Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 September 2019
Sec. Schizophrenia

White Noise Speech Illusions: A Trait-Dependent Risk Marker for Psychotic Disorder?

Elaine Schepers*Elaine Schepers1*Richel LousbergRichel Lousberg1Sinan Guloksuz,Sinan Guloksuz1,2Lotta-Katrin PriesLotta-Katrin Pries1Philippe DelespaulPhilippe Delespaul1Gunter KenisGunter Kenis1Jurjen J. Luykx,,Jurjen J. Luykx3,4,5Bochao D. LinBochao D. Lin4Alexander L. RichardsAlexander L. Richards6Berna AkdedeBerna Akdede7Tolga BinbayTolga Binbay7Vesile Alt&#x;nyazarVesile Altınyazar8Berna Yal&#x;netinBerna Yalınçetin9Güvem Gümü-AkayGüvem Gümüş-Akay10Burin CihanBurçin Cihan11Haldun SoygürHaldun Soygür12Halis UlaHalis Ulaş7Eylem ahin CankurtaranEylem Şahin Cankurtaran13Semra Ulusoy KaymakSemra Ulusoy Kaymak14Marina M. Mihaljevic,Marina M. Mihaljevic15,16Sanja Andric PetrovicSanja Andric Petrovic16Tijana MirjanicTijana Mirjanic17Miguel Bernardo,,Miguel Bernardo18,19,20Bibiana Cabrera,Bibiana Cabrera18,20Julio Bobes,,,Julio Bobes20,21,22,23Pilar A. Saiz,,,Pilar A. Saiz20,21,22,23María Paz García-Portilla,,,María Paz García-Portilla20,21,22,23Julio Sanjuan,Julio Sanjuan20,24Eduardo J. Aguilar,Eduardo J. Aguilar20,24Jos Luis Santos,José Luis Santos20,25Estela Jimnez-Lpez,Estela Jiménez-López20,26Manuel ArrojoManuel Arrojo27Angel CarracedoAngel Carracedo28Gonzalo Lpez,Gonzalo López20,29Javier Gonzlez-Peas,Javier González-Peñas20,29Mara Parellada,Mara Parellada20,29Nadja P. Maric,Nadja P. Maric15,16Cem AtbaoluCem Atbaşoğlu30Alp UcokAlp Ucok31Kksal AlptekinKöksal Alptekin7Meram Can SakaMeram Can Saka30Celso Arango,Celso Arango20,29Bart P.F. RuttenBart P.F. Rutten1Jim van Os,,Jim van Os1,3,32
  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Translational Neuroscience, UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 5GGNet Mental Health, Apeldoorn, Netherlands
  • 6MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • 7Department of Psychiatry, Dokuz Eylül University School of Medicine, Izmir, Turkey
  • 8Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Adnan Menderes University, Aydin, Turkey
  • 9Department of Neuroscience, Health Sciences Institute, Dokuz Eylül University, Izmir, Turkey
  • 10Ankara University Brain Research Center, Ankara, Turkey
  • 11Department of Psychology, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey
  • 12Turkish Federation of Schizophrenia Associations, Ankara, Turkey
  • 13Güven Çayyolu Healthcare Campus, Ankara, Turkey
  • 14Atatürk Research and Training Hospital Psychiatry Clinic, Ankara, Turkey
  • 15Faculty of Medicine, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia
  • 16Clinic for Psychiatry CCS, Belgrade, Serbia
  • 17Special Hospital for Psychiatric Disorders Kovin, Kovin, Serbia
  • 18Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 19Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi I Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain
  • 20Biomedical Research Networking Centre in Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain
  • 21Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain
  • 22Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria del Principado de Asturias, Oviedo, Spain
  • 23Mental Health Services of Principado de Asturias, Oviedo, Spain
  • 24Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Clínico Universitario de Valencia, School of Medicine, Universidad de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
  • 25Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Virgen de la Luz, Cuenca, Spain
  • 26Health and Social Research Center, Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha, Cuenca, Spain
  • 27Department of Psychiatry, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria, Complejo Hospitalario Universitario de Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 28Fundación Publica Galega de Medicina Xenómica (SERGAS), Universidad de Santiago de Compostela, CIBERER, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 29Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
  • 30Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Ankara University, Ankara, Turkey
  • 31Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
  • 32Department of Psychosis Studies, King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom

Introduction: White noise speech illusions index liability for psychotic disorder in case–control comparisons. In the current study, we examined i) the rate of white noise speech illusions in siblings of patients with psychotic disorder and ii) to what degree this rate would be contingent on exposure to known environmental risk factors (childhood adversity and recent life events) and level of known endophenotypic dimensions of psychotic disorder [psychotic experiences assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE) scale and cognitive ability].

