Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 08 May 2020
Sec. Biomechanics and Control of Human Movement

Sustained Isometric Wrist Flexion and Extension Maximal Voluntary Contractions Similarly Impair Hand-Tracking Accuracy in Young Adults Using a Wrist Robot

  • 1Faculty of Science, Ontario Tech University, Oshawa, ON, Canada
  • 2Faculty of Applied Health Sciences, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, Canada
  • 3Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genoa, Italy
  • 4Faculty of Health Sciences, Ontario Tech University, Oshawa, ON, Canada

Due to their stabilizing role, the wrist extensor muscles demonstrate an earlier onset of performance fatigability and may impair movement accuracy more than the wrist flexors. However, minimal fatigue research has been conducted at the wrist. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine how sustained isometric contractions of the wrist extensors/flexors influence hand-tracking accuracy. While gripping the handle of a three-degrees-of-freedom wrist manipulandum, 12 male participants tracked a 2:3 Lissajous curve (±32° wrist flexion/extension; ±18° radial/ulnar deviation). A blue, circular target moved about the trajectory and participants tracked the target with a yellow circle (corresponding to the handle's position). Five baseline tracking trials were performed prior to the fatiguing task. Participants then exerted either maximal wrist extension or flexion force (performed on separate days) against a force transducer until they were unable to maintain 25% of their pre-fatigue maximal voluntary contraction (MVC). Participants then performed 7 tracking trials from immediately post-fatigue to 10 min after. Performance fatigability was assessed using various metrics to account for errors in position-tracking, error tendencies, and movement smoothness. While there were no differences in tracking error between flexion/extension sessions, tracking error significantly increased immediately post-fatigue (Baseline: 1.40 ± 0.54°, Post-fatigue: 2.02 ± 0.51°, P < 0.05). However, error rapidly recovered, with no differences in error from baseline after 1-min post-fatigue. These findings demonstrate that sustained isometric extension/flexion contractions similarly impair tracking accuracy of the hand. This work serves as an important step to future research into workplace health and preventing injuries of the distal upper-limb.

Introduction

Work by Holmes et al. (2015) demonstrated that the wrist extensor muscles of the forearm contribute more to joint rotational stiffness (JRS) than the wrist flexors during external wrist perturbations. As greater JRS tends to result in a greater resistance to sudden disturbances (Cholewicki and McGill, 1996; Brown and Potvin, 2007), the wrist extensor muscles have been labeled as the primary stabilizers of the wrist. Further evidence for this hypothesis comes from studies assessing muscle activity, or from studies calculating co-contraction from muscle activity, both of which are surrogate measures for joint stiffness (De Serres and Milner, 1991; Cholewicki and McGill, 1996; van Loon et al., 2001; Franklin and Milner, 2003). The wrist extensors exhibit significantly greater co-contraction during both handgrip forces and wrist exertions than the flexors (Forman et al., 2019). The wrist extensors also demonstrate less task-dependency; they exhibit high levels of activity regardless of task-parameters (Mogk and Keir, 2003; Forman et al., 2019). This continuous, elevated activity predisposes the wrist extensors to an earlier onset of fatigue (Hägg and Milerad, 1997) and is likely the primary reason why they demonstrate a higher incidence of overuse injuries than the flexors (Shiri et al., 2006). For instance, the prevalence of lateral epicondylitis (which affects the wrist extensors) is ~1–3% in the average population (Allander, 1974; Verhaar, 1994; Shiri et al., 2006, 2007), but can vary wildly in different occupational settings. In tennis players, the prevalence is thought to be closer to 35–40%, although this number seems to increase with age (Gruchow and Pelletier, 1979; Carroll, 1981). In mild cases, lateral epicondylitis can be treated with improved rest, physical therapy, and custom braces, but in severe cases, can result in prolonged work absence and require invasive surgery. Both the prevalence and the severity of lateral epicondylitis are worse than medial epicondylitis (affects the wrist flexors; approximate prevalence of 0.4% in the general population; Shiri et al., 2006).

Given the broad scope of fatigue as a field of study, and the inconsistency in which fatigue is defined, recent literature has proposed a taxonomy to provide clearer communication between studies (Kluger et al., 2013; Enoka and Duchateau, 2016). According to this work, fatigue should be defined as a symptom in which both physical and cognitive function may be limited through interactions of perceived fatigability and performance fatigability (Enoka and Duchateau, 2016). Perceived fatigability refers to the subjective state of the individual and thus involves subjective measures, while performance fatigability is measured through objective laboratory-based assessments characterizing the functional decline of performance (Marrelli et al., 2018). Performance fatigability (modulated by both muscle contractile function and by voluntary muscle activation, or classically termed peripheral and central fatigue; Kluger et al., 2013), can manifest experimentally as decreased movement accuracy (Missenard et al., 2008b), impaired proprioception acuity (Pedersen et al., 1999; Mugnosso et al., 2019), decreased co-contraction during precision movements (Gribble et al., 2003; Missenard et al., 2008b), and decreased peak contractile speed and torque generation (de Haan et al., 1989). These factors not only compromise joint stability but also contribute to greater signal-dependent noise (SDN; signal meaning the optimal, ideal force required to perform a task, and noise meaning any deviation from that ideal) (Missenard et al., 2008a). The result is an overall increase in force variability, which reduces the accuracy of precision movements. Greater movement error has real-world implications. While the consequences of performance fatigability can contribute to the development of chronic overuse injuries, impairments to movement accuracy may lead to performance decrements and greater risks of suffering acute injuries (Parijat and Lockhart, 2008). Understanding how performance fatigability manifests, and the specific ways that it impairs performance, is an important step in mitigating its potentially harmful effects. This is particularly important in the context of the distal upper-limb, as the hand makes the final interface with the external environment.

However, there is currently limited research into how performance fatigability develops in the forearm, with most work centered around office mouse use (Huysmans et al., 2008). Additionally, we are aware of only one study that has examined performance fatigability between opposing muscle groups (Jaric et al., 1997). In this study, agonist muscle fatigue caused greater velocity, acceleration, and deceleration deficits in the agonist than the antagonist (minimal differences were seen whether the agonists were the elbow flexors or extensors). There is insufficient literature to conclude what influence performance fatigability of the forearm has on hand-tracking accuracy. The potential specificity of performance fatigability between forearm muscle groups is also unknown. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to examine hand-tracking accuracy prior to and following a single bout of maximal, sustained, isometric wrist extension, or wrist flexion contraction. Hand-tracking error was examined while performing on a three-degrees-of-freedom wrist manipulandum, and hand movement incorporated wrist flexion/extension and radial/ulnar deviation. Tracking error was expected to increase following sustained contractions of either muscle group. However, it was hypothesized that tracking error would be greater following wrist extension fatigue, given the evidence that the wrist extensors contribute more to wrist stability than the flexors.

Methods

Participants

Experimental procedures were approved by the research ethics boards (REB) of Brock University (REB# 16-263) and Ontario Tech University (REB# 15044). Written consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiment. Twelve right-handed males (Height: 180.2 ± 7.7 cm; Weight: 77.4 ± 10.4 kg; Age: 23.9 ± 2.7 years) were recruited for this study. Participants were excluded if they presented with any upper-body, neuromuscular injuries.

Experimental Setup

Participants were seated with their dominant forearm supported in a three-degrees-of-freedom wrist manipulandum (WristBot, Genoa, Italy; Masia et al., 2009; Iandolo et al., 2019) with their hand firmly gripping the device's handle (Figure 1A). All participants had previous experience using the manipulandum (Forman et al., 2020). The manipulandum was positioned at a comfortable distance so that subjects neither leaned forwards nor sideways. While upper-limb position was not controlled between participants, upper-limb joint angles were manually assessed using a goniometer and matched between the two experimental sessions (elbow extension: 134.0 ± 3.4°; shoulder flexion: 33.0 ± 7.4°; shoulder external rotation: 36.0 ± 4.2°). As a group, participants' grasp distance (distance from the wrist crease to the middle of the manipulandum's handle) was 7.9 ± 0.5 cm. The position of the handle was digitally displayed to participants on a computer monitor as a blue circle that could be moved horizontally (wrist flexion/extension) and vertically (radial/ulnar deviation of the wrist) by moving the handle of the WristBot. For all tracking trials, participants were instructed to overlay their blue circle (by moving the device's handle) on the monitor with a yellow target circle that moved along a set path. This path was a 2:3 Lissajous curve that was ±32° in the x-axis (flexion-extension) and ±18° in the y-axis (radial-ulnar deviation). The circular yellow target took 20 s to complete one full cycle of the Lissajous curve (see Figure 1B for an example of the monitor display). Thus, movement velocity was controlled by the target and consistent across trials. A single lap of the Lissajous curve represented a single tracking trial. The tracking trials were non-fatiguing as no resistance was provided to participants from the manipulandum. Trials were performed both prior to and following a single bout of maximal sustained isometric contraction.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Experimental setup for the tracking trials. Participant's forearm is positioned atop the WristBot support, and their hand is gripping the handle of the device. (B) Example of the 2:3 Lissajous curve. The white circle represents the target as it moves around the curve, while the gray circle represents the real-time position of the handle. Data collected during the initial dotted-line portion was not analyzed. (C) Experimental setup for MVCs and the sustained isometric fatigue trial. In this example, the participant was setup for wrist flexion MVCs/wrist flexion fatigue.

