Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 August 2020
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Sensory Information Processing Abnormalities in Schizophrenia and Related Neuropsychiatric Disorders View all 36 articles

Early Visual Processing Is Associated With Social Cognitive Performance in Recent-Onset Schizophrenia

  • 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
  • 2Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California–Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of California–Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Background: Early-stage visual processing deficits are evident in chronic schizophrenia. Consistent with a cascade model of information processing, whereby early perceptual processes have downstream effects on higher-order cognition, impaired visual processing is associated with deficits in social cognition in this clinical population. However, the nature of this relationship in the early phase of illness is unknown. Here, we present data from a study of early visual processing and social cognitive performance in recent-onset schizophrenia (ROSz).

Method: Thirty-two people with ROSz and 20 healthy controls (HC) completed a visual backward masking task using stimuli of real world objects (Object Masking) to assess early-stage (i.e., 0–125 ms post-stimulus onset) visual processing. Subjects also completed two tasks of social cognition, one assessing relatively low-level processes of emotion identification (Emotion Biological Motion, EmoBio), and another assessing more complex, higher-order theory of mind abilities (The Awareness of Social Inference Test, TASIT). Group differences were tested with repeated measures ANOVAs and t-tests. Bivariate correlations and linear regressions tested the strength of associations between early-stage visual processing and social cognitive performance in ROSz.

Results: For Object Masking, the mask interfered with object identification over a longer interval for ROSz than for HC [F (3.19, 159.35) = 8.51, p < 0.001]. ROSz were less accurate on the EmoBio task [t (50) = −3.36, p = 0.001] and on the TASIT compared to HC [F (1, 50) = 38.37, p < 0.001]. For the TASIT ROSz were disproportionately impaired on items assessing sarcasm detection [F (1, 50) = 4.30, p = 0.04]. In ROSz, better Object Masking performance was associated with better social cognitive performance [r EmoBio = 0.45, p < 0.01; r TASIT = 0.41, p < 0.02]. Regression analyses did not provide significant support for low-level social cognition mediating the relationship between visual processing and high-level social cognition.

Conclusion: Early-stage visual processing, low-level social cognition, and high-level social cognition were all significantly impaired in ROSz. Early-stage visual processing was associated with performance on the social cognitive tasks in ROSz, consistent with a cascade model of information processing. However, significant cascading effects within social cognition were not supported. These data suggest that interventions directed at early visual processing may yield downstream effects on social cognitive processes.

Introduction

Schizophrenia is associated with marked impairment across a variety of information processing domains, spanning from very early stages of perceptual processing, through complex, higher-order cognitive processes. Deficits in early-stage visual processing have consistently been reported in chronic schizophrenia (16). Consistent with a cascade model of information processing, whereby disruptions in early perceptual processes have downstream consequences for higher-order cognition and functioning, impaired visual processing is associated with deficits in social cognition (i.e., processing of social stimuli including emotion identification and mental state attribution) in this clinical population (714). Moreover, structural equation modeling analyses have demonstrated that social cognition mediates the relationship between visual processing and community functioning in people with schizophrenia (7, 12, 15). Thus, visual processing and social cognitive abilities are important components of the pathway toward functional recovery in schizophrenia.

Social cognitive deficits are well-documented in people with recent-onset schizophrenia (ROSz), with meta-analytic reviews reporting large effect sizes that are on par with those obtained from chronic phase schizophrenia samples (1618). In contrast, considerably less is known about visual perception abnormalities in the early phase of illness. Similarly, the nature of the relationship between visual processing and social cognition in the early phase of illness is unknown. The available evidence strongly suggests visual processing abnormalities in ROSz (1923), with patients exhibiting significantly impaired contour integration (19), visual perception organization (20), and motion processing (21) relative to healthy adults. However, the magnitude of impairment may be attenuated relative to what is observed in chronic phase schizophrenia (19, 24, 25).

The earliest stages of visual processing can be probed behaviorally with visual masking tasks (see (26) for a comprehensive review). In these tasks, a rapidly presented target stimulus is either shortly preceded by (for forward masking) or shortly followed by (for backward masking) a masking stimulus which interferes with processing of the target. Depending on the type of paradigm used, the masking stimuli may spatially overlap the target, or it may surround, but not touch, the target. The duration of the interval between the target and mask is brief (i.e., 0–500 ms), and is varied across trials. Accuracy for identifying the target, or some aspect of the target (e.g., target location, a feature of the target), is assessed yielding a masking function. For backward masking, the typical response function is S-shaped, with very poor accuracy at short intervals between target and mask, and improved performance as the interval between target and mask increases.

