; Rolando et al., 2013). Up to now, however, such effectors have been found exclusively in intracellular parasites like Legionella pneumophilia, which have direct contact to the intracellular environment to modulate host-cell transcription. In addition, RNA-based mechanisms targeting host transcriptional regulation have been added to the growing list of microbial effects on host transcription (Liu et al., 2012). This study found that E. coli is capable of producing short non-coding RNAs that act similarly to short interfering RNAs (siRNAs) on some host mRNAs. This interaction does not rely on intracellular localization of the symbiont, yet it is still unclear how the E. coli non-coding RNAs bridge the species barrier to interfere with C. elegans mRNA.

Most research in this regard has focused on parasites, i.e., viruses and bacterial pathogens (Minárovits, 2009; Paschos and Allday, 2010; Al Akeel, 2013; Silmon de Monerri and Kim, 2014), and it remains and open but intriguing question whether pathogens in the brain have means to alter transcription in neurons, which in turn could have an effect on host behavior. Most recently this prediction seems to have realized in the finding that Toxoplasma gondii infection results in DNA-hypomethylation at the promoter of the arginine vasopressin (Avp) gene and therefore increased expression of this gene in the amygdala of infected rats, which was sufficient for inducing the behavior-manipulation phenotype (Hari Dass and Vyas, 2014). Furthermore, it is unclear if similar mechanisms exist in any of the gut microbes. However, these pioneering studies demonstrate the various ways microbes use to interact with the host's epigenome.

In conclusion, there are several potential routes for microbes to interact with host cellular function and even behavior and some of these may be mediated by alterations of the epigenetic gene regulation in the brain.

Confusing Interactions – Who is the Puppet, who the Puppeteer?

“MOST UNFORTUNATELY, IN THE LIVES OF PUPPETS THERE IS ALWAYS A BUT THAT SPOILS EVERYTHING.”

(Carlo Collodi, 1883, The adventures of Pinocchio)

Out of the 5 canonical ways different species can interact with each other (mutualism, commensalism, rivalry, predation and parasitism), parasitism long seemed to have the greatest impact on accelerating evolution. This has been acknowledged by the so-called “Red Queen Hypothesis,” a concept used to describe rapid host-parasite co-evolution by paraphrasing a character of Lewis Carroll's novel Alice's Adventures in Wonderland. Other forms of symbiosis, especially mutualism, have been neglected in this regard, although they now emerge to be just as important (Ezenwa et al., 2012).

However, in many cases it turns out to be challenging to determine where a given microorganism is located on what appears to be a mutualism-parasitism spectrum, unless the co-evolutionary trajectory is fully known. In addition, changes in the environment may revise the nature of the association. For example, it has been hypothesized that Mycobacterium tuberculosis, a well-known pathogen today, may have contributed to human evolution as a beneficial symbiont by providing a source of essential nicotinaminde during meat shortages (Williams and Dunbar, 2014). Further evidence for a strong dependency on environmental conditions determining how a particular microbe is associated with the host comes from a recent study on Helicobacter pylori, that appears to exhibit significantly differing virulence in two independent human populations (Kodaman et al., 2014). Thus, a causal timeline of symbiosis is hard to reconstruct in retrospective and even what appears as obligate mutualism today may not have started as mutually beneficial.

Classification of host-microbe interactions is further complicated by the need to define benefits and disadvantages for either side. As such, it is highly debatable whether strict commensalism actually exists. Also in mutualistic relationships, many cases may not be clear-cut. In such a relationship, both species mutually benefit from one another. For example, while the gut microbiota benefits from a constant supply of nutrients and a relatively high ambient temperature, allowing fast metabolic turnover, the host benefits from increased nutrient availability through enzymatic activity that is not coded in the host genome (e.g., digestion of fiber). But, since these advantages of the relationship may have compensated possible disadvantages during evolution, negative side effects of the association may be ignored or not recognized as detrimental in retrospect today.

In this respect, an exciting question regarding the evolution of social behavior is: Who is the puppet and who the puppeteer in social transmission of microbes? On the one hand, microbes that develop means of faster and more frequent transmission will be positively selected for. This may include co-evolutionary mechanisms and developmental cues, such as described in this article, which lead to increased sociability of a population or species or even direct behavioral manipulation of the individual host. This further relates to the question: What's in it for the microbiota? Why do they provide cues for development, why do they influence behavior? A recent attempt to answer some of these questions has been put forward, arguing that feeding behavior of the host might be manipulated by the special interests of particular microbes residing in the gut (Alcock et al., 2014). Another, simple but provocative explanation, derived from data discussed in this article, is that the bacteria promote social behavior in mammals and group living to more easily spread to new hosts and thereby reproduce more efficiently.

Indeed, fascinating examples of host-microbe interaction are found in parasites that are known to manipulate host behavior in order to reproduce and spread. Researchers find a growing variety of parasites in all domains of life that depend on altering host behavior for completion of their complex life cycles. These include members from a range of phyla such as the small liver fluke Dicrocoelium dendriticum, the zombie-ant fungus Ophiocordyceps unilateralis, or the Gordian worm Paragordius tricuspidatus, but also more common ones such as the rabies virus, which induces aggression as well as water avoidance behavior in mammals, or the ubiquitous protozoan Toxoplasma gondii, best known for inducing attraction of rodents toward cat urine scent, but also implicated in schizophrenia (Berdoy et al., 2000; Webster, 2001; Vyas et al., 2007; Libersat et al., 2009; Cézilly et al., 2010; Thomas et al., 2010; Flegr, 2013; Webster et al., 2013; for further reading see the special issue on neural parasitology in the January 2013 issue of the Journal of Experimental Biology). Interestingly, the underlying neurological mechanisms for these cases have yet to be revealed in detail. Given the very intimate contact with intracellular neuronal processes, it is intriguing to speculate that host-behavior alteration may be achieved by manipulating the transcriptional machinery of host neurons.

However, the answer to why a normal, healthy, and rather non-parasitic microbiota may provide developmental cues that facilitate social behavior may be a lot less spectacular than this. The problem is that we tend to think about this matter within a teleological framework and impose intentions on the bacteria. Furthermore, we tend to forget all eukaryotic evolution has always occurred in the presence of microbiota: Animals have never lived, and could never live, germ-free outside laboratory isolators. From this perspective the question is just the wrong question to ask and there is probably no satisfactory answer to it, because the microbiome just became part of multicellular bodies while these formed and now share a long history of co-evolution together. Given this tight association, it seems that we cannot simply impose opposing intentions for the microbiota and the host, since we also would not do this for genuine host-tissue cells.

Conclusions & Future Perspectives

The Microbiota as “Episymbionts” within a Holobiont

“ALL EVOLUTION IS CO-EVOLUTION”

(Stuart Kauffman, 1995)

In this article, we present potential mechanisms for host-microbe interactions through molecular epigenetic processes and offer arguments to suggest that alterations of the microbiome and epigenetic modifications as well as RNA-based regulation of gene-expression are linked in shaping brain evolution and neurodevelopment. Apart from presenting evidence for rather direct influence on neuroendocrine and neurotransmitter systems, we further suggest that some microbial products can modulate the epigenetic landscape of the host brain. These possibilities include regulators of the activity of histone-modifying enzymes either through metabolic alterations or direct interactions between bacteria-secreted molecules (such as SCFAs) and host signaling pathways. Together these mechanisms provide important and time-critical cues for host neurodevelopment and thereby continue to influence social behavior in a co-evolutionary and co-developmental manner. At the moment, it is unclear if any of the behavioral phenotypes that are associated with altered microbial colonization, including models for ASDs, are due to decreased availability of SCFAs and/or other metabolites with epigenetic modifying function. Evidently, some parasites are able to highjack the host-cell's epigenetic machinery and it may prove worthy to look for similar effectors in symbiotic or commensal bacteria to assess translatable, therapeutic potential of such mechanisms.

