Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 22 January 2020
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Current MEG Research in Psychiatry View all 11 articles

Decreased Task-Related HRV Is Associated With Inhibitory Dysfunction Through Functional Inter-Region Connectivity of PFC in Major Depressive Disorder

Hongliang ZhouHongliang Zhou1Zongpeng DaiZongpeng Dai2Lingling HuaLingling Hua1Haiteng JiangHaiteng Jiang2Shui TianShui Tian2Yinglin HanYinglin Han1Pinhua LinPinhua Lin1Haofei WangHaofei Wang1Qing Lu,*Qing Lu2,3*Zhjjian Yao,*Zhjjian Yao1,4*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, China
  • 2School of Biological Sciences & Medical Engineering, Southeast University, Nanjing, China
  • 3Child Development and Learning Science, Key Laboratory of Ministry of Education, Nanjing, China
  • 4Nanjing Brain Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China

The regulation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) can improve cognitive function in major depressive disorders (MDD). Heart rate variability (HRV) derives from the dynamic control of the ANS and reflects the balance between the activities of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems by measuring tiny changes in adjacent heart beats. Task-related HRV may reflect the association between the flexibility of cognition and ANS function. The study was to investigate the neural mechanism of interactions between ANS and cognitive function in MDD with Magnetoencephalography (MEG) measurements. Participants included 20 MDD patients and 18 healthy controls (HCs). All participants were measured with a go/no-go task MEG. HRV indices, the standard deviation of the average normal-to-normal (NN) interval calculated over short periods (SDANN) and the square root of the mean squared differences of successive NN intervals (RMSSD), were derived from the raw MEG data. Results showed that MDD patients showed decreased SDANN and RMSSD. In MDD patients, both resting-state and task-related RMSSD were related to inhibitory and control dysfunction. In the go/no-go task, many areas in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) are responsible for an individual’s inhibitory function. A brain MEG functional connectivity analysis revealed that there were significant differences in four brain regions within the prefrontal cortex (PFC) between MDD patients and HCs. Task-related RMSSD in HCs were related to the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), while in MDD patients, these values were not related to the above functional connectivity but were related to the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus and insula. However, the resting-state RMSSD value was not related to these significant difference functional connectivity networks in all participants. It concludes that the decreased task-related HRV is associated with inhibitory dysfunction through functional inter-region connectivity in the PFC in MDD, and the task-related HRV can be used as an index of the association between MDD and autonomic dysregulation.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a serious health care problem worldwide, according to the report of illness-induced disability (1). MDD is a more severe state of depressive symptomatology in which patients present multiple depressive symptoms and show significant distress or impaired functioning. Of the many depressive symptoms, cognitive impairments are common in MDD. Cognitive function is an individual’s ability to process information. Cognitive dysfunction is prevalent and is associated with the early onset of depression and with longer depressive episodes in MDD patients; it also has an adverse impact on treatment outcomes, as well as on functional recovery (2). Inhibitory and control function is a critical cognitive function that can be defined as interference control, prepotent response inhibition, and resistance to interference. Prepotent response inhibition is the ability to suppress or withhold a (motor) response, which can be measured by the go/no-go task (3).

Inhibitory and control function includes two levels: a central level, which is related to the cerebral cortex, and a peripheral level, which is mediated by the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (4). At the central level, many studies using neuroimaging methods have reported that in the go/no-go task, many areas in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), such as BA9, BA10, and BA47, are responsible for an individual’s inhibitory function (59). At the peripheral level, the ANS, especially the vagus nerve, plays an important role in an individual’s inhibitory and control function (4). Many studies have shown that the decreased vagus tone is associated with abnormal inhibitory and control function under the emotional stop-signal task or the Stroop test (10, 11).

Magnetoencephalography (MEG) has high temporal and spatial resolution and has been used to investigate the neural mechanism of many cognitive functions (12). Functional connectivity measures how brain regions are temporally coordinated and is employed to probe brain network architecture (13). MEG has been used for investigations of brain cortical activity and functional connectivity in MDD (14). MEG in MDD patients allows inter-regional and intraregional connectivity interactions in brain function to be related to cognition, which can provide mechanistic insights into interactions between the ANS and cognitive function.

Individual ANS functioning relies on a balance between the activities of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems. Although cardiac automaticity is originated in pacemaker tissues, heart rate and rhythm largely depend on the control of ANS (15). By releasing neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine, epinephrine, and norepinephrine, the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems can affect heart beats which lead to the changes of heart rate variability (HRV). HRV derives from the dynamic control of the ANS and reflects the balance between the activities of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems by measuring tiny changes in adjacent heart beats. The measurement of HRV contains time domain methods, frequency domain methods and rhythm pattern analysis. Time domain methods and frequency domain methods are mostly applied to clinical researches. Although frequency domain methods are computed by decomposing waveform of electrocardiogram (ECG)–RR intervals (interbeat interval or RRIs) and are considered to better distinguish the influence of the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous systems, they may be affected by different therapeutic regimes and other factors easily comparing with time domain methods which may also reflect the activities of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems (1618). The standard deviation of the average normal-to-normal (NN) intervals (SDANN), which is calculated over a short period (usually within 5 min), and the square root of the mean squared differences of successive normal sinus intervals (RMSSD) are components of HRV from the time-domain analysis. SDANN represents sympathetic function, and RMSSD represents the flexibility of vagal (parasympathetic) tone (19, 20). It is accepted that HRV is decreased in MDD (2123), and decreased HRV might be associated with abnormal perseveration cognition (24). A recent study proved that yoga could be an add-on treatment for MDD by adjusting HRV (25). In addition, HRV biofeedback (HRVB), which has been extensively applied in the clinic, can improve cognition and other symptoms of MDD (2630). It is worth noting that according to the different experimental conditions, HRV can be measured as the resting-state HRV or task-related HRV. Previous studies that used resting-state HRV investigated the association between the ANS and cognition in MDD, and confirmed that resting-state HRV can be used as a characteristic biological marker for MD (23, 24, 31).

