Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol., 16 September 2020
Sec. Biomechanics
This article is part of the Research Topic The Relationship Between Neural Circuitry and Biomechanical Action View all 27 articles

Load Magnitude and Locomotion Pattern Alter Locomotor System Function in Healthy Young Adult Women

\r\nKellen T. Krajewski*Kellen T. Krajewski1*Dennis E. DeverDennis E. Dever1Camille C. JohnsonCamille C. Johnson2Qi MiQi Mi1Richard J. SimpsonRichard J. Simpson3Scott M. GrahamScott M. Graham4Gavin L. MoirGavin L. Moir5Nizam U. AhamedNizam U. Ahamed1Shawn D. FlanaganShawn D. Flanagan1William J. AnderstWilliam J. Anderst2Chris ConnaboyChris Connaboy1
  • 1Neuromuscular Research Laboratory and Warrior Human Performance Research Center, Department of Sports Medicine and Nutrition, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 2Biodynamics Laboratory, Department of Orthopedic Surgery, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 3Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 4School of Applied Sciences, Edinburgh Napier University, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
  • 5Exercise Science Department, East Stroudsburg University, East Stroudsburg, PA, United States

Introduction: During cyclical steady state ambulation, such as walking, variability in stride intervals can indicate the state of the system. In order to define locomotor system function, observed variability in motor patterns, stride regulation and gait complexity must be assessed in the presence of a perturbation. Common perturbations, especially for military populations, are load carriage and an imposed locomotion pattern known as forced marching (FM). We examined the interactive effects of load magnitude and locomotion pattern on motor variability, stride regulation and gait complexity during bipedal ambulation in recruit-aged females.

Methods: Eleven healthy physically active females (18–30 years) completed 1-min trials of running and FM at three load conditions: no additional weight/bodyweight (BW), an additional 25% of BW (BW + 25%), and an additional 45% of BW (BW + 45%). A goal equivalent manifold (GEM) approach was used to assess motor variability yielding relative variability (RV; ratio of “good” to “bad” variability) and detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) to determine gait complexity on stride length (SL) and stride time (ST) parameters. DFA was also used on GEM outcomes to calculate stride regulation.

Results: There was a main effect of load (p = 0.01) on RV; as load increased, RV decreased. There was a main effect of locomotion (p = 0.01), with FM exhibiting greater RV than running. Strides were regulated more tightly and corrected quicker at BW + 45% compared (p < 0.05) to BW. Stride regulation was greater for FM compared to running. There was a main effect of load for gait complexity (p = 0.002); as load increased gait complexity decreased, likewise FM had less (p = 0.02) gait complexity than running.

Discussion: This study is the first to employ a GEM approach and a complexity analysis to gait tasks under load carriage. Reduction in “good” variability as load increases potentially exposes anatomical structures to repetitive site-specific loading. Furthermore, load carriage magnitudes of BW + 45% potentially destabilize the system making individuals less adaptable to additional perturbations. This is further evidenced by the decrease in gait complexity, which all participants demonstrated values similarly observed in neurologically impaired populations during the BW + 45% load condition.

Introduction

Bipedal ambulation requires the complex integration of multisensory information (optical, somatic and vestibular) that is used to coordinate actions within specific environments in order to achieve goal-directed movement (Alexander, 1992; Warren et al., 2001; Bent et al., 2004a; Pandy and Andriacchi, 2010; Matthis et al., 2017). Perceptions of continuously obtained multisensory information yield opportunities to act (affordances) resulting in a perception-action coupling, with a specific movements success predicated on the modulation (tuning and weighting) of the afferent signals that provide (or fail to) appropriate affordances for the task (Gibson, 1966, 1979; Hollands and Marple-Horvat, 1996; Warren et al., 2001; Bent et al., 2004a,b; Rossignol et al., 2006; Peters et al., 2017). In conjunction with sensory “reafference,” feedforward mechanisms stimulate coordinative structures or muscle synergies that produce a desired movement that achieves a locomotion task goal (Kim et al., 2011; Bizzi and Cheung, 2013; Minassian et al., 2017). Collectively, the reciprocal cooperation of feedback (afferent) and feedforward (efferent) subcomponents executing a locomotion task is known as the locomotor system. The function of the locomotor system reflects the emergent properties of the organization of the degrees of freedom during locomotor tasks, with specific macroscopic pattern of organization being influenced by the confluence of cost functions (i.e., metabolic efficiency and energy dampening), task, organism (including feedback and feedforward processes) and environmental constraints (i.e., gravity, uneven terrain) (Turvey, 1990; Newell and Vaillancourt, 2001; Davids et al., 2003; Sánchez et al., 2016; Caballero et al., 2019; Shafizadeh et al., 2019). Optimal locomotor system function is represented by biomechanical output that is both stable and adaptive to perturbation (Davids et al., 2003; West and Scafetta, 2003; Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013; Seifert et al., 2013). A common perturbation to bipedal locomotion, especially in military populations, is load carriage, especially “combat load” magnitudes of 20–30 kg (Taylor et al., 2016; Krajewski et al., 2020). How the locomotor system accommodates increasing load magnitudes to successfully execute locomotion task goals still remain unclear (LaFiandra et al., 2003; Attwells et al., 2006; Walsh et al., 2018). Thus, measuring the responses of biomechanical variables to the perturbation of additional loading during locomotory tasks provides valuable insight to the global functional state of the locomotor system.

Variability in the observed movement patterns (motor variability) represents the observed variation in a movement solution, when attempting to accomplish the same goal/task, such as using different segment coupling patterns to perform a step (Bernstein, 1967; Latash et al., 2002, 2010; Latash, 2016). The multitude of joints and muscles in the lower extremity lead to a large number of degrees of freedom that lends itself to equifinality; infinite number of movement solutions to accomplish the same task (Bernstein, 1967; Gelfand and Latash, 1998; Latash et al., 2010). A goal equivalent manifold (GEM; equifinality technique) approach seeks to quantify the “good” (plotted tangential to the GEM [δT]) versus “bad” (plotted perpendicular to the GEM [δP]) motor variability to further discriminate optimal performance (known as relative variability [the ratio of “good” motor variability to “bad” motor variability]) (Dingwell et al., 2010; Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013; Sedighi and Nussbaum, 2019). Recent theories have demonstrated that motor variability not only leverages equifinality, making the system more adaptable and stable to perturbation (i.e., overcoming varying terrain or recovering from a slip/trip) (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013; Dingwell et al., 2017), it also has other cost function benefits (Gates and Dingwell, 2008). Specifically, by capitalizing on a larger workspace (greater relative variability) of movement patterns to perform steady-state (constant locomotion velocity) behaviors, energy can be dispersed through more supportive, anatomical structures, whereas limited motor variability (lower relative variability) may lead to site-specific mechanical overloading (cumulative mechanical stress) that can result in musculoskeletal injury (MSI) (Baida et al., 2018; Nordin and Dufek, 2019). Likewise, motor variability can distribute positive mechanical workloads across a greater number of muscle fibers improving metabolic efficiency by reducing the fatigue of a specific subset of muscle fibers (Gates and Dingwell, 2008).

Regulation of cyclical movements during steady-state behavior such as corrections of stride-to-stride fluctuations further elucidates the state of the locomotor system (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013). Stride regulation is determined by statistical persistence assessment [alpha coefficients (Dingwell et al., 2017)] of deviations tangential (good variability) [δT] and orthogonal (bad variability) [δP] to the goal manifold (Dingwell et al., 2017). A seminal investigation by Dingwell et al. (2017) demonstrated that elderly individuals classified as low risk fallers and healthy young adults had the same amount of relative variability (ratio of “good” to “bad” motor variability) and used similar stride regulation strategies indicative of a minimal intervention principle (δTα > 1; δPα < 0.5) (Dingwell et al., 2017). Furthermore, it was suggested that changes in stride-regulation strategy to an absolute position control (POS) model [δTα and δPα < 0.5] (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013) may be the determinant of fall risk (Dingwell et al., 2017). The latter finding was determined with computational modeling (based on a minimum intervention principle) and is still theoretical at this point (Dingwell et al., 2017), but the use of a POS regulation strategy may indicate perception heavily tuned on their exact position, neglecting/overpowering other important information which will impact affordances perception. In the case of military personnel, especially infantry, load carriage is only one perturbation that must be overcome in addition to uneven terrain, enemy threats and decision making. Thus, the quantification of regulation strategy of the system used for stride to stride fluctuations acts as an indirect assessment of the perception-action loop function namely: (i) the ability to (re)calibrate information-action in a dynamic environment, (ii) (re)weighting the relative importance of information sources as they become available, and (iii) modulate based on the relative importance in relation to the successful maintenance of a functionally useful action-response (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013; Roerdink et al., 2019).

