Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Anim. Sci., 12 May 2023
Sec. Animal Welfare and Policy

Animal welfare at slaughter: perceptions and knowledge across cultures

  • 1Humane and Sustainable Food Lab, School of Medicine, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, United States
  • 2Harvard Law School, Animal Law & Policy Program, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States
  • 3School of Veterinary Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 4Laboratório de Etologia Aplicada e Bem-Estar Animal, Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Florianópolis, Brazil
  • 5Asia Animal Happiness Consultancy, Puchong, Malaysia
  • 6Institute of Animal Science, College of Agriculture and Food Science, University of the Philippines Los Baños, Los Baños, Philippines
  • 7Himachal Pradesh Animal Husbandry Department, Veterinary Polyclinic, Shahpur, Himachal Pradesh, India
  • 8Department of Physiology, Faculty of Veterinary and Animal Sciences, The Islamia University of Bahawalpur, Bahawalpur, Pakistan
  • 9Department of Veterinary Medicine, Bangladesh Agricultural University, Mymensingh, Bangladesh
  • 10Department of Animal Physiology, Federal University of Agriculture, Abeokuta, Nigeria
  • 11Departamento de Medicina Veterinaria, Universidad Católica de Temuco, Temuco, Chile
  • 12Biomedical Research Institute, Darfur University College, Nyala, South Darfur, Sudan
  • 13Centre for Planetary Health and Food Security, Griffith University, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 14School of Environmental and Rural Science, University of New England, Armidale, NSW, Australia
  • 15United States Department of Agriculture - Agricultural Research Service, Livestock Behavior Research Unit, West Lafayette, IN, United States

Most people around the world eat meat and billions of animals are slaughtered each year to meet that demand. For many, eating meat is a biocultural activity steeped in tradition and formative in cuisines and identity. Eating meat, however, comes with a myriad of ethical and practical considerations. In tandem with animal rights and environmental sustainability concerns surrounding the impact of animal slaughter for meat, animal welfare science has identified numerous ways animals may suffer on an individual level during various methods of slaughter. Practices of slaughter differ around the world and the degree to which culture and regional interpretations of religion impact consumer expectations and perceptions of suffering at slaughter are relatively unknown. We begin to address some of these gaps by conducting a survey of 4,291 members of the general public to assess knowledge and attitudes around animal welfare at slaughter and religious slaughter, across 14 culturally and religiously diverse countries in local languages; Australia, Philippines, Nigeria, United Kingdom and United States (English), Bangladesh (Bengali), Brazil (Portuguese), Chile (Spanish), China (Traditional Chinese), India (Hindi and English), Malaysia (Bahasa Malay, Chinese and English), Pakistan (Urdu), Sudan (Arabic) and Thailand (Thai). Our results demonstrate that in highly developed countries where exposure to slaughter is low, comfort witnessing slaughter and knowledge about animal welfare at slaughter and the local application of stunning is also low. Cultural and religious differences exist by country, however in all countries except Bangladesh, the majority of participants stated that it mattered to them that animals do not suffer during slaughter, and in most countries, participants would prefer that animals be rendered unconscious before they are slaughtered (preslaughter stunning); including in countries where this practice is not currently widespread. These findings suggest that concern for the reduction of animal suffering during slaughter is a universal human tendency, rather than a cultural development, while opinion of how best to achieve this (i.e., to stun or not to stun) may be cultural and tied to local interpretations of religious slaughter requirements. The findings of this study serve as an indication for meat industries and governments that continual review and improvement of animal welfare processes at slaughter are required to continue to meet evolving general public sentiment.

1 Introduction

Meat eaters account for an estimated 86% of the human population (Buchholz, 2021), collectively consuming over 337 million tonnes each year (Food and Agricultural Organisation, 2020). Conventionally, science has attributed the path of human evolution and the development of larger brains of Homo-Sapiens as opposed to Homo-Erectus to the practice of eating other animals (Leroy and Praet, 2015). In a contemporary context, the industrialised and intensive farming systems that have developed to achieve the scale needed to meet demands for meat eating have been at the centre of environmental, ethical, nutritional and agronomical criticisms both in a growing body of scientific literature and in some societies more generally (Smil, 2004; Henning, 2011; Bramble and Fischer, 2015; Machovinaab et al., 2015). Once credited as the practice that allowed us to evolve into the species we are today, the structures created to support modern meat-eating are now presented as a key contributor to the demise of our species through climate change.

Aside from evolutionary, scientific and ethical debate, meat-eating remains a commonplace human activity. Over 73 billion non-human animals (hereafter animals, excluding fish) are slaughtered to supply the demand of humans each year, and that number is increasing for much of the world (Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development, and Food and Agricultural Organisation, 2022; Orzechowski, 2022).

Eating meat is a biocultural activity (Leroy and Praet, 2015) steeped in tradition, at the foundation of cuisines, and formative in identity and the experience of heritage. Slaughter, as the active process of ending the life of an animal, is variably approached with rites, rituals, utility and stigma, depending on the culture. What remains consistent in the process is that an animal will have their life brought to an end and, most frequently, this will be in a commercial slaughterhouse at the conclusion of an intensive farming process. Slaughter of animals for human food is an ethical issue, but whether the slaughter of a healthy animal without pain and suffering is a welfare issue is still open to debate and is discussed in detail elsewhere (Yeates, 2010). Although human societies and beliefs that influence slaughter vary across the world, the physiology and ability to experience pain and suffering within species subjected to slaughter tends to be similar. The experience of the animal during this process can be approached with the application of animal welfare science, using investigatory methods based on physiology and behaviour. As such, animal welfare scientists can also provide key information on approaches that may mitigate some of the suffering experienced by animals during the processes to which we subject them. One such method is the introduction of pre-slaughter stunning. The process of pre-slaughter stunning, usually via the application of a sudden electrical current or cranial bolt, aims to instantaneously render an animal unconscious and insensible until death (Fletcher, 1999; European Food Safety Authority (EFSA), 2004). Applied correctly, preslaughter stunning eradicates pain and suffering for individual animals. It is currently considered ‘best practice’ to increase animal welfare during slaughter (Federation of Veterinarian of Europe (FVE), 2002; Gibson et al., 2009; Gregory et al., 2010), and in some areas of the world such as Australia (Wood, 2013), Europe (European Commission, 2013), United Kingdom (UK Statutory Instruments, 1995), New Zealand (Ministry for Primary Industries, N.Z 2018), Brazil (Hötzel and Vandresen, 2022) and the United States of America (United States Department of Agriculture, 1958) required by law in most instances. Conversely, the slaughter of fully conscious animals is a common practice in lesser developed areas of the world mainly due to economic reasons (Fuseini and Sulemana, 2018) but also due to strict religious beliefs most commonly associated with Islam and Judaism (Jalil et al., 2018). This practice is also allowed by derogation in countries with stunning legislation in place. This manuscript assumes the position of current animal welfare and neurophysiological science consensus that correctly applied pre-slaughter stunning measures reduce suffering during slaughter.

Although the animal has the most to lose as a result of the common human practice of slaughter, the animals themselves are obviously not afforded input. Choices surrounding the manner in which they are raised, and die, are made by humans, and humans are not always guided by science (Hornsey, 2020; Lewandowsky and Oberauer, 2021). Meat consumption and, importantly, slaughter processes are varied by way of culture and are impacted by religion, power dynamics, social dynamics, biocultural cuisines and identity. It is therefore important to understand perceptions according to socio-political region (Neo and Emel, 2017).