Because task-related HRV may reflect the association between the flexibility of cognition and ANS function, task-related HRV can be used as a sensitive index to reflect inhibitory control function at the peripheral level (32). However, to date, no studies on the association between the ANS and cognition (i.e. inhibitory control function) in MDD, which was shown to be involved in task-related HRV, have been reported. Most importantly, the neural mechanism of interactions between the ANS and inhibitory and control function in MDD remains unclear.

In this study, both resting-state HRV and task-related HRV were used to assess ANS function; a go/no-go task was employed to measure cognitive function (inhibitory and control function), and MEG was used to measure brain cortical activity and functional connectivity. To guarantee cognition along different psychophysiological dimensions, task-related HRV indices were derived from the raw MEG data. The purpose of this study is to 1) uncover the neural mechanism of interactions between the ANS and inhibitory and control function in MDD and 2) confirm whether task-related HRV can be used as an index of the association between MDD and autonomic dysregulation.

Materials and Methods

Time and Setting

The experiment was completed in the Department of Psychiatry, Affiliated Nanjing Brain Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, People’s Republic of China, from January 1, 2017, to July 28, 2019. All experimental procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee on Human Studies, Affiliated Nanjing Brain Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, People’s Republic of China and were conducted depending on the Declaration of Helsinki (Ethical review number: 2016KY12). All patients and healthy controls signed an informed consent form, and they received 20 United States dollars as a reward after finishing the experiment. Considering that some patients could not sign the consent form, legal guardians who could represent these patients were also asked to provide consent for all experimental procedures.

Diagnostic Approaches and Participants

This study included an MDD group and an HC group. The criteria for the MDD group were as follows: 1) only meet the criteria of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth edition (DSM-5) for major depression; 2) age from 18 years old to 65 years old; 3) Hamilton Depression Scale (17-item edition, HAMD) scores ≥ 17; 4) no fewer than two episodes of depression; 5) had not taken medication for the last two weeks; 6) were not treated with electroconvulsive therapy within the last month; 7) no diagnosis of alcohol, drug, or other substance dependency, any kind of head injury, neurological disorder, or systemic disease that might have an effect on the central nervous system; and 8) no contraindications for the MEG measurements. The criteria for the HC group were as follows: 1) did not meet the criteria of any DSM-5 axis I disorder or personality disorders according to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5, Chinese version); 2) age from 18 years old to 65 years old; 3) HAMD (17-item edition) scores ≤ 7; 4) no history of any kind of psychiatric disorder; 5) no diagnosis of alcohol, drug, or other substance dependence; and 6) no diagnosis of any kind of head injury, neurological disorder or systemic disease that might have an effect on the central nervous system. All participants needed to be free of any form of cardiac disease, lung disease, beta-blocker medication, or any other disease that could influence the physiological data, and they were forbidden to drink any coffee or tea on the day of the test session. In addition, to reduce the stress caused by MEG examination, patients were informed the whole process of the research and were asked to fit the environment in advance after 8 min resting.

According to the above criteria, twenty patients with MDD were chosen from thirty-four inpatients, and eighteen healthy persons were chosen from thirty healthy volunteers. These HCs were citizens who lived in Nanjing city, Jiangsu Province, People’s Republic of China. All participants were Chinese.

On the day of the experiment, with the assistance of a psychiatric resident physician, a psychiatric associate chief physician was asked to collect medication information, demographic and clinical characteristics, and confirm/exclude the MDD diagnosis of participants. We used the Annett Handedness Scale to evaluate handedness. The standards of handedness were as follows: Annett score (1) = right, (2–7) = mixed, and (8) = left. All participants were right handed.

