- 1Department of Global Health Promotion, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
- 2Institute of Education, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
- 3Department of Medical Education Research and Development, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
- 4Department of Pharmacology, National Research Institute for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan
- 5Department of Social Medicine, National Research Institute for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan
Background: Children who are exposed to natural disasters are at greater risk of developing mental and behavior problems. Prior studies have suggested that positive parenting practices could prevent child mental and behavior problems among those who were exposed to natural disasters. Parent–child interaction increases oxytocin level in parents and infants; however, studies assessing the change in oxytocin level after positive parent-child interaction and its effect on child behavior problems among preadolescents who were exposed to natural disasters are lacking. This study investigated whether playful interaction stimulated oxytocin levels in 34 mother–child dyads who experienced the 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake in Kesennuma City in Miyagi Prefecture, Japan, and the effect of the maternal oxytocin changes on child behavior problems.
Methods: Participants were recruited in 2012 after the Great East Japan Earthquake. Annual surveys were conducted from 2012 to 2017. Salivary oxytocin level was assessed before and after the playful interaction in 2015. Behavior problems were evaluated by caregivers, using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) in 2017. Fixed effect regression analyses were conducted to determine the effect of playful mother–child interaction on oxytocin level by comparing the change in the 10 min after the interaction with the change in the 10 min before the interaction. We also examined the effect of maternal oxytocin changes before and after the playful interaction on the onset of child behavior problems in 2017.
Results: A significant increase in maternal oxytocin level was detected following playful interaction, especially among mothers of first-born boys (2.63 pg/mg protein. 95% CI: 0.45, 4.81). Maternal psychological distress and trauma were also negatively associated with an increase of oxytocin levels. The increase in maternal oxytocin level was significantly associated with lower externalizing problem score of children 2 years later.
Conclusion: Our results might suggest a rational for potential preventive intervention for child behavior problems through playful mother–child interaction after natural disasters.
Introduction
Numerous studies have shown an increasing frequency and severity of natural disasters in many parts of the world (1–5). Children who are exposed to natural disasters are at greater risk of developing posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depressive symptoms, anxiety, and behavior problems (6–10).
Previous studies have tested a wide variety of interventions with the goal of alleviating deleterious effects of disaster experiences on mental health, emotional well-being, and behavior in children (9, 11–13). These preventive measures include cognitive behavioral approaches, provision of social support, and development of coping skills. Many of the interventions are provided in schools by mental health professionals or school personnel (9, 11, 13). Notably, an association between parenting practices and favorable parent–child relationships and child mental symptoms after disaster exposure has been observed (7, 14, 15). These studies suggest programs that promote positive parenting practices prevent child mental and behavior problems among those who were exposed to natural disasters.
Prior studies reported that parenting behaviors were partly affected by oxytocin (16–18). Intranasal administration of synthetic oxytocin led to the induction of maternal behavior in rats (19) and increased gazing behavior in dogs (20). In humans, intranasal administration of oxytocin was associated with improvement in protective behavior towards their children in depressed mothers (16, 21). Feldman et al. reported that parental interaction with their infant by itself increased oxytocin level in parents and infants (17). Stimulating natural secretion of oxytocin by parent–child interaction would be better suited to large-scale parenting programs among disaster survivors, because it would be more accessible and economical than intranasal administration of oxytocin. However, studies assessing the change in oxytocin level after parent–child interaction among preadolescents who were exposed to natural disasters are lacking.
Moreover, to develop a practical intervention after natural disaster, heterogeneity of subjects needs to be considered. The effectiveness of parenting intervention may depend on factors such as specific trauma exposure and the birth order or sex of the child. Assessment of change in oxytocin level due to parenting among those who were exposed to natural disasters, stratified by trauma type, and the children's characteristics, may provide the rationale for parenting intervention after natural disasters.
The Great East Japan Earthquake and tsunami struck Japan on 11 March 2011, causing more than 15,000 deaths and damaging over 400,000 houses, together with the evacuation of nearly 470,000 people (22, 23). About 18 months after the earthquake, we recruited children aged 3–5 years old at the time of the earthquake to investigate the impact of the earthquake on young children (24). The objectives of the study were: 1) to examine whether experimental playful interaction stimulated oxytocin level in mothers and children who were exposed to the Great East Japan Earthquake in Kesennuma city in Miyagi, Japan, 2) to examine the effect of the interaction on short-term psychophysiological status using heart rate variability (HRV), and 3) to assess the effect of maternal oxytocin changes due to interaction on the onset of child behavior problems 2 years later.
Methods
Sample
Participants included 34 mother–child dyads from the cohort in Kesennuma, Miyagi, Japan, who were recruited using a multistage sampling method after the Great East Japan Earthquake in 2012. The detail of the cohort was described elsewhere (24). We selected Kesennuma city, because it was severely affected by the earthquake, the resulting tsunami, and fire. Preschools in the selected municipality were invited to participate, and two of 16 preschools agreed to participate. Mothers with children aged 3 to 5 years old were invited to join the study by the principals or staff of the preschools in 2012.