Experimental Protocol

This experiment consisted of two separate testing sessions. Each session was separated by 7 days and consisted of either (1) maximal sustained isometric wrist flexion, or (2) maximal sustained isometric wrist extension (order was pseudorandomized across sample; 6 participants started with flexion, 6 started with extension). A visual overview of the experimental protocol can be seen in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic of the experimental protocol. This protocol was repeated for both the wrist flexion fatigue session and the wrist extension fatigue session (sessions separated by 7 days). Gray bars represent MVCs while white bars represent a single tracking completion of the Lissajous curve.

Upon obtaining informed consent, participants were seated in front of a table-mounted force transducer (Model: BG 500, Mark-10 Corporation, New York, USA). In this same seat, the WristBot rested at the participant's right side. The transducer was raised above the table to allow participants to place their right hand underneath (Figure 1C). For the wrist flexion session, the transducer made contact with the distal anterior surface of the metacarpal bones (top of the palm), while for the wrist extension session, the transducer made contact with the distal posterior surface of the metacarpal bones (back of the most proximal knuckles). This placement for both sessions was marked on the hand with a black marker to match alignment throughout the experiment. For both sessions, the angle of the wrist was maintained at neutral (neither flexed nor extended). Participants then performed two maximal voluntary contractions (MVCs) held for 3–4 s and separated by 1 min of rest. For the MVCs, participants were required to maximally flex/extend (flex on flexion fatigue day/extend on extension fatigue day) their wrist upwards against the force transducer with their right hand open (phalanges extended). Participants were instructed to maintain an open hand throughout the MVC and to keep their forearm firmly upon the support pads (to isolate wrist forces and limit any assistance from the elbow flexors). Their left hand was placed on the table beside them, supinated and palm open so as not to provide them with any additional assistance. Participants were provided with ample verbal encouragement from the researchers for both MVCs as well as visual feedback of their MVC force. The greater of the two trials was deemed their true MVC.

While remaining in the same seat, participants placed their right hand in the WristBot's support and grasped the handle of the manipulandum. Relevant joint angles of the upper-limb (see Experimental setup above) were assessed at this time. Due to the novelty of the tracking task, it was vitally important to limit the influence of motor learning on performance outcomes in the present study. To accomplish this, participants first performed 12 practice tracking trials with each trial separated by 1 min of rest. [12 trials were deemed sufficient based on preliminary pilot work. In these pilot sessions, 5 participants performed 20 trials of the Lissajous curves with 1 min of rest between trials. Mean tracking error (see Data analysis for explanation) rapidly decreased after the first two trials but only gradually improved after trial 3. Group tracking error did not significantly improve after trial 12]. Following the 12 practice trials, participants were given 5 min of rest. Baseline (pre-fatigue) tracking trials were then performed with a total of 5 trials separated by 1 min of rest each.

For the fatigue-inducing trial, participants placed their right hand back underneath the table-mounted force transducer. Fatigue was then induced by a maximal sustained isometric wrist flexion/extension (on separate days) MVC. The MVC was performed following the same guidelines as mentioned above (hand open on both days). The cut-off criteria for the sustained MVC was when participants could no longer maintain 25% of their pre-fatigue MVC force. This cut-off criteria was not disclosed to participants, who were instead told that they would be exerting maximal force for ~1–2 min. They were to relax only once the researchers (who were actively watching the force readings) told them to stop. Ample verbal encouragement was provided to participants throughout the fatigue-inducing trial.

Following the 25% cut-off, participants immediately returned their right hand to the WristBot and performed their first post-fatigue tracking trial. While the time between the end of the fatigue-inducing trial and the start of the first post-fatigue tracking trial was not measured, it is estimated that it took ~5 s to get participants back into the WristBot and begin tracking. The first tracking trial was labeled as “0” min post-fatigue. Additional tracking trials also occurred at 1, 2, 4, 6, 8, and 10 min post-fatigue. An MVC was performed immediately after the tracking trials at 2, 6, and 10 min post-fatigue to assess wrist flexion/extension force recovery (flexion MVCs on flexion fatigue day/extension MVCs on extension fatigue day). These MVCs were not sustained, and only lasted ~3–4 s.

Data Analysis

Kinematic data of both the manipulandum's handle (which represents the participant's hand position) and the monitor-displayed target were sampled at 100 Hz and analyzed off-line (Matlab 2015b, Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Because participants had to “catch-up” to the target once each tracking trial began, the initial portion of the Lissajous curve (right before the first turn; the dotted section in Figure 1B) was not assessed in this study. A 6th order Savitzky-Golay filter was used to smooth the positional data in the x and y-axis (Squeri et al., 2010). The Savitzky-Golay is a polynomial fitting filter that works through the means of linear least squares. This filter is segmented in that it fits separate polynomials to a subset of data points within a predetermined window length. The window length in the present study was set to 170 ms which functions as an equivalent 11 Hz low-pass filter (Squeri et al., 2010). From this data, two groups of metrics were used to quantify performance. These include: (1) tracking error and various subtypes, and (2) movement smoothness.

Tracking Error: Was calculated as the square root of the displacement between the position of the handle and the position of the target and is calculated by the following formula:

|e|= (Hx-Tx)2+(Hy-Ty)2

where |e| is the Euclidean distance, and H and T are the positions (x, y coordinates) of the handle and target, respectively. The error at each data point was summed over the full tracking trial and divided by the total number of samples to give mean tracking error. To provide additional insight into possible error patterns, we also separated tracking error into the 4 main movement directions: left, right, up, and down on the computer monitor, which was made possible primarily by flexion of the wrist, extension of the wrist, radial deviation, and ulnar deviation, respectively.

Longitudinal Component: Is a measure of whether the handle is ahead of or behind the target at each data point. If the resulting value is positive, then the handle is ahead of the target relative to the target's own trajectory, and is given by the following formula:

ul=1T˙x2+T˙y2 [T˙xT˙y]= [uxluyl]δl= e · ul

where ul is the unit vector of the trajectory of the target at each data point, and δl is the longitudinal component of the tracking error. In order to establish whether the handle is ahead of or behind the target, the direction of the trajectory of the target must first be established. The first derivative is taken of the target displacement to find the tangent vector to the trajectory. The norm of the obtained vector is calculated. Then the tangent vector is normalized to obtain the unit vector. ul is then multiplied against the error vector measured from earlier (e, see Tracking Error) which gives either a positive (handle is ahead of the target) or negative (handle is behind the target) value.

Normal Component: Is a measure of whether the handle is to the right or left of the target at each data point. If the resulting value is positive, then the handle is to the right of the target relative to the target's own trajectory, and is given by the following formula:

un=[uyl-uxl]δn= e · un

like the longitudinal component, in order to establish whether the handle is to the right or left of the target, the direction of trajectory of the target must first be established. This is given by the ul equation described earlier. un is simply the orthogonal of ul and is then multiplied by the error vector. This then gives either a positive (handle is to the right of the target) or negative (handle is to the left of the target) value.