Methods: The white noise task was used as an experimental paradigm to elicit and measure speech illusions in 1,014 patients with psychotic disorders, 1,157 siblings, and 1,507 healthy participants. We examined associations between speech illusions and increasing familial risk (control -> sibling -> patient), modeled as both a linear and a categorical effect, and associations between speech illusions and level of childhood adversities and life events as well as with CAPE scores and cognitive ability scores.

Results: While a positive association was found between white noise speech illusions across hypothesized increasing levels of familial risk (controls -> siblings -> patients) [odds ratio (OR) linear 1.11, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.02–1.21, p = 0.019], there was no evidence for a categorical association with sibling status (OR 0.93, 95% CI 0.79–1.09, p = 0.360). The association between speech illusions and linear familial risk was greater if scores on the CAPE positive scale were higher (p interaction = 0.003; ORlow CAPE positive scale 0.96, 95% CI 0.85–1.07; ORhigh CAPE positive scale 1.26, 95% CI 1.09–1.46); cognitive ability was lower (p interaction < 0.001; ORhigh cognitive ability 0.94, 95% CI 0.84–1.05; ORlow cognitive ability 1.43, 95% CI 1.23–1.68); and exposure to childhood adversity was higher (p interaction < 0.001; ORlow adversity 0.92, 95% CI 0.82–1.04; ORhigh adversity 1.31, 95% CI 1.13–1.52). A similar, although less marked, pattern was seen for categorical patient–control and sibling–control comparisons. Exposure to recent life events did not modify the association between white noise and familial risk (p interaction = 0.232).

Conclusion: The association between white noise speech illusions and familial risk is contingent on additional evidence of endophenotypic expression and of exposure to childhood adversity. Therefore, speech illusions may represent a trait-dependent risk marker.

Introduction

The positive symptoms of psychotic disorder are characterized by altered attribution of meaning to internal or external stimuli. It is thought that psychosis may occur across a spectrum of severity, representing a dimension of human variation extending into the general population (1, 2). Research has demonstrated that psychotic experiences—in the form of attenuated reality distortion including perceptual abnormalities and persecutory ideas—can be demonstrated in the general population (2) across the world (3). Subthreshold psychotic experiences in the general population are mostly transient in nature (46), but in some individuals, persistent psychotic experiences may be predictive of psychotic disorder (6, 7). According to the psychosis–proneness–persistence–impairment model, psychosis expression may become persistent and clinically relevant, depending on developmental, environmental, and cognitive factors (1, 8). Furthermore, it has been suggested that the two ends of the psychosis spectrum, from subthreshold mental variation to severe impairment, show a degree of etiological continuity. This refers to the notion that genetic (9) and environmental risk factors (10, 11) for psychotic disorder also drive variation at the level of subtle psychotic experiences in the non-ill population.

Theoretical accounts of the cognitive mechanism of hallucinations (perceptions in the absence of a stimulus) suggest that abnormal perception originates from an imbalance in top–down and bottom–up processing (12). Altered perceptions may arise when a higher priority is given to top–down processing (perceptual expectation, prior knowledge and mental imagery), at the expense of bottom–up information (sensory input) (13, 14). In this context, illusions (misinterpretations of an external stimulus) may originate from perceptual expectations associated with top–down processing (14).

Experimental illusion studies have been designed around the paradigm of hearing voices in white noise, giving rise to a speech illusion representing the tendency to attribute meaning to neutral sensory input. Hoffman and colleagues, studying a population of individuals at psychometric risk of psychotic disorder, suggested that speech illusions may signal an increased risk of psychotic disorder (13). Galdos and colleagues reported on the white noise task, showing differences between individuals with psychotic disorders and healthy participants (15), later replicated by Catalan and colleagues (16). There was a suggestion that white noise speech illusions may represent an intermediate phenotype in that a prevalence of 30% was found in patients with psychotic disorder, 14% in siblings of patients, and 9% in the general population. Hypothetically, when self-reported psychotic experiences [e.g., Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE) positive scale] and white noise speech illusions tap into the same dimension of psychotic disorder, a positive association is expected. However, in the general population, no clear association has been observed, indicating that variations of speech illusions in the general population may not signal increased risk to develop a disorder (17, 18). A finding supporting this notion is that while risk factors for subclinical psychosis expression generally correspond with risk factors for psychotic disorder (10, 11), white noise speech illusions were not associated with either childhood adversity or life events in the general population (18). In conclusion, underlying mechanisms of white noise speech illusions may be different in patients and the general population. However, in non-ill individuals with a higher-than-average genetic risk for psychotic disorder, such as siblings of patients, white noise speech illusions may be associated with psychotic experiences as an expression of genetic risk, which may be even stronger if there is additional evidence of environmental exposure under a model of gene–environment interaction (11, 19). Furthermore, the subclass of affectively salient speech illusions (speech illusions with emotional impact) may be more strongly associated with psychotic experiences and psychosis risk. Thus, in a sample of healthy children, hallucinations during the last month were associated with white noise speech illusions that were affectively salient but not white noise speech illusions that were not affectively salient (20). Similarly, in the case–sibling–healthy participant study by Galdos and colleagues, stronger associations were apparent with affective speech illusions (15). It has been suggested that affective salience might characterize speech illusions in individuals at risk for clinical outcomes (20, 21) and may be mediated in part by cognitive alterations (22), although another study did not find evidence for this (15). Therefore, further research is required to evaluate differences in psychopathology, cognition, and affective valence of speech illusions in patients, siblings, and healthy participants.