MEG Task and Procedure

Go/No-Go Task

The go/no-go task was edited by BrainX software, which was based on DirectX (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA). One hundred eighty stimuli were used in this task, and the total duration of the task was 300 s. Three types of stimuli were used in this task: a red lamp picture, a green lamp picture, and a gray cross. These stimuli were shown serially on a white background against a black background (1.5 × 1.5 cm in size) on a computer screen. The whole task contained both go and no-go trials. In a go trial, the participant was shown a gray cross on a white background that was presented for 2,500 millisecond (ms) followed by a green lamp picture. The green lamp picture remained on the screen for either 150 or 500 ms, decided at random. In a no-go trial, a red lamp picture was presented for 400 ms duration, followed by a gray cross picture and then a green lamp picture. The duration of the gray fixation cross presentation was 2,500 ms and that of the green lamp was 150 ms. After every picture was presented, there was an interval time of 300 ms. The duration of no-go trials accounts for 25% of the entire task. There were 20 go trials and 5 no-go trials in this experiment in the practice phase, and the data in the practice phase were not recorded or analyzed (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 A cartoon illustrating the go/no-go task. ms, millisecond. (A) This schematic representation of a no-go trial begins with the presentation of a gray fixation cross (go stimulus), followed by a green lamp (go stimulus). Then, a red lamp, which indicates a no-go trial, is shown, and participants need to respond. The no-go trials accounted for 25% of the entire session, and every picture was followed by a 300 ms interval. (B) The go trials consisted of a gray fixation cross whose duration was 2,500 ms and a green lamp. The duration of the green lamp was randomly either 150 or 500 ms.

Behavioral Data Measurements

The behavioral data included the accuracy rate (hit rate, i.e. the percentage of correct responses), reaction times (RTs), and false alarms (error rate, i.e. the opposite of the intended action was selected on accident). The accuracy rate and RTs were recorded for go trials, and false alarms were recorded for no-go trials.

MEG Data Acquisition

A 275-channel whole-head CTF MEG system (Omega 2000, VSM Med Tech Inc., Port Coquitlam, Canada) was used for the MEG recordings. The sampling rate of the machine was adjusted to 1,200 Hz. Any metal on participants’ bodies was removed, and people were asked to lay in the supine position in a special magnetically shielded room. With a third-order synthetic gradient, the system could cancel background and noise interference effectively. During MEG measurements, the movement of head motion and localization could be tolerated within 5 millimeter (mm). The T1-weighted imaging data were obtained from Siemens Verio 3.0 Tesla Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scanner (Erlangen, Germany). The T1-weighted axial image parameters were as follows: repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 1,900/2.48 ms, thickness/gap = 1.0/0 mm, field of view (FOV) = 240 × 240 mm2, matrix = 256 × 256 × 192, and voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3. Three energizing coils were set at the nasion, left preauricular, and right preauricular locations to measure the participant’s head location within the scanner and for offline co-registration of MRI and MEG data.

Pre-Processing

The Fieldtrip toolbox (http://www.ru.nl/fcdonders/fieldtrip/) in a MATLAB software environment (http://www.mathworks.com) was used to pre-process MEG data. After removing the frequency band between 49.5 and 50.5 Hz, which indicated big noise that could affect the authenticity of the data, the raw MEG data were separated into three parts: go epochs, no-go epochs and interval epochs. Only correct no-go epochs were used in the MEG analysis. The time range of 200 ms pre-stimulus and 900 ms post-stimulus during no-go epochs was selected. The SPM megheadloc function was applied to screen the data for head motion by removing any epochs with motion greater than 5 mm or when intertrial movement was >10 mm. Then, we used independent component analysis (ICA) to eliminate eye movement and cardiac artifacts. The 1- to 120-Hz frequency band was chosen for analysis.

Source Reconstruction and Functional Connectivity Calculations

The minimum-norm estimates algorithm was used to complete source reconstruction after pre-processing the raw MEG data (33). We used averaged envelope correlation (AEC) to compute the functional connectivity networks within brain regions between MDD patients and HCs under the go/no-go task. The filtered time series gotten after source reconstruction were transformed by Hilbert transform to get the analytic signal, and then the absolute value of the signal was extracted and divided into several parts. The time consistency between each two parts of them was calculated by average correlation value with Pearson correlation analysis. Considering that there had been no similar study carried out before, according to previous studies (34, 35), we chose the prefrontal lobe, which is the area primarily reactive to the go/no-go task. Significantly, the anterior cingulate cortex was specifically chosen for its tight association with HRV. Although there are some doubts surrounding exploring deep brain areas by MEG, considering that the important role of the insula plays in HRV, this region was chosen as an auxiliary brain area for the explanation of our results. The brain areas chosen to compute function connectivity in the no-go epoch are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Brain regions chosen from previous studies.

HRV Data Acquisition and Analysis

To guarantee HRV could reflect task-related state more precisely, ECG record was directly collected from the MEG raw data under the go/no-go task after ICA (36). There were two reasons to choose time domain analysis. Firstly, the bandpass frequency range of MEG data was set from 0 HZ to 120 HZ, which could disturb the indices of frequency domain analysis. Secondly, time domain analysis is not sensitive to the usage of medication other than tricyclic and tetracyclic antidepressants (37), which could minimize the influence of medication. By recording continuous ECG data, each QRS complex was detected with the pan-tompkin function in the MATLAB software environment, and the normal-to-normal (NN) intervals which were all intervals between adjacent QRS complexes resulting from instantaneous heart rate were determined. And HRV components SDANN and RMSSD were selected to be analyzed. The pan-tompkin function in the MATLAB software environment was applied to lock the R wave. Then, the RMSSD and SDANN were measured with the following formulas: SDANN (which represents the activity of sympathetic nervous system) =1Ni1N(RRiR¯R¯)2 and RMSSD (which represents the activity of parasympathetic nervous system) =1N1i=1N-1(RRi+1RRi)2 (38). Resting-state HRV was derived from the recording data of HRV biofeedback software after 8 min resting at the same time (about 2 o’clock in the afternoon) on the next day to reduce the effect of circadian rhythm on experiment. Although the data contain all indices of HRV, to match task-related HRV, only the SDANN and RMSSD values were used for analysis.