Components of the locomotor system operate/evolve over different time scales and configure in a heterarchical organization when functioning optimally (Bak et al., 1987; Turvey, 1990; Bak and Paczuski, 1995; Newell and Vaillancourt, 2001; Davids et al., 2003; Van Orden et al., 2003). A heterarchical organization of a dynamical system is considered to be complex (interaction of many independent subcomponents that yield an emergent behavior) and a perturbation of one subcomponent is less likely to affect the system globally (West and Shlesinger, 1989; Bak and Paczuski, 1995; Marks-Tarlow, 1999; Torre et al., 2007; Torre and Balasubramaniam, 2009). Thus quantification of system complexity indicates the state of dynamical system health (Iyengar et al., 1996; Gisiger, 2001; Goldberger et al., 2005; Hausdorff, 2007; Van Orden et al., 2009; Nourrit-Lucas et al., 2015; Torre et al., 2019) through non-linear signal processing techniques to determine the fractal structure of a time-series, which exhibits self-similarity at different time scales (Stadnitski, 2012). These fractals display long-range correlations, or learning behavior of current iterations from previous iterations (Hausdorff et al., 1995). Time-series structures of gait dynamics (stride length [SL] and stride time [ST]) that yield long range correlations (pink noise) have been linked to healthy functioning adults (Hausdorff et al., 1997, 1999; Hausdorff, 2007; Delignières and Torre, 2009; Ducharme et al., 2018); however, very strong long-range correlations exhibit over-regularity (brown noise) (Gisiger, 2001). Signals that are completely stochastic (white noise) demonstrate no correlation between strides and have been associated with individuals suffering central neurological impairment (Hausdorff et al., 1997; Hausdorff, 2009; Moon et al., 2016). Moreover, white noise has also been observed when imposing a frequency on cyclical steady-state behavior (Terrier et al., 2005; Terrier and Dériaz, 2012; Hunt et al., 2014; Ducharme et al., 2018; Roerdink et al., 2019). Interestingly, warfighters are encouraged to utilize a walking pattern during a velocity that exceeds the gait transition velocity (GTV), colloquially known as forced marching (FM) that is an unnatural (imposed frequency) gait. Little is known how load magnitude, especially military relevant loads (20–60 kg) (Taylor et al., 2016), and this imposed locomotion affect gait complexity in healthy individuals.

To date a load magnitude perturbation is evidenced only in terms of increased mechanical [greater ground reaction forces (GRF) (Birrell et al., 2007; Seay et al., 2014b) and joint kinetics (Knapik et al., 2004; Seay et al., 2014a,b; Liew et al., 2016; Willy et al., 2016, 2019; Lenton et al., 2019; Loverro et al., 2019; Wills et al., 2019; Krajewski et al., 2020)] and physiological [increased heart rate and ratings of perceived exertion (Simpson et al., 2010, 2011, 2017; Huang and Kuo, 2014)] demands compared to unloaded bipedal ambulation. The majority of these studies consisted of male dominated samples, leaving females underrepresented in load carriage research (Loverro et al., 2019). In addition, females are at twice the risk of MSI (Molloy et al., 2020), with a high incidence (∼78%) of MSI observed during basic combat training (recruits), the majority (30–64%) of those MSI suffered during load carriage conditioning in basic training (Jensen et al., 2019; Lovalekar et al., 2020) suggesting that individuals with little to no experience with load carriage tasks are of greater interest. However, there is a paucity of information regarding the effects of load carriage on motor control (LaFiandra et al., 2003; Attwells et al., 2006; Walsh et al., 2018). Importantly, previous work has focused on average behavior of spatiotemporal gait parameters (LaFiandra et al., 2003; Attwells et al., 2006) and have yet to elucidate key features of a healthy locomotor system such as motor variability, stride to stride regulation and the complexity of the system. Therefore, the purpose of this investigation was to determine the interactive effects of load magnitude and locomotion pattern on motor variability, stride regulation and gait complexity during bipedal ambulation in recruit aged females. It is hypothesized, based on an affordance-based control theory (Davids et al., 2003; Mukherjee and Yentes, 2018) that as load increases and the use of an unnatural (imposed) locomotion (FM) will constrain the locomotor system decreasing the number of affordances available which will be reflected by the reduction in relative variability (the ratio of “good” motor variability to “bad” motor variability). Likewise, increases in load and utilization of FM will lead to stricter regulation strategies. Lastly, individuals gait complexity will decrease as load increases and during the execution of the FM locomotion pattern.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

All participants were read and signed informed consent that has been approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of The University of Pittsburgh. They were notified of potential risks and benefits associated with participation in the study.

Subjects and Protocol

Eleven healthy, recreationally active young adult females (see Table 1 for participant characteristics) participated in this study. Recreationally active was defined as engaging in moderate physical activity a minimum of two times a week for at least 30 min, similar to comparable recruits. Moreover, women novice to load carriage and forced marching were chosen to represent a female recruit population, replicating initial exposure to load carriage tasks. Subjects were screened to exclude individuals who reported spine and lower extremity musculoskeletal injury, neurological disorder or pregnant.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject Characteristics and exercise status.

The procedures for this investigation have been previously described in detail (Krajewski et al., 2020). Briefly, participants ran (RN) and forced marched (FM) on a instrumented split-belt treadmill (Bertec Corporation, Columbus, OH, United States) for 1 min at three different loaded conditions: Bodyweight (BW), plus an additional 25% of BW (BW + 25%), and plus an additional 45% of BW (BW + 45%) [which represents 20–30 kg “combat” loads in average young adult females (Taylor et al., 2016)] at 10% above their GTV (BW: 2.08 ± 0.25 m/s; BW + 25%: 2.02 ± 0.22 m/s; BW + 45%: 1.93 ± 0.23 m/s). All participants wore provided combat boots (Speed 3.0 Boot, 5.11 Tactical, Irvine, CA, United States) to control for influences of footwear on kinematics (Telfer et al., 2017) and loaded conditions were executed with an anterior-posterior loaded weight vest (Short Plus Style Vest, MIR, United States) to control for effects of center of mass (COM) location (Seay et al., 2014a; Loverro et al., 2019). All trials were randomized by load condition and then by locomotion pattern. Participants were given up to 5 min between each trial to control for effects of fatigue. During RN trials, participants were instructed to move “naturally” or how they felt most comfortable to maintain treadmill velocity. For FM trials, participants were instructed to maintain a walking gait regardless of the treadmill velocity. Each trial yielded ∼130 strides (120–180) dependent on the locomotion pattern and velocity. Prior to familiarization and data collection, participants filled out an activity questionnaire and body composition was assessed with dual energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) [Lunar iDXA, General Electric, Boston, MA, United States].

Data Collection and Processing

Three retro-reflective markers were placed on each boot (calcaneus, 1st and 5th metatarsophalangeal [MTP] joints) [see Figure 1 for subject experimental set up]. Kinematic data was collected via 12 infrared cameras (Vicon Motion Systems Ltd., Oxford, United Kingdom) sampling at 100 Hz. Kinetic data was collected via an instrumented split-belt treadmill sampling at 1000 Hz that was synchronized with the motion analysis system. Using the Vicon Nexus® 2.0 software (Vicon Motion Systems Ltd., Oxford, United Kingdom), a custom labeling template was created for the marker configuration used in the study. Once all static and motion trials were reconstructed, the labeling template was used to auto label the static trials captured for each load condition (BW, BW + 25% and BW + 45%) which were then used to auto label their respective motion trials (RN and FM). Gap filling methods in Nexus 2.0 were used to correct any breaks in trajectory data due to marker occlusion. Data was then exported, and post processed in Visual 3D (C-Motion Inc., Germantown, MD, United States). Further analysis [GEM decomposition and Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (Atlas Collaboration et al., 2014)] was conducted with custom MatlabTM 2019a (Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) scripts. Kinematic and kinetic data were filtered with a second order Butterworth low-pass filter (cut-off frequencies of 6 Hz and 40 Hz for the kinematic and kinetic signals, respectively). Heel strike was defined as the time when vertical component of the ground reaction force exceeded a 50N threshold.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Subject Set-Up. Exemplar set up of a participant with their +45% load in the anterior-posterior weight vest. Solid dots represent retroreflective markers. Markers at the medial/lateral epicondyles (knee) and medial/lateral malleoli (ankle) removed after static calibration trial capture. Markers at the calcaneus and 1st/5th metatarsophalangeal (MTP) joints defined the foot segment.