Prior studies of visual backward masking in ROSz indicate impaired performance relative to healthy adults (22, 25), an association with duration of untreated psychosis (i.e., short duration of untreated psychosis associated with better performance; (27)), and stability of performance over 6–24 months (22, 23). Here, we assessed visual backward masking and test the association between early visual processing and performance on social cognitive tasks that involve processing of visual cues in people with ROSz and healthy adults. We hypothesized that visual backward masking performance and social cognitive task performance would be significantly impaired in the ROSz sample compared to healthy adults. In addition, we hypothesized that visual backward masking performance would be significantly correlated with social cognitive task performance in the patient group.

Method

Participants

Thirty-two people with recent-onset schizophrenia (ROSz) and 20 healthy controls (HC) participated in this study. The patient participants were recruited from the UCLA Aftercare Research Program, an outpatient research clinic for ROSz. Inclusion criteria were: 1) onset of a first psychotic episode within 24 months of program entry, 2) fulfillment of DSM-IV (28) criteria for schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, depressed type, or schizophreniform disorder, 3) age of 18 to 45 years, and 4) sufficient fluency in English to allow for valid completion of the testing protocol. These participants met criteria for schizophrenia (n = 24), schizoaffective disorder, depressed type (n = 3), or schizophreniform disorder (n = 5). All ROSz participants were prescribed atypical antipsychotic medication, and chlorpromazine (CPZ) equivalent dosing information (29, 30) can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the study participants.

The HC sample were recruited via advertisements (e.g., online classified ads, flyers in the community), and had no current psychiatric diagnosis, no lifetime history of any psychotic disorder, bipolar disorder, or recurrent depressive disorder, and no history of a psychotic disorder among their first degree relatives. For all subjects, current substance or alcohol use disorder, history of head injury with loss of consciousness, seizure disorder, and/or IQ below 70 were exclusionary. Demographic information for the study participants is presented in Table 1.

Clinical and Cognitive Characterization

DSM-IV diagnoses were made using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (31) and SCID-II (32). For the patient sample, current (i.e., within the 2 week period prior to testing) psychiatric symptoms were assessed by trained raters with the 24-item Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (33). Each clinical rater achieved a median Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) of 0.80 or higher across all BPRS items compared with the criterion ratings and participated in a quality assurance program (34). For the SCID, clinical raters demonstrated an overall kappa coefficient, kappa sensitivity, and kappa specificity of 0.75 or greater, and a diagnostic accuracy kappa coefficient of 0.85 or greater. For all participants, cognitive performance was assessed using the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) (35). The variable of interest was the age and gender-corrected neurocognitive composite score, which reflects performance on tasks assessing speed of processing, attention/vigilance, working memory, verbal learning, visual learning, and reasoning and problem solving.

Visual Processing

All participants completed a backward masking task using common household objects as targets (“Object Masking”; see Figure 1) (36). The task was run in Eprime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA) on a Dell Alienware Aurora R4 Intel Core i7 PC, using a Nvidia GeForce GTX 560 Ti video card. All stimuli were presented on a 120 Hz Asus V0236 LCD 23” monitor. Target stimuli from one of six different objects were presented for 8 ms. Target stimuli were followed by a masking stimulus (overlapping black and white curved lines that spatially overlapped the target location) that was presented for 75 ms after a variable inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 8–125 ms (in 17 ms increments). There were 12 trials per ISI, plus 12 unmasked trials (i.e., target was presented without a mask). After each trial, a list of the six objects appeared and participants verbally reported which object they thought the target was and the tester entered the response into the computer. Because participants gave verbal responses, reaction times were not recorded. The dependent measure was accuracy, measured as proportion correct. Mean accuracy across ISIs was the variable of interest for the correlation and regression analyses (10).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Object Masking task.