Data and Statistical Analysis

Data are shown as the means (standard deviation, SD). Statistical Program for Social Sciences software version 19.0 (SPSS, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) was applied to statistical analysis. Independent-samples t-tests and paired-sample t-tests were applied to compare mean age, education, The Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD, 17-item edition) scores, the Hamilton Anxiety Scale (HAMA) scores, behavioral data, HRV values, and functional connectivity of brain regions between the MDD and the HC groups, and the Pearson chi-square test was used to investigate the gender ratio. A general linear correlation method was used to investigate the relationship between HRV indices and significant difference functional connectivity networks, and HAMD scores used Pearson’s r in the MDD patients and the HCs. To reduce the likelihood of false positives, the false-discovery rate (FDR) correction was used to correct multiple comparisons (39). Alpha values of 0.05 were considered to have significance.

Results

Demographic Characteristics of Participants

As shown in Table 2. There were no significant differences between the demographic characteristics of the participants in the MDD group and the HC group. The HAMA scores were higher in the MDD group than in the HC group; however, there were no significant differences. The HAMD scores of the MDD group were higher than those of the HC group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants.

Behavioral Data Analysis

As shown in Table 3, RTs for go trials in the MDD group were longer than those in the HC group; the hit rate for go trials in the MDD group was lower than that in the HC group, and the rate of false alarms for no-go trials in the MDD group was higher than that in the HC group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Comparisons of behavioral data between MDD and HC group [present as mean (SD)].

HRV Indices Analysis

Comparisons of HRV Indices Between Different States

As shown in Figure 2A, by paired-sample t-test, there were no differences in SDANN values in the resting state and task-related state (in the Go/No-go task) in the HC group or in the MDD group (for HC group, t = 1.661,p = 0.115; for MDD group, t = 1.856, p = 0.079). However, there were significant differences in the RMSSD value in both the HC group and the MDD group (for the HC group, t = 5.667, p = 0.000028; for the MDD group, t = 2.993, p = 0.007); the RMSSD value in both the HC group and the MDD group in the resting state were lower than that in the task-related state.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 (A) Comparing HRV indices in the task-related state or resting state within the HC group or MDD group, there was no difference in the SDANN value, but there was a significant difference in the RMSSD value. (B) Comparing HRV indices in the resting state or the task-related state between the HC and MDD group, there were significant differences in both the RMSSD and SDANN values. T-RMSSD, task-related RMSSD, R-RMSSD, resting-state RMSSD; T-SDANN, task-related SDANN, R-SDANN, resting-state SDANN.

Comparisons of HRV Indices Between the HC Group and the MDD Group

As shown in Figure 2B, by independent sample t-test, there were significant differences in both the SDANN and RMSSD values in the resting state (for SDANN, t = 3.034, p = 0.004; for RMSSD, t = 2.712, p = 0.010) and in the task-related state (for SDANN, t = 3.932, p = 0.000368; for RMSSD, t = 3.965, p = 0.000334) between HCs and MDD patients. Both SDANN and RMSSD values in MDD patients were lower than those in HCs in both the resting state and the task-related state.

Correlation Analysis Between HRV Indices and Behavioral Data

The general linear correlation method was used to investigate the relationship among HRV indices (resting-state SDANN and RMSSD values and task-related SDANN and RMSSD values) and behavioral data (RTs, hit rate, and false alarms). The results showed that only the resting-state RMSSD value and the task-related RMSSD value were correlated with RTs in the MDD group (for resting-state RMSSD: r = −0.476, p = 0.034; for task-related RMSSD: r = −0.522, p = 0.018).

MEG Analysis

As shown in Figure 3, compared to the brain regions in the HC group, there were three regional functional connectivities in the brain that passed FDR correction in the MDD group, including the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and right superior frontal gyrus (42, 60, 2) (t = −3.573, p = 0.001027), the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and left superior frontal gyrus (−48, 28, −16) (t = 3.972, p = 0.000375), and the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and anterior cingulate cortex (4, 26, −14) (t = −3.851, p = 0.000569).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Functional connectivity networks of significant differences. In the no-go trials, the average activity of functional connectivity networks within PFC in the MDD group (20 subjects) was higher than that in the HC group (18 subjects), which means that MDD patients have to activate more connections than HCs to control their behavior.

Correlation Analysis Between the RMSSD Value and Significant Difference Functional Connectivity Networks

Task-Related RMSSD Value

A general linear correlation method was used to investigate the relationship between task-related HRV indices and the three above mentioned brain regional functional connectivity networks; this analysis showed that in the HC group, the task-related RMSSD value is positively correlated with the functional connectivity between left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and anterior cingulate cortex (4, 26, −14) (r = 0.605, p = 0.008), while in the MDD group, the task-related RMSSD value is not correlated with above functionally connected regions (r = −0.117, p = 0.625), and, instead, the task-related RMSSD value is negatively correlated with the functional connectivity between left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and insula (44, −6, −4) (r = −0.577, p = 0.008). However, we didn’t find correlation between task-related RMSSD and the functional connectivity between left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and left insula (−35.13, 6.65, 3.44), (−32, 16, −16), (−34, 12, −14) (r = −0.17, −0.197, −0.115; p = 0.306, 0.235, 0.317).

