- 1Compassionate Mind Research Group, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
- 2The Centre for Advanced Imaging, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
- 3Orbisant Analytics, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
- 4School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Central Queensland University, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
Research has shown that engaging in self-reassurance, a compassionately motivated cognitive relating style, can down-regulate neural markers of threat and pain. Whilst important, the relationship between neural and self-report markers of reassurance are largely unknown. Here we analyzed previously published fMRI data which measured neural responses when participants engaged in self-reassurance toward a mistake, setback, or failure. Within the present paper, we identified correlations between regions of interest extracted during self-reassurance with fMRI and self-report data. Using generalized additive modelling, we show that participants with greater inadequate forms of self-criticism exhibited greater neural activation within the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and anterior insula (AI). Furthermore, a relationship between greater fears of expressing compassion to the self and neural activation within the MPFC returned non-significant after correction for multiple comparisons. No significant relationships were observed between brain activation and hated and reassuring forms of self-criticism. Our results identify preliminary evidence for neural activity during self-reassurance as correlated with self-report markers, and we outline a method for modelling neural and self-report data which can be applied to future studies in compassion science, particularly with a clinical sample.
Introduction
Self-reassurance is a way of being compassionate to one’s own suffering, and has been operationalized as a way to be soothing, encouraging, and supportive to oneself in the face of setbacks (1–3). In contrast, self-criticism is considered to be a self-relating style embedded in a competitive or “rank-based” motivational system. Studies have consistently found that self-criticism, insecure striving, and fears of receiving compassion from others have all been linked to increased depressive symptoms (4–6). Importantly, this association no longer exists for individuals with higher levels of self-reassurance, suggesting a buffering effect on mental-health via self-reassurance (1). According to social mentality theory (7), self-reassurance is a cognitive relating style that stems from the compassionate motivational system. The function of self-reassurance is to soothe and calm the individual by activating the person’s caring/affiliative affect regulation system, also known as the “tend and befriend” or “rest and digest” system (8, 9). Accordingly, the physiological function of self-reassurance is to activate the parasympathetic system in times of perceived threat (2). When locked in a competitive or “rank-based” motivational system, as opposed to a compassionate one, individuals find self-criticism easy and self-reassurance difficult (10, 11).Typically, the function of self-criticism is to correct errors, but it can also serve a function to punish mistakes, and although both significantly predict depressive symptoms, it is the latter which is particularly powerful (3, 12).
Importantly, compassion is a motivation, which involves having a sensitivity to suffering in self and others, with a commitment to try and alleviate and prevent it (13). Cultivating a compassionate motivational system, can enable an individual to cultivate supportive and encouraging abilities and their internal supportive “physiological infrastructure” (e.g., parasympathetic system and the vagus) (14–16) in which the buffering impact of self-reassurance can work, opening the opportunity for the individual in times of distress to be helpful rather than hurtful or harmful (17). Indeed, there are now therapies developed, such as Compassion Focused Therapy (8) aimed at motivational switching to help individuals who struggle with high levels of self-criticism and shame. But how self-reassurance may work on a neural level, and how this may link to self-report data is less well known.
Within the current paper, we report on an fMRI paradigm which has been reported on previously (2, 18). This experiment investigated the neurophysiological correlates of compassionate mind training, and with fMRI, assessed participant’s neural responses when generating self-relating styles towards mistakes, setbacks and/or failures. Comparisons between activation patterns for self-critical versus self-reassuring relating styles have been reported elsewhere (2, 19, 20), The aim of the present paper is to explore how regions of the brain activated during self-reassurance may be attenuated by fears of compassion and forms of self-criticism.