Figural Error: Is a measure of how accurately the participant's trajectory adheres to the ideal target trajectory (or how well the participant recreates the target path/shape; Conditt et al., 1997). This measure is insensitive to speed, meaning that it does not matter if an individual is ahead of or behind the target. The measure is given by the following equations:

distAB(i)= minjAi-Bj i=1, 2, ndistBA(j)= miniAi-Bj j=1, 2, m        FEAB= i=1ndistAB(i)+ j=1mdistBA(j)n+m

where “A” and “m” are the time series and total samples of the target trajectory and “B” and “n” are the time series and total samples of the handle trajectory. The first equation calculates the distance between a single data point of the target (denoted by j) and every data point of the handle (in the present study, 2,000 samples) before moving to the next target data point. The minimum distance of all these comparisons is then taken. [For example, if at sample number 100, the position of the handle was directly overlaying some portion of the Lissajous curve (this could be at any point along the target trajectory) the minimum distance would be zero]. The second equation is the same, but in reverse, and compares every data point of the target against a single data point of the handle. The final equation adds the sum of all the minimum distances and divides it by the sum of the two samples. A final figural error score of 0 would indicate that the handle was directly overlaying the target trajectory throughout the entire trial.

Jerk Ratio: Following filtering with the 6th order Savitzky-Golay filter, the displacement data of both the handle and the target were taken to the 3rd differential in order to obtain jerk. The jerk ratio was calculated as the integrated squared jerk (ISJ) (Platz et al., 1994) of the handle divided by the ideal ISJ of the target. ISJ was defined as (Hx2+Hy2)dt and integrated over the entire tracking trial. As the jerk ratio in the present study is a comparison of the handle to the target, a value of 1 would represent movement that is as smooth as possible. Any value greater than 1 signifies movement that is less smooth than the movement of the target.

Statistics

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (SPSS, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Assumptions of sphericity were tested with Mauchley's test of sphericity, and in cases where violated, degrees of freedom were corrected with Greenhouse-Geisser. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA (fatigue day x measurement time) was conducted for MVC data, all tracking error metrics, and jerk ratio measures to identify differences between both the two fatigue sessions as well as between baseline and following sustained isometric fatigue. In cases where a main effect of measurement time was found, post-hoc pairwise comparisons were conducted with a Bonferroni correction. Effect sizes (ES) were evaluated using partial ETA squared calculated as the division of the sum of squares of the effects (SSEffect) by both the SSEffect and the sum of squares of the error (SSError). Significance level was set at P < 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed on kinematic data in degrees (°). However, to more clearly communicate experimental findings, data has been shown in some figures as normalized to baseline measures. Group data is reported as mean ± SD.

Results

MVC Force

Participants produced significantly more absolute force in wrist flexion than wrist extension during the pre-fatigue MVCs (Flexion: 176.1 ± 38.2 N, Extension: 157.6 ± 34.6 N, P < 0.05). Absolute MVC force significantly decreased following the fatigue-inducing trial, regardless of testing session, and remained significantly reduced from baseline all the way to 10-min post-fatigue (P < 0.05 for all three post-fatigue time points). Normalized to baseline pre-fatigue MVC force, Figure 3A shows relative MVC force in the post-fatigue recovery period. In this recovery period, there were no significant differences between the flexion or extension sessions [F(3, 33) = 3.084, P = 0.11, ES = 0.22], nor was there an interaction effect of session and time [F(3, 33) = 2.25, P = 0.13, ES = 0.17]. However, there was a main effect of time [F(3, 33) = 53.86, P < 0.05, ES = 0.83] with relative MVC forces different from each other at all time points (P < 0.05 for all comparisons). This indicates that at each subsequent time point, MVC force was significantly recovering from the previous time point.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Group averages of relative (% of baseline) MVC force between the wrist flexion and wrist extension sessions. Gray lines depict wrist flexion MVC force collected on the wrist flexion fatigue day, while black lines represent wrist extension MVC force collected on the wrist extension fatigue day. The x-axis denotes the time of collection; the numbers refer to time of collection after fatigue. The dotted-line represents pre-fatigue (or baseline) MVC force. * denotes a significant difference of MVC force at one time point to all other time points. (B) Time to exhaustion for all 12 participants on both testing sessions. Black lines represent the 6 participants who took longer to fatigue on the extension day; light gray lines denote the 6 participants who took longer to fatigue on the flexion day. Lastly, the thick, dark gray line shows the group average.

Time to Exhaustion

Group data (Figure 3B, dashed line) demonstrated that there was no difference in the time it took for participants to reach their 25% of pre-MVC force cut-off (Flexion: 76.1 ± 26.8 s, Extension: 73.1 ± 19.3 s, P = 0.65). However, there was tremendous variability across participants, with 6 participants taking longer to fatigue during extension (black lines of Figure 3B) and 6 participants taking longer to fatigue during flexion (light gray lines of Figure 3B).

Tracking Error

Figure 4 shows group data for mean tracking error calculated over the full Lissajous curve. While statistical analyses found no difference between the extension and flexion sessions [F(7, 77) = 0.06, P = 0.81, ES = 0.01], there was a main effect of time on tracking error [F(7, 77) = 12.35, P < 0.05, ES = 0.53], with error significantly worse from baseline immediately post-fatigue (Baseline: 1.40 ± 0.54°, 0: 2.02 ± 0.51°, P < 0.05). Although group means never returned (or fell below) baseline error (even up to 10 min post-fatigue), tracking error recovered rapidly and was not significantly different from baseline at, or following, 1 min post-fatigue. To examine if there were any movement-specific trends, tracking error was separated into the four primary movement directions (Figure 5). However, results were mostly similar to data calculated over the full curve. There was no difference in tracking error between the extension and flexion sessions for any movement direction, although all four directions showed a main effect of time [Flexion movement: F(7, 77) = 6.32, P < 0.05, ES = 0.37; Extension movement: F(7, 77) = 4.04, P < 0.05, ES = 0.27; Radial deviation: F(7, 77) = 4.42, P < 0.05, ES = 0.29; Ulnar deviation: F(7, 77) = 8.99, P < 0.05, ES = 0.45]. Error significantly increased immediately post-fatigue in both the flexion (Baseline: 1.40 ± 0.71°, 0: 2.02 ± 0.65°, P < 0.05) and radial (Baseline: 1.39 ± 0.57°, 0: 1.90 ± 0.56°, P < 0.05) directions, but was not significantly different from baseline at, or following, 1-min post-fatigue. Error was also significantly greater immediately post-fatigue during extension (Baseline: 1.36 ± 0.48°, 0: 1.80 ± 0.56°, P < 0.05) and ulnar (Baseline: 1.40 ± 0.56°, 0: 2.00 ± 0.63°, P < 0.05) movement, however, error was also greater at 2-min post-fatigue (Extension: Baseline: 1.36 ± 0.48°, 0: 1.60 ± 0.48°, P < 0.05; Ulnar: Baseline: 1.40 ± 0.56°, 0: 1.59 ± 0.43°, P < 0.05).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group averages of mean tracking error calculated over the entire Lissajous curve (excluding the dotted-line portion). Tracking error is normalized to baseline (shown by the dashed horizontal line), and data points are shown in minutes after fatigue (0–10). Black lines represent tracking error from the wrist extension fatigue session, while gray lines represent tracking error from the wrist flexion fatigue session. * denotes a significant difference of both extension and flexion compared to baseline.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Group averages of mean tracking error (A) only when the wrist was flexing, (B) only when the wrist was extending, (C) only when the wrist was moving in radial deviation, and (D) only when the wrist was moving in ulnar deviation. Tracking error is normalized to baseline (shown by the dashed horizontal lines in each graph), and data points are shown in minutes after fatigue (0–10). Black lines represent tracking error from the wrist extension fatigue session, while gray lines represent tracking error from the wrist flexion fatigue session. * denotes a significant difference of both extension and flexion compared to baseline.

Longitudinal and Normal Error Components

Figures 6A,B depict group data of the longitudinal (ahead or behind) and normal (right or left) components, respectively, of the tracking error. As a group, participants tended to rush ahead of the target as it moved around the Lissajous curve. Even at baseline, the longitudinal component averaged 0.24 ± 0.61° between the two testing sessions. This tendency increased immediately post-fatigue, with participants significantly farther ahead than at baseline (Baseline: 0.24 ± 0.61°, 0: 0.85 ± 0.70°, P < 0.05). However, there was no difference in the longitudinal component between testing sessions, and the error across both sessions was not significantly different from baseline at, or following, 1-min post-fatigue. Regarding the normal component of error, data seemed to hover around 0 for all conditions, meaning that as a group, participants missed the target to the right and to left to a nearly equal extent. The normal component of error was not significantly different between the two testing sessions [F(7, 77) = 0.52, P = 0.50, ES = 0.05], nor did it significantly change over time [F(7, 77) = 1.61, P = 0.21, ES = 0.13].