The following variables were calculated: Stride length (SL) was computed as the distance covered from heel strike to ipsilateral heel strike; Stride time (ST) was computed as the time elapsed from heel strike to ipsilateral heel strike; Stride speed (SS) was computed as the quotient of SL/ST; Velocity (v) was computed as the average SS over all n strides of a time-series. Average values (Means), standard deviations (SD) and DFA scaling exponents (α-value) were calculated for SL, ST and SS across all trials.

Goal Equivalent Manifold Decomposition

Methods utilized for GEM decomposition have been described in detail by Dingwell et al. (2010). However, to further elaborate the process: firstly, SL and ST time-series for each trial was normalized to unit variance [dividing by its own standard deviation]. A specific operating point was computed for ST as Eq. (1):

S T * = < S T > n (1)

Where <■> represents the average across all n strides of the time series. The specific operating point for SL was computed as Eq. (2):

S L * = v S T * (2)

Here v represents the velocity of the treadmill for that specific trial. The new centered operating point was then computed as Eqs. (3) and (4):

S T n = S T n - S T * (3)

and

S L n = S L n - S L * (4)

Lastly, deviations tangential to the goal manifold were represented as δT and deviations perpendicular to the goal manifold were represented as δP. These deviations were calculated with a linear coordinate transformation as Eq. (5):

[ δ T δ P ] = 1 1 + v 2 [ 1 v - v 1 ] [ S T n S L n ] (5)

Where, the σ of δT and δP were determined for each load and locomotion condition. Relative variability was calculated as the ratio between σδT/σδP. Therefore, a relative variability magnitude of 1 represents equal amounts of “good” versus “bad” variability; <1 represents more “bad” variability; and >1 represents more “good” variability. Additionally, DFA scaling exponents (α) were computed for δT and δP. Scaling exponents for δT and δP are interpreted as follows: α < 0.5 represents anti-persistence (alteration in one direction more likely followed by an alteration in opposite direction); α > 0.5 represents statistical persistence (alteration in one direction more likely followed by an alteration in same direction); and α = 0.5 represents uncorrelated (alteration in one direction has same likelihood of being followed by alteration in either direction) (Dingwell et al., 2010, 2017).

Complexity Analysis

Complexity analysis was executed utilizing fractal methods, specifically DFA (Peng et al., 1993; Hausdorff et al., 1995; Delignières et al., 2006; Stadnitski, 2012) on SL and ST gait variables (∼130 consecutive strides). Refer to the aforementioned references for greater detail but briefly: DFA creates a one-dimensional signal x(i), i = 1,…, L, where x is the initial signal of size L, and an integrated signal (x) is calculated according to the Eq. (6):

Y ( k ) = i = 1 k ( B ( i ) - B a v g ) (6)

where Bavg is the mean value of the signal (B = signal value at specific time point). The unified time series Y is then divided into segments (boxes that don’t overlap) of length l, and the linear approximation Yl is then obtained through a least-squares fit of each segment separately (trend of each section).

The mean fluctuation (root mean square) of the incorporated and detrended time-series is computed using Eq. (7):

F ( l ) = 1 L k = 1 L ( Y ( k ) - Y l ( k ) ) 2 (7)

The aforementioned calculations are repeated for a range of l. The goal of this analysis is to identify the relation between F(l) and the size of segment l because this relationship serves as an indicator of a scaling phenomenon. In general, F(l) increases with increases in the range of segment l. A double plot logarithmic graph (log (F(l) vs logl) is then formed, and this graph is used to acquire the scaling exponent (α). A linear dependency implies the existence of self-fluctuations, and F(l) which is the slope of line outlines the scaling α exponent, which increases with l based on a power law, as detailed in Eq. (6):

F ( l ) - l α = > l o g ( ( F ( l ) ) - α × l o g ( l ) (8)

DFA ultimately yields a scaling exponent (α) which represent the correlational structure of the signal. White noise (uncorrelated or completely stochastic) is represented as α = 0.5; Pink noise (positive long-range correlations) is represented as α = 1.0; Brown noise (persistent long-range correlations or too much regularity) is represented as α = 1.5 (Peng et al., 1995). Classifications based upon a range of α were employed to provide greater clarity as values are rarely the exact values listed above. “Suboptimal self-organization” was represented by α < 0.75; “Optimal self-organization” was represented by α = 0.75 – 1.30; “Impaired self-organization” was represented by α > 1.30. These values were based upon previously established ranges that classified populations (healthy, elderly, and impaired) as either white, pink or brown noise (Hausdorff et al., 1996, 1997, 1999; Ravi et al., 2020). The values attained during the RN with BW condition was considered the baseline because it is the natural locomotion pattern used 10% above GTV and unperturbed by an external load (no added load carriage). Change classifications were then determined for each individual (change from the baseline condition) as either “positive change” in complexity (“Suboptimal self-organization” or “Impaired self-organization” to “Optimal self-organization”), “negative change” (“Optimal self-organization” to “Suboptimal self-organization” or “Impaired self-organization”), “no change positive” (“Optimal self-organization” to “Optimal self-organization”) and “no change negative” (“Suboptimal self-organization” or “Impaired self-organization” to “Suboptimal self-organization” or “Impaired self-organization”).

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics (mean and SD) were reported for all the variables. In order to determine interactive effects of load and locomotion on relative variability and gait complexity a two-way repeated measure analysis of variance (RMANOVA) for Load × Locomotion (3 × 2) was conducted separately. Additionally, to further elucidate findings regarding relative variability, tangential and perpendicular variability were assessed within locomotion pattern with a 3 × 2 (Load × Direction) RMANOVA. If interactions were significant, simple main effects were performed (paired t-tests for locomotion/direction stratified by load and RMANOVA for load stratified by locomotion/direction). If no significant interaction was observed, only main effects were analyzed. Post hoc analysis using Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons were conducted when necessary.

To determine interactive effects of load and locomotion on stride regulation a three-way Load × Locomotion × Direction (3 × 2 × 2) RMANOVA was conducted on DFA scaling exponents of δT and δP. If a significant three-way interaction was observed then two-way RMANOVAs were conducted for load by locomotion (3 × 2), load by direction (3 × 2) and locomotion by direction (2 × 2). If a two-way interaction was observed, then simple main effects were analyzed (RMANOVA for load and paired t-tests for locomotion and direction). If no significant two-way interaction was observed, only main effects were analyzed. Post hoc analysis using Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons were conducted when necessary. Lastly, if no significant three-way interaction was observed, only main effects were analyzed. Post hoc analysis using Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons were conducted when necessary.

Partial eta squared (η2P) was calculated as a measure of effect size given the within-subject design (Bakeman, 2005; Richardson, 2011), with magnitudes of effect interpreted as: 0.01–0.085 (small effect); 0.09–0.24 (moderate effect); and > 0.25 (large effect) (Cohen et al., 2003). Additionally, frequencies of complexity classifications and change classifications are reported to qualitatively examine individual responses. The alpha level was set at 0.05 (p ≤ 0.05).

Results

Relative Variability

See Table 2 for mean and SD of all GEM related outcomes. There was no significant interaction between load and locomotion for relative variability of motor control (F2,20 = 0.167, p = 0.85, η2P = 0.02). Load had a significant influence on relative variability reducing the number of successful or “good” movement solutions, exemplified as relative variability magnitude decreasing as load magnitude increased confirmed by the main effect of load (F2,20 = 5.50, p = 0.01, η2P = 0.36); with post hoc analysis revealing BW + 45% (1.28 ± 0.05) being significantly (p = 0.02) less than BW (1.55 ± 0.07). Additionally, FM demonstrated a more relative variability compared to running indicated by the main effect of locomotion (F1,10 = 8.90, p = 0.01, η2P = 0.47) with estimated marginal means for FM (1.53 ± 0.08) being greater than RN (1.27 ± 0.04).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. GEM outcomes (mean ± standard deviation).

The interaction between load and direction during RN was not statistically significant (F2,20 = 2.33, p = 0.12, η2p = 0.19). There was no significant main effect of load (F2,20 = 3.05, p = 0.07, η2p = 0.23). While not significant, as load increased mean tangential (“good”) variability decreased (BW = 1.13 ± 0.10, BW + 25% = 1.10 ± 0.06, BW + 45% = 1.06 ± 0.07) and mean perpendicular (“bad”) variability increased (BW = 0.84 ± 0.13, BW + 25% = 0.88 ± 0.08, BW + 45% = 0.94 ± 0.08). However, regardless of load, tangential variability was always greater evidenced by the main effect of direction (F1,10 = 60.91, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.86), with estimated marginal means revealing variability along the tangential (1.10 ± 0.01) was greater than along the perpendicular (0.88 ± 0.02).