Resting-State RMSSD Value

A general linear correlation method was used to investigate the relationship between the resting-state RMSSD value and the three above mentioned regional functional connectivity networks in the brain; this analysis showed that the RMSSD value was not correlated with these significant difference brain regions in either the HC group or the MDD group (for HC group: r = 0.225, 0.173, 0.166; p = 0.369, 0.493, 0.511; for MDD group: r = −0.205, −0.008, −0.99; p = 0.495, 0.855, 0.532).

Correlation Analysis Between the HAMD Value and Significant Difference Functional Connectivity Networks or HRV Indices in the MDD Group

By Pearson correlation analysis, the HAMD scores in the MDD group were positively correlated with the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus (−28, 56, −2) and right superior frontal gyrus (42, 60, 2) (r = 0.516, p = 0.020); the HAMD scores in the MDD group were negatively correlated with HRV indices (for task-related RMSSD: r = −0.555, p = 0.011; for task-related SDANN: r = −0.559, p = 0.010; for resting-state RMSSD: r = −0.497, p = 0.026).

Discussion

This study is the first to employ brain MEG functional connectivity analysis with a go/no-go task, by combining HRV indices, to uncover the neural mechanism of interactions between the ANS and inhibitory and control function in MDD patients. Additionally, this study is the first to confirm that task-related HRV can be used as an index of the association between MDD and autonomic dysregulation.

ANS, which is generally regarded made of sympathetic system and parasympathetic system, is one of the most important factors that are associated with a wide range of somatic and mental disease. Although pacemaker tissues play an important role in cardiac automaticity, heart rate, and rhythm largely depended on the neurotransmitters (acetylcholine, epinephrine, and norepinephrine) secreted by sympathetic nervous system and parasympathetic nervous system. HRV, which represent the rhythm of heart, has been used as an indicator of central–peripheral neural feedback (40). SDANN and RMSSD are two important indices in HRV which reflect sympathetic nervous system and parasympathetic nervous system respectively. There are growing studies to discover the relationship between ANS and central nervous system (CNS) based on neurovisceral integration model, and investigators have identified functional units within the CNS (such as PFC) which are related to several cognitive functions such as executive function, memory, attention, etc. (41). Notably, PFC also plays an important role in inhibition and control function, and there are several researches to find vagally mediated HRV is associated with inhibition and control function.

Consistent with previous studies (22, 4244), MDD patients present inhibitory and control dysfunction and decreased resting-state RMSSD values. Compared to the task-related state, all participants displayed lower resting-state HRV values. Additionally, MDD patients showed lower HRV indices (SDANN and RMSSD values) than HCs in both the resting state and the task-related state. Furthermore, in MDD patients, both the resting-state and the task-related RMSSD value were related to inhibitory and control dysfunction. A brain MEG functional connectivity analysis in MDD patients revealed that four brain regions within the PFC had significant difference functional connectivity networks. Meanwhile, task-related RMSSD values in HCs were related to the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), while in MDD patients, this measure was not related to the above mentioned functionally connected regions but was instead related to the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus and insula. However, the resting-state RMSSD value was not related to significant difference functional connectivity networks in all participants.

Our results also support that resting-state HRV can be used as a biomarker of inhibitory and control function in MDD patients. A previous study indicated that task-related HRV may reflect inhibitory and control function at the peripheral level (32). Our results support the theory that the decreased vagus nerve function might be the mechanism of the decreased HRV indices in MDD patients. In addition, comparing HRV indices in the resting state with those during the task (the go/no-go task), the RMSSD value was closely related to the vagus function and, thus, might be a better marker to index impaired inhibitory and control function than that of the SDANN value, which is associated with the sympathetic nervous system in MDD patients. Our results also confirm that the task-related HRV may reflect the association between the flexibility of cognition and ANS function.

The fundamentals of the PFC function are the “inhibition” of the ordinary and stereotyped responses in order to pursue the novelty “execution”. Although PFC plays an important role in so many functions (such as inhibitory, work memory, regulation of visceral feeling, etc.), in this study, the function of inhibitory was highlighted. The PFC plays a critical role in inhibitory and control function, which can be projected to BA8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 44, 45, 46, and 47 Brodmann areas (7, 8). The left middle frontal gyrus (BA10) and right superior frontal gyrus (BA47), seen in this study, belong to the PFC. It is widely recognized that the PFC plays an important role in the regulation of HRV (4447). A previous study has shown that the middle frontal gyrus and superior frontal gyrus are sensitive brain regions related to inhibitory and control function (4851). In addition, a study reported that the right superior frontal gyrus is one of the targets of erythropoietin treatment, which can effectively improve the executive function of MDD patients (52). As an important part of the limbic system, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is not only associated with the regulation of inhibitory and control function but also with the regulation of breathing and heart rate. The reduction in dopamine D2 receptors in the ACC is an important pathological mechanism of MDD, and increasing ACC dopamine D2 receptor levels can effectively improve executive function in MDD patients (53). In the go/no-go task, abnormal regional functional connectivity networks in the brain are the neural mechanism of cognitive dysfunction (inhibitory control dysfunction) in MDD. The appearance of “visceral brain” components suggest that decreased HRV indices resulting from the sympathetic-vagus nerve balance disorder are associated with inhibitory and control dysfunction.