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Group averages of the mean (A) longitudinal component of the tracking error, and (B) the normal component of the tracking error. For both metrics, error is shown in degrees (°) and data points are shown from pre-fatigue to 10 min-post. Black lines represent tracking error from the wrist extension fatigue session, while gray lines represent tracking error from the wrist flexion fatigue session. * denotes a significant difference of both extension and flexion compared to baseline.

Figural Error and Jerk Ratio

Group data on figural error is shown in Figure 7A, and much like tracking error, demonstrated a main effect of time [F(7, 77) = 7.10, P < 0.05, ES = 0.392], with no difference between the flexion and extension sessions [F(7, 77) = 1.55, P = 0.24, ES = 0.12]. Figural error significantly increased immediately post-fatigue (Baseline: 0.74 ± 0.19°, 0: 0.94 ± 0.22°, P < 0.05), but was not significantly different from baseline at, or following, 1-min. Jerk ratios (representing movement smoothness) also demonstrated a main effect of time [F(7, 77) = 3.37, P < 0.05, ES = 0.23], although interestingly, pairwise comparisons revealed no differences between any two time points. There was also no difference between the flexion and extension test sessions on jerk ratios [F(7, 77) = 0.07, P = 0.79, ES = 0.01].

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. (A) Group averages of figural error, and (B) group averages of the jerk ratio. Error is relative to baseline (shown by the dashed horizontal lines in each graph), and data points are shown in minutes after fatigue (0–10). Black lines represent tracking error from the wrist extension fatigue session, while gray lines represent tracking error from the wrist flexion fatigue session. * denotes a significant difference of both extension and flexion compared to baseline.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to have examined sustained isometric MVCs in opposing forearm muscle groups and their subsequent influence on precision movements of the hand and wrist. Results demonstrated that hand-tracking accuracy was impaired across nearly all analysis metrics, although most of these measures recovered within 1-min. Increased tracking error seemed to be mostly consistent throughout the full trace; minimal differences were seen when examined in the four primary directions. A tendency to rush (longitudinal component) while tracking may explain part of the increased tracking error; left/right error tendencies averaged out to be the same following fatigue. However, the most interesting finding of this study may be the lack of differences between the flexion and extension fatigue sessions. Across all metrics, there were no differences in tracking proficiency whether the wrist flexors or the wrist extensors were fatigued. This raises new questions regarding the functional roles of the wrist flexors and extensors in both sustained contractions and in the execution of fine motor skills.

Flexors vs. Extensors

Prior to the fatigue-inducing trial, participants produced significantly more absolute wrist flexion MVC force than wrist extension, although when normalized (Figure 3A), there were no differences in MVC force recovery. For every other metric, there were also no differences between the two sessions. Participants produced equal tracking error between the wrist flexion and extension sessions and even demonstrated similar error tendencies (Figures 6, 7). This finding was surprising, given the differences in anatomy and physiological roles of the two muscle groups. The wrist flexors possess a physiological cross-section area (PCSA) that is approximately twice as large as the wrist extensors (Flexors: ~24.8 cm2, Extensors: ~11.8 cm2; Lieber et al., 1990, 1992; Jacobson et al., 1992). The wrist flexors also cumulatively possess larger moment arms than the wrist extensors (Gonzalez et al., 1997). Since PCSA is a strong predictor of a muscle's strength, these two factors indicate that the wrist flexors are capable of generating a peak moment in excess of 2:1 of the extensors (Gonzalez et al., 1997). Muscle lines of action, at least in the most superficial forearm muscles, only compound this force-generating disparity. Both the flexor carpi radialis (FCR) and ulnaris (FCU) possess a mostly direct line of action toward wrist flexion (Bawa et al., 2000). In contrast, the extensor carpi radialis (ECR) and ulnaris (ECU) are more closely aligned with radial and ulnar deviation, respectively (Bawa et al., 2000). Consequently, the wrist extensors must function at a higher percentage of maximal activation in order to counterbalance the activity of the stronger flexors, particularly in movements that are closer to the flexors' more direct line of action. This has been repeatedly shown experimentally, whereby the wrist extensors exhibit significantly higher levels of muscle activity than the flexors across a multitude of handgrip and wrist forces (Snijders et al., 1987; Mogk and Keir, 2003; Forman et al., 2019). These recruitment characteristics have contributed to the notion that the wrist extensors make a greater contribution to wrist joint stability (Holmes et al., 2015). They are also the primary reason why the wrist extensors exhibit an earlier onset of fatigue than the wrist flexors (Hägg and Milerad, 1997). Considering all of the above, it was well within reason to hypothesize that separately inducing fatigue through wrist flexion and wrist extension would result in unique performance impairments.

The most plausible explanation for this lack of difference may arise from the recruitment characteristics of the wrist extensors. In a recent study (Forman et al., 2019), we demonstrated that during isolated wrist extension trials, the wrist flexors averaged just 2.8% of maximal activity (vs. 23.2% for the extensors). However, in the pure wrist flexion trials, the wrist extensors averaged 9.8% of maximal activity (compared to 28.1% in the flexors). To summarize, the wrist extensors were highly active even in pure wrist flexion exertions, while the opposite was not true. Thus, in the present study, it is entirely possible that prolonged wrist extension successfully isolated the wrist extensor muscles (i.e., the wrist flexor muscles were not fatigued). However, during prolonged wrist flexor fatigue, literature would suggest that both wrist flexors and extensors may have fatigued simultaneously (Forman et al., 2019). If so, then the wrist flexors were only fatigued in a single session of the present study, while the wrist extensors were fatigued in both. The two sessions would therefore be more similar than previously assumed. However, this also further complicates the comparison between the two muscle groups in regards to fatigue; it would be very difficult to isolate the two using any form of exercise. Future investigations examining antagonist fatigue metrics following agonist fatigue (i.e., is wrist extension MVC impaired following prolonged wrist flexion, and vice versa?), or using alternative fatigue measures (such as surface electromyography), would help address these questions.

Finally, it is possible that during both the flexion and extension fatigue-inducing sessions, compensation from synergistic muscles diminished the influence of fatigue on tracking accuracy measures. For instance, in studies that have induced isolated fatigue in single muscles (i.e., vastus lateralis), the muscle activity of adjacent/synergistic muscles increases during synchronous muscles actions (i.e., knee extension) (Akima et al., 2002; Stutzig et al., 2012; Stutzig and Siebert, 2015a,b). Thus, while fatigue of the wrist flexors or extensors might be uniquely detrimental to tracking accuracy of the hand/wrist in isolation, compensation from synergistic muscles may diminish these differences during the execution of complex motor tasks. It is unclear if this was the case in the present study, given that the fatigue-inducing task likely fatigued forearm muscles on a “global” scale (the prime movers and synergists might have fatigued simultaneously). Regardless, this suggestion requires further investigation.

Time to Exhaustion

To fairly compare tracking error between fatigue-inducing contractions of the wrist flexors and extensors, a relative cut-off criterion (25% of MVC for each session) was established. Interestingly, although there was large variability between participants, the duration of the fatigue-inducing trial averaged across our sample was ultimately not different between days; the wrist flexors took just as long to reach the 25% cut-off as the extensors. This is surprising given the differences in function and muscle architecture of the two muscle groups. Direct assessments of joint stiffness (Holmes et al., 2015) and investigations using muscle activity and co-contraction as surrogate measures for joint stiffness (Hägg and Milerad, 1997; Mogk and Keir, 2003; Forman et al., 2019) suggest that the wrist extensors contribute more to wrist stability than the flexors. While we are not aware of any study that has directly quantified muscle fiber typing between the wrist flexors/extensors of the forearm (fiber typing research in the forearm is overall scarce; Fugl-Meyer et al., 1982; McIntosh et al., 1985; Mizuno et al., 1994), stabilizing muscles of the trunk are predominantly composed of type 1 fibers (Mannion et al., 1997; Boyd-Clark et al., 2001; Arbanas et al., 2009). Thus, considering the stabilizing behavior of the wrist extensors, and the possibility that they possess a greater ratio of type 1 muscle fibers (at this time, this point is pure speculation), it was hypothesized that the flexors would fatigue faster than the extensors. Given our experimental setup (forearm supinated for flexion fatigue; pronated for extension), it is possible that forearm posture contributed to this lack of difference (La Delfa et al., 2015; Yoshii et al., 2015). However, while the average time to exhaustion was not different between sessions, there was large variability between participants; 6 participants took longer to fatigue during flexion; 6 took longer to fatigue during extension. It is possible that certain anthropometric characteristics, such as hand size, hand length, or forearm muscle moment arms predisposed some individuals to fatiguing earlier in one session over the other. Additionally, the participants recruited in the present study were highly active (varsity athletes and strength athletes). Differences in training status, or differences in inter-flexor/extensor strength, may have also contributed to a certain fatigue predisposition.