Additionally, we argue that the microbiota is a key interface for gene-environment interactions. These interactions may lie at the heart of incompatible genotype-enterotype combinations and supposedly have critical consequences for the study of disease-associated genetic risk factors with yet unrevealed function. Such incompatibilities will also have implications for donor screening in fecal transplantation therapies.

With these functions in mind, we notice that the microbiota also shares some important characteristics in its interaction with the host that compare to classical epigenetic mechanisms such as histone modifications, DNA methylation and ncRNA-mediated regulation. These characteristics include (1) vertical transmission (transgenerational inheritance) of (acquired) microbes, (2) response to environmental stimuli and facilitation of gene-environment interactions, (3) determination of gene expression programs and developmental regulation, and (4) reversibility.

Hence, it is intriguing to look at the canonical epigenetic mechanisms as mediators of developmental signals sent by the microbiota as well as to also consider the symbionts themselves as epigenetic entities, as we and others have previously argued (Gilbert et al., 2010; Fitzpatrick, 2014; Stazi and Toccaceli, 2014; Stilling et al., 2014). However, the microbiome is clearly more complex and dynamic than an epigenetic signal since symbionts have their own genomes that can respond to adaptation where a molecular epigenetic modification itself is not subject to natural selection. Yet, while the analogy may not be perfect in details, this perspective is useful to understand how the microbiome represents a further interface for environmental influence and a dynamic source for transgenerational developmental regulation. It furthermore places the microbiota in line with other mechanisms that accelerate short-term environmental adaptation and may be especially helpful in unifying different theories of host-microbe co-evolution and the evolution of the “social brain.” In fact, this viewpoint may hold some answers to fundamental questions in the fields of epigenetics and neuroepigenetics (Bohacek et al., 2013; Sweatt, 2013). The host-specific microbial composition could be considered as part of the “parental effects” that were suggested to prepare offspring for an unpredictable environment (Cameron et al., 2008; Badyaev and Uller, 2009). Careful experimental design, including cross-fostering and in vitro-fertilization studies, sided by longitudinal experimental evolution studies are necessary to understand how long transgenerational effects may persist and to establish brain-gut-microbe bidirectional communications as a part of a “soft-inheritance” paradigm (Sweatt, 2013).

Harmonization of the Hologenome Theory and Apparent Lamarckism with the Neo-Darwinist Perspective

“ALL THE ACQUISITIONS OR LOSSES WROUGHT BY NATURE ON INDIVIDUALS, THROUGH THE ENVIRONMENT IN WHICH THEIR RACE HAS LONG BEEN PLACED, […] ALL THESE ARE PRESERVED BY REPRODUCTION TO THE NEW INDIVIDUALS WHICH ARISE […]”

(Jean-Baptiste Lamarck, 1809)

This quote summarizes the second law of the Lamarckian theory of evolution, which tried to explain evolutionary change by inheritance of acquired traits during the lifetime of an individual. Recent experimental findings, such as those discussed in this review, as well as the general interest in epigenetic transmission of phenotypic variation have repeatedly urged authors to partly resurrect this theory or at least raise the question, to what extent these Lamarckian aspects contribute to adaptation in a complementary way to the Darwin and Wallace theory of evolution (or, more accurately, the modern synthesis of genetics and evolution), (e.g., Smythies et al., 2014; Szyf, 2014). However, we feel that this perspective on epigenetics is of little avail for two main reasons. First, non-genetic information is ultimately coded by genetic information, i.e., there are genes for epigenetic processes, such as genes coding for histone- or DNA-modifying enzymes. These chromosomal genes clearly underlie the common, accepted Mendelian inheritance patterns and will be subject to natural selection or neutral evolution (drift) just as any other gene. Second, while the ability to pass environmental information to the next generation represents another source of variability that natural selection can act on, it is unlikely that the outcome of an epigenetic inheritance is stabilized and fixed through evolution. Together, these arguments imply that it will rather be the possibility to epigenetically inherit environmental information itself, i.e. the genes that encode the specific epigenetic machinery, that is positively selected for to promote flexibility. We have thus argued here that epigenetic mechanisms should rather be viewed as the evolutionary answer to nature's inherent unpredictability rather than constituting an alternative mode of evolution. This form of meta-adaptation or adaptability, which Mattick has referred to as “evolution has learnt how to learn” (Mattick, 2009), can be defined as the ability of a genetic system to produce and maintain potentially adaptive epigenetic variation and regulation. This definition is derived as an analogy to the concept of evolvability, defined as “the ability of the genetic system to produce and maintain potentially adaptive genetic variants” (Hansen, 2006; Pigliucci, 2008)

Appreciation grows for the fact that animals live in a bacterial world (McFall-Ngai et al., 2013), and no longer can the animal be viewed as separate from the microbes it requires to subsist, reproduce, and evolve over time. One principle that highlights this theme is the hologenome concept, which combines the host's multicellular genome and associated microbiome into a self-contained unit of selection that does not obviate other units of selection (Booth, 2014; Rosenberg and Zilber-Rosenberg, 2014), but recognizes that portions of the genome and microbiome are inseparable for encoding a viable organism and thus subject to co-evolution in similar ways to genes co-evolving within the nuclear genome. Indeed, Fitzpatrick recently identified circumstances under which this premise is fulfilled by deriving a mathematical framework based on ordinary population genetics, which provides further support for inclusion of non-genetic inheritance (such as symbionts, cultural traits and other epigenetic traits) in evolutionary concepts (Fitzpatrick, 2014).

In order to harmonize the different concepts of host-microbe co-dependencies mentioned in this article, it may help to take a gene-centric point of view—and thus consider symbiont genomes as extended chromosomes of the participating genomes within a holobiont. From this point of view, genes promoting the symbiosis by modulating their mutualistic counterpart would be positively selected, if this in turn promotes the overall fitness of the holobiont. The eukaryotic proportion provides a self-sustaining vehicle for the hologenome, while the microbial genes contribute by supplementing available genetic information (e.g., metabolic pathways) but also by modulating development and evolution of the holobiont in response to environmental stimuli.

Closing Remarks

“NO SELF IS OF ITSELF ALONE. […] THE ‘I’ IS CHAINED TO ANCESTRY BY MANY FACTORS […]. THIS IS NOT MERE ALLEGORY, BUT AN ETERNAL MEMORY.”

(Erwin Schrödinger, 1918)

In this theoretical article, we have gathered arguments for an integrated view on brain development and evolution, symbiosis, and RNA biology, which together frame social behavior (Figure 2): Pro-social behavior relies on proper brain development, which requires precisely timed gene expression, orchestrated by epigenetic regulatory mechanisms, such as histone modifications as well as the ncRNAome and other plastic RNA-based mechanisms (Figure 3). Given a potential co-evolution of social behavior in mammals and their microbiomes, brain development is furthermore particularly vulnerable to microbial signals. Gilbert et al. suggested that “we have outsourced certain developmental signals” to our microbiota (Gilbert et al., 2010). However, we need to consider that eukaryotic evolution has probably never seen a period without the presence of microbes, so that these particular developmental signals have never been produced “in-house” but are a result of co-evolution of host and microbiota. Gilbert and colleagues thus have a play on Kaufmann's evolutionary comment in stating that “Almost all development may be co-development” (Gilbert et al., 2010). In fact, together with Lombardo's theory of “access to mutualistic endosymbionts” (Lombardo, 2008), this perspective on brain-gut-microbiota co-development may hold a missing link in the “expensive-tissue hypothesis” (Aiello and Wheeler, 1995), in that it would explain how a socially-transmitted diverse and thus highly efficient microbiota could compensate for decreasing gastrointestinal tract size during primate brain enlargement by increasing nutrient availability from a broader range of sources.