Unlike in HCs, HRV indices are not associated with the functional connectivity between the left middle frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex in MDD patients. We suggest that MDD patients have abnormal activation in regional brain functional connectivity, decreasing the regulation of sympathetic-vagus nerve function by the ACC. Namely, this abnormal activation decreases the regulation HRV indices by the ACC; however, to compensate for the decreased regulation of HRV indices by the ACC, as the regulation of the vital center of the body, the insula strengthens the regulation of sympathetic-vagus nerve function. In addition, the functional connectivity networks between insula (no matter in right hemisphere or left hemisphere) and PFC were higher in MDD patients than that in HCs, which supported our idea that the functional connectivity between insula and PFC could compensate for abnormal functional connectivity networks within PFC. However, it is worth noting that, we only took insula into consideration as an auxiliary brain region to explain our results in that there was still controversy about discovering the function about deep brain regions with MEG.

Limitations

There are some limitations to this study. Firstly, because of the small sample size, our results are preliminary. Secondly, although MEG data integrated with task-related HRV indices has verified that task-related HRV is a characteristic biological marker of MDD in this study, whether task-related HRV can be used as a genuine diagnostic indicator for MDD depends on multidimensional research. Future studies should integrate MEG with other technologies, such as genetic and immunohistochemical techniques, to further confirm the relationship between task-related HRV indices and MDD. Finally, our study only investigated the neural mechanism of interactions between ANS and inhibitory and control function in MDD with MEG measurements. Because inhibitory and control function was only one of neurocognitive functions, other aspects of neurocognitive such as working memory, sustained attention, etc. should be conducted in the future study.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the decreased task-related HRV is associated with inhibitory control dysfunction through functional inter-region connectivity in the PFC in major depressive disorder, and the task-related HRV be used as an index of the association between MDD and autonomic dysregulation.

Our findings have important implications for understanding the pathophysiology of MDD and task-related HRV could be used to predict abnormal inhibitory and control function in MDD patients. In addition, integrated with task-related HRV, MEG may provide an image-guided tool for intervention.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee on Human Studies, Affiliated Nanjing Brain Hospital of Nanjing Medical University, Nanjing, People’s Republic of China. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

HZ: data curation, formal analysis, methodology, writing original draft, writing review and editing. LH, HJ, ZD, ST, YH, PL, HW: data curation, formal analysis, methodology. QL, ZY: data curation, formal analysis, methodology, writing review and editing.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China [grant number 81871066 and 81571639]; the Jiangsu Provincial Medical Innovation Team of the Project of Invigorating Health Care Through Science, Technology and Education [grant number CXTDC2016004]; and the Jiangsu Provincial Key Research and Development Program [grant number BE2018609]. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the editors and reviewers of this manuscript for the many helpful suggestions.

References

1. Ferrari AJ, Somerville AJ, Baxter AJ, Norman R, Patten SB, Vos T, et al. Global variation in the prevalence and incidence of major depressive disorder: a systematic review of the epidemiological literature. Psychol Med (2013) 43:471–81. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Papakostas GI. Cognitive symptoms in patients with major depressive disorder and their implications for clinical practice. J Clin Psychiatry (2014) 75:8–14. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13r08710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Wesse JR. Prepotent motor activity and inhibitory control demands in different variants of the go/no-go paradigm. Psychophysiology (2018) 55(3):e12871. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ottaviani C. Brain-heart interaction in perseverative cognition. Psychophysiology (2018) 55:e13082. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Fuster JM, Bressler SL. Past makes future: role of PFC in prediction. Cogn Neurosci (2015) 27:639–54. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Barbas H, García-Cabezas MÁ. How the prefrontal executive got its stripes. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2016) 40:125–34. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Barbier EL, Marrett S, Danek A, Vortmeyer A, van Gelderen P, Duyn J, et al. Imaging cortical anatomy by high-resolution MR at 3.0T: detection of the stripe of Gennari in visual area 17. Magn Reson Med (2002) 48:735–8. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zilles K, Amunts K. Centenary of Brodmann’s map mdash conception and fate. Nat Rev Neurosci (2010) 11:139–45. doi: 10.1038/nrn2776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Criaud M, Boulinguez P. Have we been asking the right questions when assessing response inhibition in go/no-go tasks with fMRI? A meta-analysis and critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:11–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Krypotos AM, Jahfari S, van Ast VA, Kindt M, Forstmann BU. Individual differences in heart rate variability predict the degree of slowing during response inhibition and initiation in the presence of emotional stimuli. Front Psychol (2011) 2:278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Robinson LJ, Gallagher P, Watson S, Pearce R, Finkelmeyer A, Maclachlan L, et al. Impairments in cognitive performance in chronic fatigue syndrome are common, not related to co-morbid depression but do associate with autonomic dysfunction. PloS One (2019) 14:e0210394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Alamian G, Hincapié AS, Pascarella A, Thiery CE, Saive AL, et al. Measuring alterations in oscillatory brain networks in schizophrenia with resting state MEG: State-of-the-art and methodological challenges. Clin Neurophysiol (2017) 128:1719–36. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yamashita M, Yoshihara Y, Hashimoto R, Yahata N, Ichikawa N, Sakai Y, et al. A prediction model of working memory across health and psychiatric disease using whole-brain functional connectivity. eLife (2018) 7:e38844. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nugent AC, Robinson SE, Coppola R, Furey ML, Zarate CA Jr. Group differences in megica derived resting state networks: application to major depressive disorder. Neuroimage (2015) 118:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Jalife J, Michaels DC. Neural control of sinoatrial pacemaker activity. In: Levy MN, Schwartz PJ, editors. Vagal Control of The Heart: Experimental Basis And Clinical Implications. Futura: Armonk (1994). p. 173–205.