Because of the limited language skills of preschool children, the effect of exposure to trauma due to the earthquake is difficult to assess and has been understudied (25). We initiated this cohort in Kesennuma, Miyagi, to clarify the effect of exposure to trauma due to the earthquake in preschool children (24, 25). In our prior study, using data from this cohort, we reported an association between facial expressions of preschool children, measured by facial expression recognition software, and child PTSD symptoms (25).
We conducted annual surveys from 2012 to 2017. Enrolled mothers provided our research coordinators with written informed consent for their child, completed the questionnaire, and conducted playful mother–child interaction. Informed consent was also obtained to publish the image from the participant for Figure 2. After excluding one mother for incomplete oxytocin measurement and five children (three for incomplete oxytocin measurement, one for incorrect measurement, and one for missing identification), an analytic sample of 33 mothers and 29 children was obtained. This study was approved by the research ethics committees at the National Center for Child Health and Development (584) and the Ethics Committee at Tokyo Medical and Dental University (M2016-148). This study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Japan's Ethical Guidelines for Epidemiological Research established by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology and the Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare.
Procedure
In the annual survey conducted in 2015, as shown in Figure 1, after the participants arrived at the examination lab, the mother and children were asked to wait in separate rooms (Time point 0: T0). During the next 10 min, HRV of mothers and children in the pre-interaction period was measured. Ten minutes after T0, the first saliva sample collection was made (Time point 1: T1). During the pre-interaction period (control period), samples were collected twice: 5 min after T1 (Time point 2: T2) and 10 min after T2 (Time point 3: T3). Similarly, during the post-interaction period, sample were collected twice: 5 min after the interaction (Time point 4: T4) and 10 min after T4 (Time point 5: T5). During the 10 min between T4 and T5, HRV of mothers and children in the post-interaction period was measured. We assessed the effect of playful mother–child interaction on oxytocin level by comparing the change after the interaction (from T4 to T5 in Figure 1) with the change before the interaction (from T2 to T3 in Figure 1).
Intervention: Playful Mother–Child Interaction (Conducted in 2015)
We developed the following procedure for playful mother–child interaction based on previous studies (26, 27) (Figure 2). The intervention was conducted on a 3.2 m2 mattress in a 50–100 m2 room in either a local health center or a nursery school in Kesennuma city. The entire intervention was video-taped for quality control.
Figure 2 Intervention: Playful Mother–Child Interaction. Intervention protocol with representative images for each procedure was shown.
1. In a standing position, the dyads conducted a session of jan-ken-pon (rock–paper–scissors), with the winner tickling the loser. The game was played repeatedly for 2 min.
2. In a sitting position, the dyads played a game that involved trying to pull off each other's socks for 2 min.
3. In a sitting position, the dyads played a session of jan-ken-pon, with the winner tickling the loser at the base of the foot. The game was played repeatedly for 2 min.
4. In a sitting position, the dyads played a session of jan-ken-pon, with the winner tickling the loser all over their body. The game was played repeatedly for 2 min.
5. Each of the dyads hugged for 20 s and told each other, “I love you”. The timing of the saliva sample collection is described in the following section for oxytocin measurement (Figure 1).
Measures
Oxytocin
We collected five saliva samples from each dyad (Figure 1). Saliva samples were collected by Salivette (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) and kept frozen at −80◦C until the assay. Salivary oxytocin level was measured using a commercial ELISA kit (Assay Designs, Ann Arbor, MI, USA) (28). The data were normalized by protein concentration in the saliva (pg/mg salivary protein).
Heart Rate Variability
In the 2015 annual survey, HRV was assessed at two time points (Figure 1), with a TAS9 pulse analyzer (YKC, Tokyo, Japan) by recording pulse rate for 5 min. In the time-domain, inter-beat intervals (R-R intervals, RRI) were calculated. Then, the standard deviation of normal to normal R-R intervals (SDNN) and the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) were assessed. For the analyses of HRV in the frequency-domain, the signal was filtered into different frequency bands: a low-frequency (LF) (0.04–0.15 Hz) and high-frequency component (HF) (0.15–0.40 Hz). The LF/HF ratio was used for further analyses (29–31). LF, HF, and LF/HF ratio were analyzed in the log scale.
Other Variables
Information on the following variables were assessed using questionnaires that were administered to mothers from September 2012 to June 2013: maternal age, child sex, birth order of the child, K6 (32, 33), Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (APQ) (34), and whether they experienced damage to their house, witnessed the tsunami, or heard the explosion of multiple oil tanks (32, 33). K6 is a six-item scale used for screening psychological distress and has a score ranging from 0 to 24 with a higher score indicating more frequent psychological distress (33). The Japanese version of K6 was used in this study (32, 33). For assessing maternal parenting, we used APQ (34), which has five subscales: involvement, positive parenting, poor monitoring, inconsistent care, and corporal punishment (35–37). The total APQ score was used for the analysis after dividing it into two categories at the median. Behavior problems were evaluated by caregivers, using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) in 2017 (38). The T score of the CBCL internalizing, externalizing, and total problem scores were calculated using the standardized distribution of Japanese children (39).