Method
Experimental Procedure and Participants
As reported previously and reproduced in part due to a CC BY license (2), 40 participants (Mean age = 22 years, SD = .49, 27 female) were invited to engage in either self-reassuring or self-critical thoughts to stimuli which describe a mistake, setback, or failure (stimuli were counterbalanced for an emotional and neutral condition). Pre-testing on 200 participants revealed emotional statements were of sufficient intensity and negative valence as compared with neutral statements. Participants underwent 20 practice trials to test the task before the scanner, and rated the intensity of self-reassurance and self-criticism they felt for each trial via button-press on an MRI-compatible fibre-optic button box within the experiment. A relevant University Human Research Ethics committee approved the experimental protocol, and all participants provided written and/or digital informed, voluntary consent, and received remuneration at the value of $60 Australian dollars for participation. Whilst we observed no participant attrition across the combined fMRI experiment and HRV training as reported previously, one participant failed to complete an fMRI session due to feeling anxious during the scan, leaving us with a total number of 40 participants who completed both fMRI and HRV components. fMRI methods, pre-processing, and analysis are reported previously (2), yet regions-of-interest (ROI) extraction is summarized below for clarity:
Group-level analyses were conducted for emotional – neutral stimuli overall. As reported previously (2), brain regions shown to be significant had their anatomical labels identified with the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL) toolbox implemented in SPM12. Next, in order to examine correlations between the level of neural activation (i.e. difference in response between emotion verses neutral) and the mindset participants engaged in (i.e. self-criticism versus self-reassurance), we performed additional ROI analyses. For each ROI, we identified peak clusters which showed significantly greater activation overall for emotion vs neutral stimuli, and used these coordinates to extract the average contrast parameter estimates (i.e. levels of activation, Beta weights) with 5 mm radius spheres centered on those peaks for each mindset (i.e., self-criticism and self-reassurance).
Fears of Compassion Scale
We utilized the fears of compassion scale within the present research (5), which has three subscales; measuring fear of compassion for the self (example item, “I fear that if I become more self-compassionate I will become a weak person”), fear of receiving compassion from others (example item, “When people are kind and compassionate towards me I feel anxious or embarrassed”), and fear of compassion for others (example item, “Being compassionate towards people who have done bad things is letting them off the hook.”). Our sample comprised an internal consistency of 0.85 for compassion toward the self, 0.90 for compassion to other, and 0.85 for receiving compassion from others.
Forms of Self-Criticism/Self-Reassuring Scale
This 22-item scale assesses participants’ thoughts and feelings about themselves during a perceived failure. Two subscales measure forms of self-criticising (inadequate self (e.g., “I think I deserve my self-criticism”) and hated self (e.g., “I have become so angry with myself that I want to hurt or injure myself”), and one of reassurance (reassured self, e.g., “I am gentle and supportive with myself.”) (21). Our sample comprised an internal consistency of 0.89 for inadequate, 0.70 for hated, and 0.86 for reassuring forms of self-criticism, respectively.
Results
Correlations and GAMs
Previously we reported how activation in a few key regions of the brain (Anterior Cingulate, Amygdala, and Anterior Insula) were down-regulated when participants engaged in self-reassurance, versus self-criticism (2). Interestingly, activation across numerous regions were shown to not change their activation based on the mindset participants engaged in (Medial Pre-Frontal Cortex, Posterior Cingulate, and Lingual Gyrus). We will continue to use these same ROIs in the present paper for consistency to correlate with self-report measures (fears of compassion to self, forms of self-criticism). All combinations of brain regions and self-report variables have been reported in a table for clarity of measures (Table 1).
Our analysis plan proceeds as follows; first, we will explore the relationships (correlations) between all ROIs and self-report measures. Second, we will use GAMs to model the significant relationships. As can be seen in Table 1 and Supplementary Figure 1, an initial correlation analysis revealed relationships with fears of self-compassion and inadequate forms of self-criticism in the MPFC and AI. Furthermore, also shown in Table 1 and depicted in Supplementary Figures 2, 3, non-significant relationships of reassuring and hated forms of criticism with MPFC and AI are reported. To follow-up the significant relationships observed for inadequate forms of criticism and brain responses, three generalized additive models (GAM) were used to test statistically the relationships between explanatory (i.e., self-report) and response variables (brain activation: ROIs). Candidate variables for modelling with GAMs were selected after inspection of correlations (Table 1) and scatterplots (Supplementary Figures 1–3) revealed relationships which would benefit from the smoothing functions implemented under GAMS. Indeed, GAMS are similar to General Linear Model (GLMs), however in GAMs, smooth functions for each covariate are added (22, 23) in circumstances where traditional linear models are insufficient. Basis functions in GAMs are summed (connected) using spline interpolation, typically with a polynomial (22, 23). A benefit of GAMS is the provision of model evidence: here, GAMS can go beyond correlations to examine how well a model may fit data which is not strictly linear, and can contribute p-values, as well as R2 and deviance explained (similar to unadjusted R2, when the link function is Gaussian). As can be seen in Figure 1 and Tables 2, 3, GAMS can appropriately model the neural and self-report data. However, note that upon multiple comparison correction (0.05/3 = 0.016), the relationship between MPFC activation and Fears of Expressing Compassion to the Self returned non-significant. We also conducted GAMs on hated and reassuring forms of criticism, however hated forms violated a degrees of freedom assumption (possible due to a floor effect in a non-clinical sample), and reassuring forms returned a non-significant GAM (Supplementary Tables 1, 2).