Google Scholar

16. Yeh TC, Kao LC, Tzeng NS, Kuo TB, Huang SY, Chang CC, et al. Heart rate variability in major depressive disorder and after antidepressant treatment with agomelatine and paroxetine: Findings from the Taiwan Study of Depression and Anxiety (TAISDA). Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2016) 64:60–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Terhardt J, Lederbogen F, Feuerhack A, Hamann-Weber B, Gilles M, Schilling C, et al. Heart rate variability during antidepressant treatment with venlafaxine and mirtazapine. Clin Neuropharmacol (2013) 36:198–202. doi: 10.1097/WNF.0b013e3182a76fbb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Brunoni AR, Kemp AH, Dantas EM, Goulart AC, Nunes MA, Boggio PS, et al. Heart rate variability is a trait marker of major depressive disorder: evidence from the sertraline vs. electric current therapy to treat depression clinical study. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2013) 16:1937–49. doi: 10.1017/S1461145713000497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord (2000) 61:201–16. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Haapaniemi TH, Pursiainen V, Korpelainen JT, Huikuri HV, Sotaniemi KA, Myllyla VV. Ambulatory ECG and analysis of heart rate variability in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2001) 70:305–10. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.70.3.305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Jandackova VK, Britton A, Malik M. Heart rate variability and depressive symptoms: a cross-lagged analysis over a 10-year period in the White hall II study. Psychol Med (2016) 46:2121–31. doi: 10.1017/S003329171600060X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Huang C, Gevirtz RN, Onton J. Investigation of vagal afferent functioning using the heartbeat event related potential. Int J Psychophysiol (2017) 2017:S0167876017301095. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hartmann R, Schmidt FM, Sander C, Hegerl U. Heart rate variability as indicator of clinical state in depression. Front Psychiatry (2018) 9:735. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ottaviani C, Shahabi L, Tarvainen M, Cook I, Abrams M, Shapiro D. Cognitive, behavioral, and autonomic correlates of mind wandering and perseverative cognition in major depression. Front Neurosci (2014) 8:433. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Meister K, Juckel G. A systematic review of mechanisms of change in body-oriented yoga in major depressive disorders. Pharmacopsychiatry (2018) 51:73–81. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-111013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Caldwell YT, Steffen PR. Adding HRV biofeedback to psychotherapy increases heart rate variability and improves the treatment of major depressive disorder. Int J Psychophysiol (2018) 131:96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lin IM, Fan SY, Yen CF, Yeh YC, Tang TC, Huang MF, et al. Heart rate variability biofeedback increased autonomic activation and improved symptoms of depression and insomnia among patients with major depression disorder. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci (2019) 17:222–32. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2019.17.2.222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Pinter A, Szatmari S, Horvath T, Penzlin AI, Barlinn K, Siepmann M, et al. Cardiac dysautonomia in depression—heart rate variability biofeedback as a potential add-on therapy. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat (2019) 15:1287–310. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S200360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Windthorst P, Mazurak N, Kuske M, Hipp Giel KE, Enck P, et al. Heart rate variability biofeedback therapy and graded exercise training in management of chronic fatigue syndrome: An exploratory pilot study. J Psychosom Res (2017) 93:6–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2016.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Jester DJ, Rozek EK. Heart rate variability biofeedback: implications for cognitive and psychiatric effects in older adults. Aging Ment Health (2019) 23:574–80. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2018.1432031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ha JH, Park S, Yoon D, Kim B. Short-term heart rate variability in older patients with newly diagnosed depression. Psychiatry Res (2015) 226:484–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Moses ZB, Luecken LJ, Eason JC. Measuring task-related changes in heart rate variability. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc (2007) 2007:644–7. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2007.4352372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hauk O. Keep it simple: a case for using classical minimum norm estimation in the analysis of EEG and MEG data. Neuroimage (2004) 21:1612–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sakaki M, Yoo HJ, Nga L, Lee TH, Thayer JF, Mather M. Heart rate variability is associated with amygdala functional connectivity with MPFC across younger and older adults. Neuroimage (2016) 139:44–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Wang Q, Tian S, Tang H, Liu X, Yan R, Hua L, et al. Identification of major depressive disorder and prediction of treatment response using functional connectivity between the prefrontal cortices and subgenual anterior cingulate: a real-world study. J effect Disord (2019) 252:365–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.04.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Garg P, Davenport E, Murugesan G, Wagner B, Whitlow C, Maldjian J, et al. Automatic 1d convolutional neural network-based detection of artifacts in MEG acquired without electrooculography or electrocardiography. Int Workshop Pattern Recognit Neuroimaging (2017) 2017:1–10. doi: 10.1109/PRNI.2017.7981506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Udupa K, Thirthalli J, Sathyaprabha TN, Kishore KR, Raju TR, Gangadhar BN. Differential actions of antidepressant treatments on cardiac autonomic alterations in depression: a prospective comparison. Asian J Psychiatr (2011) 4:100–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2011.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Camm AJ, Malik M, Bigger JT, Breithardt G, Cerutti S, Cohen RJ. Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. task force of the European society of cardiology and the North American society of pacing and electrophysiology. Eur Heart J (1996) 17:354–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1542-474X.1996.tb00275.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Storey JD. The positive false discovery rate: a bayesian interpretation and the q-value. Ann Stat (2003) 31:2013–35. doi: 10.1214/aos/1074290335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2009) 33:81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Forte G, Favieri F. Heart rate variability and cognitive function: a systematic review. Front Neurosci (2019) 13:710. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Erickson K, Drevets WC, Clark L, Cannon DM, Bain EE, CA Zarate Jr, et al. Mood-congruent bias in affective go/no-go performance of unmedicated patients with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry (2005) 162:2171–3. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bermpohl F, Fregni F, Boggio PS, Thut G, Northoff G, Otachi PT, et al. Effect of low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on an affective go/no-go task in patients with major depression: role of stimulation site and depression severity. Psychiatry Res (2006) 141:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Huang M, Frantz J, Moralez G, Sabo T, Davis PF, Davis SL, et al. Reduced resting and increased elevation of heart rate variability with cognitive task performance in concussed athletes. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2019) 34:45–51. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Chang WH, Lee IH, Chi MH, Lin SH, Chen KC, Chen PS, et al. Prefrontal cortex modulates the correlations between brain-derived neurotrophic factor level, serotonin, and the autonomic nervous system. Sci Rep (2018) 8:2558. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20923-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Silva WQAD, Fontes EB, Forti RM, Lima ZL, Elsangedy HM. Affect during incremental exercise: the role of inhibitory cognition, autonomic cardiac function, and cerebral oxygenation. PloS One (2017) 12:e0186926. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Wendt J, Neubert J, Koenig J, Thayer JF, Hamm AO. Resting heart rate variability is associated with inhibition of conditioned fear. Psychophysiology (2015) 52:1161–6. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lavagnino L, Mwangi B, Bauer IE, Cao B, Selvaraj S, Prossin A, et al. Reduced inhibitory control mediates the relationship between cortical thickness in the right superior frontal gyrus and body mass index. Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41:2275–82. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.26