A study conducted with university students in Malaysia, Thailand, The People’s Republic of China (henceforth China) and Vietnam found that regional religion had a large impact on attitudes to slaughter, but that in general these stakeholders cared about animal welfare at slaughter (Ling et al., 2016). This finding is echoed in Europe, where members of the public overwhelmingly stated that they care about welfare at slaughter (Kjærnes and Lavik, 2008). ‘Humane slaughter practices’ feature as a top concern for citizens in most European countries (Miele et al., 2011), and they believe that slaughter processes need improvement (Tomasevic et al., 2020). Likewise, members of the public in Spain and Mexico converge on animal welfare as a value in considering meat production (Estévez-Moreno et al., 2021). Additionally, some previous studies demonstrate that livestock industry stakeholders also express concern for animal welfare during slaughter (World Animal Protection, 2016; Sinclair et al., 2017; Sinclair et al., 2019b). In China, where pre-slaughter stunning is not mandated and often not conducted, livestock stakeholders identified pre-slaughter stunning as the one most important livestock animal welfare issue to be addressed during slaughter and transport (Li et al., 2018), and in another study in Bangladesh, China, India, Malaysia, Thailand and Vietnam, livestock leaders were mostly willing to embrace pre-slaughter stunning in a variety of pre-determined conditions (Sinclair et al., 2019a). During this latter study, livestock stakeholders in Malaysia, a predominantly Muslim nation, made the point that members of the public were largely unaware of farming processes, including slaughter (Sinclair et al., 2019a). This perceived lack of knowledge of the general public is not limited to one nation, and particularly seems to be the case when considering the practices of religious slaughter (Jalil et al., 2018). Despite regional differences, the fact that consumers in Europe expect meat production processes to take animal welfare and other social and ethical attributes into account (Boogaard et al., 2011; Van Loo et al., 2014; European Commission, 2016; Grunert et al., 2018) remains important for world trade and for predicting the transformative nature of trends in animal industries outside of Europe.

In general, little is known about the knowledge and preferences of the general public in relation to slaughter and animal welfare outside of Europe, and even less so in regard to the process of pre-slaughter stunning. The present study seeks to address this gap by surveying members of the public in 14 countries selected for their geographical dispersion, cultural diversity and varied predominant religions; Australia, Bangladesh, Brazil, Chile, China, India, Malaysia, Nigeria, Pakistan, Philippines, Sudan, Thailand, the United Kingdom (henceforth UK) and the United States of America (henceforth USA). We present our general findings in relation to declared meat eating, the perceived importance of reducing suffering during slaughter, knowledge and perceptions of pre-slaughter stunning and comfort levels in witnessing slaughter, and then present nation-specific contextual discussions and a legislation reference table. It is anticipated that the findings of this study will offer a useful resource to domestic livestock industries and governments in each nation, and to domestic and international advocates for improved animal protection and welfare.

2 Method

The findings presented in this manuscript pertain to our survey questions relating to meat, slaughter and pre-slaughter stunning. Findings associated with general perceptions to animal welfare and analysed by species are presented in an earlier companion manuscript (Sinclair et al., 2022a), with another manuscript presenting findings relating to hen welfare and egg production systems (Sinclair et al., 2022b).

2.1 Research ethics

This research was granted ethical approval by the University of Queensland in Australia (2020002752). Data collection was conducted between April and October 2021. Due to the active COVID-19 pandemic, additional precautions were taken, and some regional variations in data collection methods were required. Potential participants were presented with a statement of consent which they were required to agree to (verbally or physically by clicking a box) before commencing the survey.

2.2 Research tool

A survey was hosted online on the Google Forms platform and consisted of 24 items and four demographic questions (identified gender, age group, education level and religion) <supplementary file>. Participants were first asked if they understood the meaning of ‘animal welfare’, after which the following definition was provided:

“The welfare of animals’ refers to how well an animal is coping with the conditions in which it lives. An animal has good welfare if its needs are being met and hence it is healthy, comfortable, well nourished, safe, able to express important behaviour and not suffering from unpleasant states such as pain, fear and distress” - adapted from World Organisation for Animal Health (OIE, 2016).

The 24 research items were designed to reveal participants’ knowledge and perceptions and consisted of a range of question styles. Some animal welfare-related statements required the participant to attribute a level of agreement utilising a 7-point Likert scale (1 – strongly disagree, 4 – neither agree nor disagree, 7 – strongly agree), or a level of importance to the welfare of various species (1 – extremely unimportant, 4 – I don’t have an opinion/don’t know the species, 7 – extremely important). All research items were pilot tested and revised with research team consultation to ensure they could be clearly translated and were appropriate within each of the countries where the study was being conducted. As a result of this consultation, some alterations were made to the research tool, to enable the identical question to be used across all countries. One example of this was in question 3 (see supplementary file), where ‘animal welfare friendly products’ was replaced with ‘products kinder to animals’, as some countries do not have a concept of the former (largely due to availability). In the versions that were completed by individuals at home facilitated through Mechanical Turk and Instagram (see 2.3 Data collection for a full description of methods), the research tool included the addition of a “mid-way attention tool” to assess that participants were answering accurately and not randomly clicking. The ‘mid-way attention tool’ took the form of an additional question (11b) in which participants were asked ‘How important is reading a question instead of simply clicking numbers to finish quickly?’, with the accompanying instruction ‘As you are at the midway point this is a check to ensure you are still reading the questions. Please select 4’. Responses other than ‘4’ were removed from the dataset prior to analysis. Once programmed online in English for use in Australia, Nigeria, Philippines, UK and USA, the survey tool was replicated for each of the countries and translated by bilingual translators in-country into other languages as suitable to each general population as follows; Bangladesh (Bengali), Brazil (Portuguese), Chile (Spanish), China (Traditional Chinese/Mandarin), India (Hindi and English), Malaysia (Bahasa Malay, Chinese and English), Pakistan (Urdu), Sudan (Arabic) and Thailand (Thai).

2.3 Data collection

The main method of data collection in this study was face-to face-collection in public spaces. Effort was made to gain varied samples within each country, city and rural areas and geographical regions and demographics were collected (Table 1). Data collectors in each country were instructed and supervised by local research familiar ‘Country Collaborators’, who are also co-authors of this study. Once randomly selected and approached by data collectors, prospective participants were asked if they were willing to complete a 5-minute survey of opinions about animals for an international academic study. If the request was accepted, the data collectors ensured the participants were over 18 and that they identified themselves as residents of the country they were in. If they did not fulfill the required criteria they were thanked for their time and collection was ceased. If they did fulfill the criteria and agreed to participate, they were advised of the confidential and anonymous nature of the research and advised that they could cease their involvement at any time during the survey. In this method of face-to-face collection, the data collectors then verbally presented each question to the participants and entered the response into the online survey tool where it was anonymously stored. While this method of collection was highly demanding of human resources, it was adopted in an attempt to reduce the biases of self-selection of animal-leaning participants. As with all methods of data collection, however, face-to-face methods of collection do carry the risk of other bias. In this instance it is important to acknowledge the ‘enumerator effect’, whereby the behaviour or characteristics of the researcher approaching the respondent impacts both the likelihood to engage with the research and the responses they may give (Meta Di Maio and Fiala, 2018). The eleven countries in which this face-to-face method was used were Australia, Bangladesh, Chile, China, India, Malaysia, Nigeria, Pakistan, Philippines, Sudan and Thailand.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of respondents (n = 4,291) in Australia, Bangladesh, Brazil, Chile, China, India, Malaysia, Nigeria, Pakistan, Philippines, Sudan, Thailand, United Kingdom and United States.

In addition to the random approach in public spaces, in some countries, it was necessary to adopt additional methods of recruitment to ensure a sufficient number of responses. Pandemic lockdowns during the data collection period sometimes limited numbers in public spaces and direct contact presented a safety risk. In Malaysia and Australia, a QR code linked to the survey tool was randomly given to members of the public with a brief explanation of the study so that they could complete the survey without being in close proximity to the data collector. Recruitment of participants in Brazil, the USA and the UK was conducted entirely online. Recruitment in Brazil was conducted utilising the social media platform Instagram (Meta Platforms et al., 2010), where an advertisement (in Portuguese) saying “We want to hear from you. Access the link to collaborate with our research on the relationship between people and animals” invited the public to participate in the survey. The Instagram account was created exclusively to share the questionnaire and had no information on the identity of the authors or the research group. We strategically targeted participants to match the age and gender distribution of the Brazilian population and the questionnaire was initially pilot tested using 20 randomly recruited participants to ensure representative distribution was achieved. Participant recruitment for data collection in the USA and the UK was exclusively conducted utilising the online platform Amazon Mechanical Turk (Bezos, 2005). Amazon Mechanical Turk offers a small payment (in this study, a scaled proportion of minimum salary for five minutes of participation time) to ‘workers’ previously enlisted with the platform in exchange for completing the survey. Amazon Mechanical Turk has been validated as a survey participant recruitment tool (Robbins et al., 2016), with samples providing more diversity with comparable quality measured against standard samples (Paolacci and Chandler, 2014).