Statistical Analyses
In order to account for the within-individual association between repeatedly measured data from the same child, fixed effect regression analyses were conducted to determine the effect of playful mother–child interaction on oxytocin level by comparing the change after the interaction with the change before the interaction. Samples were also stratified by maternal and child characteristics, such as sex of the child, birth order of the child, K6, and whether they experienced damage to their house, witnessed the tsunami, or heard the explosion of multiple oil tanks, to examine whether the effects varied depending on maternal and child characteristics. Fixed effect regression analyses were also conducted to determine the effect of the playful mother–child interaction on HRV in mothers and children. We fit linear regression models to assess the association between maternal change in oxytocin level following playful mother–child interaction in 2015 and the CBCL score 2 years later, adjusting for mother's age, sex of the child, APQ, and child traumatic experiences due to the earthquake. All analyses were conducted using STATA SE statistical package, version 14 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX, USA).
Results
Characteristics of the Sample Population
Table 1 shows the characteristics of the sample population. The mean maternal age was 38.5 years (SD: 5.1). About 40%–60% of mothers experienced hardships caused by the earthquake such as damage to the house, experience of living in a shelter, witnessing the resulting destructive tsunami, and hearing the explosion of multiple oil tanks. About one-third of the mothers reported psychological distress as defined by K6 score of 5 or more (32, 33). The mean age of the children was 8.8 years (SD: 0.9), with two-thirds being female, and about half being a first-born child.
Mean Oxytocin Levels of Mothers and Children at Each Time Point
Table 2 shows the mean oxytocin levels of the mothers and children at each time point of the measurement. A negative change in maternal mean oxytocin level in the pre-interaction period was detected (from 10.14 to 9.31 pg/mg protein). A decreasing trend was observed in both the mothers with boys and mothers with girls. In the post-interaction period, a positive change in maternal mean oxytocin level was detected during the same 10 min under resting conditions (from 9.47 to 10.06 pg/mg protein). Although the increasing trend was observed in both the mothers with boys and mothers with girls, it was more evident in mothers with boys (mothers with boys: from 9.48 to 10.88 pg/mg protein; mothers with girls: from 9.46 to 9.53 pg/mg protein).
In the children, a negative change in oxytocin level from T2 to T3 was detected (from 9.63 to 9.42 pg/mg protein). Similarly, a negative change in the child mean oxytocin level from T4 to T5 in the post-interaction period was detected (from 9.55 to 8.83 pg/mg protein).
Maternal Oxytocin Response Following Mother–Child Interaction
Figure 3 shows the maternal oxytocin responses due to playful mother–child interaction. An increase in maternal oxytocin level was detected among all mothers after the playful mother–child interaction of 1.42 pg/mg protein (95% CI 0.18, 2.66, p=0.026). In mothers with boys, we observed a significant increase in maternal oxytocin level of 2.63 pg/mg protein (95% CI 0.45, 4.81, p=0.022), whereas in mothers with girls, the change was not significant (coefficient 0.63 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -0.90, 2.17, p=0.397) (Figure 3A). The sample was stratified using the sex and birth order of the child (Figure 3B). In mothers with a male first-born, we detected a significant increase of 4.24 pg/mg protein (95% CI 1.66, 6.83, p=0.006); however, in mothers with a female first-born, the change was minimal and not significant (coefficient -0.42 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -2.99, 2.14, p=0.714). We did not find significant changes in mothers with a non-first-born boy or girl (non-first-born boy: coefficient -1.20 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -2.95, 0.54, p=0.116; non-first-born girl: coefficient 1.54 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -0.80, 3.87, p=0.170) (Figure 3B).
Figure 3 Maternal oxytocin response following playful mother–child interaction. (A) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by child sex. *P < 0.05. (B) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying according to sex of the child as well as the birth order of the child. *P < 0.05. (C) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by the presence of damage to the house by the earthquake. *P < 0.05. (D) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by maternal experience of having lived in a shelter after the earthquake. *P < 0.05. (E) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by the maternal experience of having witnessed tsunami after the earthquake. *P < 0.05. (F) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by the maternal experience of having heard the explosion of multiple oil tanks after the earthquake. *P < 0.05. (G) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by maternal psychological distress as assessed by K6 score (≤4 vs ≥5). *P < 0.05. (H) Change in maternal oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by maternal psychological distress as assessed by K6 score (≤4 vs ≥5) and the presence of damage to the house by the earthquake. *P < 0.05. All data and graph bars are expressed as the mean ± standard error. Saliva oxytocin values are in pg/mg protein.
In mothers whose house was not damaged by the earthquake (Figure 3C), we observed a significant increase in maternal oxytocin level of 2.46 pg/mg protein (95% CI 0.76, 4.16, p=0.007). However, in mothers whose house was damaged by the earthquake, the increase was small and not significant (coefficient 0.32 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.52, 2.15, p=0.718). In mothers who lived in a shelter after the earthquake (Figure 3D), the change was smaller and not significant (coefficient 0.98 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -0.67, 2.63, p=0.221), whereas the change was larger and significant in those who did not live in a shelter (coefficient 1.92 pg/mg protein, 95% CI 0.25, 3.59, p=0.027). In mothers who witnessed the destructive tsunami after the earthquake (Figure 3E), the change was smaller and not significant (coefficient 1.32 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -0.12, 2.77, p=0.070), whereas the change was larger and significant in mothers who did not witness the tsunami (coefficient 2.47 pg/mg protein, 95% CI 0.26, 4.69, p=0.032). Similarly, in mothers who heard the explosion of multiple oil tanks (Figure 3F), the change was smaller and not significant (coefficient 1.31 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -0.29, 2.91, p=0.102), whereas the change was larger and significant in mothers who did not hear the explosion (coefficient 2.28 pg/mg protein, 95% CI 0.20, 4.35, p=0.034).