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kleerekooper I, Van Rooij SH, Van d WWPM, de Leeuw M, Kahn RS, Vink M. The effect of aging on fronto-striatal reactive and proactive inhibitory control. NeuroImage (2016) 132:51–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Faul L, Fogleman ND, Mattingly KM, Depue BE. Inhibitory control mediates a negative relationship between body mass index and intelligence: a neurocognitive investigation. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci (2019) 19:392–408. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00695-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Xu H, Liu L, Tian Y, Wang J, Li J, Zheng J, et al. A disinhibitory microcircuit mediates conditioned social fear in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron (2019) 102:668–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Miskowiak KW, Vinberg M, Glerup L, Paulson O, Knudsen GM, Ehrenreich H, et al. Neural correlates of improved executive function following erythropoietin treatment in mood disorders. Psychol Med (2016) 46:1679–91. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Larisch R, Klimke A, Vosberg H, Löffler S, Gaebel W, Müller-Gärtner HW. In Vivo evidence for the involvement of Dopamine-D2 receptors in striatum and anterior cingulate gyrus in major depression. Neuroimage (1997) 5:251–60. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: major depressive disorders, magnetoencephalography, task-related heart rate variability, functional connectivity, go/no-go task

Citation: Zhou H, Dai Z, Hua L, Jiang H, Tian S, Han Y, Lin P, Wang H, Lu Q and Yao Z (2020) Decreased Task-Related HRV Is Associated With Inhibitory Dysfunction Through Functional Inter-Region Connectivity of PFC in Major Depressive Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 10:989. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00989

Received: 28 October 2019; Accepted: 12 December 2019;
Published: 22 January 2020.

Edited by:

Yoji Hirano, Kyushu University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Yutaka Kato, Tsutsuji Mental Hospital, Japan
Murat İlhan Atagun, Yıldırım Beyazıt University, Turkey

Copyright © 2020 Zhou, Dai, Hua, Jiang, Tian, Han, Lin, Wang, Lu and Yao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Qing Lu, bHVxQHNldS5lZHUuY24=; Zhjjian Yao, emp5YW9AbmptdS5lZHUuY24=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.