Social Cognition

Low-level emotion identification was assessed with Emotion in Biological Motion (EmoBio) (37), and higher-order theory of mind abilities were assessed with Part 3 of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT) (38). For EmoBio, the ability to perceive emotion based on limited cues from body motion (i.e., gait, limb movement, speed of movement) was assessed using the point-light walker stimuli (39) and adapted for clinical trials use (37). Twenty-four point-light walker clips of 5–10 s in length were presented on a computer screen. Participants were asked which of five emotional states (fear, anger, happiness, sadness, or neutral) best described the movement of the walker. The five choices for emotional state were presented on the computer screen immediately after presentation of the clip. The dependent measure was accuracy, measured as proportion correct.

The TASIT Part 3 is comprised of 16 audio-video vignettes of adults interacting with each other. After each vignette, participants respond to four yes/no questions probing the beliefs and intentions of the characters. Half of the vignettes involve a character lying to another character, and half of the vignettes involve a character making sarcastic comments. Participants detect lies and sarcasm based on integration of multiple sources of social information including facial affect, vocal prosody, body motion, mental state attributions, social knowledge, and contextual cues. The dependent measure was accuracy, measured as proportion correct for the Lies and Sarcasm conditions.

Data Analysis

The data were screened for univariate outliers using box plots, and for skewness with histograms. Group differences for the visual processing and social cognition tasks were tested with repeated measures ANOVAs and t-tests. When assumptions of sphericity were violated in the repeated measures analyses, a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used. Bonferroni corrections were used to control the familywise error rate of the post-hoc analyses. For the post-hoc paired t-tests of the Object Masking data, critical α = 0.05/14 tests or 0.004. For post-hoc t-tests of the TASIT data, critical α = 0.05/4 tests or 0.01. In the ROSz group, bivariate correlations and multiple linear regression (method = enter) tested the strength of associations between early-stage visual processing and social cognitive task performance. For the regression analyses, the data were screened for multivariate outliers using Mahalanobis distances (critical α = 0.001). Effect sizes were interpreted as small (r = 0.10, partial η2 = 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.20), medium (r = 0.30, partial η2 = 0.09, Cohen’s d = 0.50), or large (r = 0.50, partial η2 = 0.25, Cohen’s d = 0.80) (40, 41).

Results

Sample Characteristics

Demographic, clinical, and cognitive characteristics of the study participants are presented in Table 1. The two groups were well-matched on demographic characteristics, including age, gender, race and ethnicity, and level of parental education. Twenty-two (69%) of the ROSz participants met criteria for positive symptom remission (i.e., score of ≤3 on BPRS hallucinations, unusual thought content, and conceptual disorganization) for the 2 week period prior to testing (42). As expected, the ROSz participants exhibited significant neurocognitive impairment relative to the HC group [t (50) = 6.20, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.75]. The magnitude of impairment was similar to our previous findings with a different ROSz patient cohort (43).

Visual Processing

Results for the Object Masking task are presented in Figure 2 and Table 2. Mean accuracy (i.e., proportion correct) for the unmasked control condition exceeded 0.90, and performance did not significantly differ between groups [p = 0.12, Cohen’s d = 0.47]. Data for the individual ISIs were negatively skewed, as is typical for backward masking tasks, and were thus log transformed prior to repeated measures analysis. Across ISIs, a typical backward masking response profile was evident [main effect of ISI: F (3.19, 159.35) = 44.08, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.47]. Accuracy was poor at brief ISIs [8 ms ISI mean accuracy = 0.32, s.d. = 0.25], indicating that the mask interfered with processing of the target stimulus, and performance steadily improved as the ISI duration increased [125 ms ISI mean accuracy = 0.94, s.d. = 0.14], reflecting escape from the masking effect.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Object Masking performance in recent-onset schizophrenia and healthy controls.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Descriptive statistics for visual processing and social cognitive task performance.

The interference by the mask was greater in ROSz compared to HC [group × ISI: F (3.19, 159.35) = 8.51, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.15]. For the HC group, follow-up paired t-tests indicated that accuracy significantly improved between 25 and 42 ms ISI [t (19) = 6.52, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.45], but tapered off between 42 and 58 ms ISI [t (19) = 1.87, p = 0.07, Cohen’s d = 0.19], reflecting escape from the masking effect. Compared to HC, the ROSz group exhibited worse performance across ISIs [group: F (1, 50) = 5.76, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.10]. Follow-up paired t-tests indicated that performance in the patient group steadily improved through 75 ms ISI [p’s < 0.001, Cohen’s d’s ≥ 0.75], before leveling off between 75 and 92 ms ISI [t (31) = 1.57, p = 0.13, Cohen’s d = 0.28] reflecting escape from the masking effect.1