We also performed subgroup analysis stratified by maternal psychological distress as assessed by K6 score (Figure 3G). In mothers who did not report psychological distress, the change was larger and significant (coefficient 2.13 pg/mg protein, 95% CI 0.52, 3.74, p=0.012), whereas the change was minimal in mothers with psychological distress (coefficient 0.14 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.46, 1.75, p=0.843). We stratified using K6 and whether the respondent experienced damage to their house by the earthquake (Figure 3H). In mothers who did not report psychological distress and whose house was not damaged by the earthquake, the change was the largest and significant (coefficient 3.40 pg/mg protein, 95% CI 1.42, 5.38, p=0.003). In contrast, in mothers who did not report psychological distress and whose house was damaged by the earthquake, the change was smaller and not significant (coefficient 0.75 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.93, 3.43, p=0.547). In mothers with psychological distress, the change was minimal and not significant, irrespective of the damage to their house (Figure 3H).
Child Oxytocin Response Following Mother–Child Interaction
Figure 4 shows child oxytocin response due to playful mother–child interaction. The change was small in both boys and girls (boys: coefficient -0.19 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.54, 1.16, p=0.757; girls: coefficient -0.66 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -2.04, 0.72, p=0.329) (Figure 4A). Figure 4B shows the results of the analyses with data stratified by the sex and birth order of the child. The changes were in the direction of a decreasing trend, although none of the changes were significant. Figures 4C–E show the oxytocin response following playful mother–child interaction according to maternal experience of the earthquake and maternal level of psychological distress. In Figure 4C, we observed some decreases in both children whose house was damaged and children whose house was not damaged by the earthquake, although the changes were not significant (damaged: coefficient -0.39 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.99, 1.21, p=0.608; not damaged: coefficient -0.61 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.90, 0.67, p=0.321). Similarly, Figure 4D shows that there were some decreases in both children of mothers who lived in a shelter and children of mothers who did not live in a shelter; however, the results were not significant (lived in a shelter: coefficient -0.72 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -2.66, 1.21, p=0.428; did not live in a shelter: coefficient -0.45 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.59, 0.70, p=0.419). As shown in Figure 4E, the change in oxytocin level was not significant irrespective of maternal K6 score (maternal K6 ≤ 4: coefficient -0.56 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -1.88, 0.76, p=0.387; maternal K6 ≥ 5: coefficient -0.45 pg/mg protein, 95% CI -2.17, 1.28, p=0.560).
Figure 4 Child oxytocin response following playful mother–child interaction. (A) Change in child oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by sex of the child. (B) Change in child oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by sex of the child as well as the birth order of the child. (C) Change in child oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by the presence of damage to the house by the earthquake. (D) Change in child oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by the maternal experience of having lived in a shelter after the earthquake. (E) Change in child oxytocin levels by playful mother–child interaction by stratifying by maternal psychological distress as assessed by K6 score (≤4 vs ≥5). All data and graph bars are expressed as the mean ± standard error. Saliva oxytocin values are in pg/mg protein.
Changes in HRV Measures Among Mothers and Children
The results of the HRV measurements and changes in the HRV measurements by playful mother–child interaction are shown in Table 3 and Supplementary Table 1, respectively. Regarding the changes in mothers following the interaction (Table 3), LF and LF/HF were significantly increased by the interaction (LF: coefficient 0.36, 95% CI 0.12, 0.61, p=0.005; LF/HF: coefficient 0.12, 95% CI 0.05, 0.19, p=0.002). A similar trend was observed irrespective of the sex of the child. Regarding changes in children, HF significantly decreased by 0.29 (95% CI -0.57, -0.01, p=0.040) while LF/HF significantly increased by 0.05 (95% CI 0.01, 0.08, p=0.008). The decrease in HF was more evident in girls (girls: coefficient -0.34, 95% CI -0.67, -0.01, p=0.045; boys; coefficient -0.20, 95% CI -0.84, 0.45, p=0.497). Root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) and standard deviation of normal to normal R-R interval (SDNN) did not change following playful mother–child interaction for both mothers and children.
Association Between Maternal Oxytocin Response After Mother–Child Interaction and Behavior Problems 2 Years Later
Finally, we assessed whether change in maternal oxytocin level following playful mother–child interaction could predict later child behavior problems (Table 4). We found that the increase of change in maternal oxytocin level by playful mother–child interaction was significantly associated with lower CBCL externalizing problem scores 2 years later (coefficient: -1.56; 95% CI -2.71, -0.41; p=0.014), after adjustment for other covariates. The increase in maternal oxytocin level was also associated with lower CBCL total and internalizing problem scores, although not reaching statistical significance.
Table 4 Association between maternal change in oxytocin level following mother–child interaction in 2015 and the CBCL score in 2 years in 2017 (n=14a).