2.4 Data analysis

The data were initially collated, organised and cleansed by removing incomplete datasets and all data from participants who did not pass the midway attention test (see 2.2 Research tool). Data were imported into Microsoft SQL Server and Microsoft Excel for cleaning, and IBM SPSS and Minitab (Minitab Statistical Software LLC, 1972) where descriptive statistics were obtained for the demographics and research items. Based on the Likert scale (1-7), all attitudinal questions were assessed for means to approximate the magnitude of agreement for statement items, or associated importance for species-specific animal welfare perceptions (Norman, 2010). Variance between countries for each question was assessed with a one-way ANOVA in addition to variance between meat-eaters and non-meat-eaters, with a post hoc Tukey pairwise analysis test performed to assess homogeneity between countries. Significance was considered at p<0.05. Where means of question responses or groups were compared, a Student’s t-test was used. Percentages of agreement were calculated by identifying and quantifying the number of participants who expressed some level of agreement (Likert scale values 5, 6 or 7), against those who expressed disagreement (Likert scale values 1, 2 or 3) or neutrality (Likert scale value 4). Both calculations are presented together in results tables to build a picture of general agreement/associated importance, and the strength of that agreement/associated importance.

3 Results

A total of 4,291 participants engaged in this study across 14 geographically and culturally diverse countries (Australia, Bangladesh, Brazil, Chile, China, India, Malaysia, Nigeria, Pakistan, Philippines, Sudan, Thailand, UK and USA). Demographic distribution within these countries is presented in Table 1. Although distribution varies across countries, the total sample was closely split by gender (49.6% were male, 47.9% were female). Most participants were between 18 and 49 years of age. Participants with a university education are overrepresented as compared to the general population, and this is a consistent effect across countries.

3.1 Do you eat meat?”

Most respondents in most countries stated that they eat meat, with an average international aggregate of 89.5% (see Table 2). Nigeria had the highest rate of meat-eating (99%), followed by Sudan (98.8%), Thailand (98.8%), and China (98.4%). India was an outlier, with only 45% eating meat, with the second and third lowest being Australia (76.4%) and the UK (87.4%).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Percentage of respondents that eat meat, by country.

3.2 General knowledge around slaughter and stunning

The statement “I think that most of the animals in <country> are fully conscious and aware at the moment they are slaughtered” was met with a large degree of uncertainty, receiving comparatively high levels of ‘I don’t know’ across most countries (see Table 3; Figure 1). Uncertainty was highest in Thailand, Malaysia, the UK, Brazil and Australia, where approximately a third of participants selected ‘I don’t know’. Participants in Pakistan, China, the USA and Bangladesh were most likely to agree with this statement.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Percentage of respondents that believe most animals are fully conscious and aware at the moment they are slaughtered, by country.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Responses to statement “I believe most animals are fully conscious and aware at the moment they are slaughtered” sorted by general agreement.

“I think that stunning an animal unconscious in the moments before they are slaughtered is better for the animal” received less uncertainty, but did vary significantly by country (p<0.001), with a very large disparity of agreement between Pakistan and Sudan at the lowest levels of agreement, and Australia, the USA, the UK, Chile, Thailand and China at the highest levels – the latter, China, being the highest (see Table 4; Figure 2).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Means and standard deviations for the level of agreement to statements regarding slaughter, by country.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Responses to statement “I think that stunning an animal unconscious the moments before they are slaughtered is better for the animal” sorted by general agreement and graded by strength (1 – Strongly Disagree, 2 – Disagree, 3 – Somewhat Disagree, 4 – Neutral, 5 – Somewhat Agree, 6 – Agree, 7 – Somewhat Agree).

3.3 Animal welfare at slaughter

Most participants in all countries except Bangladesh agreed that animal welfare at slaughter mattered to them (see Table 4; Figure 3). The highest levels of agreement were found, in descending order, in Chile, Brazil, Australia, the UK and Thailand. The lowest level of agreement was in Bangladesh, with a neutral mean of 4.09 (neither agree or disagree). Interestingly, ANOVA analysis for responses to “it matters to me that animals do not suffer during slaughter” found no significant difference between meat-eaters and non-meat-eaters across all countries, meaning meat-eaters were not less likely to agree that reduction of suffering was important to them. ‘It matters to me that animals do not suffer during slaughter’ presented a country effect (p<0.001), meaning that participants in each country tended to answer in the same way as their fellow citizens, which was different from most other countries.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Level of agreement to statement “It matters to me that animals do not suffer during slaughter” sorted by general agreement and graded by strength (1 – Strongly Disagree, 2 – Disagree, 3 – Somewhat Disagree, 4 – Neutral, 5 – Somewhat Agree, 6 – Agree, 7 – Somewhat Agree).

One of the more polarising statements by country was ‘I am comfortable to watch an animal slaughtered for meat’. Participants in Thailand, Brazil, Chile, Australia, India and the Philippines had the highest rates of disagreement, with a mean below 2.5 (disagree). The most likely to state they were comfortable to watch slaughter were participants from Pakistan, followed by the USA and China (see Table 4; Figure 4). A significant country effect is present in regard to this statement (p<0.001), and a test for homogeneity of variances confirmed the significance for both mean and median responses.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Level of agreement to statement “I am comfortable to watch an animal slaughtered for meat” sorted by general agreement and graded by strength (1 – Strongly Disagree, 2 – Disagree, 3 – Somewhat Disagree, 4 – Neutral, 5 – Somewhat Agree, 6 – Agree, 7 – Somewhat Agree).

3.4 Preferences around stunning and religious slaughter

In response to the statement “I think stunning animals unconscious will reduce the taste of the meat”, participants in all countries disagreed, except for those in Pakistan, Bangladesh, the USA and Sudan, listed in order from highest strength of disagreement (see Table 4; Figure 5). Similarly, participants in Bangladesh, Pakistan, Sudan and Nigeria stated that they would not prefer to eat meat from animals that are stunned before slaughter (see Table 4; Figure 6), again listed in order from highest strength of disagreement. All of these countries have majority or significant Muslim populations, as reflected in the demographics table (see Table 1). The highest rate of agreement regarding a preference for meat from animals that have been stunned prior to slaughter is again found in Chile, Australia, the UK, the USA, China, Thailand and Brazil (see Table 4; Figure 6).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 Responses to statement “I think stunning animals unconscious prior to slaughter will reduce the taste of the meat” sorted by general agreement and graded by strength (1 – Strongly Disagree, 2 – Disagree, 3 – Somewhat Disagree, 4 – Neutral, 5 – Somewhat Agree, 6 – Agree, 7 – Somewhat Agree).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6 Responses to statement “I would prefer to eat meat from animals that are made unconscious for slaughter” sorted by general agreement and graded by strength (1 – Strongly Disagree, 2 – Disagree, 3 – Somewhat Disagree, 4 – Neutral, 5 – Somewhat Agree, 6 – Agree, 7 – Somewhat Agree).

Given that slaughter and killing are often approached from a religious lens in addition to, and sometimes interwoven with, the cultural context, we also applied additional analysis by religion across countries. When considering the statement “I would prefer to eat meat from animals that are killed using religious method of slaughter”, a Tukey post-hoc analysis demonstrated that similar responses could be identified by three distinct groups; (1) Atheist and non-Abrahamic religions, (2) Christians and (3) Muslims. When considering the earlier statement “I think that stunning an animal unconscious the moments before they are slaughtered is better for the animal” those similarities were reduced to two groups: (1) Muslims and (2) all others including atheists. Unsurprisingly, participants in Pakistan, Bangladesh and Sudan agreed that “I would prefer to eat meat from animals that are killed using religious methods for slaughter”, with Malaysian participants also leaning this way, along with, surprisingly, US Americans (see Table 4; Figure 7). The preference for eating meat from religiously slaughtered animals was closely correlated with the perception that stunning would affect meat quality, in particular, its taste (R2 = 0.789, p<0.001, Figure 8).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7 Responses to statement “I would prefer to eat meat from animals that are killed using religious methods for slaughter” sorted by general agreement and graded by strength (1 – Strongly Disagree, 2 – Disagree, 3 – Somewhat Disagree, 4 – Neutral, 5 – Somewhat Agree, 6 – Agree, 7 – Somewhat Agree).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8 Scatter Plot modelling “I think stunning animals unconscious will reduce the taste of the meat” (X) against “I would prefer to eat meat from animals that are killed using religious methods for slaughter” (R2 = 0.789, p<0.001).