It is now important to determine our psychobiotic endosymbionts, i.e., symbionts with a beneficial effect on mental health and neurodevelopment (Dinan et al., 2013), which constitute a positive relationship during development and which may aid treatment in case of disease. Understanding our long-term relationship with these beneficial friends will also have important implications for future research to provide lifestyle recommendations such as diet, hygiene and behavior in certain critical periods in life, including pregnancy and early education.

We may not yet fully understand the epigenetic potential of our bacterial friends and what their social benefits are, nonetheless we are beginning to appreciate the extent to which host-microbe interactions drive brain evolution and development.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The Alimentary Pharmabiotic Centre is a research centre funded by Science Foundation Ireland (SFI), through the Irish Government's National Development Plan. Roman M. Stilling is supported by the Irish Research Council (IRC) through a Government of Ireland Postdoctoral Fellowship. Seth R. Bordenstein is supported by awards from NIH (R01 GM085163) and NFS (DEB 1046149). Timothy G. Dinan and John F. Cryan are supported by SFI (grant no is 07/CE/B1368 and 12/RC/2273). Timothy G. Dinan and John F. Cryan are also supported by the Irish Health Research Board, Health Research Awards (HRA_POR/2011/23) and (HRA_POR/2012/32).

References

Aiello, L. C., and Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 36, 199–221. doi: 10.1086/204350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al Akeel, R. (2013). Role of epigenetic reprogramming of host genes in bacterial pathogenesis. Saudi J. Biol. Sci. 20, 305–309. doi: 10.1016/j.sjbs.2013.05.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcock, J., Maley, C. C., and Aktipis, C. A. (2014). Is eating behavior manipulated by the gastrointestinal microbiota? Evolutionary pressures and potential mechanisms. Bioessays 36, 940–949. doi: 10.1002/bies.201400071

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alenghat, T., Osborne, L. C., Saenz, S. A., Kobuley, D., Ziegler, C. G. K., Mullican, S. E., et al. (2013). Histone deacetylase 3 coordinates commensal-bacteria-dependent intestinal homeostasis. Nature 504, 153–157. doi: 10.1038/nature12687

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arai, J. A., Li, S., Hartley, D. M., and Feig, L. A. (2009). Transgenerational rescue of a genetic defect in long-term potentiation and memory formation by juvenile enrichment. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 29, 1496–1502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5057-08.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arpaia, N., Campbell, C., Fan, X., Dikiy, S., van der Veeken, J., de Roos, P., et al. (2013). Metabolites produced by commensal bacteria promote peripheral regulatory T-cell generation. Nature 504, 451–455. doi: 10.1038/nature12726

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arumugam, M., Raes, J., Pelletier, E., Le Paslier, D., Yamada, T., Mende, D. R., et al. (2011). Enterotypes of the human gut microbiome. Nature 473, 174–180. doi: 10.1038/nature09944

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ausubel, F. M. (2005). Are innate immune signaling pathways in plants and animals conserved? Nat. Immunol. 6, 973–979. doi: 10.1038/ni1253

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badyaev, A. V., and Uller, T. (2009). Parental effects in ecology and evolution: mechanisms, processes and implications. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1169–1177. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0302

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, M. T., and Coe, C. L. (1999). Maternal separation disrupts the integrity of the intestinal microflora in infant rhesus monkeys. Dev. Psychobiol. 35, 146–155.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, M. T., Dowd, S. E., Galley, J. D., Hufnagle, A. R., Allen, R. G., and Lyte, M. (2011). Exposure to a social stressor alters the structure of the intestinal microbiota: implications for stressor-induced immunomodulation. Brain. Behav. Immun. 25, 397–407. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.10.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 68–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, G. (2014). Integrating the roles of long and small non-coding RNA in brain function and disease. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 410–416. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.196

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, G., and Mattick, J. S. (2012). The role of regulatory RNA in cognitive evolution. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 497–503. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belshaw, R., Pereira, V., Katzourakis, A., Talbot, G., Paces, J., Burt, A., et al. (2004). Long-term reinfection of the human genome by endogenous retroviruses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 4894–4899. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0307800101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, A. K., Kelly, S. A., Legge, R., Ma, F., Low, S. J., Kim, J., et al. (2010). Individuality in gut microbiota composition is a complex polygenic trait shaped by multiple environmental and host genetic factors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 18933–18938. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007028107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berdoy, M., Webster, J. P., and Macdonald, D. W. (2000). Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii. Proc. Biol. Sci. 267, 1591–1594. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1182

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhavsar, A. P., Guttman, J. A., and Finlay, B. B. (2007). Manipulation of host-cell pathways by bacterial pathogens. Nature 449, 827–834. doi: 10.1038/nature06247

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bierne, H. (2013). Nuclear microbiology—bacterial assault on the nucleolus. EMBO Rep. 14, 663–664. doi: 10.1038/embor.2013.105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bierne, H., and Cossart, P. (2012). When bacteria target the nucleus: the emerging family of nucleomodulins. Cell. Microbiol. 14, 622–633. doi: 10.1111/j.1462-5822.2012.01758.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bierne, H., Hamon, M., and Cossart, P. (2012). Epigenetics and bacterial infections. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2:a010272. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a010272

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaxter, M. (2007). Symbiont genes in host genomes: fragments with a future? Cell Host Microbe. 2, 211–213. doi: 10.1016/j.chom.2007.09.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blow, M., Futreal, P. A., Wooster, R., and Stratton, M. R. (2004). A survey of RNA editing in human brain. Genome Res. 14, 2379–2387. doi: 10.1101/gr.2951204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohacek, J., Gapp, K., Saab, B. J., and Mansuy, I. M. (2013). Transgenerational epigenetic effects on brain functions. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 313–320. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, A. (2014). Symbiosis, selection, and individuality. Biol. Philos. 29, 657–673. doi: 10.1007/s10539-014-9449-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borody, T. J., and Khoruts, A. (2012). Fecal microbiota transplantation and emerging applications. Nat. Rev. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 9, 88–96. doi: 10.1038/nrgastro.2011.244

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borre, Y. E., O'Keeffe, G. W., Clarke, G., Stanton, C., Dinan, T. G., and Cryan, J. F. (2014). Microbiota and neurodevelopmental windows: implications for brain disorders. Trends Mol. Med. 20, 509–518. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2014.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boto, L. (2012). Horizontal gene transfer in the acquisition of novel traits by metazoans. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281:20132450. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2450

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brucker, R. M., and Bordenstein, S. R. (2012a). Speciation by symbiosis. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 443–451. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.03.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brucker, R. M., and Bordenstein, S. R. (2012b). The roles of host evolutionary relationships (genus: Nasonia) and development in structuring microbial communities. Evol. Int. J. Org. Evol. 66, 349–362. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01454.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brucker, R. M., and Bordenstein, S. R. (2013a). The capacious hologenome. Zoology (Jena) 116, 260–261. doi: 10.1016/j.zool.2013.08.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brucker, R. M., and Bordenstein, S. R. (2013b). The hologenomic basis of speciation: gut bacteria cause hybrid lethality in the genus Nasonia. Science 341, 667–669. doi: 10.1126/science.1240659