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study that examined oxytocin responses following playful mother–child interaction among survivors of natural disasters. We detected a significant increase in maternal oxytocin level after the interaction, especially among mothers with a first-born boy; however, the maternal oxytocin response was blunted by maternal psychological distress and trauma due to the earthquake. We also found that the mother–child interaction was associated with increased sympathetic activity relative to parasympathetic activity among mothers and children. The increase in maternal oxytocin level was also associated with lower CBCL externalizing problem score 2 years later.
Prior studies found that parental interaction with infants aged 4–6 months old increased oxytocin level in the parents (17), and in mothers who provided high levels of affectionate interaction with their infants, although the increase was not detected in mothers who provided a low level of affectionate interaction (40). In Japan, a first-born boy is often given better treatment compared with his younger siblings, especially in the northeastern region of Japan where Tohoku is located (41, 42). A higher status is also given to the first-born son because he will be the main heir of the family and is responsible for looking after elderly parents (41). Although we did not assess the norm of each family, the level of interaction provided by the mother may vary according to the sex and the birth order of the child, leading to the different maternal oxytocin response levels after the interaction. Future research may need to evaluate the level of affection during the interaction.
We also assessed how maternal psychological distress and trauma from the earthquake could affect oxytocin response following playful mother–child interaction. We found that maternal oxytocin response was blunted among mothers who reported psychological distress. This is consistent with previous studies that showed that the oxytocin system can be disrupted by psychological distress (43, 44). Moreover, a blunted oxytocin response due to trauma was observed even among mothers who did not report psychological distress. Although inconclusive, previous studies have reported that the oxytocin system can be disrupted by past traumatic experience (16, 45). Current findings add to the literature that having either past traumatic experience or psychological distress may have deteriorating effects on the oxytocin system. These results suggest that assessing oxytocin level among disaster survivors along with their psychological distress and trauma could be important for monitoring the effectiveness of parenting programs.
In our analysis, there were no significant changes in child oxytocin level after playful mother–child interaction. Feldman et al. reported that parental interaction with their infants increased the oxytocin level of the infants (17). A prior study reported that oxytocin response following mother–child interaction was blunted in children who experienced neglect and lived in orphanages immediately after birth (46), and among children of depressed mothers (47). In a study with children aged 7–16 with primary DSM-5 anxiety disorders, oxytocin levels did not change significantly after mother–child interaction (27). Since the children in our study were all exposed to the earthquake, the traumatic exposure could be one reason for the blunted oxytocin changes. Furthermore, the blunted response may also be explained by the difference of age range of the children in our study (8.8 years old on average), which was older than those in previous studies [e.g., infants (17, 40), or toddlers/preschoolers (28, 46, 48)]. Children in our study may have felt embarrassed to take part in playful parenting interaction, which may have inhibited the predicted boost to oxytocin level. In addition, the timing of measuring oxytocin level may have been too long for children of this age range in our study and the oxytocin level may have returned to its normal level. Future studies are warranted to examine whether the reason for the blunted oxytocin response is a consequence of traumatic exposure, age, experimental settings, or other factors. Future studies should include a control group of children who were not exposed to the earthquake, assess oxytocin level over several time points, and implement this intervention in a more natural setting such as their own home.
We assessed the effect of playful mother–child interaction on HRV, using mothers and children as a surrogate for psychophysiological effects in the short term (31). Maternal LF and LF/HF were significantly increased by the interactions, while no significant changes were detected in maternal HF. Regarding changes in children, HF was significantly decreased while LF/HF was significantly increased. The decrease in HF was more evident in girls. LF is a marker for a combination of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity, HF for parasympathetic activity, and LF/HF for a combination of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity (31, 49). Thus, the change that was observed among mothers and children can be interpreted as sympathetic activity being activated, relative to parasympathetic activity, by the playful mother–child interaction. While numerous studies have shown that intranasal administration of oxytocin can activate parasympathetic activity in human adults (50, 51), Tracy et al. recently reported that administration of oxytocin may also reduce parasympathetic regulation in people under cognitive stress (52). Our study adds to the literature that playful mother–child interaction accompanied with a natural increase of oxytocin level is associated with changes in the autonomic nervous system among mothers and children traumatized by natural disasters. Future studies may be needed to examine whether oxytocin itself or other factors could mediate the effect of the interaction on the changes in HRV that were observed in our study.
We also examined the effect of maternal oxytocin changes before and after the playful interaction on the onset of child behavior problems using CBCL score 2 years later. The analysis revealed that the increase in maternal oxytocin level associated with the interaction was significantly associated with lower CBCL externalizing problem scores 2 years later, after adjustment for other covariates. To interpret the findings, it is possible that the increase of maternal oxytocin level associated with the interaction may have increased the quality of mother–child interaction in daily life. Other studies have also suggested that oxytocin administration to mothers may improve parenting, especially responding to infant behavior cues (53). An alternative interpretation is that the change of maternal oxytocin level associated with the interaction can be interpreted as a proxy measurement of the functional level of the pre-existing oxytocin system that was determined by oxytocin-related genes. Previous studies have found that maternal oxytocin level may be determined by CD38 and oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms (e.g., OXTR rs53576), which are associated with child behavior problems (54, 55). Nonetheless, we confirmed that only 10 min of playful mother–child interaction increased maternal oxytocin level, which was associated with a decrease in child externalizing problem scores. Our result provides rationale for future larger-scale studies to evaluate the effect of an intervention using playful mother–child interaction to prevent child behavior problems.