4 Discussion

The welfare state of farmed animals is directly determined by on-farm, transportation and peri-slaughter practices; however, those practices are influenced by consumer attitudes expressed in supermarkets, voting booths and societal culture at large (Bell et al., 2017; Alonso et al., 2020). This study is the first research to illuminate consumer attitudes and societal culture around animal slaughter within many of the countries included herein. To the awareness of the authors, this is also the first time a comparative analysis has been conducted on attitudes to slaughter across culturally-diverse, key world regions. To acknowledge the impact of individual cultures we need to compare and understand across cultures, so this approach is important. Although efforts were made to obtain samples across geographical regions and residential zonings (rural vs metropolitan) the samples are not fully representative of the entire community within country. For this reason, full demographics are reported (Table 1), and the results and following discussion is designed to serve as a generalized snapshot of perceptions to slaughter across countries. Likewise, in some areas of the world the methods and practices of slaughter are diverse, largely depending on the size of establishment, but also depending on customs, technical skills and tools. While acknowledging the existence of domestic diversity of practice this study aimed to understand awareness and perspectives on the what is understood to be the most common practices of slaughter (Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Current ‘state of play’ summary reference table regarding the incorporation of pre-slaughter stunning in legislation and practice in each of the countries included in this study.

Although some of our findings are in line with known phenomena, such as generalised discomfort when witnessing slaughter, a significant proportion of findings serve to challenge any practice of applying perceived general sentiment across borders and specifically, into regions that host agricultural industries in earlier stages of development. Similarities of response across all of the diverse countries in this study may be considered commonalities of being human, and the many differences in which there was a significant country effect may be considered a product of culture together with socio-political and economic region.

With the exception of a mean neutral response from participants in Bangladesh, most participants agreed that avoiding suffering during slaughter mattered to them. How to achieve a reduction of suffering and what role religious slaughter plays was far less clear for participants in general, and knowledge levels regarding actual practice were very low in some countries. Crucially, reported comfort levels in witnessing slaughter varied considerably by culture.

4.1 General understanding around slaughter and stunning

The disparity between participants who believed that animals were most frequently slaughtered while fully conscious in their country (i.e., not rendered unconscious through stunning) versus the respective local practices that are believed to be most common and the legislative mandate status of the practice demonstrated a particular disconnection in some countries. For reference, we tabulated in-country perceptions of pre-slaughter stunning use against what we understand to be the most frequent actual use in-country, in addition to relevant federal law, to present further information by country (see Table 5).

In the USA, Australia and the UK most participants (between 79.9%-93.2%) assumed animals were fully conscious at slaughter when in reality, with a small exception of religiously slaughtered animals, pre-slaughter stunning is mandated by law and routinely practiced in all of these countries. Most participants in Pakistan, China, Nigeria and Bangladesh (between 74.8%-98.4%), also tended to agree that stunning is not routinely applied in their countries. In each of these countries, however, stunning is not believed to be routinely conducted or mandated. In each of these countries slaughter on occasion is conducted openly and exposure to slaughter both visually and practically is comparatively more common and therefore awareness around the process is increased. In contrast, slaughter in the USA, Australia and the UK is strictly legislated in highly industrialised processes than remain almost entirely hidden from public view (Leroy and Praet, 2017).

Many participants stated that they ‘don’t know’ if pre-slaughter stunning is frequently conducted in their country, particularly in Australia, Brazil, India, Malaysia, Philippines and the UK. This may suggest that confusion or purposeful ignorance plays a large role in perceptions of slaughter for many people, coupled with low levels of exposure and awareness around the slaughter process and, for these countries, a willingness to admit that. A notable exception to this was the USA and, to a lesser degree, Chile. Participants in the USA were particularly confident that animals weren’t stunned prior to slaughter (77.7%) when in fact it is legislated, with comparatively few stating ‘I don’t know’. This could be attributable to an individualism-tied, cultural tendency towards generalized self-competence (the belief in personal ability and efficacy) (Tafarodi and Swann, 1996), generalized confidence (Handy, 2001) and self-esteem that is not necessarily accurately correlated with ability or knowledge (Shtulman, 2010; Zoller Booth and Gerard, 2011), and therefore a strong preference against declaring ignorance. It is also possible that the use of the word ‘slaughter’ in the USA has connotations beyond large-scale slaughter operations and is instead understood as ‘killing’ in general, rather than the specific meaning of slaughter in terms of killing of livestock for meat. The term ‘slaughter’ is used in media hyperbole to describe killing via regular mass shooting events and may also be biased by exposure to the national pastime of wild game hunting.

In response to the statement that pre-slaughter stunning was better for the welfare of the animals, as current scientific understanding suggests, general disagreement existed in Pakistan and Sudan (see below for the role of religion), and neutrality and uncertainty led the way in Bangladesh, Brazil, India, Malaysia and the Philippines. This is not clearly explainable by ways of exposure and may in part be due to exposure to pre-slaughter stunning being incorrectly applied, or the connotation that the practice itself is violent and cruel; for example, by the addition of electronarcosis and thus extending what could be a one-step process of violence into two steps. Incorrect application of pre-slaughter stunning significantly reduces welfare and increases suffering, and witnessing unsuccessful or even successful attempts to stun animals using blunt instruments such as pipes, wooden logs or otherwise could result in the conclusion that pre-slaughter stunning is violent and cruel. In addition to the consideration of exposure, a lack of advocacy for pre-slaughter stunning or laws often result in reduced knowledge of the role pre-slaughter stunning can play in the mitigation of some suffering, and methods employed to achieve instantaneous insensibility (Riaz et al., 2021). The lack of knowledge and understanding of animal welfare problems facing farmed animals more generally as demonstrated in this study is reminiscent of findings in Europe, where the general public receives most of their information from the media (Miele, 2010), and in the very few studies conducted outside of Europe that investigate knowledge around slaughter and pre-slaughter stunning (Fuseini and Sulemana, 2018; Jalil et al., 2018).

4.2 Comfort witnessing slaughter

Although most participants state that they care that animals do not suffer, the purported levels of comfort, and conversely, uneasiness in witnessing slaughter was highly polarised by country. In this regard, the findings of this study could support an assumption of the universality of compassion or empathy as a human trait, while suggesting that approaches to death and killing are deeply cultural.

Participants in Thailand stated they were not comfortable witnessing slaughter; 93.3% of total Thai participants identified as Buddhist, a philosophical way of life that aims to practice non-violence and underpins the gentle culture frequently associated with Thailand (Phelps, 2004). Likewise, 86.2% of total participants from India identified as Hindu, a karmic world religion that teaches the principle of ‘ahimsa’ – non-harm, which includes violence to people, animals and oneself (Kumar, 2021). In addition to Thailand and India, participants in Australia, Brazil, Chile and the UK were the most likely to report feeling uncomfortable about witnessing slaughter. Considering participants in these countries were also the most likely to lead a vegetarian diet, report that avoiding suffering at slaughter mattered to them, and to report high levels of concern for farmed and companion animal welfare in general (Sinclair et al., 2022a), this is not surprising. In many of these countries exposure to animal slaughter of animals is less frequent as generations become more distant from rural upbringing. With slaughter largely conducted by large scale licensed facilities away from public view in Australia, Brazil, Chile and the UK, participants in these countries are also the least likely to have witnessed slaughter. Witnessing, and thus possibly becoming desensitised to slaughter, is more likely in Pakistan and China where participants were first and third most likely to state that they were comfortable watching slaughter. Although most animals in China are processed in large scale slaughter operations, wet markets in which live animals are purchased and slaughtered directly for members of the public are relatively common (Zhong et al., 2020), as is urban slaughter and ritual slaughter at celebratory festivals such as Eid in Pakistan (Mallhi et al., 2020). Transparency around trade of healthy animals and assurances of food safety, meat quality and freshness may also motivate meat-eaters in China to witness slaughter (Zhong et al., 2020), and the same is true in Pakistan (Waqas and Hong, 2019). Likewise, the celebratory association of sacrificial slaughter at the completion of the Hajj pilgrimage (the religion of Islam’s Fifth Pillar of worship) could contribute to higher comfort levels in witnessing slaughter. Underpinned by cognitive dissonance in western-based cultures (Rothgerber, 2020), this avoidance of thinking about or seeing the animals before, during or after slaughter (until the resulting unrecognisable packaged meat products) is not always shared in other countries. In addition to the existence of wet markets whereby animals are openly slaughtered in front of the public, this sentiment is further illustrated by successful institutions in China such as pork industry operated ‘Pig Planet’ in Jiaxing City, Zhejiang Province, which connects the animal to the end product (Qinglian Food Co. Ltd., 2021). At Pig Planet, families are invited to learn about pigs, build an appreciation for their intelligence and likability, and then safely move through walkways in the slaughter plant to observe the slaughter process, and able to purchase fresh meat products (Qinglian Food Co. Ltd., 2021). Given the lack of need to turn away, this cultural difference presents a substantial opportunity to establish animal welfare as a key element of product quality. From this, the unique opportunity presented by this desire to personally ensure the quality and value of purchases through transparency also places discerning Chinese consumers in a power position to keep domestic industry accountable.