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, C. M., Chu, H., Rueter, S. M., Hutchinson, L. K., Canton, H., Sanders-Bush, E., et al. (1997). Regulation of serotonin-2C receptor G-protein coupling by RNA editing. Nature 387, 303–308. doi: 10.1038/387303a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, X., Lin, P., Jiang, P., and Li, C. (2013). Characteristics of the gastrointestinal microbiome in children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 25, 342–353. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2013.06.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, N. M., Shahrokh, D., Del Corpo, A., Dhir, S. K., Szyf, M., Champagne, F. A., et al. (2008). Epigenetic programming of phenotypic variations in reproductive strategies in the rat through maternal care. J. Neuroendocrinol. 20, 795–801. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01725.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Candido, E. P., Reeves, R., and Davie, J. R. (1978). Sodium butyrate inhibits histone deacetylation in cultured cells. Cell 14, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(78)90305-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cézilly, F., Thomas, F., Médoc, V., and Perrot-Minnot, M.-J. (2010). Host-manipulation by parasites with complex life cycles: adaptive or not? Trends Parasitol. 26, 311–317. doi: 10.1016/j.pt.2010.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, P. V., Hao, L., Offermanns, S., and Medzhitov, R. (2014). The microbial metabolite butyrate regulates intestinal macrophage function via histone deacetylase inhibition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 2247–2252. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322269111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, H., Pamp, S. J., Hill, J. A., Surana, N. K., Edelman, S. M., Troy, E. B., et al. (2012). Gut immune maturation depends on colonization with a host-specific microbiota. Cell 149, 1578–1593. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.04.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claesson, M. J., Jeffery, I. B., Conde, S., Power, S. E., O'Connor, E. M., Cusack, S., et al. (2012). Gut microbiota composition correlates with diet and health in the elderly. Nature 488, 178–184. doi: 10.1038/nature11319

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, G., Grenham, S., Scully, P., Fitzgerald, P., Moloney, R. D., Shanahan, F., et al. (2013). The microbiome-gut-brain axis during early life regulates the hippocampal serotonergic system in a sex-dependent manner. Mol. Psychiatry 18, 666–673. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.77

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crow, T. J. (1995). Aetiology of schizophrenia: an evolutionary theory. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 10(Suppl. 3), 49–56.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Crow, T. J. (1997). Is schizophrenia the price that Homo sapiens pays for language? Schizophr. Res. 28, 127–141. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00110-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crumeyrolle-Arias, M., Jaglin, M., Bruneau, A., Vancassel, S., Cardona, A., Daugé, V., et al. (2014). Absence of the gut microbiota enhances anxiety-like behavior and neuroendocrine response to acute stress in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 42, 207–217. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cryan, J. F., and Dinan, T. G. (2012). Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 701–712. doi: 10.1038/nrn3346

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cryan, J. F., and O'Mahony, S. M. (2011). The microbiome-gut-brain axis: from bowel to behavior. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. Off. J. Eur. Gastrointest. Motil. Soc. 23, 187–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2010.01664.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, C., and Moran, N. A. (2006). Molecular interactions between bacterial symbionts and their hosts. Cell 126, 453–465. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.07.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Da Silva, W. C., Bonini, J. S., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Medina, J. H., Izquierdo, I., and Cammarota, M. (2008). Inhibition of mRNA synthesis in the hippocampus impairs consolidation and reconsolidation of spatial memory. Hippocampus 18, 29–39. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20362

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dave, M., Higgins, P. D., Middha, S., and Rioux, K. P. (2012). The human gut microbiome: current knowledge, challenges, and future directions. Transl. Res. J. Lab. Clin. Med. 160, 246–257. doi: 10.1016/j.trsl.2012.05.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davie, J. R. (2003). Inhibition of histone deacetylase activity by butyrate. J. Nutr. 133, 2485S–2493S.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawkins, R. (1983). The Extended Phenotype: The Long Reach of the Gene. Oxford; New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Day, J. J., and Sweatt, J. D. (2011). Epigenetic mechanisms in cognition. Neuron 70, 813–829. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derrien, T., Johnson, R., Bussotti, G., Tanzer, A., Djebali, S., Tilgner, H., et al. (2012). The GENCODE v7 catalog of human long noncoding RNAs: analysis of their gene structure, evolution, and expression. Genome Res. 22, 1775–1789. doi: 10.1101/gr.132159.111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desbonnet, L., Clarke, G., Shanahan, F., Dinan, T. G., and Cryan, J. F. (2014). Microbiota is essential for social development in the mouse. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 146–148. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.65

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dethlefsen, L., McFall-Ngai, M., and Relman, D. A. (2007). An ecological and evolutionary perspective on human-microbe mutualism and disease. Nature 449, 811–818. doi: 10.1038/nature06245

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias, B. G., and Ressler, K. J. (2014). Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 89–96. doi: 10.1038/nn.3594

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinan, T. G., Stanton, C., and Cryan, J. F. (2013). Psychobiotics: a novel class of psychotropic. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 720–726. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in light of evolution. Am. Biol. Teach. 35, 125–129. doi: 10.2307/4444260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domazet-Loso, T., and Tautz, D. (2008). An ancient evolutionary origin of genes associated with human genetic diseases. Mol. Biol. Evol. 25, 2699–2707. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msn214

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, A. E. (2014). Symbiosis as a general principle in eukaryotic evolution. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 6:a016113. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a016113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dracheva, S., Elhakem, S. L., Marcus, S. M., Siever, L. J., McGurk, S. R., and Haroutunian, V. (2003). RNA editing and alternative splicing of human serotonin 2C receptor in schizophrenia. J. Neurochem. 87, 1402–1412. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.02115.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubrovsky, B. (2002). Evolutionary psychiatry. adaptationist and nonadaptationist conceptualizations. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 26, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(01)00243-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dulac, C. (2010). Brain function and chromatin plasticity. Nature 465, 728–735. doi: 10.1038/nature09231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evol. Anthropol. 6, 178–190.

Google Scholar

Dunning Hotopp, J. C., Clark, M. E., Oliveira, D. C., Foster, J. M, Fischer, P., Muñoz Torres, M. C., et al. (2007). Widespread lateral gene transfer from intracellular bacteria to multicellular eukaryotes. Science 317, 1753–1756. doi: 10.1126/science.1142490

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eskandarian, H. A., Impens, F., Nahori, M.-A., Soubigou, G., Coppée, J.-Y., Cossart, P., et al. (2013). A role for SIRT2-dependent histone H3K18 deacetylation in bacterial infection. Science 341:1238858. doi: 10.1126/science.1238858

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ezenwa, V. O., Gerardo, N. M., Inouye, D. W., Medina, M., and Xavier, J. B. (2012). Animal behavior and the microbiome. Science 338, 198–199. doi: 10.1126/science.1227412

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, A. (2014a). Epigenetic memory: the lamarckian brain. EMBO J. 33, 945–967. doi: 10.1002/embj.201387637

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, A. (2014b). Targeting histone-modifications in Alzheimer's disease. What is the evidence that this is a promising therapeutic avenue? Neuropharmacology 80C, 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.01.038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, A., Sananbenesi, F., Mungenast, A., and Tsai, L.-H. (2010). Targeting the correct HDAC(s) to treat cognitive disorders. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 31, 605–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2010.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, A., Sananbenesi, F., Wang, X., Dobbin, M., and Tsai, L.-H. (2007). Recovery of learning and memory is associated with chromatin remodelling. Nature 447, 178–182. doi: 10.1038/nature05772

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, B. M. (2014). Symbiote transmission and maintenance of extra-genomic associations. Front. Microbiol. 5:46. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2014.00046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flegr, J. (2013). Influence of latent Toxoplasma infection on human personality, physiology and morphology: pros and cons of the Toxoplasma-human model in studying the manipulation hypothesis. J. Exp. Biol. 216(Pt 1), 127–133. doi: 10.1242/jeb.073635