There was no significant interaction between load and direction for FM (F2,20 = 1.79, p = 0.19, η2p = 0.15). There was no main effect of load (F2,20 = 2.75, p = 0.09, η2p = 0.22). While not significant, as load increased mean tangential (“good”) variability decreased slightly (BW = 1.20 ± 0.08, BW + 25% = 1.15 ± 0.08, BW + 45% = 1.15 ± 0.06) and mean perpendicular (“bad”) variability increased (BW = 0.74 ± 0.013, BW + 25% = 0.81 ± 0.12, BW + 45% = 0.81 ± 08). Lastly, regardless of load, tangential variability was always greater than perpendicular variability indicated by the main effect of direction (F1,10 = 90.10, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.90), with estimated marginal means revealing tangential variability (1.17 ± 0.02) was greater than perpendicular variability (0.79 ± 0.02). See Figure 2 for exemplar plots of SL and ST combinations along the goal manifold.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) Exemplar Plots. Exemplar plots (S4) represent a time series of consecutive strides (∼156 strides). Solid dots represent the different combinations of SL and ST for each stride. The solid line represents the goal manifold, which in this case is the velocity of the treadmill. Therefore, the assumed goal of the participant is to maintain horizontal velocity so they do not drift off the end of the treadmill belt. The dashed lines superior and inferior to the solid line represent the ±5% error of the goal manifold. Dots that are tangential (along the solid goal manifold) are variations that still achieve the task goal (‘good’ variability). Dots that are perpendicular to the solid goal manifold line are variations that fail to achieve the goal manifold. Perpendicular coordinates will result in the participant moving forward or backward on the treadmill belt. (A,B) demonstrate more tangential variability as indicated by the larger spread along the goal manifold. Conversely, (C,D) exhibit a much tighter formation indicative of less variation and stricter stride regulation. Furthermore, in contrast of (A), (C) exhibits more stride variants that lie beyond the ±5% error range. Not surprisingly, this participant had complexity classifications of ‘optimal’ and ‘suboptimal’ for (A,C) respectively.

Scaling Exponents (α): ST & Sp

See Table 2 for all means and SD of scaling exponents. There was no significant 3-way interaction between load, locomotion and direction (F2,20 = 1.96, p = 0.17, η2p = 0.16). As load increased control of stride-to-stride fluctuations became more strict (and therefore corrected more quickly) evidenced by the main effect of load (F2,20 = 8.87, p = 0.002, η2p = 0.47), with post hoc analysis revealing that BW (0.6 ± 0.08) was significantly (p = 0.02) greater than BW + 45% (0.04 ± 0.11). Additionally, running exhibited less control of stride-to-stride fluctuations indicated by the main effect of locomotion (F1,10 = 8.57, p = 0.02, η2p = 0.46), with estimated marginal means revealing that running (0.46 ± 0.07) was greater than FM (0.19 ± 0.08). Lastly, “bad” variations (perpendicular to the goal manifold) were controlled and corrected more quickly than “good” variations (tangential to goal manifold) as evidenced by the main effect of direction (F1,10 = 67.12, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.87), with estimated marginal means revealing that persistence along the tangential (0.47 ± 0.06) was greater than the perpendicular (0.18 ± 0.07).

Complexity Analysis (α): SL, ST

See Table 3 for mean and SD of scaling exponents. There was no significant interaction between load and locomotion for SL (F2,20 = 0.03, p = 0.97, η2p = 0.003). As load magnitude increased, gait complexity decreased independent of locomotion pattern as evidence by the main effect of load (F2,20 = 8.74, p = 0.002, η2p = 0.4 = 7), with post hoc pairwise comparisons revealing BW (0.69 ± 0.06) was significantly (p = 0.02) greater than BW + 45% (0.1 ± 0.16). Additionally, FM reduced gait complexity compared to running independent of load magnitude indicated by the main effect of locomotion (F1,10 = 7.59, p = 0.02, η2p = 0.43), with estimated marginal means revealed running (0.59 ± 0.06) was greater than FM (0.23 ± 0.13).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Complexity outcomes (mean ± standard deviation) and class frequency (# of occurrences).

There was no significant interaction between load and locomotion for ST (F2,20 = 0.43, p = 0.36, eta = 0.10). The increase in load magnitude decreased gait complexity independent of locomotion pattern as evidenced by the main effect of load (F2,20 = 6.52, p = 0.007, η2p = 0.40), with post hoc analysis revealing that BW (0.67 ± 0.10) was greater (p = 0.03) than BW + 45% (−0.10 ± 0.19). Lastly, FM reduced gait complexity compared to running independent of load magnitude as indicated by the main effect of locomotion (F1,10 = 9.66, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.75), with estimated marginal means revealing running (0.76 ± 0.11) was greater than FM (−0.003 ± 0.15).

See Table 3 for frequency of observed complexity classifications by condition and Figure 3 for the frequency of each classification change of all conditions combined. For SL and ST 43.64% and 49.09% of the observed changes from baseline were negative changes (“Optimal self-organization” to “Suboptimal’ or ‘Impaired.” Only two participants demonstrated a positive change with 63% of those occurrences accounted for by one participant. Positive change classifications only occurred at the BW + 25% load condition and not the BW + 45%. Two of the participants with positive changes from baseline reported having prior experience in a multitude of exercise modalities that would improve lower limb muscular endurance and anaerobic conditioning (see Table 1). For SL only 10.91% of changes were classified as “no change positive” with 58% of the occurrences accounted for by two individuals. Both individuals performed a greater variety of exercise modalities including resistance training and trained more frequently and for longer periods of time. Lastly, only a single subject demonstrated an “Impaired” classification at baseline (RN at BW). This participant trained for the shortest durations (30 min maximum) only 2–3 times a week (see Table 1 for more detailed characteristics).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Frequency of Change Classifications. Positive Change = ‘Suboptimal’ to ‘Optimal’ or ‘Impaired’ to ‘Optimal’ (only 2 subjects, 50% of incidences 1 subject); Negative Change = ‘Optimal’ to ‘Suboptimal’ or ‘Optimal’ to ‘Impaired’; No Change Positive = ‘Optimal’ to ‘Optimal’ (2 subjects accounted for 59% of all incidences); No Change Negative = ‘Suboptimal’ to ‘Suboptimal’, ‘Suboptimal’ to ‘Impaired’, ‘Impaired’ to ‘Impaired’, or ‘Impaired’ to ‘Suboptimal’.

Discussion

The objectives of this investigation were to determine the interactive effects of load magnitude and locomotion pattern on motor variability, stride-to-stride control/regulation and complexity. Load magnitude significantly altered relative variability independent of locomotion pattern evidenced by the significant main effect of load. As load increased relative variability decreased with BW + 45% (1.28σ) having 21% less relative variability compared to BW (1.55σ), suggesting individuals are better able to leverage either the system degeneracy, or indeed, redundancy of coordinative patterns employed to execute a stride during unloaded bipedal ambulatory tasks. The higher relative variability ratios are achieved by a greater variance tangential the goal manifold (“good” variability) and a reduction in variance perpendicular to the goal manifold (“bad” variability) [see Figure 2]. Importantly, these findings alone do not necessarily indicate more stable performance (task execution), but that during unloaded conditions there is a greater workspace of solutions that can be utilized to accomplish the goal task (maintain velocity). Coupled with the observed complexity scaling exponents ∼1 for SL/ST at BW and BW + 25% load conditions suggest that individuals display an optimally organized locomotor system fully leveraging equifinality. Furthermore, the lack of significant differences between BW and BW + 25% load conditions indicates that the latter load condition is insufficient to impose a constraint on the locomotor system that results in altered motor variability. At BW + 45%, the locomotor system has a diminished ability to leverage “good” motor variability. The lack of observed “good” variability, may limit energy dispersion across multiple structures up the kinetic chain when energy is highest [due to increased forces from load (Birrell et al., 2007; Lloyd, 2010; Liew et al., 2016; Willy et al., 2016, 2019; Krajewski et al., 2020)] increasing cumulative mechanic stress MSI risk (Nordin and Dufek, 2019; Baggaley et al., 2020).