In following a logic-based trend that considers exposure, resulting in desensitisation and culturally-driven consumer demands and celebrations, the USA was an outlier. Surprisingly, a majority of participants from the USA stated that they were comfortable witnessing slaughter; however, slaughter is rarely incorporated into daily life in this country and exposure is low. Although the reason for this divergent result is unclear, it could be associated with pride in the potential perception of a ‘frontiersman’ heritage and associated with game hunting, in which many members of the public participate in shooting animals as recreation (Bellesiles, 2000) or as an activity in self-sufficiency. The familiar nature of game hunting may also underpin a misinformed perception that ‘slaughter’ entails shooting at a distance and not intimate, close quarters, cut-and-bleed that is routine in slaughter operations. The popularity of permaculture and trending survivalist shows in the USA may also create a perception of slaughter tied to a social desirability to demonstrate self-sufficiency and capability to slaughter animals that stems from this.

4.3 The role of religion

Humans do not have a universal way of dealing with the issue of death; the construct and process of death is approached in variable ways around the globe (Palgi and Abramovitch, 1984). For this reason, many of the findings in relation to this study are unsurprising.

By way of reductive summary, religious slaughter varies from slaughter that is mainstream in highly developed countries. The two most commercially relevant types of religious slaughter are Shechita for Kosher (“ritually pure”) in Judaism and Halal (“lawful”) in Islam (Anil, 2012), with the latter being highly relevant to the present study. Both of these processes of religious slaughter involve the reciting of rites and cutting the carotid artery of restrained animals who die as a result of exsanguination. In Islam, the belief that the animals need to be well at the time of their ritual death causes disagreement amongst Muslim authorities as to the acceptability of rendering animals unconscious before the cut. Many Muslim authorities deem reversible methods of pre-slaughter stunning (such as percussive stun bolt to the skull or electrical stun) acceptable, as the animal is otherwise in full health and could theoretically recover from the stun; however, this acceptance is not universal (Farouk et al., 2014; Sinclair et al., 2019a). Perceived meat quality is impacted by ‘spiritual quality’ for Kosher and Halal consumers (Farouk et al., 2014) and, as supported by the findings of this study, participants in some Muslim majority nations (Bangladesh, Pakistan and Sudan) prefer meat from religiously slaughtered animals that have not been stunned and also believe that stunning adversely affects taste (see Table 4).

Muslim-majority Malaysia presented interesting findings in relation to religious slaughter that did not fit this trend. A notable proportion of Malaysians agreed they prefer animals to be unconscious and would prefer meat from animals made unconscious prior to slaughter; however, a similar proportion also agreed that they would prefer to eat meat from animals that have been subject to religious slaughter. Although some animals are routinely pre-slaughter stunned in Malaysia, a large amount are not, and this conflicting finding echoes previous findings that Malaysians are generally confused about halal slaughter and what it entails (Jalil et al., 2018; Sinclair et al., 2019a). Malaysia is a more developed Muslim country and exposure to slaughter is low in most areas, which is likely to contribute to this difference when compared with the lesser developed Muslim countries of Bangladesh, Pakistan and Sudan.

In considering Muslim-majority countries, participants in Bangladesh deviated from those in Malaysia, Pakistan and Sudan by reporting low levels of agreement that animal welfare during slaughter mattered to them. Only 50% of Bangladeshi participants stated it matters compared with 80% of participants in the other three countries. This could be due to the belief that strict ‘halal’ slaughter is not only better for humans and their spiritual selves but also for the animals in that the divinely mandated process, and the local interpretation of it, could not possibly cause unavoidable harm. In this way, personal responsibility, and therefore concern, to avoid suffering could have been diffused and instead allocated to divine powers. This hypothesis is supported by the findings of previous research with livestock leaders in Bangladesh, where sentiment was shared that Halal methods were unquestionably ‘better for the animals’ (Sinclair et al., 2019a).

While religiosity is likely to contribute to reduced levels of concern, this could also be impacted by developmental status. Bangladesh has the lowest rating on the United Nations ‘Human Development Index’ of the countries included in this study, with higher levels of poverty (United Nations Development Programme, 2018); therefore, animal welfare during slaughter may be considered less important than issues such as reliable sources of dietary sustenance and human welfare. Bangladeshis may simply not have the luxury of developing discussions about animal welfare during slaughter or engaging with the possibility of higher welfare products.

Personal preference to place trust in the perceived unquestionable correctness of a religious belief over the relative consensus of current animal welfare science is also supported by our findings in Sudan, Pakistan and to some degree Bangladesh again, whereby participants mostly disagreed that rendering animals unconscious prior to slaughter was better for the animals. The preference to be guided by religion may also explain unexpected findings in the USA, where a majority of participants identified as Christian (63%). Although Christianity does not contain religious slaughter rites or processes, a notable proportion of Americans stated that they would prefer to eat meat from animals subject to religious slaughter, suggesting a connotation that ‘religious’ is better, or more correct. USA participants, however, also mostly agreed that pre-slaughter stunning was better for the animals and that they would prefer meat from stunned animals although, in line with most Muslim countries in our study, USA participants also agreed that stunning reduced the taste of the meat. This potentially suggests that, as with Malaysia, understanding of what religious slaughter is may be reduced and that exposure to slaughter is low, despite a large portion of Americans stating that they are comfortable to watch slaughter.

In considering religious slaughter in the context of trade, Brazil and Australia are lead exporters of halal meat. This study presents attitudes of citizens within these countries towards animal suffering during slaughter as inconsistent with the abstaining of pre-slaughter stunning required to attend these markets. This indicates a risk to the social license of the meat industry in these countries, as the public becomes growingly aware of the practices used in animal production and slaughter.

Generally, religion clearly plays a significant role in the perception of animal welfare during slaughter, and that perception may diverge from scientific understanding that conscious animals suffer considerably more during slaughter. Special understanding and respect should be applied in collaboration and dialogue with religious leaders when looking to improve animal welfare during religious slaughter around the world, with the goal of applying scientific knowledge and advancements in ways that can be eventually embraced by the community. Ensuring respectful conversations regarding religious slaughter that are inclusive of interest groups with diverging attitudes will be integral to any solution-focussed collaboration.

4.4 Trending towards higher welfare globally

In many areas of the world, citizens are increasingly expressing concern for the welfare of farm animals (Thiermann and Babcock, 2005; European Commission, 2007; European Commission, 2016). This can compel governments to introduce minimum requirements to protect animals and industries to shift practices or become burdened with negative brand associations or abandonment (Hansen and Østerås, 2019). In many countries, the increased concern is thought to be fueled by a mixture of increasing intensification and industrialization of animal industries and animal abuse scandals, alongside shifting socio-ethical considerations, strong human bonds with companion animals, and the increasing scientific body of knowledge around the physiological and psychological states of animals (Clark et al., 2016; Robbins et al., 2016; Vargas-Bello-Pérez et al., 2017). Our previously published findings support the notion that animal welfare for both farmed and companion animals is receiving groundswell support (Sinclair et al., 2022a), including in lesser-developed nations and nations in which the dedicated scientific field of animal welfare has not yet become widespread.

These findings are echoed when considering the limited studies that specifically address attitudes to slaughter in the countries we have investigated. In Brazil, 72% of study participants believe fish should also be included in humane slaughter regulations (Rucinque et al., 2017), and Chinese consumers self-report a willingness to pay for animal welfare improved meat products (Lai et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2019) and Brazilian (Hötzel and Vandresen, 2022) and Chilean (Schnettler et al., 2009) consumers are generally opposed to controversial management practices in beef production.