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsythe, P., and Kunze, W. A. (2013). Voices from within: gut microbes and the CNS. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 70, 55–69. doi: 10.1007/s00018-012-1028-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, D. N., and Pace, N. R. (2008). Gastrointestinal microbiology enters the metagenomics era. Curr. Opin. Gastroenterol. 24, 4–10. doi: 10.1097/MOG.0b013e3282f2b0e8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, T. B., Russig, H., Weiss, I. C., Gräff, J., Linder, N., Michalon, A., et al. (2010). Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzenburg, S., Walter, J., Künzel, S., Wang, J., Baines, J. F., Bosch, T. C. G., et al. (2013). Distinct antimicrobial peptide expression determines host species-specific bacterial associations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, E3730–E3738. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1304960110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraune, S., and Bosch, T. C. G. (2007). Long-term maintenance of species-specific bacterial microbiota in the basal metazoan Hydra. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 13146–13151. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703375104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funkhouser, L. J., and Bordenstein, S. R. (2013). Mom knows best: the universality of maternal microbial transmission. PLoS Biol. 11:e1001631. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001631

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furusawa, Y., Obata, Y., Fukuda, S., Endo, T. A., Nakato, G., Takahashi, D., et al. (2013). Commensal microbe-derived butyrate induces the differentiation of colonic regulatory T cells. Nature 504, 446–450. doi: 10.1038/nature12721

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gapp, K., Jawaid, A., Sarkies, P., Bohacek, J., Pelczar, P., Prados, J., et al. (2014). Implication of sperm RNAs in transgenerational inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 667–669. doi: 10.1038/nn.3695

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, S. F., McDonald, E., Boyle, N., Buttino, N., Gyi, L., Mai, M., et al. (2010). Symbiosis as a source of selectable epigenetic variation: taking the heat for the big guy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365, 671–678. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0245

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Govindarajan, N., Agis-Balboa, R. C., Walter, J., Sananbenesi, F., and Fischer, A. (2011). Sodium butyrate improves memory function in an Alzheimer's disease mouse model when administered at an advanced stage of disease progression. J. Alzheimers Dis. 26, 187–197. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110080

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gräff, J., and Tsai, L.-H. (2013). The potential of HDAC inhibitors as cognitive enhancers. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 53, 311–330. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-011112-140216

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. J. Theor. Biol. 7, 17–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90039-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamon, M. A., and Cossart, P. (2008). Histone modifications and chromatin remodeling during bacterial infections. Cell Host Microbe 4, 100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.chom.2008.07.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, T. F. (2006). The evolution of genetic architecture. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 37, 123–157. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.37.091305.110224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari Dass, S. A., and Vyas, A. (2014). Toxoplasma gondii infection reduces predator aversion in rats through epigenetic modulation in the host medial amygdala. Mol. Ecol. doi: 10.1111/mec.12888. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heijtz, R. D., Wang, S., Anuar, F., Qian, Y., Björkholm, B., Samuelsson, A., et al. (2011). Normal gut microbiota modulates brain development and behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3047–3052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010529108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holliday, R. (1987). The inheritance of epigenetic defects. Science 238, 163–170. doi: 10.1126/science.3310230

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hrvoj-Mihic, B., Bienvenu, T., Stefanacci, L., Muotri, A. R., and Semendeferi, K. (2013). Evolution, development, and plasticity of the human brain: from molecules to bones. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:707. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00707

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, E. Y., McBride, S. W., Hsien, S., Sharon, G., Hyde, E. R., McCue, T., et al. (2013). Microbiota modulate behavioral and physiological abnormalities associated with neurodevelopmental disorders. Cell 155, 1451–1463. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Human Microbiome Project Consortium. (2012). Structure, function and diversity of the healthy human microbiome. Nature 486, 207–214. doi: 10.1038/nature11234

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Igaz, L. M., Bekinschtein, P., Vianna, M. M. R., Izquierdo, I., and Medina, J. H. (2004). Gene expression during memory formation. Neurotox. Res. 6, 189–204. doi: 10.1007/BF03033221

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igaz, L. M., Vianna, M. R. M., Medina, J. H., and Izquierdo, I. (2002). Two time periods of hippocampal mRNA synthesis are required for memory consolidation of fear-motivated learning. J. Neurosci. 22, 6781–6789.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R. (2010). The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron 65, 768–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khatri, N., and Man, H.-Y. (2013). Synaptic activity and bioenergy homeostasis: implications in brain trauma and neurodegenerative diseases. Front. Neurol. 4:199. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00199

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilgore, M., Miller, C. A., Fass, D. M., Hennig, K. M., Haggarty, S. J., Sweatt, J. D., et al. (2010). Inhibitors of class 1 histone deacetylases reverse contextual memory deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 35, 870–880. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.197

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kodaman, N., Pazos, A., Schneider, B. G., Piazuelo, M. B., Mera, R., Sobota, R. S., et al. (2014). Human and Helicobacter pylori coevolution shapes the risk of gastric disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 1455–1460. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318093111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, J. E., Spor, A., Scalfone, N., Fricker, A. D., Stombaugh, J., Knight, R., et al. (2011). Succession of microbial consortia in the developing infant gut microbiome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108(Suppl. 1), 4578–4585. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000081107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koren, O., Goodrich, J. K., Cullender, T. C., Spor, A., Laitinen, K., Bäckhed, H. K., et al. (2012). Host remodeling of the gut microbiome and metabolic changes during pregnancy. Cell 150, 470–480. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.07.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornblihtt, A. R., Schor, I. E., Alló, M., Dujardin, G., Petrillo, E., and Muñoz, M. J. (2013). Alternative splicing: a pivotal step between eukaryotic transcription and translation. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 14, 153–165. doi: 10.1038/nrm3525

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosik, K. S., Rapp, P. R., Raz, N., Small, S. A., Sweatt, J. D., and Tsai, L.-H. (2012). Mechanisms of age-related cognitive change and targets for intervention: epigenetics. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 741–746. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovacs, A., Ben-Jacob, N., Tayem, H., Halperin, E., Iraqi, F. A., and Gophna, U. (2011). Genotype is a stronger determinant than sex of the mouse gut microbiota. Microb. Ecol. 61, 423–428. doi: 10.1007/s00248-010-9787-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landry, C. D., Kandel, E. R., and Rajasethupathy, P. (2013). New mechanisms in memory storage: piRNAs and epigenetics. Trends Neurosci. 36, 535–542. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.05.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., et al. (2011). City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 474, 498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature10190

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledford, H. (2008). Language: disputed definitions. Nature 455, 1023–1028. doi: 10.1038/4551023a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. K., and Mazmanian, S. K. (2014). Microbial learning lessons: SFB educate the immune system. Immunity 40, 457–459. doi: 10.1016/j.immuni.2014.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Jia, H., Cai, X., Zhong, H., Feng, Q., Sunagawa, S., et al. (2014). An integrated catalog of reference genes in the human gut microbiome. Nat. Biotechnol. 32, 834–841. doi: 10.1038/nbt.2942

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Bammann, H., Li, M., Liang, H., Yan, Z., Phoebe Chen, Y.-P., et al. (2013). Evolutionary and ontogenetic changes in RNA editing in human, chimpanzee, and macaque brains. RNA 19, 1693–1702. doi: 10.1261/rna.039206.113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libersat, F., Delago, A., and Gal, R. (2009). Manipulation of host behavior by parasitic insects and insect parasites. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 54, 189–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.54.110807.090556

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, L., Shen, S., Jiang, P., Sato, S., Davidson, B. L., and Xing, Y. (2010). Evolution of alternative splicing in primate brain transcriptomes. Hum. Mol. Genet. 19, 2958–2973. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddq201

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., Wang, X., Wang, H.-D., Wu, J., Ren, J., Meng, L., et al. (2012). Escherichia coli noncoding RNAs can affect gene expression and physiology of Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat. Commun. 3, 1073. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2071

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lombardo, M. P. (2008). Access to mutualistic endosymbiotic microbes: an underappreciated benefit of group living. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 479–497. doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0428-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luckey, T. D. (1972). Introduction to intestinal microecology. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 25, 1292–1294.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Luco, R. F., Allo, M., Schor, I. E., Kornblihtt, A. R., and Misteli, T. (2011). Epigenetics in alternative pre-mRNA splicing. Cell 144, 16–26. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.11.056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukas, M., Toth, I., Veenema, A. H., and Neumann, I. D. (2013). Oxytocin mediates rodent social memory within the lateral septum and the medial amygdala depending on the relevance of the social stimulus: male juvenile versus female adult conspecifics. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 916–926. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.09.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyte, M. (2011). Probiotics function mechanistically as delivery vehicles for neuroactive compounds: microbial endocrinology in the design and use of probiotics. Bioessays 33, 574–581. doi: 10.1002/bies.201100024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyte, M., and Freestone, P. P. E. (2010). Microbial Endocrinology Interkingdom Signaling in Infectious Disease and Health. New York, NY: Springer Available online at: http://public.eblib.com/EBLPublic/PublicView.do?ptiID=571526 [Accessed October 20, 2013].