To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies have examined the impact of secretion of oxytocin by playful mother–child interaction on later child behavior problems. Since stimulating natural secretion of oxytocin by playful mother–child interaction would be more accessible and economical than intranasal administration of oxytocin, our results suggest a potential preventive intervention for child behavior problems after natural disaster. Further studies are warranted to investigate whether the effect of stimulating natural secretion of oxytocin by playful mother–child interaction varies by frequency and duration of intervention; the severity, duration, and frequency of the trauma; and by the sex of the parents and children in the session.
Our study has several limitations. First, the mothers and children who participated in the study were not a representative sample of the area that was affected by the earthquake, and we could not follow-up all the participants. Mother–child dyads who were severely affected by the disaster may not have participated because they were not able to, while mother–child dyads who experienced minor disruption and damage may not have been interested in participating. Second, because the CBCL was filled out by parents, child behavior problems that were unknown to parents were not be captured (e.g., behavior problems at school). Third, measuring oxytocin level 5 min after the interaction may have been too long for children in our study. The oxytocin level may have returned to its baseline level. Future studies are needed to assess oxytocin level over several time points in both mothers and children. Such studies should also include a control group of children who were not exposed to the earthquake. Fourth, we were not able to assess the effect of father–child interaction. Fifth, sample size for the analysis that assessed the association between maternal oxytocin changes and child behavior problems was small (n=14). Finally, we did not assess parenting style and oxytocin level before the earthquake, thus the response to playful mother–child interaction may be determined by parenting style or oxytocin levels before the earthquake. Nonetheless, the results that are presented in this study shed light on the prevention of child behavior problems after natural disaster.
In conclusion, we found that playful mother–child interaction increased oxytocin level in mothers but not in children among disaster survivors. We also found that maternal oxytocin response could vary depending on maternal and child characteristics. To clarify the reason for the blunted oxytocin response in children in this study, future studies are needed to assess oxytocin level over several time points in both mothers and children, and should include a control group. Finally, playful mother–child interaction could affect the autonomic nervous system in mothers and children, and the change in oxytocin was related to CBCL externalizing problem score 2 years later. Our results suggest a potential preventive intervention for child behavior problems through playful mother–child interaction after a natural disaster.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the research ethics committees at the National Center for Child Health and Development (584), the Ethics Committee at Tokyo Medical and Dental University (M2016-148). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s), and minor(s)' legal guardian/next of kin, for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author Contributions
NN contributed analyses and interpretation of the data, and drafted, reviewed and revised the manuscript. KN contributed sample processing and oxytocin measurements, reviewed and revised the manuscript. TF contributed conceptualization and the design of the study, interpretation of the data, and revisions to the draft and final versions of the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Funding
This study was supported by a grant from the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare (H24-jisedai-shitei-007) and Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (19H04879).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank all the participants who contributed to this study. We also thank the child psychiatrists and psychologists who provided extra mental health support to participants when requested during interviews. In addition, we thank the research assistants, Ms. Maiko Osawa, Ms. Hisayo Saeki, Ms. Rie Mizuki, Naho Morisaki, Tsuguhiko Kato, Takahiro Miki, Airi Amemiya, Sakurako Okuzono, and Aya Isumi, who coordinated the logistics for this study and helped to collect data.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00477/full#supplementary-material
Supplementary Table 1 | Mean heart rate variability of mothers and children in pre-interaction period and post-interaction period.
References
1. Hoyos CD, Agudelo PA, Webster PJ, Curry JA. Deconvolution of the factors contributing to the increase in global hurricane intensity. Science (2006) 312(5770):94–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1123560
2. Morgan O, Ahern M, Cairncross S. Revisiting the tsunami: health consequences of flooding. PloS Med (2005) 2(6):e184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020184
3. Adikari Y, Yoshitani J. (2009). Global trends in water-related disasters: an insight for policymakers. World Water Assessment Programme Side Publication Series, Insights. The United Nations, UNESCO. International Centre for Water Hazard and Risk Management (ICHARM).