The findings in this manuscript support this trend, with a large cross-cultural dataset demonstrating that most participants all over the world declare that it matters to them that animals do not suffer during slaughter. The findings also draw attention to the significant amount of trust the general public place in meat processors to ensure animal welfare standards are continually improved. Given this diffusion of great responsibility onto meat industries to uphold the evolving expectations of constituents and consumers everywhere, they have a responsibility and a sustainability imperative to continue serving their customers in a way that meets their evolving expectations. These expectations involve ensuring the industries provide the animals they raise and slaughter with the highest welfare interventions possible in their locale. Depending on the developmental status of domestic agricultural industries, this may look different. This could include tailored consultation with agricultural experts in animal welfare, implementing slaughter training programs, upgrading and exchanging slaughter and lairage processes and equipment, and monitoring for animal welfare and quality. If adopting the position that correctly applied pre-slaughter stunning techniques can eradicate suffering during slaughter, then applying these techniques is one crucial way to meet customer expectations. This could be specifically indicated in countries where participants readily agreed that the application of pre-slaughter stunning was better for the animals, yet, the practice has not been adopted into practice or introduced into legislation (China, Nigeria and Thailand).

It is important to note that although participants in this study state that it matters to them that animals do not suffer during slaughter, this does not equate to any knowledge regarding how to mitigate this suffering. This dynamic draws attention to the need for clear communication with consumers through reliable and transparent labelling. It is also clear that customers place large amounts of trust in producers, retailers and governments to act on their interests to the best of current scientific knowledge. Livestock stakeholders are encouraged to consult local and international resources on improving welfare during slaughter and best practices according to species. In regions and operations with budget constraints, in addition to the application of pre-slaughter stunning techniques, considerable improvements can be made for little to no additional cost, such as improving stockmanship (Hemsworth et al., 2009; Grandin, 2020b), and ensuring slaughter implements such as knives are sharp and well maintained to increase the speed of exsanguaniation and reducing the time to unconsciousness. Although measures such as the latter can serve to reduce time in suffering, it is important to note that a critical period of acute suffering (ie. approximately 14 seconds in sheep and 20 seconds in cattle after cleanly and swiftly severing both jugular and carotoid arteries) in which the animals are still conscious remains present where slaughter is devoid of stunning (Gibson et al., 2009; Gregory et al., 2010).

Taking measures to improve welfare at slaughter begins to address the evolving interests of citizens and consumers and can also bring benefits to operations. Although the purchase of equipment to apply pre-slaughter stunning, for example, involves an outlay of expenditure, it has the capacity to increase efficiencies, reduce injuries to workers otherwise tasked with slaughtering stressed and unpredictable animals, and meat quality is increased in animals not subject to severe stress (Linares et al., 2007; D’Eath et al., 2010; Paranhos da Costa et al., 2012; Schwartzkopf-Genswein et al., 2012; Grandin, 2020a).

4.5 Limitations

Some of the limitations of this study are common to survey methodologies. The data collected in surveys rely on self-reporting, which can be impacted by social desirability bias, and other biases. Survey respondents in some Asian nations, such as China, have a tendency to avoid extreme responses (Harzing et al., 2012), which may impact the findings. Additionally, in many countries participant demographics are not directly representative of the general population, with a balance in favour of more educated and younger participants, and a varied balance of male to female depending on the country. Particular limitation is noted with the dataset of Australian participants, and the self-selection of participants in Brazil. Despite randomized participant recruitment in Australia, a higher than societally representative sample of participants who do not eat meat (23%) was included (compared to 12.1% in reality). This is likely to have slightly skewed results in Australia towards pro-animal responses, and therefore caution is advised in interpreting the results for this country. Lastly, this study focusses on perceptions by country, based on previous research that demonstrates culture as a significant factor in attitudes to animal welfare. The authors acknowledge that regional and sub cultures can differ, and that geographical difference occur within countries. This could also be considered the case between rural and urban participants within geographical regions, and we did not measure for residential zoning in this present study. The authors also wish to draw attention to the diversity of language; although translations were careful and a definition of animal welfare was shared, connotations of specific translations may vary across regions. For example, ‘animal welfare’, ‘suffering’, ‘slaughter’ and ‘stunning’ may translate into a generic or existing concepts in local language that slightly differ. The subtlety of language is an important consideration specifically for international organizations interpreting these findings and developing training, awareness or campaign initiatives. Local collaboration is urged and a deeper investigation of language and meanings as they are locally understood is recommended. This study is designed as generalized snapshot of perceptions to slaughter across countries, and therefore findings should be interpreted as such.

5 Conclusion

Although important key differences exist in knowledge around slaughter, comfort witnessing slaughter, and sentiments towards religious slaughter and pre-slaughter stunning, one important commonality existed. Most participants, despite their cultural, geographical and religious differences, stated that it mattered to them that animals do not suffer as they are slaughtered. Although slaughter is met with potentially purposeful confusion in a bid to maintain cognitive dissonance and protection from vicarious traumas of suffering, this finding suggests that compassion and empathetic aversion to the suffering of animals is an underlying human trait.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Queensland Human Ethics Committee. The participants provided their informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MS conceptualised the study, sourced funding, recruited the research team, administered the project, developed the research tool, analysed the data and wrote the paper. NL, ML, AS, MI, OI, GN, and AA coordinated local data collection, provided advice on the localisation of methodologies, provided interpretation of results within the local context and proof-read the paper. MH coordinated local data collection, provided advice on the localisation of methodologies, provided interpretation of results within the local context and edited the paper. MC conducted deeper analysis of the data and contributed to data interpretation. GB conducted data collection, provided methodological advice and edited the paper. JM gave methodological advice, provided interpretation of results within the local context, contributed to writing the paper and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the Brooks McCormick Jnr. Harvard Law School Animal Law & Policy Program, which, with the support of Tiny Beam Fund, hosted the lead investigator and author (Dr. Michelle Sinclair) during this research project. The authors also wish to sincerely thank Open Philanthropy who facilitated research funding provisions, and Dr Huw Golledge who offered methodological and editorial insight. The authors also wish to draw attention to the concentrated efforts of the in-country data collection teams, who made this study possible through professionalism and determination, despite challenging setbacks associated with the pandemic, civil unrest and travel restrictions.

Conflict of interest

Author NL was employed by Asia Animal Happiness Consultancy.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The findings and conclusions in this publication are those of the authors and should not be construed to represent any official USDA or U.S. Government determination or policy. Mention of any trade name, proprietary product or specific equipment does not constitute a guarantee or warranty by USDA-ARS and does not imply its approval to the exclusion of other products that may also be suitable. The USDA-ARS is an equal opportunity and affirmative action employer, and all agency services are available without discrimination.

References

Alonso M. E., González-Montaña J. R., Lomillos J. M. (2020). Consumers' concerns and perceptions of farm animal welfare. Animals 10 (3), 385. doi: 10.3390/ani10030385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anil M. H. (2012). Religious slaughter: A current controversial animal welfare issue. Anim. Front. 2. doi: 10.2527/af.2012-0051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell E., Norwood F. B., Lusk J. L. (2017). Are consumers wilfully ignorant about animal welfare? Anim. Welfare 26 (4), 399–402. doi: 10.7120/09627286.26.4.399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellesiles M. A. (2000). Arming America: the origins of a national gun culture (New York: U.S.A: Alfred A. Knopf).

Google Scholar

Bezos J. (2005). Amazon mechanical turk (Seattle, USA: Amazon Wed Services).

Google Scholar

Boogaard B. K., Boekhorst L. J. S., Oosting S. J., Sørensen J. T. (2011). Socio-cultural sustainability of pig production: citizen perceptions in the Netherlands and Denmark. Livestock Sci. 140, 189–200. doi: 10.1016/j.livsci.2011.03.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bramble B., Fischer B. (2015). The moral complexities of eating meat (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Buchholz K. (2021). Eating meat is the norm almost everywhere. Available at: https://www.statista.com/chart/24899/meat-consumption-by-country/.

Google Scholar

Clark B., Stewart G. B., Panzone L. A., Kyriazakis I., Frewer L. J. (2016). A systematic review of public attitudes, perceptions and behaviours towards production diseases associated with farm animal welfare. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 29, 455–478. doi: 10.1007/s10806-016-9615-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Eath R. B., Turner S. P., Kurt E., Evans G., Thölking L., Looft H., et al. (2010). Pigs’ aggressive temperament affects pre-slaughter mixing aggression, stress and meat quality. Animal 4 (4), 604–616. doi: 10.1017/S1751731109991406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Maio M., Fiala N. (2018). Be wary of those who ask: a randomized experiment on the size and determinants of the enumerator effect. Available at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3299769.