Contrary to the proposed hypothesis, the FM (RV = 1.52) resulted in 19.6% greater relative variability than running (RV = 1.27). However, this finding alone should be interpreted with caution; the greater variability simply denotes a larger set of motor solutions observed for FM compared to running. Similar observations were made by Black et al. (2007) demonstrating greater motor variability in adolescents with down syndrome compared to aged-matched neurotypical adolescents, indicating that during unperturbed steady-state gait, those with down-syndrome needed to utilize their full movement solution potential in order to maintain velocity (Black et al., 2007). In addition, Black et al. (2007) demonstrated that the greater motor variability exhibited during unperturbed gait suggested a locomotor system operating closer to the action boundaries that would be more likely to fail (fall) with additional pertubrations (Black et al., 2007). Thus, greater relative variability alone may not indicate more task performance stability or motor-system health. In females during FM there is a greater contribution of frontal plane moments (adduction/abduction) to knee total joint moment, indicative of potentially deleterious movements (Krajewski et al., 2020). Although a greater range of movement solutions were utilized during FM compared to running, they may have included more movement solutions that while successful in executing the goal task (maintaining velocity), are maladaptive/deleterious with respect to joint/tissue health (Zabala et al., 2013; Asay et al., 2018; Baggaley et al., 2020; Krajewski et al., 2020). Future work should focus on elucidating the movement pattern structures (segment couplings) in more detail, which coincide with relative variability, its magnitude and structure.

Additionally, the unfamiliarity of the task for this sample (they had little to no experience with FM) may have demonstrated a greater exploration of state-space as a consequence of learning to perform this locomotion pattern (Newell and Vaillancourt, 2001; Pacheco and Newell, 2017). The greater relative variability exhibited by FM compared to running may be indicative of a locomotor system trying to adapt to improve mechanical efficiency or reduce pain sensations associated with each stride. By obstructing the natural bifurcation of locomotion (participants executing FM at a velocity 10% above their GTV), the system is in search of an order parameter (segment coupling pattern) that adheres to cost function (decrease metabolic cost/increase mechanical efficiency) of the task control parameter (horizontal velocity) (McGinnis and Newell, 1982; Newell and Vaillancourt, 2001; Davids et al., 2003; Caballero et al., 2019; Shafizadeh et al., 2019). Consequently, the observed complexity for FM was α = 0.11 (“suboptimal”), supporting the notion that the observed greater relative variability in FM compared to running, represented a state-space exploratory behavior. In conjunction with the low system complexity, the locomotor system is less stable to additional perturbations. Whilst imposed locomotion (FM) and load carriage are themselves perturbations dynamic military environments present even more perturbations (uneven terrain, enemy threats, orders from local commanders). If motor learning occupies a large percentage of the system workspace, it potentially results in the failure to identify external perturbations [due to competition over feedback/feedforward resources (Woollacott and Shumway-Cook, 2002; Al-Yahya et al., 2011; De Sanctis et al., 2014)] making the locomotor system less stable and more susceptible to failure (slips/trips/falls, identification of threats). Future research should compare novice and experienced individuals with load carriage to determine if there is a difference in relative variability and complexity while completing ambulatory tasks with load.

Stride-to-stride regulation demonstrated less control for unloaded conditions as evidenced by the main effect of load (p = 0.002). The BW + 45% load condition exhibited (δTα = 0.22, δpα = −0.11) significantly more regulation than BW (δTα = 0.74, δpα = 0.45). Alpha coefficient (α) values less than.5 indicate statistical anti-persistence, representing much stricter control because the subsequent stride variation is more likely to be the opposite composition (combination of SL and ST) than the previous. Thus, movements were corrected much quicker and more often with the addition of substantial (BW + 45%) load carriage independent of locomotion pattern. Moreover, the main effect of direction (p < 0.001) exhibited more strict regulation perpendicular to the manifold (“bad” variability) compared to tangential (“good” variability) [see Table 1 for δT (α) and δp (α) means]. The combination of less control of “good” variability and more control of “bad” variability [resembling an ideal minimum intervention principle (MIP) model [δTα > 1; δpα < 0.5] (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013)] is indicative of a stride-regulation strategy of a healthy system recognizing movement variations that impede the execution of the task goal (maintain velocity). Further, the “system controller” minimally intervenes, minimizing control effort theoretically freeing up system capacity for other components of the locomotor system (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013). In fact, the BW results (δTα = 0.74, δpα = 0.45) of this investigation were similar to stride regulation findings of healthy adults (δTα = ∼0.90, δpα = ∼0.42) by Dingwell et al. (2017). If the individual were unhealthy, a failure to properly regulate “bad” motor variability potentially results in them unable to successfully execute the task. However, as load increased both δTα and δpα decreased indicating a change in stride-regulation strategy that reflected an absolute position control (POS) model [δTα and δpα < 0.5] (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013), postulating that maximal control effort was used thus reducing the capacity of other locomotor system components (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013).

Locomotion pattern also affected stride-to-stride regulation independent of load, with FM (δTα = 0.36, δpα = 0.04) demonstrating stricter control compared to running (δTα = 0.60, δpα = 0.35). Even “good” motor variability (δTα) was tightly regulated for FM evidenced by the δTα < 0.5. Once again, the stride-regulation strategy utilized for FM at all load conditions more closely mimicked that of a POS model (Cusumano and Dingwell, 2013). A stricter regulation of stride-to-stride intervals coupled with greater relative variability of FM at BW + 45% compared to RN BW + 45% may further evidence the participant learning by exploring state-space and freezing/unlocking different degrees of freedom quickly (Bernstein, 1967; Sporns and Edelman, 1993; Newell and Vaillancourt, 2001). Thus in response to the load and imposed locomotion perturbations a structural reorganization of the locomotor system occurs, with potentially greater reliance on supraspinal input that disrupt feed forward mechanisms of gait (further supported by the observed α < 0.5 for FM of SL and ST) due to the competition over feedback/feedforward resources (Woollacott and Shumway-Cook, 2002; Al-Yahya et al., 2011; De Sanctis et al., 2014). It is likely that the perturbations associated with FM and load elicit more system capacity dedicated to control in a relatively unperturbed state (walking on a treadmill) potentially overpowering other system components important to navigating dynamic environments. This may have important consequences to military populations, as loaded ambulatory tasks are often undertaken in dynamic circumstances requiring the integration of multiple information sources to understand the context within which the action takes place, i.e., quickly navigating across an open area of the battle space while aiming and firing a weapon.

When assessing complexity of gait dynamics (SL and ST) there was a main effect of load (p = 0.002 and p = 0.007), with complexity decreasing as load increased. The BW + 45% condition (SLα = 0.1; STα = −0.1) exhibited significant less complexity than BW (SLα = 0.69; STα = 0.67). Whilst, group mean comparison demonstrated a decrease in complexity, the group mean for BW still represents a “suboptimal” organization (α < 0.75) (Hausdorff et al., 1997; Hausdorff, 2007). However, when observing the individual results, eight of the participants had “optimal” complexity or.75 < α < 1.30 during BW load conditions (see Tables 1, 3). Moreover, only five participants had “optimal” complexity for BW + 25% and every participant exhibited “suboptimal” complexity for the BW + 45% load condition. “Optimal” complexity or scaling exponents (α) ∼1 represent long-range correlations (pink noise) that is indicative of skilled performance and utilization of prior stride information (i.e., proprioceptive) to influence future strides (Hausdorff et al., 1996, 1997; Delignières and Torre, 2009; Nourrit-Lucas et al., 2015). Likewise, the fractal structure of “optimal” represents an independence of fluctuations at different time scales meaning a perturbation of one system component will not likely affect the global system (locomotor system as a whole) (West and Shlesinger, 1989; Bak and Paczuski, 1995; Marks-Tarlow, 1999; Torre et al., 2007). Therefore, the data observed within the present study indicates that a load carriage magnitude of at least BW + 45% reduces the system stability and adaptability, predisposing the system to failure (falling) in the presence of additional perturbations (i.e., increased fall risk with uneven terrain) in females with limited/no load carriage experience. Importantly, BW + 45% for this female sample was 26.6 ± 4.7kg, which represents a typical combat load (20 kg) (Taylor et al., 2016), a load used during operations that will bombard individuals with perturbations (terrain obstacles[debris], enemy threats, officer commands/questions) that if not actioned correctly could result in serious MSI or death. The latter suggests the need for further research to determine if experience/training with load carriage improves system complexity that can better handle additional adaptations.