Figure 1 GAM results of significant correlations. (A) MPFC ~ Fears: Smooth function for fears of expressing compassion to self significantly predicts MPFC reassurance signal. (B) MPFC ~ Forms: Smooth function for inadequate forms of criticism significantly predicts MPFC reassurance signal. (C) AI ~ Forms: Smooth function for inadequate forms of criticism significantly predicts AI reassurance signal. (D) Rough spatial location of MPFC (2 46 36) and Left AI (−26 10 −14) ROIs. Coordinates reported in MNI-space.
Discussion
Within the present research we explored relationships between neural markers of self-reassurance and fears of compassion and forms of self-criticism. Using the ROIs outlined in the previous paper (2), we used GAMs to fit the extent of neural activation within each ROI from self-report markers of self-reassurance’s inhibitors (fears of compassion and forms of self-criticism). An initial correlation matrix followed-up with GAMs revealed significant relationships between neural markers of self-reassurance during fMRI and fears of compassion and forms of self-criticism (Figure 1). Specifically, participants with greater fears of expressing compassion toward the self, have greater neural activation within the MPFC when engaging in self-reassurance (Figure 1A) (however, a correction for multiple comparisons returned non-significant). In addition, participants with greater inadequate forms of self-criticism were shown to have greater neural activation within the MPFC and AI (Figures 1B, C).
Implications of Results
First, the MPFC is a node in the default-mode network, which is involved with mentalizing and self-referential thoughts (24). Previous work has linked alterations in DMN function within major depression, particularly through increased functional connectivity of these regions with other neural networks such as the salience network and central executive (attention) networks (25–28). Indeed, that we observed both increased MPFC (Figure 1B) as well as AI response (Figure 1C) may imply that inadequate forms of self-criticism is an index of functional co-activation of these regions. Accordingly, from the DMN and depression literature described above, it is possible that we are identifying within a healthy control sample a subset of participants who might be tending towards depressive-like rumination, as indexed from greater self-report inadequate forms of criticism. However, future work with a larger sample, and with a between participants approach (i.e., clinically depressed versus healthy controls), would need to test this theory a priori with a measure of functional connectivity of these brain regions (MPFC, AI).
Second, the AI is a node within the salience network, responsible in-part for processing salient negative events within the environment. This region has been reported extensively within the empathy and compassion neuroscience literature to date (29–31). What is interesting, however, is that greater activation of this region during engagement in self-reassurance, can be indexed from greater levels of inadequate forms of self-criticism. What this self-report scale measures is the degree to which an individual feels inadequate and inferior, and therefore their self-criticism is used to correct and improve the self, in order to try harder, to achieve goals and to maintain certain standards (32, 33). Accordingly this relationship might indicate that participants with greater scores on this self-report scale might actually be “beating themselves up”, importantly, “for their perceived own good” for the imagined mistakes, setbacks or failures so that they prevent them happening again (34), as may be seen in the increase in activation of the AI. However, whilst inferring mental states from fMRI data as ‘reverse inference’ has been criticized (35), future work with an MVPA machine-learning approach might be able to decode the mind-states of participants when engaged in this paradigm (35–37).
Limitations and Future Directions
That we did not find significant relationships between hated and reassuring forms of self-criticism and brain markers are also interesting. As can be seen in Supplementary Figures 2, 3, the correlation between reassuring and hated forms of criticism and brain activation was non-significant. However, inspection of the scatterplots of these scales revealed a floor effect for hated forms, and a ceiling effect for reassuring forms. That we found these results can speak to aspects of our sample, namely, within a healthy population is that our sample tended toward greater self-reassuring, and less hated forms of self-criticism, which has also been found previously (12). Indeed, non-clinical samples have been shown to rate the hated-self subscale at floor (12). Yet it is curious how inadequate forms of self-criticism was associated with neural responses when engaged in self-reassurance. Future work perhaps with a clinically depressed sample to examine self-reassurance in comparison to a healthy control sample, might reveal a relationship between hated forms of criticism and brain activity when using the GAM method.