Performance Fatigability and Hand Tracking

In the present study, isometric fatigue significantly impaired hand-tracking performance immediately after the cessation of the fatigue trial. Tracking deficits mostly recovered within 1-min post-fatigue, although tracking in certain directions (wrist extension and ulnar deviation) remained impaired for up to 2 min. A shift in error tendencies was also observed; participants were more inclined to be ahead of the target immediately following fatigue as compared to baseline. While there was no net change in left/right error tendencies throughout the study, Figure 7A suggests that participants were tracking with greater absolute left/right error following fatigue. The equation used to calculate figural error is insensitive to time, and thus, any increase in figural error must be due to greater deviations to the left/right of the target pathway.

This is not the first study to report fatigue-induced accuracy impairments. Indeed, these findings are well-supported by literature (Jaric et al., 1999; Huysmans et al., 2008; Missenard et al., 2008b). Although separate laboratory groups have attributed fatigue-induced accuracy deficits to isolated mechanisms, the underlying cause is almost certainly multi-factorial. Literature has reported numerous factors, including decreased force availability (Jones et al., 2002), greater signal-dependent noise (Missenard et al., 2008a), slower contractile speed (de Haan et al., 1989), decreased co-contraction during precision movements (Gribble et al., 2003; Missenard et al., 2008b), and impaired proprioception (Pedersen et al., 1999; Mugnosso et al., 2019). Force availability is likely linked with signal-dependent noise (SDN), where “signal” is the optimal force output to execute a given task, and “noise” is any force deviation from that ideal. Should a fatigue-inducing task decrease available force (i.e., decrease MVC), a larger relative portion of available force is then required to complete tasks with absolute force requirements (the force required to move the handle in the present study). This is noteworthy as SDN (force variability) increases linearly with higher force outputs (Jones et al., 2002) and is greater still following fatigue (Missenard et al., 2008a). Greater fatigue-induced force variability would have assuredly contributed to tracking error in the present study where accurate tracking required precise movements and speed. Speed itself may help explain why participants tended to rush the target (Figure 6A). Both maximal velocity and maximal torque decrease following fatigue (Buttelli et al., 1996); muscle relaxation-time is similarly prolonged (de Haan et al., 1989). In precision tasks, this has manifested experimentally as a reduction in peak velocity when rapidly moving to a known target (Jaric et al., 1997). In the present study, it is possible that participants altered their tracking strategy following fatigue. To compensate for a potential loss of available speed, participants may have opted to remain ahead of the target (even at the expense of error) so as to avoid the poorer alternative of falling behind and struggling to catch-up.

While neither co-contraction nor proprioception were assessed in this study, both factors have been reported as significant contributors to movement accuracy. Greater co-contraction increases limb impedance (Osu and Gomi, 1999), reduces kinematic variability (Selen et al., 2005), and subsequently leads to improved accuracy (Gribble et al., 2003). This is all relevant given that performance fatigability impairs co-contraction to a similar extent as tracking error, even when force availability is controlled for (Missenard et al., 2008b). Finally, extensive evidence has demonstrated that fatigue results in significant joint position sense impairment (Carpenter et al., 1998; Pedersen et al., 1999; Björklund et al., 2003; Roberts et al., 2003). This impairment arises from numerous factors, including, but not limited to, decreased discharge rate of muscle spindles (Macefield et al., 1991), decreased activity of golgi tendon organs (Hutton and Nelson, 1986), and alterations in central pathways (Sharpe and Miles, 1993; Zabihhosseinian et al., 2015). The extent to which co-contraction and proprioception may have influenced the findings of the present study is unclear, and future investigations quantifying these measures at the forearm would add valuable insight.

Central and Peripheral Mechanisms

In the presence of performance fatigability, changes occur at all levels of the neuromuscular pathway, from the central nervous system (CNS), the neuromuscular junction (NMJ), and the muscle fibers themselves. Research utilizing isometric fatigue-inducing tasks would suggest that these mechanisms likely had some influence over our study's findings. At the level of the motor unit, ample literature has shown that performance fatigability decreases motor unit firing rates (Grimby and Hannerz, 1977; Petrofsky, 1980; Petrofsky and Lind, 1980; Bigland-Ritchie et al., 1983; Woods et al., 1987; Peters and Fuglevand, 1999; Gandevia, 2001). A similar decrease in spinal excitability (Taylor et al., 1996, 2000; Butler et al., 2003; Taylor and Gandevia, 2008) suggests that alpha motoneurones are inhibited, either through intrinsic motoneuron adaptations or peripheral inhibitory pathways (Heckman and Enoka, 2012), in the presence of fatigue. These changes can all occur without a subsequent reduction in force (in non-maximal contractions), which is made possible by a simultaneous increase in motor unit recruitment (Edwards and Lippold, 1956; Scherrer and Bourguignon, 1959; Eason, 1960; DeVries, 1968; Lynn et al., 1978; Johnson et al., 2004; Hendrix et al., 2009). Greater recruitment likely results from an increase in descending neural drive, as studies utilizing transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) have shown motor evoked potentials (MEPs) to increase following fatigue (Søgaard et al., 2006; Klass et al., 2008; Lévénez et al., 2008). Increased neural drive (or cortical excitability) is thought to act as a compensatory mechanism to ensure adequate recruitment in the presence of decreased spinal excitability. Interestingly, in these studies, both corticospinal and spinal excitability change rapidly following the cessation of a fatigue-inducing task, either returning to baseline or overcorrecting in ~1-min post-fatigue. This is remarkably similar to the patterns observed in the present study, whereby metrics of tracking error significantly worsened immediately post-fatigue but mostly recovered following 1-min of recovery. It is therefore possible that fatigue-induced changes in central pathways altered voluntary activation in the present study, which may have driven patterns of tracking accuracy post-fatigue.

In terms of peripheral mechanisms, resting twitch force evoked by electrical stimulation of motor point decreases following a sustained MVC (Gandevia et al., 1996). Since some studies have found that the compound muscle action potential (Mwave) is unchanged following either sustained (Bigland-Ritchie et al., 1986) or intermittent (Neyroud et al., 2014) MVCs, this decrease in force is at least partially explained by changes at the intramuscular level. This may be the result of reduced sarcoplasmic reticulum release/impaired renewal of intracellular calcium and a reduced myofibrillar calcium sensitivity (Westerblad et al., 1991; Fitts, 1994; Glaister, 2005; Allen et al., 2008). Importantly, as voluntary activation typically recovers to near pre-fatigue levels within 30 s of recovery from sustained MVCs (Gandevia et al., 1996; Hunter et al., 2006, 2008; Kennedy et al., 2013, 2015), any lasting impairment in voluntary force production is likely due to intramuscular mechanisms. Thus, in the present study, fatigue-induced changes to intramuscular systems are likely the primary reason why MVC force remained lower from baseline all the way up to 10-min post-fatigue. However, this reduction in available MVC force was insufficient to impair tracking accuracy, at least beyond 1–2 min post-fatigue.