In addition to a main effect of load, locomotion patterns also affected the complexity of SL and ST (p = 0.02 and p < 0.001, respectively). As hypothesized, the natural locomotion pattern running (SLα = 0.59; STα = 0.76), exhibited greater complexity than FM (SLα = 0.23; STα < −0.01). Considering that running is the extant locomotion observed at a velocity exceeding GTV, it is not surprising more participants (seven) had “optimal” complexity during RN conditions (BW and BW + 25% only). Five of the participants did have “optimal” complexity for FM but at the BW and BW + 25% conditions only. These five participants (S1, S7, S8, S10, and S11) engaged in moderate amounts of exercise time per week and engaged in a multitude of exercise modalities that included a form of anaerobic conditioning and/or strength training (see Table 1). Furthermore, one participant (S3) had an “impaired” complexity at baseline (running at BW). Interestingly, this participant performed the least amount of exercise time per week and engaged in the least varied modes of exercise [see Table 1]. Thus, the movement poverty in terms of time and coordination diversity, may impact system adaptability as reflected in the “impaired” and “suboptimal” complexity during minimally perturbed steady-state behaviors. Moreover, the complexity outcomes demonstrated varying individual responses highlighting its potential utility as a mechanical “biomarker” of the current state of the locomotor system and training adaptation response. Future research should compare complexity of different fitness level groups (i.e., highly aerobic versus highly anaerobic fit individuals) during loaded ambulatory tasks to further elucidate characteristics of “optimal” performers.

The primary limitations of this study are its small, sex specific (female) sample and the number of consecutive data points (∼130). While this investigation cannot make inferences about sex specific responses to load carriage in the absence of a male cohort, females are an under represented population in load carriage literature (Loverro et al., 2019). Therefore, findings regarding motor variability, stride regulation and gait complexity of this study can only be generalized to healthy recruit-aged females (18–33 years old) whom are novices to load carriage and forced marching. Future research should compare males versus females and recruit versus experienced individuals (deployable soldiers) to determine if the unfamiliarity to the tasks and equipment were confounding the observed responses to load magnitude and locomotion pattern. Likewise, it is advised to perform fractal analysis with a minimum of 512 consecutive data points (Delignières et al., 2006), however, 128 consecutive strides has been determined to be within 6% of the actual scaling value (Hausdorff et al., 1997). In addition, performing steady-state behavior with load carriage while minimizing the effects of fatigue is difficult beyond several minute trials. Moreover, our findings on the effect of locomotion pattern on gait complexity are similar to investigations regarding imposed frequencies and complexity (Delignieres et al., 2004; Ducharme et al., 2018). Nevertheless, while not ideal for complexity, the ∼130 consecutive data points is a robust time-series for GEM decomposition (Dingwell et al., 2010) and provides the first quantitative data on load magnitude’s influence on gait complexity. Lastly, upon the completion of the study we learned that one participant (S5) completed the experimental protocols with a distal avulsion of the semitendinosus. Interestingly, this participant exhibited “optimal” complexity for running at BW and BW + 25%. Although alone this data is inconclusive and may represent an isolated incident, but it does suggest that gait complexity of SL and ST may be an inappropriate factor to assess musculoskeletal health. The complexity of SL and ST specifically may only represent the global function of the locomotor system at achieving the task goal.

In conclusion, there are no interactive effects of load magnitude and locomotion pattern on motor variability, stride regulation and gait complexity. But load and locomotion do independently alter the function of the locomotor system. As load increases there is a reduction in relative variability (good:bad motor variability), gait complexity (α < 0.5) and strides become more tightly controlled. FM further reduces gait complexity and mimics stride regulation strategies of BW + 45% load conditions. Moreover, despite more relative variability for FM compared to running, this appears to be a consequence of state-space exploration, as supported by the increased stride control/error correction and “suboptimal” complexity. While complexity of SL and ST may be indicative of locomotor system function in terms of achieving a task goal (maintaining horizontal velocity in the case of this investigation), the variability/complexity of these factors do not appear to represent the health of the musculoskeletal system (i.e., state of joint/tissue health and whether an injurious movement pattern is being used). Therefore, additional order parameters (different gait variables) should be investigated to identify a marker of global MSI risk. Likewise, research should be conducted with longer trials to confirm the complexity findings of this investigation and elucidate the role of fatigue. Soldiers ultimately operate in dynamic environments with lots of perturbations under substantial load carriage where poor movement/slips, trips and fall can produce MSI or even death. The findings of this study indicate that locomotor system function is altered by FM and BW + 45% load, resulting in reduced motor variability and a system with less stability/adaptability that is potentially more susceptible to failure with additional perturbations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of University of Pittsburgh. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KK, QM, NA, and CC analyzed the data. CC, WA, and KK designed the study and wrote the manuscript. KK, CJ, and DD were responsible for the data collection and data processing. RS, SG, GM, QM, SF, and WA contributed to writing and review and editing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded by the Freddie H. Fu award.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alexander, R. M. (1992). A model of bipedal locomotion on compliant legs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 338, 189–198. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Yahya, E., Dawes, H., Smith, L., Dennis, A., Howells, K., and Cockburn, J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asay, J. L., Erhart-Hledik, J. C., and Andriacchi, T. P. (2018). Changes in the total knee joint moment in patients with medial compartment knee osteoarthritis over 5 years. J. Orthop Res. 36, 2373–2379. doi: 10.1002/jor.23908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atlas Collaboration, Aad, G., Abbott, B., Abdallah, J., Abdel Khalek, S., Abdinov, O., et al. (2014). Measurement of the muon reconstruction performance of the ATLAS detector using 2011 and 2012 LHC proton-proton collision data. Eur. Phys. J. C Part Fields 74:3130.

Google Scholar

Attwells, R. L., Birrell, S. A., Hooper, R. H., and Mansfield, N. J. (2006). Influence of carrying heavy loads on soldiers’ posture, movements and gait. Ergonomics 49, 1527–1537. doi: 10.1080/00140130600757237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baggaley, M., Esposito, M., Xu, C., Unnikrishnan, G., Reifman, J., and Edwards, W. B. (2020). Effects of load carriage on biomechanical variables associated with tibial stress fractures in running. Gait Posture 77, 190–194. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baida, S. R., Gore, S. J., Franklyn-Miller, A. D., and Moran, K. A. (2018). Does the amount of lower extremity movement variability differ between injured and uninjured populations? A systematic review. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 28, 1320–1338. doi: 10.1111/sms.13036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, P., and Paczuski, M. (1995). Complexity, contingency, and criticality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 6689–6696. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.15.6689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, P., Tang, C., and Wiesenfeld, K. (1987). Self-organized criticality: an explanation of the 1/f noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 59, 381–384. doi: 10.1103/physrevlett.59.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behav. Res. Methods 37, 379–384. doi: 10.3758/bf03192707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bent, L. R., Inglis, J. T., and McFadyen, B. J. (2004a). When is vestibular information important during walking? J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1269–1275. doi: 10.1152/jn.01260.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bent, L. R., McFadyen, B. J., and Inglis, J. T. (2004b). Is the use of vestibular information weighted differently across the initiation of walking? Exp. Brain Res. 157, 407–416.

Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The Coordination and Regulation of Movements. London: Pergamon Press.