A possible candidate for the links between self-reassurance and inadequate self-criticism might be at the core of each self-relating style is it aims to regulate in order to correct behaviour and prevent bad things happening in the future. Some have drawn parallels to an inner dominant-subordinate relationship, which is developed to ensure one stays focused and does not become arrogant and disliked by others (38). However, when it comes to self-hatred criticism, this is a relating style that aims to destroy or eliminate. Some have argued it is an internal relating style that regards oneself as an enemy or an out-group. In this instance submissiveness and appeasement (a result of inadequate self-criticism) may not work at mitigating the attack (38). Rather the self-hatred attack aims to remove the “toxin”. Indeed, self-hatred and wanting to remove aspects of the self has been found in those who self-harm (38).
An extension of our results would be to conduct an fMRI paradigm which measures neural responses to self-hatred, as well as self-reassurance, generated toward aspects of the self that are liked versus disliked. Indeed, it would be fascinating to run this experiment in populations such as those with high self-hatred to aspects of self, such as body dysmorphia, body-weight shame, or even in populations of eating disorders (39–42) and to examine if interventions such as Compassion Focused Therapy, particularly designed to work with these populations (40, 43) may help attenuate these neural responses. Specifically, we propose that the same neural mechanisms which code for “neural pain” differently for in-groups and out-groups, may likewise by stimulated when considering aspects of the self that are hated. For example, research has examined the effects of neural responses to pain, particularly within the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC), when viewing videos of one’s own race versus another race in suffering (44). Whilst a typical neural bias toward processing own versus other race was evident, activation to other race’s pain within the ACC significantly increased upon greater levels of physical contact with the outgroup, thereby tuning one’s own ACC to respond to the other group’s pain as if they were an ingroup (44). We hypothesize that a clinical sample with high self-report hatred to aspects of the self, would replicate this effect of suppressing neural empathy for pain toward outgroups, to the exact parts of the self that they hate, wish to get rid of, destroy, and expel (34). Critically, we also suggest that a Compassion Focussed Therapy intervention (45) would help to reduce this neural suppression, and allow processing of what was once hated as now something to incorporate as “ingroup”—as part of the self.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The University of Queensland Health and Behavioural Sciences, Low & Negligible Risk Ethics Sub-Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
JJK, RC and JNK conceived and designed the initial experiment. JJK and TH conducted data analyses. JJK, TB, TH and JNK wrote the paper.
Funding
JNK was supported by a University of Queensland Research and Teaching Fellowship. JJK was supported by an Australian Postgraduate Scholarship.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.566141/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Petrocchi N, Dentale F, Gilbert P. Self-reassurance, not self-esteem, serves as a buffer between self-criticism and depressive symptoms. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2019) 92(3):394–406. doi: 10.1111/papt.12186
2. Kim J, Parker S, Doty J, Cunnington R, Gilbert P, Kirby J. Neurophysiological and behavioural markers of compassion. Sci Rep (2020) 10(1):6789. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63846-3
3. Gilbert P, Clarke M, Hempel S, Miles JNV, Irons C. Criticizing and reassuring oneself: An exploration of forms, styles and reasons in female students. Br J Clin Psychol (2004) 43(1):31–50. doi: 10.1348/014466504772812959
4. Zuroff DC, Fournier MA, Moskowitz DS. Depression, Perceived Inferiority, and Interpersonal Behavior: Evidence for the Involuntary Defeat Strategy. J Soc Clin Psychol (2007) 26(7):751–78. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2007.26.7.751
5. Gilbert P, Mcewan K, Matos M, Rivis A. Fears of compassion: Development of three self-report measures. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2011) 2011:239–55. doi: 10.1348/147608310X526511
6. Kirby JN, Day J, Sagar V. The ‘Flow’ of compassion: A meta-analysis of the fears of compassion scales and psychological functioning. Clin Psychol Rev (2019) 70:26–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.03.001
7. Gilbert P. Compassion as a social mentality. In: Gilbert P, editor. Compassion: Concepts, Research and Applications. UK: Taylor & Francis (2017). p. 31–8.
8. Gilbert P. The origins and nature of compassion focused therapy. Br J Clin Psychol (2014) 53(1):6–41. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12043
9. Sue Carter C, Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal and ECP. The Roots of Compassion: An Evolutionary and Neurobiological Perspective. In: Seppälä EM, Simon-Thomas E, Brown SL, Worline MC, Cameron CD, Doty JR, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Compassion Science. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2017).