Social Cognition

Results for the social cognitive tasks are presented in Figure 3 and Table 2. Scores were normally distributed and free of outliers. For the EmoBio task, the ROSz group were less accurate compared to HC [t (50) = 3.36, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.10]. Similarly, for the TASIT, the ROSz group exhibited overall poorer performance compared to HC [F (1,50) = 38.37, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.43]. There was a significant interaction between TASIT condition and group [F (1,50) = 4.30, p = 0.04, partial η2 = 0.08]. Compared to HC, ROSz exhibited impaired performance on lie [t (50) = −3.54, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = −1.00] and sarcasm [t (50) = −5.17, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −1.46] detection. However, the ROSz group were disproportionately impaired on items assessing sarcasm detection [t (30) = 3.09, p = 0.004, Cohen’s d = 0.55] compared to HC [t (19) = 0.13, p = 0.90, Cohen’s d = 0.03].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Social cognitive task performance in recent-onset schizophrenia and healthy controls. *Denotes group contrast p < 0.05. **Denotes group × condition interaction effect p < 0.05.

Relationship Between Visual Processing and Social Cognition

A scatterplot of the relationship between Object Masking performance and social cognitive task performance in the patient sample is displayed in Figure 4. Scores for mean Object Masking performance averaged across ISIs were normally distributed and free of outliers. In ROSz, better Object Masking performance was associated with better social cognitive task performance. This was true for both low- and high-level tasks, and the correlations were of similar magnitude (r EmoBio= 0.45, p = 0.01; r TASIT = 0.41, p = 0.02).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Scatterplot of Object Masking performance and social cognitive task performance in recent-onset schizophrenia.

Regression analyses were conducted to test whether the relationship between visual processing and higher-level social cognition (i.e., TASIT part 3 performance) was mediated by lower-level social cognition (i.e., Emo Bio). The data were free of multivariate outliers. Object Masking performance was a significant predictor of the proposed mediator [Emo Bio, β = 0.45, t (29) = 2.70, p < 0.001] and the outcome variable [TASIT, β = 0.41, t (29) = 2.41, p = 0.02]. However, Emo Bio performance was not a significant predictor of TASIT performance [β = 0.28, t (29) = 1.58, p = 0.13]. Thus, these data do not provide significant support for mediation.2

Discussion

In this study, we evaluated early visual processing, assessed with visual backward masking, and social cognitive task performance in people with recent-onset schizophrenia (ROSz) and healthy controls (HC). We hypothesized that visual backward masking performance and social cognitive task performance would be significantly impaired in ROSz compared to HC. In addition, we hypothesized that visual backward masking performance would be significantly correlated with social cognitive task performance in the ROSz group. Our first hypothesis was supported. Congruent with the findings of Favrod et al. (25) and Perez et al. (22), early visual processing was significantly impaired in the early phase of schizophrenia. The masking effect was exaggerated in the ROSz group. Compared to HC, the ROSz group showed greater interference from the mask, as indicated by lower performance accuracy across ISIs, and the patient group required longer ISIs to escape the masking effect. Similarly, we found support for our second hypothesis: social cognition was significantly impaired in ROSz. The ROSz group exhibited an impaired ability to identify emotional states from body movement cues (low-level social cognition), and impaired theory of mind (high-level social cognition).

An association between visual processing and social cognition has previously been demonstrated in chronic phase schizophrenia (714). This relationship is hypothesized to reflect a cascading effect of disruptions of early stages of information processing on downstream cognitive functions (7, 12, 44). Consistent with a cascade model, early visual processing was associated with performance on the social cognitive tasks in this ROSz sample. Thus, our third hypothesis was supported. Consistent with prior findings in chronic schizophrenia (7, 914), the strength of the association between visual processing and social cognition was moderate in magnitude. Moreover, the relationship was consistent across low- and high-level social cognition processes.

Although the data supported a cascading effect between visual processing and social cognition, a cascading effect within social cognition, i.e., with low-level social cognition performance predicting high-level social cognition, was not supported by the regression analyses. Our results are in contrast to theoretical accounts and empirical data for a hierarchical stream within social information processing (45, 46). However, a major limitation of the current study was the small sample size, which likely rendered the analyses underpowered to detect mediation.