MacFabe, D. F., Cain, N. E., Boon, F., Ossenkopp, K.-P., and Cain, D. P. (2011). Effects of the enteric bacterial metabolic product propionic acid on object-directed behavior, social behavior, cognition, and neuroinflammation in adolescent rats: relevance to autism spectrum disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 217, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marazzi, I., Ho, J. S. Y., Kim, J., Manicassamy, B., Dewell, S., Albrecht, R. A., et al. (2012). Suppression of the antiviral response by an influenza histone mimic. Nature 483, 428–433. doi: 10.1038/nature10892

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, T. M., Wall, R., Ross, R. P., Fitzgserald, G. F., Ryan, C. A., and Stanton, C. (2010). Programming infant gut microbiota: influence of dietary and environmental factors. Curr. Opin. Biotechnol. 21, 149–156. doi: 10.1016/j.copbio.2010.03.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, M. R., Nagaraja, H. N., Camerlengo, T., Joshi, V., and Kumar, P. S. (2013). Deep sequencing identifies ethnicity-specific bacterial signatures in the oral microbiome. PLoS ONE 8:e77287. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077287

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattick, J. S. (2009). Has evolution learnt how to learn? EMBO Rep. 10, 665. doi: 10.1038/embor.2009.135

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McFall-Ngai, M., Hadfield, M. G., Bosch, T. C. G., Carey, H. V., Domazet-Lošo, T., Douglas, A. E., et al. (2013). Animals in a bacterial world, a new imperative for the life sciences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 3229–3236. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1218525110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLoughlin, G. (2002). Is depression normal in human beings? A critique of the evolutionary perspective. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 11, 170–173. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-0979.2002.00244.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McQuown, S. C., and Wood, M. A. (2011). HDAC3 and the molecular brake pad hypothesis. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.04.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meaney, M. J., and Szyf, M. (2005). Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 7, 103–123.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellios, N., and Sur, M. (2012). The emerging role of microRNAs in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Front. Psychiatry 3:39. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, K. D., Saletore, Y., Zumbo, P., Elemento, O., Mason, C. E., and Jaffrey, S. R. (2012). Comprehensive analysis of mRNA methylation reveals enrichment in 3′ UTRs and near stop codons. Cell 149, 1635–1646. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.05.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer-Lindenberg, A., and Tost, H. (2012). Neural mechanisms of social risk for psychiatric disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 663–668. doi: 10.1038/nn.3083

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mika, F., and Hengge, R. (2013). Small Regulatory RNAs in the control of motility and biofilm formation in E. coli and Salmonella. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 14, 4560–4579. doi: 10.3390/ijms14034560

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minárovits, J. (2009). Microbe-induced epigenetic alterations in host cells: the coming era of patho-epigenetics of microbial infections. A review. Acta Microbiol. Immunol. Hung. 56, 1–19. doi: 10.1556/AMicr.56.2009.1.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ming, X., Stein, T. P., Barnes, V., Rhodes, N., and Guo, L. (2012). Metabolic perturbance in autism spectrum disorders: a metabolomics study. J. Proteome Res. 11, 5856–5862. doi: 10.1021/pr300910n

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moeller, A. H., Degnan, P. H., Pusey, A. E., Wilson, M. L., Hahn, B. H., and Ochman, H. (2012). Chimpanzees and humans harbour compositionally similar gut enterotypes. Nat. Commun. 3, 1179. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2159

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montiel-Castro, Augusto, J., Báez-Yáñez, Mario, G., and Pacheco-López, G. (2014). Social neuroeconomics: the influence of microbiota in partner-choice and sociality. Curr. Pharm. Des. 20, 4774–4783. doi: 10.2174/1381612820666140130210631

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montiel-Castro, A. J., González-Cervantes, R. M., Bravo-Ruiseco, G., and Pacheco-López, G. (2013). The microbiota–gut–brain axis: neurobehavioral correlates, health and sociality. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:70. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00070

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, C., and Leutz, A. (2001). Chromatin remodeling in development and differentiation. Curr. Opin. Genet. Dev. 11, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/S0959-437X(00)00175-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munck, A., Guyre, P. M., and Holbrook, N. J. (1984). Physiological functions of glucocorticoids in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions. Endocr. Rev. 5, 25–44. doi: 10.1210/edrv-5-1-25

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murata, K., Fox-Talbot, K., Qian, Z., Takahashi, K., Stahl, G. L., Baldwin, W. M., et al. (2007). Synergistic Deposition of C4d by complement-activating and non-activating antibodies in cardiac transplants. Am. J. Transplant. 7, 2605–2614. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2007.01971.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, A., van Schaik, C. P., and Isler, K. (2011). Energetics and the evolution of human brain size. Nature 480, 91–93. doi: 10.1038/nature10629

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation? Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 14–20. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neufeld, K. M., Kang, N., Bienenstock, J., and Foster, J. A. (2011). Reduced anxiety-like behavior and central neurochemical change in germ-free mice. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 23, 255, e119. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2010.01620.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, S.-Y., Lin, L., Soh, B. S., and Stanton, L. W. (2013). Long noncoding RNAs in development and disease of the central nervous system. Trends Genet. 29, 461–468. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2013.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niu, Y., Zhao, X., Wu, Y.-S., Li, M.-M., Wang, X.-J., and Yang, Y.-G. (2013). N6-methyl-adenosine (m6A) in RNA: an old modification with a novel epigenetic function. Genomics Proteomics Bioinformatics 11, 8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.gpb.2012.12.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, R. M., Dinan, T. G., and Cryan, J. F. (2012). Little things on which happiness depends: microRNAs as novel therapeutic targets for the treatment of anxiety and depression. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 359–376. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.162

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Mahony, S. M., Marchesi, J. R., Scully, P., Codling, C., Ceolho, A.-M., Quigley, E. M. M., et al. (2009). Early life stress alters behavior, immunity, and microbiota in rats: implications for irritable bowel syndrome and psychiatric illnesses. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 263–267. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overballe-Petersen, S., and Willerslev, E. (2014). Horizontal transfer of short and degraded DNA has evolutionary implications for microbes and eukaryotic sexual reproduction. Bioessays 36, 1005–1010. doi: 10.1002/bies.201400035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantoja-Feliciano, I. G., Clemente, J. C., Costello, E. K., Perez, M. E., Blaser, M. J., Knight, R., et al. (2013). Biphasic assembly of the murine intestinal microbiota during early development. ISME J. 7, 1112–1115. doi: 10.1038/ismej.2013.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parent, A., and Carpenter, M. B. (1996). Carpenter's Human Neuroanatomy. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.