Practical Implications

Given the results of the present study, one could be mistaken for concluding that the effects of fatigue dissipated after 1–2 min, given that most tracking metrics recovered by 1-min. However, it should be reiterated that MVC force was still significantly reduced from baseline up to 10-min post-fatigue (Figure 3A). Thus, participants were not tracking better because they were no longer fatigued; they were tracking better despite still being fatigued. Similar work has shown that fatigue may not change kinematic measures, despite fatigue manifesting in surface electromyography (Mugnosso et al., 2019). This is particularly relevant for industries that may rely on performance as an indicator of fatigue, from athletic training and health to occupational settings. If fatigue is only identified once movement precision has noticeably worsened, then fatigue was likely present well-beforehand. Delayed identification of workplace fatigue could have the potential to exacerbate the development of chronic overuse injuries. This, however, should be considered in relation to the sample population examined in this study. Collected from young, active adults, the findings of this study may not be ideally suited for generalization to some workplace settings. In one sense, certain workers performing repetitive tasks could be classified as “industrial athletes,” which might lead to a number of similarities to the present sample. In another sense, since such large performance detriments were observed in a young and active sample, these findings might be more pronounced in an older or more vulnerable working population.

Understanding how performance fatigability manifests in the wrist flexors/extensors is an important first step in addressing the earlier fatigue onset (Hägg and Milerad, 1997) and the higher incidence of injury that has characterized the wrist extensors (Shiri et al., 2006). The findings of the present study seem to indicate that fatigue of either muscle group results in similar accuracy losses, as performance metrics were equally impaired between sessions. However, this may have occurred due to difficulties in isolating the muscles of the forearm, given that the wrist extensors exhibit high muscle activity even as the antagonists (Mogk and Keir, 2003; Forman et al., 2019).

Study Limitations

In the present study, fatigue was induced via a sustained isometric MVC. Given the known differences of how maximal vs. submaximal contractions influence central (Taylor and Gandevia, 2008) and peripheral (Søgaard et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2007) pathways, the results of the present study should not be generalized to lower intensity fatiguing tasks. Likewise, as this study induced fatigue through an isometric contraction, these findings should not be generalized to fatigue induced by dynamic contractions. The present study also induced fatigue using controlled postures. Not only was the wrist flexion day performed in forearm supination and the wrist extension day performed in forearm pronation (forearm posture may have influenced the development of fatigue), but the wrist angle was also maintained at neutral (0° of flexion/extension). Had the wrist been held at a different angle throughout the sustained MVC, there might have been differences in tracking accuracy (Place et al., 2005; Behrens et al., 2019).

Finally, while the objective of the present study was purely to examine the consequences of performance fatigability on precision hand/wrist movements, our discussion has proposed a number of underlying mechanisms to explain our findings. It should be clarified that none of these mechanisms were quantified, and their contribution to the present results are speculative. Future investigations utilizing techniques such as electromyography (EMG) or TMS following a similar fatigue-inducing task would add valuable insight to this work.

Conclusion

This report was the first investigation to examine performance fatigability of wrist flexion/extension and its influence on hand tracking accuracy. Contrary to our hypothesis, there were no differences in hand tracking errors between the two testing sessions, which raises questions as to how precisely forearm muscles can be isolated in fatigue-inducing studies. Tracking error was impaired similarly for the two muscle groups immediately following a sustained MVC, but mostly recovered at 1-min post-fatigue. However, MVC force remained lower from baseline for all post-fatigue measures, indicating that participants were capable of accurate tracking in the presence of force deficits.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Brock University Bioscience Research Ethics Board and the Ontario Tech University Ethics Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Experimental procedures were developed by DF, GF, MM, JZ, BM, and MH. Data was collected by DF and GF and analyzed by DF, GF, MM, and MH. Manuscript was prepared, edited, and approved by DF, GF, MM, JZ, BM, and MH.

Funding

MH was supported by an NSERC Discovery Grant and the Canada Research Chairs program. DF was supported by an NSERC CGS Doctoral scholarship. GF was supported by an NSERC PGS Doctoral scholarship.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Akima, H., Foley, J. M., Prior, B. M., Dudley, G. A., and Meyer, R. A. (2002). Vastus lateralis fatigue alters recruitment of musculus quadriceps femoris in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 92, 679–684. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00267.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allander, E. (1974). Prevalence, incidence, and remission rates of some common rheumatic diseases or syndromes. Scand. J. Rheumatol. 3, 145–153. doi: 10.3109/03009747409097141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, D. G., Lamb, G. D., and Westerblad, H. (2008). Skeletal muscle fatigue: cellular mechanisms. Physiol. Rev. 88, 287–332. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00015.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbanas, J., Starcevic Klasan, G., Nikolic, M., Jerkovic, R., Miljanovic, I., and Malnar, D. (2009). Fibre type composition of the human psoas major muscle with regard to the level of its origin. J. Anat. 215, 636–641. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01155.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bawa, P., Chalmers, G. R., Jones, K. E., Søgaard, K., and Walsh, M. L. (2000). Control of the wrist joint in humans. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 83, 116–127. doi: 10.1007/s004210000270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrens, M., Husmann, F., Mau-Moeller, A., Schlegel, J., Reuter, E.-M., and Zschorlich, V. R. (2019). Neuromuscular properties of the human wrist flexors as a function of the wrist joint angle. Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol. 7:181. doi: 10.3389/fbioe.2019.00181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigland-Ritchie, B., Furbush, F., and Woods, J. J. (1986). Fatigue of intermittent submaximal voluntary contractions: central and peripheral factors. J. Appl. Physiol. 1985 61, 421–429. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1986.61.2.421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigland-Ritchie, B., Johansson, R., Lippold, O. C., Smith, S., and Woods, J. J. (1983). Changes in motoneurone firing rates during sustained maximal voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. 340, 335–346. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björklund, M., Crenshaw, A. G., Djupsjöbacka, M., and Johansson, H. (2003). Position sense acuity is diminished following repetitive low-intensity work to fatigue in a simulated occupational setting: a critical comment. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 88, 485–486. doi: 10.1007/s00421-002-0753-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyd-Clark, L. C., Briggs, C. A., and Galea, M. P. (2001). Comparative histochemical composition of muscle fibres in a pre- and a postvertebral muscle of the cervical spine. J. Anat. 199, 709–716. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-7580.2001.19960709.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S. H. M., and Potvin, J. R. (2007). Exploring the geometric and mechanical characteristics of the spine musculature to provide rotational stiffness to two spine joints in the neutral posture. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, J. E., Taylor, J. L., and Gandevia, S. C. (2003). Responses of human motoneurons to corticospinal stimulation during maximal voluntary contractions and ischemia. J. Neurosci. 23, 10224–10230. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-32-10224.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buttelli, O., Seck, D., Vandewalle, H., Jouanin, J. C., and Monod, H. (1996). Effect of fatigue on maximal velocity and maximal torque during short exhausting cycling. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 73, 175–179. doi: 10.1007/bf00262828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, J. E., Blasier, R. B., and Pellizzon, G. G. (1998). The effects of muscle fatigue on shoulder joint position sense. Am. J. Sports Med. 26, 262–265. doi: 10.1177/03635465980260021701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, R. (1981). Tennis elbow: incidence in local league players. Br. J. Sports Med. 15, 250–256. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.15.4.250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cholewicki, J., and McGill, S. M. (1996). Mechanical stability of the in vivo lumbar spine: implications for injury and chronic low back pain. Clin. Biomech. 11, 1–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Conditt, M. A., Gandolfo, F., and Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (1997). The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 554–560. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.1.554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Haan, A., Jones, D. A., and Sargeant, A. J. (1989). Changes in velocity of shortening, power output and relaxation rate during fatigue of rat medial gastrocnemius muscle. Pflugers Arch. 413, 422–428. doi: 10.1007/bf00584493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Serres, S. J., and Milner, T. E. (1991). Wrist muscle activation patterns and stiffness associated with stable and unstable mechanical loads. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 451–458. doi: 10.1007/BF00228972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVries, H. A. (1968). Method for evaluation of muscle fatigue and endurance from electromyographic fatigue curves. Am. J. Phys. Med. 47, 125–135.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Eason, R. G. (1960). Electromyographic study of local and generalized muscular impairment. J. Appl. Physiol. 15, 479–482. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1960.15.3.479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, R. G., and Lippold, O. C. (1956). The relation between force and integrated electrical activity in fatigued muscle. J. Physiol. 132, 677–681. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1956.sp005558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enoka, R. M., and Duchateau, J. (2016). Translating fatigue to human performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 2228–2238. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitts, R. H. (1994). Cellular mechanisms of muscle fatigue. Physiol. Rev. 74, 49–94. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1994.74.1.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forman, D. A., Forman, G. N., Robathan, J., and Holmes, M. W. R. (2019). The influence of simultaneous handgrip and wrist force on forearm muscle activity. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 45, 53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2019.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forman, G. N., Forman, D. A., Avila-Mireles, E. J., Zenzeri, J., and Holmes, M. W. R. (2020). Investigating the muscular and kinematic responses to sudden wrist perturbations during a dynamic tracking task. Sci. Rep. 10:4161. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61117-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, D. W., and Milner, T. E. (2003). Adaptive control of stiffness to stabilize hand position with large loads. Exp. Brain Res. 152, 211–220. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1540-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fugl-Meyer, A. R., Eriksson, A., Sjöström, M., and Söderström, G. (1982). Is muscle structure influenced by genetical or functional factors? A study of three forearm muscles. Acta Physiol. Scand. 114, 277–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1982.tb06983.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandevia, S. C. (2001). Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol. Rev. 81, 1725–1789. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.4.1725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandevia, S. C., Allen, G. M., Butler, J. E., and Taylor, J. L. (1996). Supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue: evidence for suboptimal output from the motor cortex. J. Physiol. 490 (Pt 2), 529–536. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaister, M. (2005). Multiple sprint work : physiological responses, mechanisms of fatigue and the influence of aerobic fitness. Sports Med. 35, 757–777. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200535090-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, R. V., Buchanan, T. S., and Delp, S. L. (1997). How muscle architecture and moment arms affect wrist flexion-extension moments. J. Biomech. 30, 705–712. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(97)00015-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gribble, P. L., Mullin, L. I., Cothros, N., and Mattar, A. (2003). Role of cocontraction in arm movement accuracy. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 2396–2405. doi: 10.1152/jn.01020.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimby, L., and Hannerz, J. (1977). Firing rate and recruitment order of toe extensor motor units in different modes of voluntary conraction. J. Physiol. 264, 865–879. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruchow, H. W., and Pelletier, D. (1979). An epidemiologic study of tennis elbow. Incidence, recurrence, and effectiveness of prevention strategies. Am. J. Sports Med. 7, 234–238. doi: 10.1177/036354657900700405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hägg, G., and Milerad, E. (1997). Forearm extensor and flexor muscle exertion during simulated gripping work — An electromyographic study. Clin. Biomech. 12, 39–43. doi: 10.1016/S0268-0033(96)00049-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heckman, C. J., and Enoka, R. M. (2012). “Motor Unit,” in Comprehensive Physiology, ed. R. Terjung (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), c100087. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c100087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendrix, C. R., Housh, T. J., Johnson, G. O., Mielke, M., Camic, C. L., Zuniga, J. M., et al. (2009). Comparison of critical force to EMG fatigue thresholds during isometric leg extension. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41, 956–964. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318190bdf7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, M. W. R., Tat, J., and Keir, P. J. (2015). Neuromechanical control of the forearm muscles during gripping with sudden flexion and extension wrist perturbations. Comput. Methods Biomech. Biomed. Engin. 18, 1826–1834. doi: 10.1080/10255842.2014.976811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, S. K., Butler, J. E., Todd, G., Gandevia, S. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2006). Supraspinal fatigue does not explain the sex difference in muscle fatigue of maximal contractions. J. Appl. Physiol. 101, 1036–1044. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00103.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, S. K., Todd, G., Butler, J. E., Gandevia, S. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2008). Recovery from supraspinal fatigue is slowed in old adults after fatiguing maximal isometric contractions. J. Appl. Physiol. 105, 1199–1209. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01246.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutton, R. S., and Nelson, D. L. (1986). Stretch sensitivity of golgi tendon organs in fatigued gastrocnemius muscle. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 18, 69–74.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Huysmans, M. A., Hoozemans, M. J. M., van der Beek, A. J., de Looze, M. P., and van Dieën, J. H. (2008). Fatigue effects on tracking performance and muscle activity. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 18, 410–419. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2006.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iandolo, R., Marini, F., Semprini, M., Laffranchi, M., Mugnosso, M., Cherif, A., et al. (2019). Perspectives and challenges in robotic neurorehabilitation. Appl. Sci. 9:3183. doi: 10.3390/app9153183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, M. D., Raab, R., Fazeli, B. M., Abrams, R. A., Botte, M. J., and Lieber, R. L. (1992). Architectural design of the human intrinsic hand muscles. J. Hand Surg. 17, 804–809. doi: 10.1016/0363-5023(92)90446-v