In this report, the following hypotheses were examined. First, speech illusions were expected to be more prevalent in patients, and to a lesser degree in siblings of patients, compared to healthy participants, indicating that speech illusions may represent a familial marker for psychosis liability. Second, we hypothesized that the association between speech illusions and patient/sibling status would be trait-dependent, i.e., more pronounced in patients and relatives with greater levels of expression of known endophenotypic dimensions associated with psychosis. These included positive psychotic experiences and alterations in cognition. In addition, we hypothesized that the association between speech illusions and patient/relative status would be stronger if there was additional evidence of early environmental exposure (childhood adversity) or late environmental exposure (recent life events). Finally, investigating differences in underlying mechanisms of non-affective and affective speech illusions, we expected to find the most pronounced differences with affective speech illusions.

Methods

The current sample was derived from Workpackage 6 of the international EU-GEI (European Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene–Environment Interactions) project, a collaborative network studying genetic and environmental influences on the development, severity, and course of psychotic disorders (23). Workpackage 6 (GxE Vulnerability & Severity) focused on the expression of genetic and environmental liability in siblings of patients, who are thought to be at higher risk to develop psychotic disorders compared to healthy participants. In Workpackage 6, participants were collected in Spain (five centers), Turkey (three centers), and Serbia (one center). The sample consisted of 1,525 healthy comparison participants, 1,261 patients with a diagnosis of psychotic spectrum disorder (the great majority medicated patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder), and 1,282 siblings of the patients. Patients were diagnosed with schizophrenia spectrum disorder according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). This diagnosis was confirmed by the Operational Criteria Checklist for Psychotic and Affective Illness (24). Participants with a diagnosis of psychotic disorder due to another medical condition, a history of head injury with loss of consciousness, and an intelligence quotient <70 were excluded. The current analyses were restricted to the participants who underwent the white noise task, leaving 1,507 healthy participants, 1,014 patients, and 1,157 siblings.

In order to achieve high quality and homogeneity in clinical, experimental, and environmental assessments, standardized instruments were administered by psychiatrists, psychologists, or trained research assistants who completed mandatory on-country training sessions and online training modules including interactive interview videos and self-assessment tools (23, 25). Both on-country and online training sessions were repeated annually to maintain high inter-rater reliability throughout the 2010–2015 study enrolment period.

The EU-GEI project was approved by the medical ethics committees of all participating countries and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent.

Cognitive Ability

A short version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) was used to measure cognitive ability, consisting of the Digit Symbol Coding subtest, uneven items of the Arithmetic subtest, uneven items of the Block Design subtest, and every third item of the Information subtest (2628). The Z-score was calculated separately for each country and sex, for each test. The score for cognitive ability was calculated as the mean of the Z-scores of the different tests, expressed as a T-score (shifted and scaled to have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10). In the interaction analyses, cognitive score was modeled as a binary variable, calculated around the 20th percentile of the healthy participant group.

CAPE Scale

CAPE (29, 30) is a questionnaire designed to rate self-reports of positive, negative, and depressive psychotic experiences. The questionnaire consists of 42 items: 20 items on positive psychotic experiences, 14 items on negative psychotic experiences, and 8 items on depressive feelings. Participants rated both frequency (0 = never to 4 = nearly always) and distress (1 = not distressed to 4 = very distressed) of psychotic experiences. The mean score of the frequency and distress scales was calculated for each domain. In the analyses, the frequency measure of the positive psychotic experiences was used. In the interaction analyses, CAPE score was modeled as a binary variable, calculated around the 80th percentile of the healthy participant group, separately for each country.

Childhood Adversity

Childhood adversity was assessed using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire Short Form (CTQ), which consists of 28 items rated on a five-point Likert scale measuring five domains of maltreatment (emotional and physical neglect along with emotional, physical, and sexual abuse) (31). The psychometric characteristics of the translated versions (Spanish, Turkish, Dutch, and Serbian) of the CTQ have been comprehensively studied (3235). Consistent with previous work in similar samples, CTQ score was modeled as a binary variable, calculated around the 80th percentile of the mean score of the healthy participant group, separately for each country (36).