4. Sharma S, Mujumdar P. Increasing frequency and spatial extent of concurrent meteorological droughts and heatwaves in India. Sci Rep (2017) 7(1):15582. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15896-3
5. Lim YK, Schubert SD, Kovach R, Molod AM, Pawson S. The Roles of Climate Change and Climate Variability in the 2017 Atlantic Hurricane Season. Sci Rep (2018) 8(1):16172. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34343-5
6. Wickrama KA, Wickrama KA. Family context of mental health risk in Tsunami affected mothers: findings from a pilot study in Sri Lanka. Soc Sci Med (2008) 66(4):994–1007. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.012
7. Wickrama KA, Kaspar V. Family context of mental health risk in Tsunami-exposed adolescents: findings from a pilot study in Sri Lanka. Soc Sci Med (2007) 64(3):713–23. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.09.031
8. Masten AS, Narayan AJ. Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: pathways of risk and resilience. Annu Rev Psychol (2012) 63:227–57. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100356
9. Pfefferbaum B, Sweeton JL, Newman E, Varma V, Nitiema P, Shaw JA, et al. Child disaster mental health interventions, part I: Techniques, outcomes, and methodological considerations. Disaster Health (2014) 2(1):46–57. doi: 10.4161/dish.27534
10. Dyregrov A, Yule W, Olff M. Children and natural disasters. Eur J Psychotraumatol (2018) 9(Suppl 2):1500823. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2018.1500823
11. Pfefferbaum B, Sweeton JL, Newman E, Varma V, Noffsinger MA, Shaw JA, et al. Child disaster mental health interventions, part II: Timing of implementation, delivery settings and providers, and therapeutic approaches. Disaster Health (2014) 2(1):58–67. doi: 10.4161/dish.27535
12. Scigliano M, Roncaglione V, Madrid PA. Developing a Comprehensive Trauma- and Resiliency- Focused Program After Superstorm Sandy in New York City. Disaster Med Public Health Prep (2019) 13(3):613–7. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2018.118
13. Tanaka E, Iso H, Tsutsumi A, Kameoka S, You Y, Kato H. School-based psychoeducation and storytelling: Associations with long-term mental health in adolescent survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci (2019) 29:e65. doi: 10.1017/S2045796019000611
14. Sriskandarajah V, Neuner F, Catani C. Parental care protects traumatized Sri Lankan children from internalizing behavior problems. BMC Psychiatry (2015) 15:203. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0583-x
15. Kelley ML, Self-Brown S, Le B, Bosson JV, Hernandez BC, Gordon AT. Predicting posttraumatic stress symptoms in children following Hurricane Katrina: a prospective analysis of the effect of parental distress and parenting practices. J Trauma Stress (2010) 23(5):582–90. doi: 10.1002/jts.20573
16. Feldman R, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Oxytocin: a parenting hormone. Curr Opin Psychol (2017) 15:13–8. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.011
17. Feldman R, Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O. The cross-generation transmission of oxytocin in humans. Horm Behav (2010) 58(4):669–76. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.005
18. Feldman R. The neurobiology of mammalian parenting and the biosocial context of human caregiving. Horm Behav (2016) 77:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.001
19. Pedersen CA, Prange AJ Jr. Induction of maternal behavior in virgin rats after intracerebroventricular administration of oxytocin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1979) 76(12):6661–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.76.12.6661
20. Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, et al. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science (2015) 348(6232):333–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1261022
21. Mah BL, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IMH, Smith R. Oxytocin promotes protective behavior in depressed mothers: a pilot study with the enthusiastic stranger paradigm. Depress Anxiety (2015) 32(2):76–81. doi: 10.1002/da.22245
22. Kizuki M, Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T, Fujiwara T. Parental Time of Returning Home From Work and Child Mental Health Among First-Year Primary School Students in Japan: Result From A-CHILD Study. Front Pediatr (2018) 6:179. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00179
23. Matsuyama Y, Fujiwara T, Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T. Self-control and dental caries among elementary school children in Japan. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol (2018) 46(5):465–71. doi: 10.1111/cdoe.12387
24. Fujiwara T, Yagi J, Homma H, Mashiko H, Nagao K, Okuyama M, et al. Clinically significant behavior problems among young children 2 years after the Great East Japan Earthquake. PloS One (2014) 9(10):e109342. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109342
25. Fujiwara T, Mizuki R, Miki T, Chemtob C. Association between facial expression and PTSD symptoms among young children exposed to the Great East Japan Earthquake: a pilot study. Front Psychol (2015) 6:1534. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01534
26. Grewen KM, Girdler SS, Amico J, Light KC. Effects of partner support on resting oxytocin, cortisol, norepinephrine, and blood pressure before and after warm partner contact. Psychosom Med (2005) 67(4):531–8. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000170341.88395.47
27. Lebowitz ER, Silverman WK, Martino AM, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R, Leckman JF. Oxytocin response to youth-mother interactions in clinically anxious youth is associated with separation anxiety and dyadic behavior. Depress Anxiety (2017) 34(2):127–36. doi: 10.1002/da.22585
28. Feldman R, Golan O, Hirschler-Guttenberg Y, Ostfeld-Etzion S, Zagoory-Sharon O. Parent-child interaction and oxytocin production in pre-schoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry (2014) 205(2):107–12. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.137513
29. Saito I, Maruyama K, Eguchi E, Kato T, Kawamura R, Takata Y, et al. Low Heart Rate Variability and Sympathetic Dominance Modifies the Association Between Insulin Resistance and Metabolic Syndrome- The Toon Health Study. Circ J (2017) 81(10):1447–53. doi: 10.1253/circj.CJ-17-0192
30. Nagasawa M, Ogawa M, Mogi K, Kikusui T. Intranasal Oxytocin Treatment Increases Eye-Gaze Behavior toward the Owner in Ancient Japanese Dog Breeds. Front Psychol (2017) 8:1624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01624
31. Laborde S, Mosley E, Thayer JF. Heart Rate Variability and Cardiac Vagal Tone in Psychophysiological Research - Recommendations for Experiment Planning, Data Analysis, and Data Reporting. Front Psychol (2017) 8:213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213
32. Furukawa TA, Kawakami N, Saitoh M, Ono Y, Nakane Y, Nakamura Y, et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res (2008) 17(3):152–8. doi: 10.1002/mpr.257
33. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SL, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med (2002) 32(6):959–76. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074
35. Miki T, Fujiwara T, Yagi J, Homma H, Mashiko H, Nagao K, et al. Impact of Parenting Style on Clinically Significant Behavioral Problems Among Children Aged 4-11 Years Old After Disaster: A Follow-Up Study of the Great East Japan Earthquake. Front Psychiatry (2019) 10:45. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00045
36. Fujiwara T, Weisman O, Ochi M, Shirai K, Matsumoto K, Noguchi E, et al. Genetic and peripheral markers of the oxytocin system and parental care jointly support the cross-generational transmission of bonding across three generations. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2019) 102:172–81. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.12.004
37. Prevatt FF. The contribution of parenting practices in a risk and resiliency model of children's adjustment. Br J Dev Psychol (2003) 21(4):469–80. doi: 10.1348/026151003322535174