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaric, S., Blesic, S., Milanovic, S., Radovanovic, S., Ljubisavljevic, M., and Anastasijevic, R. (1999). Changes in movement final position associated with agonist and antagonist muscle fatigue. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 80, 467–471. doi: 10.1007/s004210050619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaric, S., Radovanovic, S., Milanovic, S., Ljubisavljevic, M., and Anastasijevic, R. (1997). A comparison of the effects of agonist and antagonist muscle fatigue on performance of rapid movements. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 76, 41–47. doi: 10.1007/s004210050210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. V. B., Edwards, S. C., Van Tongeren, C., and Bawa, P. (2004). Properties of human motor units after prolonged activity at a constant firing rate. Exp. Brain Res. 154, 479–487. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1678-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. E., Hamilton, A. F., and Wolpert, D. M. (2002). Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1533–1544. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. S., Fitzpatrick, S. C., Gandevia, S. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2015). Fatigue-related firing of muscle nociceptors reduces voluntary activation of ipsilateral but not contralateral lower limb muscles. J. Appl. Physiol. 118, 408–418. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00375.2014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. S., McNeil, C. J., Gandevia, S. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2013). Firing of antagonist small-diameter muscle afferents reduces voluntary activation and torque of elbow flexors: small-diameter afferents reduce voluntary drive to elbow flexors. J. Physiol. 591, 3591–3604. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.248559

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klass, M., Lévénez, M., Enoka, R. M., and Duchateau, J. (2008). Spinal mechanisms contribute to differences in the time to failure of submaximal fatiguing contractions performed with different loads. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1096–1104. doi: 10.1152/jn.01252.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluger, B. M., Krupp, L. B., and Enoka, R. M. (2013). Fatigue and fatigability in neurologic illnesses: proposal for a unified taxonomy. Neurology 80, 409–416. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f07be

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

La Delfa, N. J., Langstaff, N. M., Hodder, J. N., and Potvin, J. R. (2015). The interacting effects of forearm rotation and exertion direction on male and female wrist strength. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 45, 124–128. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2014.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lévénez, M., Garland, S. J., Klass, M., and Duchateau, J. (2008). Cortical and spinal modulation of antagonist coactivation during a submaximal fatiguing contraction in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 554–563. doi: 10.1152/jn.00963.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieber, R. L., Fazeli, B. M., and Botte, M. J. (1990). Architecture of selected wrist flexor and extensor muscles. J. Hand Surg. 15, 244–250. doi: 10.1016/0363-5023(90)90103-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieber, R. L., Jacobson, M. D., Fazeli, B. M., Abrams, R. A., and Botte, M. J. (1992). Architecture of selected muscles of the arm and forearm: anatomy and implications for tendon transfer. J. Hand Surg. 17, 787–798. doi: 10.1016/0363-5023(92)90444-t