Google Scholar

Birrell, S. A., Hooper, R. H., and Haslam, R. A. (2007). The effect of military load carriage on ground reaction forces. Gait Posture 26, 611–614. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bizzi, E., and Cheung, V. C. (2013). The neural origin of muscle synergies. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7:51. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, D., Smith, B. A., Wu, J., and Ulrich, B. D. (2007). Uncontrolled manifold analysis of segmental angle variability during walking: preadolescents with and without down syndrome. Exp. Brain Res. 183, 511–521. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1066-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caballero, C., Davids, K., Heller, B., Wheat, J., and Moreno, F. J. (2019). Movement variability emerges in gait as adaptation to task constraints in dynamic environments. Gait Posture 70, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., and Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cusumano, J. P., and Dingwell, J. B. (2013). Movement variability near goal equivalent manifolds: fluctuations, control, and model-based analysis. Hum. Mov. Sci. 32, 899–923. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davids, K., Glazier, P., Araujo, D., and Bartlett, R. (2003). Movement systems as dynamical systems: the functional role of variability and its implications for sports medicine. Sports Med. 33, 245–260. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200333040-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Sanctis, P., Butler, J. S., Malcolm, B. R., and Foxe, J. J. (2014). Recalibration of inhibitory control systems during walking-related dual-task interference: a mobile brain-body imaging (MOBI) study. Neuroimage 94, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delignieres, D., Lemoine, L., and Torre, K. (2004). Time intervals production in tapping and oscillatory motion. Hum. Mov. Sci. 23, 87–103. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2004.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delignières, D., Ramdani, S., Lemoine, L., Torre, K., Fortes, M., and Ninot, G. (2006). Fractal analyses for ‘short’ time series: a re-assessment of classical methods. J. Math. Psychol. 50, 525–544. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2006.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delignières, D., and Torre, K. (2009). Fractal dynamics of human gait: a reassessment of Hausdorff et al. (1996)’s data. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 1272–1279. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.90757.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dingwell, J., John, J., and Cusumano, J. P. (2010). Do humans optimally exploit redundancy to control step variability in walking? PLoS Comput. Biol. 6:e1000856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dingwell, J., Salinas, M., and Cusumano, J. (2017). Increased gait variability may not imply impaired stride-to-stride control of walking in healthy older adults. Gait Posture 55, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ducharme, S., Liddy, J. J., Haddad, J. M., Busa, M. A., Claxton, L. J., and Van Emmerik, R. E. (2018). Association between stride time fractality and gait adaptability during unperturbed and asymmetric walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 58, 248–259. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, D. H., and Dingwell, J. B. (2008). The effects of neuromuscular fatigue on task performance during repetitive goal-directed movements. Exp. Brain Res. 187, 573–585. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1326-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, I. M., and Latash, M. L. (1998). On the problem of adequate language in motor control. Motor Control 2, 306–313. doi: 10.1123/mcj.2.4.306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Gisiger, T. (2001). Scale invariance in biology: Coincidence or footprint of a universal mechanism? Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 76, 161–209. doi: 10.1017/s1464793101005607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberger, A., Peng, C. K., and Lipstiz, L. A. (2005). What is physiologic complexity and how does it change with aging and disease? Neurobiol. Aging 23, 23–26. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00266-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J., Peng, C. K., Ladin, Z. V. I., Wei, J. Y., and Goldberger, A. L. (1995). Is walking a random walk? Evidence for long-range correlations in stride interval of human gait. J. Appl. Physiol. 349–358. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1995.78.1.349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). Gait dynamics, fractals and falls: finding meaning in the stride-to-stride fluctuations of human walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 555–589. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M. (2009). Gait dynamics in Parkinson’s disease: common and distinct behavior among stride length, gait variability, and fractal-like scaling. Chaos 19:026113. doi: 10.1063/1.3147408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Mitchell, S. L., Firtion, R., Peng, C. K., Cudkowicz, M. E., Wei, J. Y., et al. (1997). Altered fractal dynamics of gait: reduced stride-interval correlations with aging and Huntington’s disease. J. Appl. Physiol. 82, 262–269. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1997.82.1.262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Purdon, P. L., Peng, C. K., Ladin, Z., Wei, J. Y., and Goldberger, A. L. (1996). Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. J. Appl. Physiol. 80, 1448–1457. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1996.80.5.1448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Zemany, L., Peng, C., and Goldberger, A. L. (1999). Maturation of gait dynamics: stride-to-stride variability and its temporal organization in children. J. Appl. Physiol. 86, 1040–1047.

Google Scholar

Hollands, M. A., and Marple-Horvat, D. E. (1996). Visually guided stepping under conditions of step cycle-related denial of visual information. Exp. Brain Res. 109, 343–356.

Google Scholar

Huang, T., and Kuo, A. D. (2014). Mechanics and energetics of load carriage during human walking. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 605–613. doi: 10.1242/jeb.091587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, N., McGrath, D., and Stergiou, N. (2014). The influence of auditory-motor coupling on fractal dynamics in human gait. Sci. Rep. 4:5879.

Google Scholar

Iyengar, N., Peng, C. K., Morin, R., Goldberger, A. L., and Lipsitz, L. A. (1996). Age-related alterations in the fractal scaling of cardiac interbeat interval dynamics. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 271, 1078–1084.

Google Scholar

Jensen, A., Laird, M., Jameson, J. T., and Kelly, K. R. (2019). Prevalence of musculoskeletal injuries sustained during marine corps recruit training. Mil. Med. 184(Suppl. 1), 511–520. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usy387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y., Tagawa, Y., Obinata, G., and Hase, K. (2011). Robust control of CPG-based 3D neuromusculoskeletal walking model. Biol. Cybern. 105, 269–282. doi: 10.1007/s00422-011-0464-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knapik, J. J., Reynolds, K. L., and Harman, E. (2004). Soldier load carriage: historical, physiological, biomechanical, and medical aspects. Mil. Med. 169, 45–56.

Google Scholar

Krajewski, K. T., Dever, D. E., Johnson, C. C., Rawcliffe, A. J., Ahamed, N. U., Flanagan, S. D., et al. (2020). Load carriage magnitude and locomotion strategy alter knee total joint moment during bipedal ambulatory tasks in recruit-aged women. J. Biomech. 105:109772. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2020.109772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFiandra, M., Wagenaar, R. C., Holf, K. G., and Obusek, J. P. (2003). How do load carriage and walking speed influence trunk coordination and stride parameters? J. Biomech. 36, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9290(02)00243-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latash, M. (2016). Biomechanics as a window into the neural control of movement. J. Hum. Kinet. 52, 7–20. doi: 10.1515/hukin-2015-0190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latash, M., Levin, M. F., Scholz, J. P., and Schöner, G. (2010). Motor control theories and their applications. Medicina 46, 382–392. doi: 10.3390/medicina46060054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latash, M., Scholz, J. P., and Schöner, G. (2002). Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability. Exerc. Sports Sci. Rev. 30, 26–31. doi: 10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenton, G., Doyle, T. L. A., Lloyd, D. G., Higgs, J., Billing, D., and Saxby, D. J. (2019). Lower-limb joint work and power are modulated during load carriage based on load configuration and walking speed. J. Biomech. 83, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, B., Morris, S., and Netto, K. (2016). The effects of load carriage on joint work at different running velocities. J. Biomech. 49, 3275–3280. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2016.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, R. (2010). Kinetic changes associated with load carriage using two rucksack designs. Ergonomics 43, 1331–1341. doi: 10.1080/001401300421770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovalekar, M., Keenan, K. A., Beals, K., Nindl, B. C., Pihoker, A. A., Coleman, L. C., et al. (2020). Incidence and pattern of musculoskeletal injuries among women and men during marine corps training in sex-integrated units. J. Sci. Med. Sport 19, S1440–S2440.

Google Scholar

Loverro, K., Hasselquist, L., and Lewis, C. L. (2019). Females and males use different hip and knee mechanics in response to symmertric military-relevant loads. J. Biomech. 95:109280. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2019.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks-Tarlow, T. (1999). The self as a dynamical system nonlinear dynamics. Psychol. Life Sci. 3, 311–345.

Google Scholar

Matthis, J. S., Barton, S. L., and Fajen, B. R. (2017). The critical phase for visual control of human walking over complex terrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, E6720–E6729.