Google Scholar

Estévez-Moreno L. X., María G. A., Sepúlveda W. S., Villarroel M., Miranda-de la Lama G. C. (2021). Attitudes of meat consumers in Mexico and Spain about farm animal welfare: a cross-cultural study. Meat Sci. 173, 108377. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2020.108377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

European Commission (2007). Attitudes of Europeans towards animal welfare: report (Brussels: Belguim: European Commission).

Google Scholar

European Commission (2013). Council regulation (EC) no 1099/2009 of 24 September 2009 on the protection of animals at the time of killing. Available at: https://eur-lex.europa.eu/eli/reg/2009/1099/oj.

Google Scholar

European Commission (2016). Attitudes of Europeans towards animal welfare: Report. (Brussels: Belguim: European Commission). Available at: https://data.europa.eu/doi/10.2875/884639.

Google Scholar

European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) (2004). Scientific report of the scientific panel for animal health and welfare on a request from the commission related to welfare of animals during transport. EFSA J 44, 1–36. doi: 10.2903/j.efsa.2004.44

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farouk M. M., Al-Mazeedi H. M., Sabow A. B., Bekhit A. E. D., Adeyemi K. D., Sazili A. Q., et al. (2014). Halal and kosher slaughter methods and meat quality: a review. Meat Sci. 98 (3), 505–519. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2014.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Federation of Veterinarian of Europe (FVE) (2002). Slaughter without stunning and food labeling: briefing note. Available at: www.veterinaire.frhttps://www.veterinaire.fr/fileadmin/user_upload/documents/outils-et-services/Index_juridique/Premiere_lettre_de_M_a_Z/Religious_slaughter_and_food_chain.pdf.

Google Scholar

Fletcher D. L. (1999). Symposium: recent advances in poultry slaughter technology. J. Poultry Sci. 78, 277–281.

Google Scholar

Food and Agricultural Organisation (2020). FAOSTAT ‘Livestock primary’ data 2020. Available at: http://www.fao.org/faostat/en/#data/QL.

Google Scholar

Fuseini A., Sulemana I. (2018). An exploratory study of the influence of attitudes toward animal welfare on meat consumption in Ghana. Food ethics 2, 57–75. doi: 10.1007/s41055-018-0028-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson T. J., Johnson C. B., Murrell J. C., Hulls C. M., Mitchinson S. L., Stafford K. J., et al. (2009). Electroencephalographic responses of halothane-anaesthetised calves to slaughter by ventral-neck incision without prior stunning. New Z. Veterinary J. 57 (2), 77–83. doi: 10.1080/00480169.2009.36882

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandin T. (2020a). “The importance of good pre-slaughter handling to improve meat quality in cattle, pigs, sheep and poultry,” in The slaughter of farmed animals: practical ways of enhancing animal welfare (Wallingford, UK: CABI).

Google Scholar

Grandin T. (2020b). Improving animal welfare: a practical approach (Wallingford: U.K: Cabi).

Google Scholar

Gregory N. G., Fielding H. R., Wenzlawowicz M.v., Holleben K.v. (2010). Time to collapse following slaughter without stunning in cattle. Meat Sci. 85, 66–69. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2009.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grunert K. G., Sonntag W. I., Glanz-Chanos V., Forum S. (2018). Consumer interest in environmental impact, safety, health and animal welfare aspects of modern pig production: results of a cross-national choice experiment. Meat Sci. 137, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2017.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handy C. (2001). Tocqueville revisited. the meaning of American prosperity. Harvard Business Rev. 79 (1), 57–63.

Google Scholar

Hansen B. G., Østerås O. (2019). Farmer welfare and animal welfare-exploring the relationship between farmer’s occupational well-being and stress, farm expansion and animal welfare. Prev. Veterinary Med. 170, 104741. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2019.104741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harzing A.-W., Brown M., Köster K., Zhao S. (2012). Response style differences in cross-national research: Dispositional and situational determinants. MIR: Manage. Int. Rev. 52 (3), 341–363.

Google Scholar

Hemsworth P. H., Barnett J. L., Coleman G. J. (2009). The integration of human-animal relations into animal welfare monitoring schemes. Anim. Welfare 18 (4), 335–345. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600000737

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henning B. (2011). Standing in livestock’s ‘“Long shadow”’: the ethics of eating meat on a small planet. Ethics Environ. 16 (2), 63–93. doi: 10.2979/ethicsenviro.16.2.63

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornsey M. J. (2020). Why facts are not enough: understanding and managing the motivated rejection of science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 29 (6), 583–591. doi: 10.1177/0963721420969364

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hötzel M. J., Vandresen B. (2022). Brazilians' attitudes to meat consumption and production: present and future challenges to the sustainability of the meat industry. Meat Sci. 192, 108893. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2022.108893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jalil N. S. A., Tawde A. V., Zito S., Sinclair M., Fryer C., Idrus Z., et al. (2018). Attitudes of the public towards halal food and associated animal welfare issues in two countries with predominantly Muslim and non-Muslim populations. PLoS One 13 (10), e0204094. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0204094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjærnes U., Lavik R. (2008). “Opinions on animal welfare and food consumption in seven European countries,” in Welfare quality reports: consumption, distribution and production of farm animal welfare. Eds. Kjærnes U., Bock B. B., Roe E., Roex J. (Cardiff, UK: Cardiff University).

Google Scholar

Kumar S. (2021). “Veganism, Hinduism, and Jainism in India: a geo-cultural enquiry,” in The routledge handbook of vegan studies (London: UK: Routledge), 10.

Google Scholar

Lai J., Wang H., Ortega D. L., Olynk Widmar N. J. (2018). Factoring Chinese consumers’ risk perceptions into their willingness to pay for pork safety, environmental stewardship, and animal welfare. Food Control 85, 423–431. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2017.09.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leroy F., Praet I. (2015). Meat traditions. the co-evolution of humans and meat. Appetite 90, 200–2011. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leroy F., Praet I. (2017). Animal killing and postdomestic meat production. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 30, 67–86. doi: 10.1007/s10806-017-9654-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewandowsky S., Oberauer K. (2021). Worldview-motivated rejection of science and the norms of science. Cognition 215, 104820. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li X., Zito S., Sinclair M., Phillips C. J. C. (2018). Perception of animal welfare issues during Chinese transport and slaughter of livestock by a sample of stakeholders in the industry. PLoS One 13 (6), e0197028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linares M. B., Bornez R., Vergara H. (2007). Effect of different stunning systems on meat quality of light lamb. Meat Sci 76, 675–681. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2007.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ling R., Zulkifli I., Lampang P., Nhiem D., Wang Y., Phillips C. (2016). Attitudes of students from south-east and east Asian countries to slaughter and transport of livestock. Anim. Welfare 25 (3), 377–387. doi: 10.7120/09627286.25.3.377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machovinaab B., Feeley K. J., Ripplec W. J. (2015). Biodiversity conservation: the key is reducing meat consumption. Sci. Total Environ. 536, 419–431. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mallhi T. H., Khan Y. H., Alotaibi N. H., Alzarea A. I., Tanveer N., Khan A. (2020). Celebrating Eid-ul-Adha in the era of the COVID-19 pandemic in Pakistan: potential threats and precautionary measures. Clin. Microbiol. Infection 26 (12), 1714–1715. doi: 10.1016/j.cmi.2020.07.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meta Platforms, Krieger M., Systrom K. (2010). Instagram (Menlo Park, California, USA).

Google Scholar

Miele M. (2010). Report Concerning Consumer Perceptions and Attitudes towards Farm Animal Welfare (Brussels, Belgium: European Animal Welfare Platform), pp. 1–16.

Google Scholar

Miele M., Veissier I., Evans A., Botreau R. (2011). Animal welfare: establishing a dialogue between science and society. Anim. welfare 20 (1), 103. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600002475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministry for Primary Industries, N.Z (2018). Code of welfare: commerical slaughter. Ed. Industries M. (Wellington: New Zealand Government).

Google Scholar

Minitab Statistical Software LLC (1972). Minitab statistical software.

Google Scholar

Neo H., Emel J. (2017). Geographies of meat: politics, economy and culture (London, UK: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Norman G. (2010). Likert scales, levels of measurement and the “laws” of statistics. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 15, 625–632. doi: 10.1007/s10459-010-9222-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development, and Food and Agricultural Organisation (2022). OECD-FAO agricultural outlook 2020-2029 (Paris, France: Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development). Available at: https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/sites/29248f46-en/index.html?itemId=/content/component/29248f46-en.