Life Events

Life events were measured with an expanded version (20 items) of the Brugha List of Threatening Experiences (37, 38). Participants rated the presence of an event during the 12-month period before onset (for patients) or before the interview (for healthy participants and siblings). The sum of life events was calculated and used as a continuous measure in the analyses.

The White Noise Task

During the white noise task, participants are exposed to three types of stimuli randomly presented across 75 fragments: 25 fragments of white noise only, 25 fragments containing white noise mixed with barely audible speech, and 25 fragments of white noise mixed with clearly audible speech. The clearly audible speech fragments had positive, negative, or neutral affective content. For example: “Sport is good for health,” “I think it is going to rain today,” or “Madrid is the capital of Spain.” Speech was adapted to each country’s native language. Each fragment had a duration of 4.3 s; the spoken sentence lasted as close as possible to 4.3 s. Sound fragments were binaurally presented through headphones. After the ending of each fragment, participants were asked to indicate what they heard by pressing on a button on the keyboard in front of them: 1, endorsed hearing speech with positive content; 2, endorsed hearing speech with negative content; 3, endorsed hearing speech with neutral content; 4, no speech heard; and 5, endorsed hearing speech but uncertain whether it was positive, negative, or neutral. The protocol was guided by the software system E-prime 1.1 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania) and took approximately 15 min. In line with the definition proposed by Catalan and colleagues (16), a speech illusion was defined broadly as a white noise fragment in which any speech was heard (option 1, 2, 3, or 5). As participants were exposed to 75 fragments, of which 25 contained white noise only, the maximum number of speech illusions was 25. First, a dichotomous speech illusion variable was calculated, with two or more speech illusions as the cutoff, as defined by Catalan and colleagues (16). Second, in order to examine the effect of affective valence, a categorical affective speech illusion variable was calculated (0 = less than two non-affective/affective speech illusions, 1 = two or more non-affective speech illusions, and 2 = two or more affective speech illusions).

Statistical Analyses

Binary logistic regression models were applied to the dichotomous speech illusion variable. As the data were hierarchically organized (individuals nested within families), familial clustering was taken into account. A first model was run with group (healthy participants = 0, siblings = 1, and patients = 2), modeled both as a linear effect and as dummy variables, as the independent variable of main interest, corrected for age, sex, cognitive ability, and country of data collection. Second, a model was run with the interaction between group and the CAPE positive scale as an endophenotypic moderator. Similarly, a model was run with the interaction between group and cognitive ability as an endophenotypic moderator. Finally, two models were executed with speech illusions explained by the interaction between group and i) childhood adversity and ii) life events, respectively, in order to test for environmental effect modification. Interaction models were followed by calculation of stratified effects, using linear combination of terms in the model with the interaction, using the Stata lincom routine. The latter models were adjusted for the CAPE positive scale, age, sex, cognitive ability, and country of data collection.

Multinomial logistic regression models, yielding ORs, were applied to the three-level categorical affective speech illusion variable described above. The models described above for the dichotomous speech illusion variable were run again, using multinomial logistic regression with the three-level categorical affective speech illusion variable. Associations were considered significant when the two-sided p-value was <0.05; a p-value between 0.05 and 0.1 was referred to as a trend. Stata version 13 was used (39).

Results

Participants

An overview of selected demographic variables, familial risk, and white noise speech illusions is provided in Table 1. Of the 3,678 participants, 1,526 (41.5%) reported two or more speech illusions. Patients had a higher rate of speech illusions (47.2%) compared to healthy participants (41.0%) and siblings (37.1%). Inspection rates in Table 1 suggest differences between countries in the rate of speech illusions; however, the within-country pattern of differences between groups was similar. The rate of two or more speech illusions with affective content was 14.6% in healthy participants, 8.2% in siblings, and 18.4% in patients.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Summary of selected sociodemographic values.

Associations Between Speech Illusion and Group

In models adjusted for age, sex, country, cognitive ability, and familial clustering of observations, a positive association was observed with white noise speech illusions across increasing level of familial risk (OR linear trend 1.11, 95% CI 1.02–1.21, p = 0.019), modeled as a linear variable. Modeling group as two dummy variables revealed that the association with white noise speech illusions was 0.93 for siblings (95% CI 0.79–1.09, p = 0.360) and 1.27 for patients (95% CI 1.07–1.51, p = 0.007).

Psychoticism Dependence of Association Between Group and White Noise Speech Illusions

White noise speech illusions were differentially associated with the dichotomous CAPE positive scale as a function of group (OR interaction 1.32, 95% CI 1.10–1.58, p = 0.003). Calculation of stratified effect sizes revealed an intermediate albeit non-significant association between speech illusions and sibling status in the high CAPE positive group (OR 1.07, 95% CI 0.75–1.52, p = 0.713) and a larger association between speech illusions and patient status in the high CAPE positive group (OR 1.56, 95% CI 1.17–2.08, p = 0.003) (Table 2). There were no significant positive associations between speech illusions and either siblings or patients in the low CAPE positive group (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Interaction between group and CAPE positive scale, cognitive ability, or childhood adversity in any white noise speech illusion.