Remediation of the perceptual and cognitive impairments associated with schizophrenia using a neuroplasticity-based, bottom-up training approach is a growing area of research, and the visual system is amenable to training. The studies conducted so far suggest that targeted visual training holds promise for remediating visual processing impairments in people with schizophrenia. Improved performance on trained visual tasks, including visual backward masking, motion perception, contrast sensitivity, visual search efficiency, visual acuity, and perceptual organization have been reported (4751). Beyond improvements on trained tasks, data from the present study suggest that interventions directed at improving early visual processing might also yield effects on downstream social cognitive processes in those with a recent onset of schizophrenia. This is a question for future research.

Conclusions

Early-stage visual processing, low-level, and high-level social cognition were all significantly impaired in ROSz. These data provide support for a cascade model of information processing between early-stage visual processing and social cognition in ROSz, but did not support significant cascading effects within social cognition.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by UCLA Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AM, MG, JW, and KN contributed conception and design of the study. AM managed data collection, organized the database, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JW, JL, ER, and MG wrote sections of the manuscript. JV and KS managed subject recruitment, and clinical and cognitive characterization of the sample. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by an individual fellowship award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR MFE120919, PI: AM) and by a NIMH Center Grant (P50 MH066286, PI: KN). AM is supported by a career development award from the NIMH (K23 MH108829).

Conflict of Interest

AM has received compensation from MedAvante-Prophase, Inc. for clinical assessment services unrelated to this project, and she has received research support from Alkermes, Inc. ER was previously an employee of Data Cubed LLC. JV has received funding from Brain Plasticity, Inc., Genentech, Inc., and has served as a consultant to Boehringer-Ingelheim, GmbH, and Brain Plasticity, Inc. KS has served on the speakers bureau for Otsuka’s Abilify Maintena, a CME speaker for Janssen Inc. Canada and PeerVoice. He has served as a consultant to Janssen Scientific Affairs, LLC, Alkermes, Inc., Medincell, Inc., and Teva Pharmaceuticals, and has received research support from Alkermes, Inc. MG and KN are officers within MATRICS Assessment, Inc., the publisher of the MCCB, but do not receive any financial remuneration for their roles. MG has been a consultant to Biogen, Click Therapeutics, Lundbeck, and Roche, and he is on the scientific board of Cadent. KN has received research grants from Janssen Scientific Affairs, LLC, Posit Science, Inc., and Genentech, Inc., and has been a consultant to Astellas, Biogen, Genentech, Janssen, Medincell, Otsuka, Takeda, and Teva.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the recent-onset schizophrenia and the community comparison subjects for their participation in this study. We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of the UCLA Aftercare Research Program staff, including Lilian Medina, B.A., Jacqueline Hayata, B.A., Michael Boucher, M.D., Laurie Casaus, M.D., John Luo, M.D., Elizabeth Arreola, B.S., Fe Asuan, B.A., Denise Gretchen-Doorly, Ph.D., Lissa Portillo, Yurika Sturdevant, Psy.D., and Luana Turner, Psy.D. We thank Jennifer Hoy, B.S., Jaime Morales, M.A., and Rachel Wein, B.A., for their assistance with data collection, and Ana Cecilia Myers, M.S. for her assistance with study coordination.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Note: Results from a follow-up repeated measures analyses in the ROSz sample that included CPZ equivalents as a covariate did not differ substantively from those reported in the manuscript (main effect of ISI, p < 0.001; main effect of CPZ, p = 0.11, ISI x CPZ, p = 0.68).
  2. ^ Note: Subsequent analyses that included CPZ equivalents did not substantively differ from the results reported above. Specifically, the association between OM and the two social cognition measures remained statistically significant after controlling for CPZ equivalents.