Paschos, K., and Allday, M. J. (2010). Epigenetic reprogramming of host genes in viral and microbial pathogenesis. Trends Microbiol. 18, 439–447. doi: 10.1016/j.tim.2010.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, M. S., and Bass, B. L. (1998). Inosine exists in mRNA at tissue-specific levels and is most abundant in brain mRNA. EMBO J. 17, 1120–1127. doi: 10.1093/emboj/17.4.1120

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peleg, S., Sananbenesi, F., Zovoilis, A., Burkhardt, S., Bahari-Javan, S., Agis-Balboa, R. C., et al. (2010). Altered histone acetylation is associated with age-dependent memory impairment in mice. Science 328, 753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1186088

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennini, M. E., Perrinet, S., Dautry-Varsat, A., and Subtil, A. (2010). Histone methylation by NUE, a novel nuclear effector of the intracellular pathogen chlamydia trachomatis. PLoS Pathog 6:e1000995. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.1000995

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pigliucci, M. (2008). Is evolvability evolvable? Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 75–82. doi: 10.1038/nrg2278

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ptashne, M. (2007). On the use of the word “epigenetic.” Curr. Biol. 17, R233–R236. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ptashne, M. (2013a). Epigenetics: core misconcept. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 7101–7103. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305399110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ptashne, M. (2013b). Faddish stuff: epigenetics and the inheritance of acquired characteristics. FASEB J. Off. Publ. Fed. Am. Soc. Exp. Biol. 27, 1–2. doi: 10.1096/fj.13-0101ufm

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, J., Li, R., Raes, J., Arumugam, M., Burgdorf, K. S., Manichanh, C., et al. (2010). A human gut microbial gene catalogue established by metagenomic sequencing. Nature 464, 59–65. doi: 10.1038/nature08821

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qureshi, I. A., and Mehler, M. F. (2012). Emerging roles of non-coding RNAs in brain evolution, development, plasticity and disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 528–541. doi: 10.1038/nrn3234

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rausch, P., Rehman, A., Künzel, S., Häsler, R., Ott, S. J., Schreiber, S., et al. (2011). Colonic mucosa-associated microbiota is influenced by an interaction of Crohn disease and FUT2 (Secretor) genotype. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 19030–19035. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1106408108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravel, J., Gajer, P., Abdo, Z., Schneider, G. M., Koenig, S. S. K., McCulle, S. L., et al. (2011). Vaginal microbiome of reproductive-age women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108(Suppl. 1), 4680–4687. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002611107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rennoll-Bankert, K. E., and Dumler, J. S. (2012). Lessons from Anaplasma phagocytophilum: chromatin remodeling by bacterial effectors. Infect. Disord. Drug Targets 12, 380–387. doi: 10.2174/187152612804142242

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinn, J. L., Kertesz, M., Wang, J. K., Squazzo, S. L., Xu, X., Brugmann, S. A., et al. (2007). Functional demarcation of active and silent chromatin domains in human HOX loci by noncoding RNAs. Cell 129, 1311–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.05.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolando, M., Sanulli, S., Rusniok, C., Gomez-Valero, L., Bertholet, C., Sahr, T., et al. (2013). Legionella pneumophila effector roma uniquely modifies host chromatin to repress gene expression and promote intracellular bacterial replication. Cell Host Microbe 13, 395–405. doi: 10.1016/j.chom.2013.03.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, K. M., Sieber, K. B., and Dunning Hotopp, J. C. (2013). A review of bacteria-animal lateral gene transfer may inform our understanding of diseases like cancer. PLoS Genet. 9:e1003877. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003877

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, E., Koren, O., Reshef, L., Efrony, R., and Zilber-Rosenberg, I. (2007). The role of microorganisms in coral health, disease and evolution. Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 5, 355–362. doi: 10.1038/nrmicro1635

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, E., and Zilber-Rosenberg, I. (2014). The Hologenome Concept: Human, Animal and Plant Microbiota. Cham; New York, NY: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Saab, B. J., and Mansuy, I. M. (2014). Neuroepigenetics of memory formation and impairment: the role of microRNAs. Neuropharmacology 80C, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.01.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salzberg, S. L., White, O., Peterson, J., and Eisen, J. A. (2001). Microbial genes in the human genome: lateral transfer or gene loss? Science 292, 1903–1906. doi: 10.1126/science.1061036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez-Elsner, T., Gou, D., Kremmer, E., and Sauer, F. (2006). Noncoding RNAs of trithorax response elements recruit drosophila ash1 to ultrabithorax. Science 311, 1118–1123. doi: 10.1126/science.1117705

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaukowitch, K., and Kim, T.-K. (2014). Emerging epigenetic mechanisms of long non-coding RNAs. Neuroscience 264, 25–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellekens, H., Clarke, G., Jeffery, I. B., Dinan, T. G., and Cryan, J. F. (2012). Dynamic 5-HT2C receptor editing in a mouse model of obesity. PLoS ONE 7:e32266. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032266

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schloissnig, S., Arumugam, M., Sunagawa, S., Mitreva, M., Tap, J., Zhu, A., et al. (2013). Genomic variation landscape of the human gut microbiome. Nature 493, 45–50. doi: 10.1038/nature11711

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schor, I. E., Fiszbein, A., Petrillo, E., and Kornblihtt, A. R. (2013). Intragenic epigenetic changes modulate NCAM alternative splicing in neuronal differentiation. EMBO J. 32, 2264–2274. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2013.167

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schor, I. E., Rascovan, N., Pelisch, F., Alló, M., and Kornblihtt, A. R. (2009). Neuronal cell depolarization induces intragenic chromatin modifications affecting NCAM alternative splicing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 4325–4330. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810666106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selkrig, J., Wong, P., Zhang, X., and Pettersson, S. (2014). Metabolic tinkering by the gut microbiome: implications for brain development and function. Gut Microbes 5, 369–380. doi: 10.4161/gmic.28681

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1978). The Stress of Life, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Shenderov, B. A. (2012). Gut indigenous microbiota and epigenetics. Microb. Ecol. Health Dis. 23. doi: 10.3402/mehd.v23i0.17195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenderov, B. A., and Midtvedt, T. (2014). Epigenomic programing: a future way to health? Microb. Ecol. Health Dis. 25. doi: 10.3402/mehd.v25.24145

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silmon de Monerri, N. C., and Kim, K. (2014). Pathogens hijack the epigenome: a new twist on host-pathogen interactions. Am. J. Pathol. 184, 897–911. doi: 10.1016/j.ajpath.2013.12.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skuse, D. H., Lori, A., Cubells, J. F., Lee, I., Conneely, K. N., Puura, K., et al. (2014). Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with human social recognition skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 1987–1992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302985111

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. M., Howitt, M. R., Panikov, N., Michaud, M., Gallini, C. A., Bohlooly, -Y. M., et al. (2013). The microbial metabolites, short-chain fatty acids, regulate colonic treg cell homeostasis. Science 341, 569–573. doi: 10.1126/science.1241165

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smythies, J., Edelstein, L., and Ramachandran, V. (2014). Molecular mechanisms for the inheritance of acquired characteristics—exosomes, microRNA shuttling, fear and stress: lamarck resurrected? Epigenomics Epigenetics 5:133. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2014.00133

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speakman, J. R. (2013). Functional analysis of seven genes linked to body mass index and adiposity by genome-wide association studies: a review. Hum. Hered. 75, 57–79. doi: 10.1159/000353585

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spor, A., Koren, O., and Ley, R. (2011). Unravelling the effects of the environment and host genotype on the gut microbiome. Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 9, 279–290. doi: 10.1038/nrmicro2540

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stazi, M. A., and Toccaceli, V. (2014). [Genome and microbiome: hologenome. Is epigenetics their link?]. Epidemiol. Prev. 38, 67–68.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Stecher, B., Denzler, R., Maier, L., Bernet, F., Sanders, M. J., Pickard, D. J., et al. (2012). Gut inflammation can boost horizontal gene transfer between pathogenic and commensal Enterobacteriaceae. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 1269–1274. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113246109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steffen, P. A., and Ringrose, L. (2014). What are memories made of? how polycomb and trithorax proteins mediate epigenetic memory. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 15, 340–356. doi: 10.1038/nrm3789