Cognition Dependence of Association Between Group and White Noise Speech Illusions

The association between white noise speech illusions and group was significantly influenced by dichotomous cognitive ability (OR interaction 1.53, 95% CI 1.27–1.84, p < 0.001). Stratified analyses revealed a significant association between speech illusions and sibling status in the low-cognition group (OR 2.03, 95% CI 1.41–2.93, p < 0.001) as well between speech illusions and patient status in the low-cognition group (OR 2.16, 95% CI 1.57–2.97, p < 0.001) (Table 2). There was a significant negative association between speech illusions and sibling status in the high-cognition group (OR 0.77, 95% CI 0.64–0.92, p = 0.004) and no significant association between speech illusions and patients in the high-cognition group (OR 0.95, 95% CI 0.76–1.18, p = 0.651) (Table 2).

Environmental Moderation of Association Between Group and White Noise Speech Illusion

The association between group and white noise speech illusion was modified by dichotomously defined childhood adversity (OR interaction 1.42, 95% CI 1.19–1.70, p < 0.001). Stratified analyses showed a positive albeit non-significant association between speech illusions and sibling status in the group exposed to childhood adversity (OR 1.19, 95% CI 0.87–1.64, p = 0.272) and a larger association between speech illusions and patient status in the exposed group (OR 1.71, 95% CI 1.27–2.30, p < 0.001) (Table 2). There were no significant positive associations between speech illusions and either sibling or patient status in the low-childhood-adversity group (Table 2).

There was no interaction between life events in the previous 12 months and group risk in the model of speech illusions (OR 1.03, 95% CI 0.98–1.09, p = 0.232).

Difference Between Non-Affective and Affective Speech Illusions

A suggestive difference was found for the association between group and non-affective white noise speech illusions (OR linear trend 1.10, 95% CI 0.99–1.21, p = 0.064), and a significant association was found between group and affective white noise speech illusions (OR 1.14 linear trend, 95% CI 1.00–1.29, p = 0.046) (Table 3). The interaction between group and CAPE positive scale was significant for both non-affective (OR 1.23, 95% CI 1.00 ≠ 1.51, p = 0.048) and affective (OR 1.57, 95% CI 1.19–2.08, p = 0.002) speech illusions. A significant interaction between group and cognitive ability was found in the model of affective speech illusions (OR interaction 2.44, 95% CI 1.85–3.21, p < 0.001), but only at trend level in the model of non-affective speech illusions (OR interaction 1.21, 95% CI 0.99–1.49, p = 0.069). For both non-affective (OR interaction 1.42, 95% CI 1.16–1.74, p = 0.001) and affective (OR interaction 1.48, 95% CI 1.13–1.93, p = 0.005) speech illusions, a significant interaction was found with childhood adversity (Table 4). No interaction was found between group and life events for either non-affective (OR interaction 1.03, 95% CI 0.98–1.09, p = 0.261) or affective (OR interaction 1.05, 95% CI 0.95–1.15, p = 0.353) speech illusions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Association between group and any non-affective and affective speech illusion.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Interaction between group and CAPE positive scale, cognitive ability, or childhood adversity in (non-)affective speech illusions.

Associations between speech illusions and sibling status were found for non-affective speech illusions in the low-cognition group and the high-childhood-adversity group, and for affective hallucinations in the high- (negative association) and low-cognition (positive association) groups, as well as a negative association in the low-childhood-adversity group and the low CAPE positive group.

Discussion

This study investigated white noise speech illusions across different levels of familial risk. The analyses demonstrated that: i) white noise speech illusions were significantly associated with familial risk, modeled as a linear effect, although there was no evidence for categorical association with sibling status and ii) the association between speech illusions and familial risk, including categorical sibling status, was stronger if there was additional evidence for early environmental exposure or trait expression of psychosis proneness and cognitive alterations. The effect size of the interaction between familial risk on the one hand and cognitive ability, psychosis proneness, and environmental exposure on the other was numerically greater for affective speech illusions than for non-affective speech illusions, but these differences were small.

Two or more speech illusions were reported by 47.2% of the patients compared to 41.0% of the healthy participants and 37.1% of the siblings. These are higher than the rates reported by Galdos et al. (15), who used another definition for speech illusions (any perceived speech illusion with positive, negative, or neutral valence). The current study used the definition by Catalan et al. (16). A sensitivity analysis, however, revealed that the rate of speech illusions using the definition by Galdos et al. (40% in healthy participants, 33% in siblings, and 43% in patients) yielded a pattern of results that was similar to the results as presented above for the definition by Catalan and colleagues.