References

1. Jahshan C, Wynn JK, McCleery A, Glahn DC, Altshuler LL, Green MF. Cross-diagnostic comparison of visual processing in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res (2014) 51:42–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Butler PD, Javitt DC. Early-stage visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2005) 18:151–7. doi: 10.1097/00001504-200503000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Braff DL, Saccuzzo DP, Geyer MA. Information processing dysfunctions in schizophrenia: studies of visual backward masking, sensorimotor gating, and habituation. In: Steinhauer SR, Gruzelier JH, Zubin J, editors, Handbook of schizophrenia, Vol. 5. Neuropsychology, psychophysiology, and information processing. Elsevier Science (1991). p. 303–334. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700039167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Cadenhead KS, Serper Y, Braff DL. Transient versus sustained visual channels in the visual backward masking deficits of schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry (1998) 43:132–8. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00316-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rund BR, Egeland J, Sundet K, Asbjørnsen A, Hugdahl K, Landrø NI, et al. Early visual information processing in schizophrenia compared to recurrent depression. Schizophr Res (2004) 68:111–8. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00193-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Mintz J. Backward Masking in Schizophrenia and Mania: I. Specifying a Mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1994) 51:939–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950120011003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Green MF, Hellemann G, Horan WP, Lee J, Wynn JK. From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2012) 69:1216–24. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Sergi MJ, Green MF. Social perception and early visual processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2003) 59:233–41. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00405-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Brittain P, ffytche DH, McKendrick A, Surguladze S. Visual processing, social cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2010) 178:270–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rassovsky Y, Horan WP, Lee J, Sergi MJ, Green MF. Pathways between early visual processing and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Psychol Med (2011) 41:487–97. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Matsumoto Y, Takahashi H, Murai T, Takahashi H. Visual processing and social cognition in schizophrenia: Relationships among eye movements, biological motion perception, and empathy. Neurosci Res (2015) 90:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sergi MJ, Rassovsky Y, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF. Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2006) 163:448–54. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Norton D, McBain R, Holt DJ, Ongur D, Chen Y. Association of Impaired Facial Affect Recognition with Basic Facial and Visual Processing Deficits in Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 65:1094–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kelemen O, Erdélyi R, Pataki I, Benedek G, Janka Z, Kéri S. Theory of mind and motion perception in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology (2005) 19:494–500. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rassovsky Y, Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Breitmeyer BG, Mintz J. Visual processing in schizophrenia: Structural equation modeling of visual masking performance. Schizophr Res (2005) 78:251–60. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39:979–92. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophr Res (2009) 109:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bora E, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res (2013) 144:31–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Keane BP, Paterno D, Kastner S, Silverstein SM. Visual integration dysfunction in schizophrenia arises by the first psychotic episode and worsens with illness duration. J Abnorm Psychol (2016) 125:43–549. doi: 10.1037/abn0000157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Silverstein S, Uhlhaas PJ, Essex B, Halpin S, Schall U, Carr V. Perceptual organization in first episode schizophrenia and ultra-high-risk states. Schizophr Res (2006) 83:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lencer R, Keedy SK, Reilly JL, McDonough BE, Harris MSH, Sprenger A, et al. Altered transfer of visual motion information to parietal association cortex in untreated first-episode psychosis: Implications for pursuit eye tracking. Psychiatry Res - Neuroimaging (2011) 194:30–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Perez VB, Shafer KM, Cadenhead KS. Visual information processing dysfunction across the developmental course of early psychosis. Psychol Med (2012) 42:2167–79. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lee J, Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, Sugar CA, Ventura J, Gretchen-Doorly D, et al. Stability of visual masking performance in recent-onset schizophrenia: An 18-month longitudinal study. Schizophr Res (2008) 103:266–74. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Feigenson KA, Keane BP, Roché MW, Silverstein SM. Contour integration impairment in schizophrenia and first episode psychosis: State or trait? Schizophr Res (2014) 159:515–20. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Favrod O, Roinishvili M, da Cruz JR, Brand A, Okruashvili M, Gamkrelidze T, et al. Electrophysiological correlates of visual backward masking in patients with first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res - Neuroimaging (2018) 282:64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Green MF, Lee J, Wynn JK, Mathis KI. Visual Masking in Schizophrenia: Overview and Theoretical Implications. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37:700–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Cuesta MJ, García de Jalón E, Campos MS, Ibáñez B, Sánchez-Torres AM, Peralta V. Duration of untreated negative and positive symptoms of psychosis and cognitive impairment in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res (2012) 141:222–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

Google Scholar

29. Rothe PH, Heres S, Leucht S. Dose equivalents for second generation long-acting injectable antipsychotics: The minimum effective dose method. Schizophr Res (2018) 193:23–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Andreasen NC, Pressler M, Nopoulos P, Miller D, Ho B-C. Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67:255–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Benjamin LS, Williams JB. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV® axis II personality disorders SCID-II. American Psychiatric Pub. (1997).