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, A., and Price, J. (2000). Evolutionary Psychiatry, A New Beginning, 2nd Edn. London; Philadelphia, PA: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Stilling, R. M., Dinan, T. G., and Cryan, J. F. (2014). Microbial genes, brain & behaviour - epigenetic regulation of the gut-brain axis. Genes Brain Behav. 13, 69–86. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stilling, R. M., and Fischer, A. (2011). The role of histone acetylation in age-associated memory impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudo, N., Chida, Y., Aiba, Y., Sonoda, J., Oyama, N., Yu, X.-N., et al. (2004). Postnatal microbial colonization programs the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system for stress response in mice. J. Physiol. 558, 263–275. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.063388

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweatt, J. D. (2013). The emerging field of neuroepigenetics. Neuron 80, 624–632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szyf, M. (2014). Lamarck revisited: epigenetic inheritance of ancestral odor fear conditioning. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 2–4. doi: 10.1038/nn.3603

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, C. M., Hong, T., van Pijkeren, J. P., Hemarajata, P., Trinh, D. V., Hu, W., et al. (2012a). Histamine derived from probiotic Lactobacillus reuteri suppresses TNF via modulation of PKA and ERK signaling. PLoS ONE 7:e31951. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031951

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, F., Poulin, R., and Brodeur, J. (2010). Host manipulation by parasites: a multidimensional phenomenon. Oikos 119, 1217–1223. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.18077.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, R. H., Meeking, M. M., Mepham, J. R., Tichenoff, L., Possmayer, F., Liu, S., et al. (2012b). The enteric bacterial metabolite propionic acid alters brain and plasma phospholipid molecular species: further development of a rodent model of autism spectrum disorders. J. Neuroinflammation 9:153. doi: 10.1186/1742-2094-9-153

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, M., McHardy, I., Ruegger, P., Goudarzi, M., Kashyap, P. C., Haritunians, T., et al. (2014). Reprograming of gut microbiome energy metabolism by the FUT2 Crohn's disease risk polymorphism. ISME J. doi: 10.1038/ismej.2014.64. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, K. (1984). Microbes, herbivory and the evolution of social behavior. J. Theor. Biol. 106, 157–169. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(84)90016-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turnbaugh, P. J., Ley, R. E., Hamady, M., Fraser-Liggett, C. M., Knight, R., and Gordon, J. I. (2007). The human microbiome project. Nature 449, 804–810. doi: 10.1038/nature06244

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turnbaugh, P. J., Ley, R. E., Mahowald, M. A., Magrini, V., Mardis, E. R., and Gordon, J. I. (2006). An obesity-associated gut microbiome with increased capacity for energy harvest. Nature 444, 1027–1031. doi: 10.1038/nature05414

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van De Vondervoort, I. I. G. M., Gordebeke, P. M., Khoshab, N., Tiesinga, P. H. E., Buitelaar, J. K., Kozicz, T., et al. (2013). Long non-coding RNAs in neurodevelopmental disorders. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 6:53. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2013.00053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Virgin, H. W. (2014). The virome in mammalian physiology and disease. Cell 157, 142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyas, A., Kim, S. K., Giacomini, N., Boothroyd, J. C., and Sapolsky, R. M. (2007). Behavioral changes induced by Toxoplasma infection of rodents are highly specific to aversion of cat odors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 6442–6447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608310104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wacklin, P., Tuimala, J., Nikkilä, J., Sebastian, T., Mäkivuokko, H., Alakulppi, N., et al. (2014). Faecal microbiota composition in adults is associated with the FUT2 gene determining the secretor status. PLoS ONE 9:e94863. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094863

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waddington, C. H. (1953). Epigenetics and evolution. Symp. Soc. Exp. Biol. 7, 186–199.

Wall, R., Cryan, J. F., Ross, R. P., Fitzgerald, G. F., Dinan, T. G., and Stanton, C. (2014). Bacterial neuroactive compounds produced by psychobiotics. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 817, 221–239. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-0897-4_10

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, D. L., and Iglesias, T. L. (2012). No evidence for the “expensive-tissue hypothesis” from an intraspecific study in a highly variable species. J. Evol. Biol. 25, 1226–1231. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02503.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D'Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 847–854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, J. P. (2001). Rats, cats, people and parasites: the impact of latent toxoplasmosis on behaviour. Microbes Infect. 3, 1037–1045. doi: 10.1016/S1286-4579(01)01459-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, J. P., Kaushik, M., Bristow, G. C., and McConkey, G. A. (2013). Toxoplasma gondii infection, from predation to schizophrenia: can animal behaviour help us understand human behaviour? J. Exp. Biol. 216(Pt 1), 99–112. doi: 10.1242/jeb.074716

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. C., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Big brains, meat, tuberculosis and the nicotinamide switches: co-evolutionary relationships with modern repercussions on longevity and disease? Med. Hypotheses 83, 79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2014.04.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woldemichael, B. T., Bohacek, J., Gapp, K., and Mansuy, I. M. (2014). Epigenetics of memory and plasticity. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 122, 305–340. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-420170-5.00011-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xing, Y., and Lee, C. (2006). Alternative splicing and RNA selection pressure–evolutionary consequences for eukaryotic genomes. Nat. Rev. Genet. 7, 499–509. doi: 10.1038/nrg1896

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Youngster, I., Sauk, J., Pindar, C., Wilson, R. G., Kaplan, J. L., Smith, M. B., et al. (2014). Fecal microbiota transplant for relapsing clostridium difficile infection using a frozen inoculum from unrelated donors: a randomized, open-label, controlled pilot study. Clin. Infect. Dis. 58, 1515–1522. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu135

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaneveld, J., Turnbaugh, P. J., Lozupone, C., Ley, R. E., Hamady, M., Gordon, J. I., et al. (2008). Host-bacterial coevolution and the search for new drug targets. Curr. Opin. Chem. Biol. 12, 109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpa.2008.01.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, G., Dahl, J. A., Niu, Y., Fu, Y., Klungland, A., Yang, Y.-G., et al. (2013). Sprouts of RNA epigenetics: the discovery of mammalian RNA demethylases. RNA Biol. 10, 915–918. doi: 10.4161/rna.24711

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziats, M. N., and Rennert, O. M. (2013). Aberrant expression of long noncoding RNAs in autistic brain. J. Mol. Neurosci. 49, 589–593. doi: 10.1007/s12031-012-9880-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoetendal, E. G., Akkermans, A. D., Akkermans-van Vliet, W. M., de Visser, J. A. G., and de Vos, W. M. (2001). The host genotype affects the bacterial community in the human gastronintestinal tract. Microb. Ecol. Health Dis. 13, 129–134. doi: 10.1080/089106001750462669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: microbiota, sociality, neurodevelopment, gene-environment interactions, non-coding RNA, epigenetics, evo-devo, transgenerational

Citation: Stilling RM, Bordenstein SR, Dinan TG and Cryan JF (2014) Friends with social benefits: host-microbe interactions as a driver of brain evolution and development? Front. Cell. Infect. Microbiol. 4:147. doi: 10.3389/fcimb.2014.00147

Received: 18 July 2014; Accepted: 03 October 2014;
Published online: 29 October 2014.

Edited by:

Emiliano J. Salvucci, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Argentina

Reviewed by:

Ingo Autenrieth, University Tubingen, Germany
Suleyman Yildirim, Istanbul Medipol University, Turkey

Copyright © 2014 Stilling, Bordenstein, Dinan and Cryan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: John F. Cryan, Department of Anatomy and Neuroscience, Western Gateway Building, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland e-mail: j.cryan@ucc.ie

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.