Speech illusions were not more prevalent in siblings of patients compared to healthy participants; overall, siblings even tended to have lower rates than healthy participants. However, the association between sibling status and white noise speech illusions did increase when there was additional evidence for trait expression of psychotic experiences, cognitive alterations, and childhood adversity, although this was not uniformly so, as sibling effects were most prominent for non-affective speech illusions in the presence of lower cognitive ability, more psychosis proneness, and environmental exposure, whereas negative sibling associations were present for affective speech illusions in the absence of lower cognitive ability, more psychosis proneness, and environmental exposure. In other words, in comparison to controls, siblings became more similar to patients only with respect to non-affective speech illusions in the subgroup selected for more risk traits and more exposure to risk factors. Therefore, white noise speech illusions appear to index a trait-dependent marker of risk: the power of white noise speech illusion to predict sibling and patient status is higher in the subgroup enriched with traits associated with genetic and environmental risk for the disorder.

Environmental risk factors (10, 11) for psychotic disorders have been observed to also drive variation at the level of subclinical psychotic experiences. The fact that the association between speech illusions and sibling and patient status, compared with controls, was stronger in the exposed subgroup may be suggestive of underlying gene–environment interaction. However, the results suggest that this would only apply to distal environmental exposures to life childhood adversity, but not proximal life stress. The window for crucial gene–environment interactions may be restricted to early developmental periods rather than the adult period (11).

Given that psychotic experiences and cognitive alterations are thought to reflect, in part, genetic risk for psychotic disorder (9, 40), the same underlying mechanism of gene–environment interaction may account for the dependence of patient–control and sibling–control speech illusion associations at higher levels of these traits. Thus, earlier studies have shown that in samples of the general population, no association exists between white noise speech illusions and the CAPE positive scale (17, 18). The CAPE positive scale mainly reflects alteration in delusional ideation, whereas the white noise task focuses on lower-prevalence alterations in perception. The non-significant association between speech illusions and the CAPE positive scale in the non-clinical population suggests that alterations in perception may not necessarily index alterations in ideation. It has been shown that in clinical populations and relatives of patients, alterations in perception are more strongly associated with alterations in ideation than in non-clinical populations (41). Thus, more evidence for association between white noise speech illusions and the CAPE positive scale in the trait-rich subgroup of sibling and patients, in comparison to controls, would be compatible with this observation.

The results did not suggest a uniform pattern of differentiation between affective and non-affective speech illusions in the pattern of associations between speech illusions, contrary to earlier research (15, 20). More work in this area is required.

There are some limitations. The CAPE positive scale and childhood trauma were modeled as binary variables, calculated around the 80th percentile of the mean score of the healthy participant group, separately for each country. Cognitive ability was modeled as a binary variable, calculated around the 20th percentile. In order to examine to what degree results were robust with regard to alternative cutoffs, sensitivity analyses were conducted with a cutoff at the 70th/30th percentile of the healthy participant group. A similar pattern of results was found.

The white noise task and associated outcomes were only administered once. It has been suggested that persistence of psychotic experiences may be particularly predictive of psychotic disorder. Therefore, a longitudinal design allowing examination of the association between white noise speech illusions and persistence of psychotic experience may be more informative.

The study was conducted across different countries and cultures, and although the pattern of white noise distribution across patients and healthy participants was similar across countries, there were differences in base rates, which may have to do with differences in language and culture impacting the experiment. As country was adjusted for, these differences will not impact effect estimates.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The EU-GEI project was approved by the Medical Ethics Committees of all participating countries and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed conception and design of the study, data collection, and organization of the database. ES, RL and JV performed statistical analysis. ES wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Supported by the European Community’s Seventh Framework Program under grant agreement No. HEALTH-F2-2009-241909 (Project EU-GEI).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

Author Halis Ulaş is no longer affiliated with Department of Psychiatry, Dokuz Eylül University School of Medicine. Author was discharged by decree 701 on July 8, 2018 because of signing Peace Petition.