38. Achenbach TM. Manual for Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Dept. of Psychiatry (1991).
39. Itani T, Kanbayashi Y, Nakata Y, Kita M, Fuji H, Kuramoto H, et al. Standardization of the Japanese version of the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18. Psychiatr Neurol Pediatr Jpn (2001) 41:243–52. (in Japanese with English abstract).
40. Feldman R, Gordon I, Schneiderman I, Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O. Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2010) 35(8):1133–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.013
41. Fukutake T. Japanese Rural Society. Trans. Dore RP, editor. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press (1967).
43. Apter-Levy Y, Feldman M, Vakart A, Ebstein RP, Feldman R. Impact of maternal depression across the first 6 years of life on the child's mental health, social engagement, and empathy: The moderating role of oxytocin. Am J Psychiatry (2013) 170(10):1161–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12121597
44. Kim S, Soeken TA, Cromer SJ, Martinez SR, Hardy LR, Strathearn L. Oxytocin and postpartum depression: delivering on what's known and what's not. Brain Res (2014) 1580:219–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.11.009
45. Donadon MF, Martin-Santos R, Osorio FL. The Associations Between Oxytocin and Trauma in Humans: A Systematic Review. Front Pharmacol (2018) 9:154. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00154
46. Wismer Fries AB, Ziegler TE, Kurian JR, Jacoris S, Pollak SD. Early experience in humans is associated with changes in neuropeptides critical for regulating social behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102(47):17237–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504767102
47. Pratt M, Apter-Levi Y, Vakart A, Feldman M, Fishman R, Feldman T, et al. Maternal Depression and Child Oxytocin Response; Moderation by Maternal Oxytocin and Relational Behavior. Depress Anxiety (2015) 32(9):635–46. doi: 10.1002/da.22392
48. Bick J, Dozier M. Mothers' and Children's Concentrations of Oxytocin Following Close, Physical Interactions with Biological and Non-biological Children. Dev Psychobiol (2010) 52(1):100–7. doi: 10.1002/dev.20411
49. Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Felmingham KL, Matthews S, Jelinek HF. Depression, comorbid anxiety disorders, and heart rate variability in physically healthy, unmedicated patients: implications for cardiovascular risk. PloS One (2012) 7(2):e30777. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030777
50. Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Kuhnert RL, Griffiths K, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Oxytocin increases heart rate variability in humans at rest: implications for social approach-related motivation and capacity for social engagement. PloS One (2012) 7(8):e44014. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044014
51. Norman GJ, Cacioppo JT, Morris JS, Malarkey WB, Berntson GG, Devries AC. Oxytocin increases autonomic cardiac control: moderation by loneliness. Biol Psychol (2011) 86(3):174–80. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.11.006
52. Tracy LM, Gibson SJ, Labuschagne I, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Giummarra MJ. Intranasal oxytocin reduces heart rate variability during a mental arithmetic task: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled cross-over study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2018) 81:408–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.08.016
53. Riem MM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Pieper S, Tops M, Boksem MA, Vermeiren RR, et al. Oxytocin modulates amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus responses to infant crying: a randomized controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 70(3):291–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.006
54. Smearman EL, Winiarski DA, Brennan PA, Najman J, Johnson KC. Social stress and the oxytocin receptor gene interact to predict antisocial behavior in an at-risk cohort. Dev Psychopathol (2015) 27(1):309–18. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000649
Keywords: oxytocin, the Great East Japan Earthquake, parenting, child behavior problems, heart rate variability
Citation: Nawa N, Nakamura K and Fujiwara T (2020) Oxytocin Response Following Playful Mother–Child Interaction in Survivors of the Great East Japan Earthquake. Front. Psychiatry 11:477. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00477
Received: 06 February 2020; Accepted: 11 May 2020;
Published: 03 June 2020.
Edited by:
David Cohen, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, FranceReviewed by:
Alexis Revet, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Toulouse, FranceNadège Bourvis, Assistance Publique Hopitaux De Paris, France
Copyright © 2020 Nawa, Nakamura and Fujiwara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Takeo Fujiwara, fujiwara.hlth@tmd.ac.jp