Google Scholar

32. Benjamin L. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II personality disorders (SCID II). New York NY Biom Res Dep (1994).

Google Scholar

33. Lukoff D, Nuechterlein KH, Ventura J. Manual for the expanded brief psychiatric rating scale. Schizophr Bull (1986) 12:594–602. doi: 10.1093/schbul/12.4.578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ventura J, Green MF, Shaner A, Liberman RP. Training and quality assurance with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale: “The drift busters”. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res (1993) 3:221–44.

Google Scholar

35. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF. MCCB: MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery. Matrics Assess Inc (2006).

Google Scholar

36. Reavis EA, Lee J, Wynn JK, Narr KL, Njau SN, Engel SA, et al. Linking optic radiation volume to visual perception in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res (2017) 190:102–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kern RS, Penn DL, Lee J, Horan WP, Reise SP, Ochsner KN, et al. Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, Part 2: trolling the depths of psychometric properties. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39:1201–10. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. McDonald S, Flanagan S, Rollins J, Kinch J. A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2003) 18:219–38. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200305000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Heberlein AS, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H. Cortical regions for judgments of emotions and personality traits from point-light walkers. J Cognit Neurosci (2004) 16:1143–58. doi: 10.1162/0898929041920423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates (1988). doi: 10.1234/12345678.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. Pearson Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education (2007).

Google Scholar

42. Nuechterlein KH, Miklowitz DJ, Ventura J, Gitlin MJ, Stoddard M, Lukoff D. Classifying episodes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Criteria for relapse and remission applied to recent-onset samples. Psychiatry Res (2006) 144:153–66. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Butler PD, Thompson JL, Seitz AR, Deveau J, Silverstein SM. Visual perceptual remediation for individuals with schizophrenia: Rationale, method, and three case studies. Psychiatr Rehabil J (2017) 40:43–52. doi: 10.1037/prj0000212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. McCleery A, Ventura J, Kern RS, Subotnik KL, Gretchen-Doorly D, Green MF, et al. Cognitive functioning in first-episode schizophrenia: MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) Profile of Impairment. Schizophr Res (2014) 157:33–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.04.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Javitt DC. When Doors of Perception Close: Bottom-up Models of Disrupted Cognition in Schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2009) 5:249–75. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ochsner KN. The Social-Emotional Processing Stream: Five Core Constructs and Their Translational Potential for Schizophrenia and Beyond. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64:48–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Vaskinn A, Andersson S, Østefjells T, Andreassen OA, Sundet K. Emotion perception, non-social cognition and symptoms as predictors of theory of mind in schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry (2018) 85:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Norton DJ, Hospital M, Mcbain RK, Ongur D, Chen Y. Perceptual training strongly improves visual motion perception in schizophrenia. Brain Cog (2011) 77(2):248–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Surti TS, Wexler BE. A pilot and feasibility study of computer-based training for visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2012) 142:248–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Surti TS, Corbera S, Bell MD, Wexler BE. Successful computer-based visual training specifically predicts visual memory enhancement over verbal memory improvement in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2011) 132:131–4. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Demmin DL, Fradkin SI, Silverstein SM. Remediation of Visual Processing Impairments in Schizophrenia: Where We Are and Where We Need to Be. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep (2019) 6:13–20. doi: 10.1007/s40473-019-00171-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: visual perception, backward masking, theory of mind, emotion identification, first episode psychosis

Citation: McCleery A, Wynn JK, Lee J, Reavis EA, Ventura J, Subotnik KL, Green MF and Nuechterlein KH (2020) Early Visual Processing Is Associated With Social Cognitive Performance in Recent-Onset Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 11:823. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00823

Received: 13 March 2020; Accepted: 30 July 2020;
Published: 25 August 2020.

Edited by:

Neal R. Swerdlow, University of California, San Diego, United States

Reviewed by:

Anja Vaskinn, Oslo University Hospital, Norway
Tim Ziermans, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2020 McCleery, Wynn, Lee, Reavis, Ventura, Subotnik, Green and Nuechterlein. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Amanda McCleery, amanda-mccleery@uiowa.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.