References

1. van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness–persistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med (2009) 39(2):179–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Verdoux H, Van Os J. Psychotic symptoms in non-clinical populations and the continuum of psychosis. Schizophr Res (2002) 54(1–2):59–65. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00352-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Nuevo R, Van Os J, Arango C, Chatterji S, Ayuso-Mateos JL. Evidence for the early clinical relevance of hallucinatory-delusional states in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2013) 127(6):482–93. doi: 10.1111/acps.12010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Dhossche D, Ferdinand R, Van der Ende J, Hofstra MB, Verhulst F. Diagnostic outcome of self-reported hallucinations in a community sample of adolescents. Psychol Med (2002) 32(4):619–27. doi: 10.1017/S003329170200555X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Dominguez MD, Wichers M, Lieb R, Wittchen HU, van Os J. Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: an 8-year cohort study. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37(1):84–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wiles NJ, Zammit S, Bebbington P, Singleton N, Meltzer H, Lewis G. Self-reported psychotic symptoms in the general population: results from the longitudinal study of the British National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Br J Psychiatry (2006) 188:519–26. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.012179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H. Children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2000) 57(11):1053–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Cougnard A, Marcelis M, Myin-Germeys I, De Graaf R, Vollebergh W, Krabbendam L, et al. Does normal developmental expression of psychosis combine with environmental risk to cause persistence of psychosis? A psychosis proneness–persistence model. Psychol Med (2007) 37(4):513–27. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. van Os J, van der Steen Y, Islam MA, Guloksuz S, Rutten BP, Simons CJ, et al. Evidence that polygenic risk for psychotic disorder is expressed in the domain of neurodevelopment, emotion regulation and attribution of salience. Psychol Med (2017) 47(14):2421–37. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Guloksuz S, van Nierop M, Lieb R, van Winkel R, Wittchen HU, van Os J. Evidence that the presence of psychosis in non-psychotic disorder is environment-dependent and mediated by severity of non-psychotic psychopathology. Psychol Med (2015) 45(11):2389–401. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature (2010) 468(7321):203–12. doi: 10.1038/nature09563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Aleman A, Bocker KB, Hijman R, de Haan EH, Kahn RS. Cognitive basis of hallucinations in schizophrenia: role of top–down information processing. Schizophr Res (2003) 64(2-3):175–85. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00060-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hoffman RE, Woods SW, Hawkins KA, Pittman B, Tohen M, Preda A, et al. Extracting spurious messages from noise and risk of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders in a prodromal population. Br J Psychiatry (2007) 191:355–6. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.031195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hugdahl K. Hearing voices: auditory hallucinations as failure of top–down control of bottom–up perceptual processes. Scand J Psychol (2009) 50(6):553–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00775.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Galdos M, Simons C, Fernandez-Rivas A, Wichers M, Peralta C, Lataster T, et al. Affectively salient meaning in random noise: a task sensitive to psychosis liability. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37(6):1179–86. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Catalan A, Simons CJ, Bustamante S, Drukker M, Madrazo A, de Artaza MG, et al. Novel evidence that attributing affectively salient signal to random noise is associated with psychosis. PLoS One (2014) 9(7):e102520. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Pries LK, Guloksuz S, Menne-Lothmann C, Decoster J, van Winkel R, Collip D, et al. White noise speech illusion and psychosis expression: an experimental investigation of psychosis liability. PLoS One (2017) 12(8):e0183695. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Schepers E, van Os J, Lousberg R. White noise speech illusions in the general population: the association with psychosis expression and risk factors for psychosis. PLoS One (2019) 14(2):e0211914. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Radhakrishnan R, Guloksuz S, Ten Have M, de Graaf R, van Dorsselaer S, Gunther N, et al. Interaction between environmental and familial affective risk impacts psychosis admixture in states of affective dysregulation. Psychol Med (2019) 49(11):1879–89. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718002635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Rimvall MK, Clemmensen L, Munkholm A, Rask CU, Larsen JT, Skovgaard AM, et al. Introducing the White Noise task in childhood: associations between speech illusions and psychosis vulnerability. Psychol Med (2016) 46(13):2731–40. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Daalman K, Boks MP, Diederen KM, de Weijer AD, Blom JD, Kahn RS, et al. The same or different? A phenomenological comparison of auditory verbal hallucinations in healthy and psychotic individuals. J Clin Psychiatry (2011) 72(3):320–5. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05797yel