10. Basran J, Pires C, Matos M, McEwan K, Gilbert P. Styles of leadership, fears of compassion, and competing to avoid inferiority. Front Psychol (2019) 9:10–4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02460
11. Kim JJ, Gerrish R, Gilbert P, Kirby JN. Stressed, depressed, and rank obsessed: Individual differences in compassion and neuroticism predispose towards rank-based depressive symptomatology. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2020) 1–24. doi: 10.1111/papt.12270
12. Halamová J, Kanovský M, Gilbert P, Troop NA, Zuroff DC, Hermanto N, et al. The Factor Structure of the Forms of Self-Criticising/Attacking & Self-Reassuring Scale in Thirteen Distinct Populations. J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2018) 40(4):736–51. doi: 10.1007/s10862-018-9686-2
13. Catarino F, Gilbert P, McEwan K, Baião R. Compassion Motivations: Distinguishing Submissive Compassion From Genuine Compassion and its Association With Shame, Submissive Behavior, Depression, Anxiety and Stress. J Soc Clin Psychol (2014) 33(5):399–412. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2014.33.5.399
14. Wagner N, Waller R. Leveraging parasympathetic nervous system activity to study risk for psychopathology: The special case of callous-unemotional traits. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2020) 118:175–85. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/vcfx5
15. Bello MD, Carnevali L, Petrocchi N, Thayer JF, Gilbert P, Ottaviani C. The compassionate vagus: A meta-analysis on the connection between compassion and heart rate variability. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2020) 166:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.016
16. Kim JJ, Parker SL, Henderson T, Kirby JN. Physiological fractals: Visual and statistical evidence across timescales and experimental states. J R. Soc Interface (2020) 3(34):1–8. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2020.0334
17. Gilbert P, Kirby JN. Building an integrative science for psychotherapy for the 21st century: Preface and introduction. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2019) 2019:1–13. doi: 10.1111/papt.12225
18. Kim JJ, Kent KM, Cunnington R, Gilbert P, Kirby JN. Attachment styles modulate neural markers of threat and imagery when engaging in self-criticism. Sci Rep (2020) 10(13776):1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-70772-x
19. Longe O, Maratos FA, Gilbert P, Evans G, Volker F, Rockliff H, et al. Having a word with yourself: neural correlates of self-criticism and self-reassurance. Neuroimage (2010) 49(2):1849–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.019
20. Lutz J, Berry MP, Napadow V, Germer C, Pollak S, Gardiner P, et al. Neural activations during self-related processing in patients with chronic pain and effects of a brief self-compassion training – a pilot study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag (2020) 34:111155. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111155
21. Gilbert P, Clarke M, Hempel S, Miles JNV, Irons C. Criticizing and reassuring oneself: An exploration of forms, styles and reasons in female students. Br J Clin (2004) 2004:31–50. doi: 10.1348/014466504772812959
22. Olival KJ, Hosseini PR, Zambrana-Torrelio C, Ross N, Bogich TL, Daszak P. Host and viral traits predict zoonotic spillover from mammals. Nature (2017) 546(7660):646–50. doi: 10.1038/nature22975
23. Van den Berge K, de Bézieux HR, Street K, Saelens W, Cannoodt R, Saeys Y, et al. Trajectory-based differential expression analysis for single-cell sequencing data. Nat Commun (2020) 11(1):1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14766-3
24. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2001) 98(7):4259–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098
25. Hamilton JP, Farmer M, Fogelman P, Gotlib IH. Depressive Rumination, the Default-Mode Network, and the Dark Matter of Clinical Neuroscience. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 78(4):224–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.020
26. Nejad AB, Fossati P, Lemogne C. Self-referential processing, rumination, and cortical midline structures in major depression. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:666(OCT). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00666
27. Posner J, Cha J, Wang Z, Talati A, Warner V, Gerber A, et al. Increased Default Mode Network Connectivity in Individuals at High Familial Risk for Depression. Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41(7):1759–67. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.342
28. Wise T, Marwood L, Perkins AM, Herane-Vives A, Joules R, Lythgoe DJ, et al. Instability of default mode network connectivity in major depression: A two-sample confirmation study. Transl Psychiatry (2017) 7(4):e1105–7. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.40
29. Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G. Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2011) 35(3):903–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009
30. Zaki J, Wager TD, Singer T, Keysers C, Gazzola V. The Anatomy of Suffering: Understanding the Relationship between Nociceptive and Empathic Pain. Trends Cognit Sci (2016) 20(4):249–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.02.003
31. Kim JJ, Cunnington R, Kirby JN. The neurophysiological basis of compassion: An fMRI meta-analysis of compassion and its related neural processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2020) 108:112–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.023
32. Duarte J, McEwan K, Barnes C, Gilbert P, Maratos FA. Do therapeutic imagery practices affect physiological and emotional indicators of threat in high self-critics? Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2015) 88:270–84. doi: 10.1111/papt.12043
33. Gilbert P, McEwan K, Irons C, Bhundia R, Christie R, Broomhead C, et al. Self-harm in a mixed clinical population: The roles of self-criticism, shame, and social rank. Br J Clin Psychol (2010) 49(4):563–76. doi: 10.1348/014466509X479771
34. Castilho P, Pinto-Gouveia J, Duarte J. Two forms of self-criticism mediate differently the shame–psychopathological symptoms link. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2017) 90(1):44–54. doi: 10.1111/papt.12094
35. Poldrack RA. Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron (2011) 72(5):692–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001
36. King JR, Dehaene S. Characterizing the dynamics of mental representations: The temporal generalization method. Trends Cognit Sci (2014) 18(4):203–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.002
37. Ashar YK, Andrews-hanna JR, Dimidjian S, Wager TD. Empathic Care and Distress: Predictive Brain Markers and Dissociable Brain Systems Article Empathic Care and Distress: Predictive Brain Markers and Dissociable Brain Systems. Neuron (2017) 94(6):1263–73.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.014
38. Gilbert P, Baldwin MW, Irons C, Baccus JR, Palmer M. Self-criticism and self-warmth: An imagery study exploring their relation to depression. J Cognit Psychother. (2006) 20(2):183–200. doi: 10.1891/jcop.20.2.183
39. Kelly AC, Carter JC. Self-compassion training for binge eating disorder: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract (2015) 88(3):285–303. doi: 10.1111/papt.12044
40. Kelly AC, Carter JC, Borairi S. Are improvements in shame and self-compassion early in eating disorders treatment associated with better patient outcomes? Int J Eat Disord (2014) 47(1):54–64. doi: 10.1002/eat.22196
41. Kelly AC, Carter JC, Zuroff DC, Borairi S. Self-compassion and fear of self-compassion interact to predict response to eating disorders treatment: A preliminary investigation. Psychother Res (2013) 23(3):252–64. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2012.717310
42. Kelly AC, Vimalakanthan K, Carter JC. Understanding the roles of self-esteem, self-compassion, and fear of self-compassion in eating disorder pathology: An examination of female students and eating disorder patients. Eat Behav (2014) 15(3):388–91. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.04.008
43. Duarte C, Stubbs J, Pinto-Gouveia J, Matos M, Gale C, Morris L, et al. The Impact of Self-Criticism and Self-Reassurance on Weight-Related Affect and Well-Being in Participants of a Commercial Weight Management Programme. Obes Facts (2017) 10(2):65–75. doi: 10.1159/000454834
44. Cao Y, Contreras-Huerta LS, McFadyen J, Cunnington R. Racial bias in neural response to others’ pain is reduced with other-race contact. Cortex (2015) 70:68–78. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.010
45. Gilbert P. A Brief Outline of the Evolutionary Approach for Compassion Focused Therapy. EC Psychol Psychiatry (2017) 3:218–27. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/1661/f7202e1c8f844db6c0a0dcfbe957bcfae092.pdf.
Keywords: compassion, fMRI, self-report, modelling, reassurance, criticism
Citation: Kim JJ, Henderson T, Best T, Cunnington R and Kirby JN (2020) Neural and Self-Report Markers of Reassurance: A Generalized Additive Modelling Approach. Front. Psychiatry 11:566141. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.566141
Received: 03 July 2020; Accepted: 01 September 2020;
Published: 23 September 2020.
Edited by:
Philipp Kanske, Technische Universität Dresden, GermanyReviewed by:
Kirsten McEwan, University of Derby, United KingdomVanessa Brown, University of Pittsburgh, United States
Copyright © 2020 Kim, Henderson, Best, Cunnington and Kirby. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jeffrey J. Kim, SmVmZnJleS5raW1AdXFjb25uZWN0LmVkdS5hdQ==