Google Scholar

Orzechowski K. (2022). Global animal slaughter statistics & charts: 2022 update (San Diego, CA: Faunalytics). Available at: https://faunalytics.org/global-animal-slaughter-statistics-charts-2022-update/.

Google Scholar

Palgi P., Abramovitch H. (1984). Death: a cross-cultural perspective. Annu. Rev. Anthropology 13, 385–417. doi: 10.1146/annurev.an.13.100184.002125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paolacci G., Chandler J. (2014). Inside the turk: Understanding mechanical turk as a participant pool. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23 (3), 184–188. doi: 10.1177/0963721414531598

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paranhos da Costa M. J. R., Huertas S. M., Gallo C., Dalla Costa O. A. (2012). Strategies to promote farm animal welfare in Latin America and their effects on carcass and meat quality traits. Meat Sci. 92 (3), 221–226. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2012.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps N. (2004). The great compassion: Buddhism and animal rights (New York, U.S.A: Lantern Books).

Google Scholar

Qinglian Food Co. L. T. D (2021). Pig planet (Qinglian Food Co. Ltd.). Available at: http://www.qinglianfood.com/index.php?m=content&c=index&a=lists&catid=17.

Google Scholar

Riaz M. N., Irshad F., Riaz N. M., Regenstein J. M. (2021). Pros and cons of different stunning methods from a halal perspective: a review. Transl. Anim. Sci. 5, 154. doi: 10.1093/tas/txab154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins J. A., Franks B., Weary D. M., v.K M. A. G. (2016). Awareness of ag-gag laws erodes trust in farmers and increases support for animal welfare regulations. Food Policy 61, 121–125. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2016.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothgerber H. (2020). Meat-related cognitive dissonance: a conceptual framework for understanding how meat eaters reduce negative arousal from eating animals. Appetite 146, 104511. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucinque D. S., Souza A. P. O., Molento C. F. M. (2017). Perception of fish sentience, welfare and humane slaughter by highly educated citizens of bogotá, Colombia and curitiba, brazil. PLoS One 12, e0168197 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnettler B., Vidal R., Silva R., Vallejos L., Sepúlveda N. (2009). Consumer willingness to pay for beef meat in a developing country: the effect of information regarding country of origin, price and animal handling prior to slaughter. Food Qual. Preference 20, 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2008.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartzkopf-Genswein K. S., Faucitano L., Dadgar S., Shand P., González L. A., Crowe T. G. (2012). Road transport of cattle, swine and poultry in north America and its impact on animal welfare, carcass and meat quality: a review. Meat Sci. 92 (3), 227–243. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2012.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shtulman A. (2010). “Confidence without competence in the evaluation of scientific claims,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 32. Available at: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1rg673m5.

Google Scholar

Sinclair M., Idrus Z., Burns G. L., Phillips C. J. C. (2019a). Livestock stakeholder willingness to embrace preslaughter stunning in key Asian countries. Animals 9 (5), 244. doi: 10.3390/ani9050224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair M., Lee N., Hötzel M. J., Luna M. T. D., Sharma A., Idris M., et al. (2022a). International perceptions of animals and the importance of their welfare. Front. Anim. Sci. 3, 960379. doi: 10.3389/fanim.2022.960379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair M., Lee N., Hötzel M. J., Luna M. T. D., Sharma A., Idris M., et al. (2022b). Consumer attitudes towards egg production systems and hen welfare across the world. Front. Anim. Sci. 3, 995430. doi: 10.3389/fanim.2022.995430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair M., Yan W., Phillips C. (2019b). Attitudes of pig and poultry industry stakeholders in guangdong province, China, to animal welfare and farming systems. Animals 9 (11), 860. doi: 10.3390/ani9110860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair M., Zito S., Idrus Z., Yan W., Nhiem D., Lampang P., et al. (2017). Attitudes of stakeholders to animal welfare during slaughter and transport in SE and e Asia. Anim. Welfare 26 (4), 417–425. doi: 10.7120/09627286.26.4.417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smil V. (2004). Eating meat: evolution, patterns, and consequences. Population Dev. Rev. 28 (4), 599–820. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2002.00599.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tafarodi R. W., Swann W. B. (1996). Individualism-collectivism and global self-esteem: evidence for a cultural trade-off. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 27 (6), 651–672. doi: 10.1177/0022022196276001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiermann A. B., Babcock S. (2005). Animal welfare and international trade. Rev Sci Tech. 24 (2), 747–755.

Google Scholar

Tomasevic I., Bahelka I., Čítek J., Čandek-Potokar M., Djekić I., Getya A., et al. (2020). Attitudes and beliefs of Eastern European consumers towards animal welfare. Animals 10 (7), 1220. doi: 10.3390/ani10071220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

UK Statutory Instruments. (1995). The welfare of animals (slaughter or killing) regulations 1995. Available at: https://www.legislation.gov.uk/uksi/1995/731/made.

Google Scholar

United Nations Development Programme (2018). Human development report (New York City, USA: United Nations).

Google Scholar

United States Congress (1978). United States Code Annotated. Title 7. Agriculture. Chapter 48 (Humane Methods of Livestock Slaughter). Available at: https://www.govinfo.gov/content/pkg/USCODE-2015-title7/pdf/USCODE-2015-title7-chap48.pdf.

Google Scholar

Van Loo E. J., Caputo V., Nayga R. M., Verbeke W. (2014). Consumers’ valuation of sustainability labels on meat. Food Policy 49 (1), 137–150. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2014.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas-Bello-Pérez E., Miranda-De La Lama G. C., Teixeira D. L., Enríquez-Hidalgo D., Tadich T., Lensink J. (2017). Farm animal welfare influences on markets and consumer attitudes in Latin America: the cases of Mexico, Chile and Brazil. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 30, 697–713. doi: 10.1007/s10806-017-9695-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waqas A., Hong C. (2019). Study on consumer behaviour and food safety of organic products in Pakistan. E3S Web Conf. 78, 02021. doi: 10.1051/e3sconf/20197802021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood A. (2013). Animal welfare under the shari’a. Macquarie Law J. 12, 155–128.

Google Scholar

World Animal Protection (2016). Consumo às cegas - percepção do consumidor sobre o bem-estar animal (London: UK: World Animal Protection).

Google Scholar

World Organisation for Animal Health (OIE) (2016). OIE terrestrial animal health code. chapter 7.1. introduction to the recommendations for animal welfare. Available at: https://www.woah.org/fileadmin/Home/eng/Health_standards/tahc/2016/en_chapitre_aw_introduction.htm.

Google Scholar

Xu L., Yang X., Wu L., Chen X., Chen L., Tsai F.-S. (2019). Consumers’ willingness to pay for food with information on animal welfare, lean meat essence detection, and traceability. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16 (19), 3616. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16193616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeates J. W. (2010). Death is a welfare issue. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 23, 229–241. doi: 10.1007/s10806-009-9199-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong S., Crang M., Zeng G. (2020). Constructing freshness: the vitality of wet markets in urban China. Agric. Hum. Values 37 (1), 175–185. doi: 10.1007/s10460-019-09987-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoller Booth M., Gerard J. M. (2011). Self-esteem and academic achievement: a comparative study of adolescent students in England and the united states. Compare: A J. Comp. Int. Educ. 41 (5), 629–648. doi: 10.1080/03057925.2011.566688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: animal welfare, international, perceptions, general public, attitudes, survey, slaughter, stunning

Citation: Sinclair M, Hötzel MJ, Lee NYP, de Luna MCT, Sharma A, Idris M, Islam MA, Iyasere OS, Navarro G, Ahmed AA, Burns GL, Curry M and Marchant JN (2023) Animal welfare at slaughter: perceptions and knowledge across cultures. Front. Anim. Sci. 4:1141789. doi: 10.3389/fanim.2023.1141789

Received: 10 January 2023; Accepted: 17 April 2023;
Published: 12 May 2023.

Edited by:

Rebecca E. Nordquist, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Coen Van Wagenberg, Wageningen University and Research, Netherlands
Carla Forte Maiolino Molento, Federal University of Paraná, Brazil

Copyright © 2023 Sinclair, Hötzel, Lee, de Luna, Sharma, Idris, Islam, Iyasere, Navarro, Ahmed, Burns, Curry and Marchant. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michelle Sinclair, bS5zaW5jbGFpcjZAdXEuZWR1LmF1; bXNpbmNsckBzdGFuZm9yZC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.