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Catalan A, de Artaza MG, Fernandez-Rivas A, Angosto V, Aguirregomoscorta F, Bustamante S, et al. Affectively salient signal to random noise might be used to identify psychosis vulnerability in severe mental disorders. Eur Psychiatry (2018) 49:37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. European Network of National Networks studying Gene–Environment Interactions in S, van Os J, Rutten BP, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Viechtbauer W, et al. Identifying gene–environment interactions in schizophrenia: contemporary challenges for integrated, large-scale investigations. Schizophr Bull (2014) 40(4):729–36. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. McGuffin P, Farmer A, Harvey I. A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness. Development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1991) 48(8):764–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810320088015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Korver N, Quee PJ, Boos HB, Simons CJ, de Haan L. Group Investigators. Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP), a multi-site longitudinal cohort study focused on gene–environment interaction: objectives, sample characteristics, recruitment and assessment methods. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res (2012) 21(3):205–21. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Blyler CR, Gold JM, Iannone VN, Buchanan RW. Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2000) 46(2-3):209–15. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00017-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Velthorst E, Levine SZ, Henquet C, de Haan L, van Os J, Myin-Germeys I, et al. To cut a short test even shorter: reliability and validity of a brief assessment of intellectual ability in schizophrenia—a control–case family study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2013) 18(6):574–93. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.731390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wechsler D. WAIS-III, Wechsler adult intelligence scale: administration and scoring manual. Psychol Corp (1997). doi: 10.1037/t49755-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2006) 114(1):55–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00741.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mossaheb N, Becker J, Schaefer MR, Klier CM, Schloegelhofer M, Papageorgiou K, et al. The Community Assessment of Psychic Experience (CAPE) questionnaire as a screening-instrument in the detection of individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res (2012) 141(2–3):210–4. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl (2003) 27(2):169–90. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hernandez A, Gallardo-Pujol D, Pereda N, Arntz A, Bernstein DP, Gaviria AM, et al. Initial validation of the Spanish Childhood Trauma Questionnaire—Short Form: factor structure, reliability and association with parenting. J Interpers Violence (2013) 28(7):1498–518. doi: 10.1177/0886260512468240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sar V, Akyuz G, Kundakci T, Kiziltan E, Dogan O. Childhood trauma, dissociation, and psychiatric comorbidity in patients with conversion disorder. Am J Psychiatry (2004) 161(12):2271–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.161.12.2271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Thombs BD, Bernstein DP, Lobbestael J, Arntz A. A validation study of the Dutch Childhood Trauma Questionnaire—Short Form: factor structure, reliability, and known-groups validity. Child Abuse Negl (2009) 33(8):518–23. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Mitkovic-Voncina M, Lecic-Tosevski D, Pejovic-Milovancevic M, Popovic-Deusic S. Linking child maltreatment history with child abuse potential: relative roles of maltreatment types. Arch Biol Sci (2014) 66:1681–7. doi: 10.2298/ABS1404681M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. van Os J, Marsman A, van Dam D, Simons CJ, Investigators G. Evidence that the impact of childhood trauma on IQ is substantial in controls, moderate in siblings, and absent in patients with psychotic disorder. Schizophr Bull (2017) 43(2):316–24. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Brugha T, Bebbington P, Tennant C, Hurry J. The List of Threatening Experiences: a subset of 12 life event categories with considerable long-term contextual threat. Psychol Med (1985) 15(1):189–94. doi: 10.1017/S003329170002105X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Brugha TS, Cragg D. The List of Threatening Experiences: the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiatr Scand (1990) 82(1):77–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. StataCorp.STATA Statistical Software: release 13. Texas: College Station (2013).

Google Scholar

40. Toulopoulou T, Picchioni M, Rijsdijk F, Hua-Hall M, Ettinger U, Sham P, et al. Substantial genetic overlap between neurocognition and schizophrenia: genetic modeling in twin samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64(12):1348–55. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Smeets F, Lataster T, van Winkel R, de Graaf R, Ten Have M, van Os J. Testing the hypothesis that psychotic illness begins when subthreshold hallucinations combine with delusional ideation. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2013) 127(1):34–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01888.x.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: white noise speech illusions, psychotic disorder, Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences, cognitive ability, childhood adversity, life events

Citation: Schepers E, Lousberg R, Guloksuz S, Pries L-K, Delespaul P, Kenis G, Luykx JJ, Lin BD, Richards AL, Akdede B, Binbay T, Altınyazar V, Yalınçetin B, Gümüş-Akay G, Cihan B, Soygür H, Ulaş H, Şahin Cankurtaran E, Ulusoy Kaymak S, Mihaljevic MM, Andric Petrovic S, Mirjanic T, Bernardo M, Cabrera B, Bobes J, Saiz PA, García-Portilla MP, Sanjuan J, Aguilar EJ, Luis Santos J, Jiménez-López E, Arrojo M, Carracedo A, López G, González-Peñas J, Parellada M, Maric NP, Atbaşoğlu C, Ucok A, Alptekin K, Can Saka M, Arango C, Rutten BPF and van Os J (2019) White Noise Speech Illusions: A Trait-Dependent Risk Marker for Psychotic Disorder? Front. Psychiatry 10:676. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00676

Received: 21 April 2019; Accepted: 21 August 2019;
Published: 25 September 2019.

Edited by:

Wim Veling, University Medical Center Groningen, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Massimo Tusconi, University of Cagliari, Italy
Gaelle Eve Doucet, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Copyright © 2019 Schepers, Lousberg, Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx, Lin, Richards, Akdede, Binbay, Altınyazar, Yalınçetin, Gümüş-Akay, Cihan, Soygür, Ulaş, Şahin Cankurtaran, Ulusoy Kaymak, Mihaljevic, Andric Petrovic, Mirjanic, Bernardo, Cabrera, Bobes, Saiz, García-Portilla, Sanjuan, Aguilar, Luis Santos, Jiménez-López, Arrojo, Carracedo, López, González-Peñas, Parellada, Maric, Atbaşoğlu, Ucok, Alptekin, Can Saka, Arango, Rutten and van Os. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elaine Schepers, e.schepers@student.maastrichtuniversity.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.