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, P. A., Bettles, N. D., Hughes, A. D., and Johnson, S. W. (1978). Influences of electrode geometry on bipolar recordings of the surface electromyogram. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 16, 651–660. doi: 10.1007/bf02442444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macefield, G., Hagbarth, K. E., Gorman, R., Gandevia, S. C., and Burke, D. (1991). Decline in spindle support to alpha-motoneurones during sustained voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. 440, 497–512. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mannion, A. F., Dumas, G. A., Cooper, R. G., Espinosa, F. J., Faris, M. W., and Stevenson, J. M. (1997). Muscle fibre size and type distribution in thoracic and lumbar regions of erector spinae in healthy subjects without low back pain: normal values and sex differences. J. Anat. 190 (Pt 4), 505–513. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-7580.1997.19040505.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marrelli, K., Cheng, A. J., Brophy, J. D., and Power, G. A. (2018). Perceived versus performance fatigability in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Front. Physiol. 9:1395. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masia, L., Casadio, M., Giannoni, P., Sandini, G., and Morasso, P. (2009). Performance adaptive training control strategy for recovering wrist movements in stroke patients: a preliminary, feasibility study. J. Neuroengineering Rehabil. 6:44. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-6-44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, J. S., Ringqvist, M., and Schmidt, E. M. (1985). Fiber type composition of monkey forearm muscle. Anat. Rec. 211, 403–409. doi: 10.1002/ar.1092110405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missenard, O., Mottet, D., and Perrey, S. (2008a). Muscular fatigue increases signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. Neurosci. Lett. 437, 154–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.03.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missenard, O., Mottet, D., and Perrey, S. (2008b). The role of cocontraction in the impairment of movement accuracy with fatigue. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 151–156. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1264-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizuno, M., Secher, N. H., and Quistorff, B. (1994). 31P-NMR spectroscopy, rsEMG, and histochemical fiber types of human wrist flexor muscles. J. Appl. Physiol. 1985 76, 531–538. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1994.76.2.531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogk, J., and Keir, P. (2003). The effects of posture on forearm muscle loading during gripping. Ergonomics 46, 956–975. doi: 10.1080/0014013031000107595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mugnosso, M., Zenzeri, J., Hughes, C. M. L., and Marini, F. (2019). Coupling robot-aided assessment and surface electromyography (sEMG) to evaluate the effect of muscle fatigue on wrist position sense in the flexion-extension plane. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:396. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neyroud, D., Vallotton, A., Millet, G. Y., Kayser, B., and Place, N. (2014). The effect of muscle fatigue on stimulus intensity requirements for central and peripheral fatigue quantification. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 114, 205–215. doi: 10.1007/s00421-013-2760-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osu, R., and Gomi, H. (1999). Multijoint muscle regulation mechanisms examined by measured human arm stiffness and EMG signals. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 1458–1468. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parijat, P., and Lockhart, T. E. (2008). Effects of lower extremity muscle fatigue on the outcomes of slip-induced falls. Ergonomics 51, 1873–1884. doi: 10.1080/00140130802567087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedersen, J., Lönn, J., Hellström, F., Djupsjöbacka, M., and Johansson, H. (1999). Localized muscle fatigue decreases the acuity of the movement sense in the human shoulder. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 31, 1047–1052. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199907000-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, E. J., and Fuglevand, A. J. (1999). Cessation of human motor unit discharge during sustained maximal voluntary contraction. Neurosci. Lett. 274, 66–70. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00666-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrofsky, J. S. (1980). Computer analysis of the surface EMG during isometric exercise. Comput. Biol. Med. 10, 83–95. doi: 10.1016/0010-4825(80)90025-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrofsky, J. S., and Lind, A. R. (1980). Frequency analysis of the surface electromyogram during sustained isometric contractions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 43, 173–182. doi: 10.1007/bf00422448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Place, N., Maffiuletti, N. A., Ballay, Y., and Lepers, R. (2005). Twitch potentiation is greater after a fatiguing submaximal isometric contraction performed at short vs. long quadriceps muscle length. J. Appl. Physiol. 1985 98, 429–436. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00664.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platz, T., Denzler, P., Kaden, B., and Mauritz, K. H. (1994). Motor learning after recovery from hemiparesis. Neuropsychologia 32, 1209–1223. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90103-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, D., Ageberg, E., Andersson, G., and Fridén, T. (2003). Effects of short-term cycling on knee joint proprioception in healthy young persons. Am. J. Sports Med. 31, 990–994. doi: 10.1177/03635465030310064001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherrer, J., and Bourguignon, A. (1959). Changes in the electromyogram produced by fatigue in man. Am. J. Phys. Med. 38, 148–158.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Selen, L. P. J., Beek, P. J., Dieën, J. H., and van (2005). Can co-activation reduce kinematic variability? A simulation study. Biol. Cybern. 93, 373–381. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0015-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharpe, M. H., and Miles, T. S. (1993). Position sense at the elbow after fatiguing contractions. Exp. Brain Res. 94, 179–182. doi: 10.1007/bf00230480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiri, R., Varonen, H., Heliövaara, M., and Viikari-Juntura, E. (2007). Hand dominance in upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders. J. Rheumatol. 34, 1076–1082.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shiri, R., Viikari-Juntura, E., Varonen, H., and Heliovaara, M. (2006). Prevalence and determinants of lateral and medial epicondylitis: a population study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 164, 1065–1074. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. L., Martin, P. G., Gandevia, S. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2007). Sustained contraction at very low forces produces prominent supraspinal fatigue in human elbow flexor muscles. J. Appl. Physiol. 103, 560–568. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00220.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snijders, C. J., Volkers, A. C., Mechelse, K., and Vleeming, A. (1987). Provocation of epicondylalgia lateralis (tennis elbow) by power grip or pinching. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 19, 518–523.

Google Scholar

Søgaard, K., Gandevia, S. C., Todd, G., Petersen, N. T., and Taylor, J. L. (2006). The effect of sustained low-intensity contractions on supraspinal fatigue in human elbow flexor muscles. J. Physiol. 573, 511–523. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.103598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squeri, V., Masia, L., Casadio, M., Morasso, P., and Vergaro, E. (2010). Force-field compensation in a manual tracking task. PLoS ONE 5:e11189. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stutzig, N., and Siebert, T. (2015a). Influence of joint position on synergistic muscle activity after fatigue of a single muscle head: fatigue of a single muscle head. Muscle Nerve 51, 259–267. doi: 10.1002/mus.24305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stutzig, N., and Siebert, T. (2015b). Muscle force compensation among synergistic muscles after fatigue of a single muscle. Hum. Mov. Sci. 42, 273–287. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stutzig, N., Siebert, T., Granacher, U., and Blickhan, R. (2012). Alteration of synergistic muscle activity following neuromuscular electrical stimulation of one muscle. Brain Behav. 2, 640–646. doi: 10.1002/brb3.87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L., Butler, J. E., Allen, G. M., and Gandevia, S. C. (1996). Changes in motor cortical excitability during human muscle fatigue. J. Physiol. 490, 519–528. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L., Butler, J. E., and Gandevia, S. C. (2000). Changes in muscle afferents, motoneurons and motor drive during muscle fatigue. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 83, 106–115. doi: 10.1007/s004210000269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L., and Gandevia, S. C. (2008). A comparison of central aspects of fatigue in submaximal and maximal voluntary contractions. J. Appl. Physiol. 104, 542–550. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01053.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Loon, E. M., Masters, R. S. W., Ring, C., and McIntyre, D. B. (2001). Changes in limb stiffness under conditions of mental stress. J. Mot. Behav. 33, 153–164. doi: 10.1080/00222890109603147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaar, J. A. (1994). Tennis elbow. Anatomical, epidemiological and therapeutic aspects. Int. Orthop. 18, 263–267. doi: 10.1007/bf00180221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerblad, H., Lee, J. A., Lannergren, J., and Allen, D. G. (1991). Cellular mechanisms of fatigue in skeletal muscle. Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol. 261, C195–C209. doi: 10.1152/ajpcell.1991.261.2.C195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, J. J., Furbush, F., and Bigland-Ritchie, B. (1987). Evidence for a fatigue-induced reflex inhibition of motoneuron firing rates. J. Neurophysiol. 58, 125–137. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.1.125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshii, Y., Yuine, H., Kazuki, O., Tung, W., and Ishii, T. (2015). Measurement of wrist flexion and extension torques in different forearm positions. Biomed. Eng. Online 14:115. doi: 10.1186/s12938-015-0110-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabihhosseinian, M., Holmes, M. W. R., and Murphy, B. (2015). Neck muscle fatigue alters upper limb proprioception. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 1663–1675. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4240-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: forearm, performance fatigability, tracking, isometric, robotics, kinematics, flexion, extension

Citation: Forman DA, Forman GN, Mugnosso M, Zenzeri J, Murphy B and Holmes MWR (2020) Sustained Isometric Wrist Flexion and Extension Maximal Voluntary Contractions Similarly Impair Hand-Tracking Accuracy in Young Adults Using a Wrist Robot. Front. Sports Act. Living 2:53. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2020.00053

Received: 08 February 2020; Accepted: 16 April 2020;
Published: 08 May 2020.

Edited by:

Adamantios Arampatzis, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Simon Steib, Technical University of Munich, Germany
Martin Behrens, University of Rostock, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Forman, Forman, Mugnosso, Zenzeri, Murphy and Holmes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael W. R. Holmes, michael.holmes@brocku.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.