Google Scholar

McGinnis, P., and Newell, K. M. (1982). Topological dynamics: a framework for describing movement and its constraints. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1, 289–305. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(82)90017-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minassian, K., Hofstoetter, U. S., Dzeladini, F., Guertin, P. A., and Ijspeert, A. (2017). The human central pattern generator for locomotion: Does it exist and contribute to walking? Neuroscientist 23, 649–663. doi: 10.1177/1073858417699790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molloy, J. M., Pendergrass, T. L., Lee, I. E., Chervak, M. C., Hauret, K. G., and Rhon, D. I. (2020). Musculoskeletal injuries and united states army readiness part I: overview of injuries and their strategic impact. Mil. Med. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usaa027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moon, Y., Sung, J., An, R., Hernandez, M. E., and Sosnoff, J. J. (2016). Gait variability in people with neurological disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Hum. Mov. Sci. 47, 197–208. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2016.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukherjee, M., and Yentes, J. M. (2018). Movement variability: a perspective on success in sports, health, and life. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 28, 758–759. doi: 10.1111/sms.13038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newell, K. M., and Vaillancourt, D. E. (2001). Dimensional change in motor learning. Hum. Mov. Sci. 20, 695–715. doi: 10.1016/s0167-9457(01)00073-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordin, A., and Dufek, J. S. (2019). Reviewing the variability-overuse injury hypothesis: Does movement variability relate to landing injuries? Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 90, 190–205. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2019.1576837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nourrit-Lucas, D., Tossa, A. O., Zelic, G., and Delignieres, D. (2015). Learning, motor skill, and long-range correlations. J. Mot. Behav. 47, 182–189. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2014.967655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, M., and Newell, K. M. (2017). Search strategies in practice: Influence of information and task constraints. Acta Psychol. 182, 9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandy, M. G., and Andriacchi, T. P. (2010). Muscle and joint function in human locomotion. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 12, 401–433. doi: 10.1146/annurev-bioeng-070909-105259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, C., Mietus, J., Hausdorff, J. M., Havlin, S., Stanley, H. E., and Goldberger, A. L. (1993). Long-range anti-correlations and non-Gaussian behavior of the heartbeat. Phys. Rev. Lett. 70, 1343–1346. doi: 10.1103/physrevlett.70.1343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, C. K., Havlin, S., Stanley, H. E., and Goldberger, A. L. (1995). Quantification of scaling exponents and crossove phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. Chaos 5, 82–87. doi: 10.1063/1.166141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, R. M., Dalton, B. H., Blouin, J. S., and Inglis, J. T. (2017). Precise coding of ankle angle and velocity by human calf muscle spindles. Neuroscience 349, 98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravi, D. K., Marmelat, V., Taylor, W. R., Newell, K. M., Stergiou, N., and Singh, N. B. (2020). Assessing the temporal organization of walking variability: a systematic review and consensus guidelines on detrended fluctuation analysis. Front. Physiol. 11:562. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.00562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, J. T. E. (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ. Res. Rev. 6, 135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roerdink, M., de Jonge, C. P., Smid, L. M., and Daffertshofer, A. (2019). Tightening up the control of treadmill walking: effects of maneuverability range and acoustic pacing on stride-to-stride fluctuations. Front. Physiol. 10:257. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossignol, S., Dubuc, R., and Gossard, J. P. (2006). Dynamic sensorimotor interactions in locomotion. Physiol. Rev. 86, 89–154. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00028.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez, C. C., Murillo, D. B., Davids, K., and Hernández, F. J. M. (2016). Variations in task constraints shape emergent performance outcomes and complexity levels in balancing. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 1611–1622. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4563-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seay, J., Fellin, R. E., Sauer, S. G., Frykman, P. N., and Bensel, C. K. (2014a). Lower extremity biomechanical changes associated with symmetrical torso loading during simulated marching. Mil. Med. 179, 85–91. doi: 10.7205/milmed-d-13-00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seay, J., Frykman, P. N., Sauer, S. G., and Gutekunst, D. J. (2014b). Lower extremity mechanics during marching at three different cadences for 60 minutes. J. Appl. Biomech. 30, 21–30. doi: 10.1123/jab.2012-0090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedighi, A., and Nussbaum, M. A. (2019). Exploration of different classes of metrics to characterize motor variability during repetitive symmetric and asymmetric lifting tasks. Sci. Rep. 9:9821.

Google Scholar

Seifert, L., Button, C., and Davids, K. (2013). Key properties of expert movement systems in sport : an ecological dynamics perspective. Sports Med. 43, 167–178. doi: 10.1007/s40279-012-0011-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafizadeh, M., Crowther, R., Wheat, J., and Davids, K. (2019). Effects of personal and task constraints on limb coordination during walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Biomech. 61, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2018.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, K. M., Munro, B. J., and Steele, J. R. (2011). Effect of load mass on posture, heart rate and subjective responses of recreational female hikers to prolonged load carriage. Appl. Ergon. 42, 403–410. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, R., Graham, S. M., Forida-James, G. D., Connaboy, C., Clement, R., and Jackson, A. S. (2010). Perceived exertion and heart rate models for estimating metabolic workload in elite British soldiers performing a backpack load-carriage task. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 35, 650–656. doi: 10.1139/h10-053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, R. J., Graham, S. M., Connaboy, C., Clement, R., Pollonini, L., and Florida-James, G. D. (2017). Blood lactate thresholds and walking/running economy are determinants of backpack-running performance in trained soldiers. Appl. Ergon. 58, 566–572. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2016.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sporns, O., and Edelman, G. M. (1993). Solving Bernstein’s problem: a proposal for the development of coordinated movement by selection. Child Dev. 64, 960–981. doi: 10.2307/1131321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stadnitski, T. (2012). Measuring fractality. Front. Physiol. 3:127. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, N. A., Peoples, G. E., and Petersen, S. R. (2016). Load carriage, human performance, and employment standards. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 41(6 Suppl. 2), S131–S147.

Google Scholar

Telfer, S., Lange, M. J., and Sudduth, A. S. M. (2017). Factors influencing knee adduction moment measurement: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Gait Posture 58, 333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrier, P., and Dériaz, O. (2012). Persistent and anti-persistent pattern in stride-to-stride variability of treadmill walking: influence of rhythmic auditory cueing. Hum. Mov. Sci. 31, 1585–1597. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2012.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terrier, P., Turner, V., and Schutz, Y. (2005). GPS analysis of human locomotion: further evidence for long-range correlations in stride-to-stride fluctuations of gait parameters. Hum. Mov. Sci. 24, 97–115. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torre, K., and Balasubramaniam, R. (2009). Disentangling stability, variability and adaptability in human performance: focus on the interplay between local variance and serial correlation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 37, 539–50. doi: 10.1037/a0020556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torre, K., Delignières, D., and Lemoine, L. (2007). Detection of long-range dependence and estimation of fractal exponents through ARFIMA modelling. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 60, 85–106. doi: 10.1348/000711005x89513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torre, K., Vergotte, G., Viel, É., Perrey, S., and Dupeyron, A. (2019). Fractal properties in sensorimotor variability unveil internal adaptations of the organism before symptomatic functional decline. Sci. Rep. 9:15736.

Google Scholar

Turvey, M. (1990). Coordination. Am. Psychol. 45, 938–953.

Google Scholar

Van Orden, G., Kloos, H., and Wallot, S. (2009). Living in the pink: intentionality wellbeing and complexity. Handb. Philos. Sci. 10, 639–689.

Google Scholar

Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., and Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 331–350.

Google Scholar

Walsh, G. S., Low, D. C., and Arkesteijn, M. (2018). Effect of stable and unstable load carriage on walking gait variability, dynamic stability and muscle activity of older adults. J. Biomech. 73, 18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., and Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 213–216. doi: 10.1038/84054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, B. J., and Scafetta, N. (2003). Nonlinear dynamical model of human gait. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 67(5 Pt 1), 051917.

Google Scholar

West, B. J., and Shlesinger, M. F. (1989). On the ubiquity of 1/f noise. Int. J. Mod. Phys. B. 3, 795–819.

Google Scholar

Wills, J., Saxby, D. J., Lenton, G. K., and Doyle, T. L. A. (2019). Ankle and knee moment and power adaptations are elicited through load carriage conditioning in males. J. Biomech. 97:109341. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2019.109341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willy, R. W., DeVita, P., Meardon, S. A., Baggaley, M., Womble, C. C., and Willson, J. D. (2019). Effects of load carriage and step length manipulation on Achilles tendon and knee loads. Mil. Med. 184, e482–e489.

Google Scholar

Willy, R. W., Willson, J. D., Clowers, K., Baggaley, M., and Murray, N. (2016). The effects of body-borne loads and cadence manipulation on patellofemoral and tibiofemoral joint kinetics during running. J. Biomech. 49, 4028–4033. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2016.10.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woollacott, M., and Shumway-Cook, A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/s0966-6362(01)00156-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabala, M. E., Favre, J., Scanlan, S. F., Donahue, J., and Andriacchi, T. P. (2013). Three-dimensional knee moments of ACL reconstructed and control subjects during gait, stair ascent, and stair descent. J. Biomech. 46, 515–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2012.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: complexity, motor variability, load carriage, motor control, regulation, biomechanics, gait

Citation: Krajewski KT, Dever DE, Johnson CC, Mi Q, Simpson RJ, Graham SM, Moir GL, Ahamed NU, Flanagan SD, Anderst WJ and Connaboy C (2020) Load Magnitude and Locomotion Pattern Alter Locomotor System Function in Healthy Young Adult Women. Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol. 8:582219. doi: 10.3389/fbioe.2020.582219

Received: 10 July 2020; Accepted: 19 August 2020;
Published: 16 September 2020.

Edited by:

Yury Ivanenko, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Shunta Togo, The University of Electro-Communications, Japan
Francesco Travascio, University of Miami, United States

Copyright © 2020 Krajewski, Dever, Johnson, Mi, Simpson, Graham, Moir, Ahamed, Flanagan, Anderst and Connaboy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kellen T. Krajewski, a3RrMjBAcGl0dC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.