- 1Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
- 2Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Objectives: Although cross-sectional investigations have found a bifactor structure of psychiatric comorbidity that includes a general psychopathology factor plus more specific factors, prospective evidence supporting the bifactor structure is still limited. We evaluated the structural stability (i.e., longitudinal invariance) of the bifactor model in comparison to an alternative structure, a correlated factors model without a general psychopathology factor. We also investigated the models’ generalizability to change processes in psychopathology.
Methods: The analyses were conducted on 10-year follow-up data from 5,001 respondents in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Invariance was evaluated through a series of nested invariance tests using confirmatory factor analysis, and the models’ generalizability to change processes was investigated using change scores of disorder status.
Results: The bifactor model and the correlated factors model exhibited an equal degree of strong structural stability over time. Only the bifactor model satisfactorily characterized the structure of temporal changes in psychopathology.
Conclusions: The bifactor structure with a general psychopathology factor is stable over time and describes temporal changes in psychopathology. The findings support the notion that the general psychopathology factor describes a transdiagnostic etiology and may therefore provide a useful target for intervention and treatment.
Introduction
Extensive comorbidity of psychiatric disorders has been well established by epidemiological studies. Early psychometric models suggested that such comorbidity would be accounted for by latent transdiagnostic factors of internalizing (mood and anxiety disorders) and externalizing (substance use, impulsivity, and antisocial behavior) but could not explain why externalizing and internalizing traits were also “comorbid” (i.e., co-occurring). More recent research has explored a bifactor model of psychopathology that includes a latent general psychopathology factor, sometimes labeled the p factor (1). The bifactor model thus consists of a broad general psychopathology factor that is presumed to underlie all psychiatric disorders and conceptually narrower specific factors of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. The general psychopathology factor captures what is common across all forms of psychiatric diagnoses and accounts for the co-occurrence of internalizing and externalizing disorders.
Even though the bifactor model has been empirically supported across a range of diverse samples (1–7), significant debate remains regarding the metastructure of psychopathology and the validity of the general psychopathology factor. A central issue is whether the overall structure of psychopathology should be modeled as a) a bifactor model with a broad general psychopathology factor and narrower specific factors that are not correlated or b) with a model that includes multiple correlated factors but no general factor of psychopathology. The externalizing and internalizing traits could co-occur due to mutual interaction, not due to a latent general factor. However, finding that a general factor model fits the data does not prove the factor’s substantive existence because a general factor could emerge simply because data features positive correlations among disorders (8). Furthermore, researchers have raised concerns regarding the bifactor model’s tendency to show superior performance in terms of model fit, suggesting that the bifactor model may be inclined to capture random noise in the data (9, 10).
Overall, despite the surge of interest in the general psychopathology factor and recent advances in understanding the structure of psychopathology, many pertinent questions regarding the nature of the bifactor model remain unanswered. Most studies examining the bifactor model have measured psychopathology at a single point in time, thereby providing an incomplete picture on the model’s longitudinal properties and on the development of psychiatric comorbidity. Although a few studies have investigated the temporal stability of the bifactor model (11–13), the model’s longitudinal invariance, that is, structural stability, has not been formally tested. Whereas temporal stability describes the extent to which the model’s latent factors predict each other at a later time point, longitudinal invariance denotes that the model’s latent factors have the same meaning over time. Establishing longitudinal invariance is thus a prerequisite to making meaningful comparisons of factor means over time. It is currently not known whether the bifactor model parameters such as factor loadings are temporally consistent or, in other words, if the relationships between psychiatric disorders and the latent factors of the bifactor model remain the same across successive measurement occasions of same individuals. Because the factors are indirectly observed latent entities, it is important to ensure they exist in a comparable form with the same meaning over time. Another question to be explored is whether the bifactor model is structurally more or less stable than for example, the traditional correlated factors model. Testing the longitudinal invariance of the bifactor model and comparing it with the invariance of a competing model without a general psychopathology factor may provide important insights into underlying etiology and into the models’ applicability.
Although longitudinal invariance establishes an age-invariant factorial structure for between-individual differences in the population, allowing one to interpret change in the factors, it gives no direct guarantees that within-individual changes follow the same structure (14). Discovering that the same longitudinally invariant structure generalizes to data on temporal changes would add evidence for a hypothesis that the modeled latent factors are situated within individuals, driving parallel changes in multiple disorders. If disorders across internalizing and externalizing spectrums change in the same direction within individuals (i.e., individuals experience parallel onsets or recoveries from disorders from both spectrums), this suggests that a general factor is driving the process by which psychiatric disorders develop across time. In contrast, if changes in internalizing disorders are not typically accompanied by changes in externalizing disorders (beyond chance rate), then within-individual change in psychopathology is not driven by a general factor but rather by underlying internalizing and externalizing factors or by disorder-specific influences. Furthermore, even if disorders across spectra do exhibit parallel changes over time, it becomes appropriate to ask whether this “parallelism” is better described by a bifactor structure or, for example, by correlated internalizing and externalizing factors. According to previous work, internalizing and externalizing factors explain the comorbidity in the onset of psychiatric disorders (15). However, whether the general psychopathology factor better accounts for such comorbidity remains to be investigated.
The central unanswered question here is whether individual differences in intraindividual patterns of change in psychiatric comorbidity can be characterized by a similar factor structure as pointwise cross-sectional individual differences. Assuming that patterns of potential confounding factors for cross-sectional individual differences do not carry over to change processes, such a finding would suggest that the latent factors reflect true underlying psychopathology mechanisms. In other words, the longitudinal applicability of the general-factor model would suggest the existence of a transdiagnostic etiological factor that might respond to treatment. Successful treatment of such etiological factor would not only treat all psychiatric disorders instead of just one but would also allow us to understand cases where diagnostic “migration” is seen instead of recovery.
To sum, despite recent progress in understanding psychiatric comorbidity, much remains to be learned about the structure of psychopathology and the validity of the general psychopathology factor. Little is known about the structural stability of the bifactor model or how the model compares with other comorbidity models in prospective data. It is also currently unknown whether change in psychopathology follows a distinct structure. Compared to cross-sectional investigations, prospective studies have the potential to offer more convincing evidence on the structure of psychiatric comorbidity and are essential to enhance our knowledge about whether the general psychopathology factor is a substantive construct or merely a statistical artifact. Within this context, the primary purpose of the present study was to examine the longitudinal invariance of the bifactor model in comparison to a traditional correlated factors model and to evaluate the models’ generalizability to change processes in psychopathology, which has not been done before, as far as we know.
Materials and Methods
Sample
Data came from the 5,001 respondents who participated in the 1990–1992 National Comorbidity Survey (NCS) (16) and the 2001–2003 NCS follow-up survey (NCS-2). The baseline NCS was a nationally representative survey of US households designed to study the prevalence and correlates of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-III-R mental and substance disorders. The survey was administered to 8,098 respondents in the noninstitutionalized civilian population, with a response rate of 82.4%. The survey was administered in two parts. Part I included the core diagnostic interview and was administered to all respondents. Part II assessed additional disorders and was administered to a probability subsample of 5,877 including all respondents aged 15–24 years, all others with any lifetime DSM-III-R disorder assessed in Part I, and a random subsample of remaining Part I respondents. Further details about the NCS design are reported elsewhere (16).
The NCS-2 attempted to trace and reinterview all of the original Part II respondents a decade after the baseline NCS using an expanded version of the baseline interview that assessed onset, course, and severity of mental disorders between the two surveys. A total of 5,463 respondents were successfully traced, of whom 166 were deceased and 5,001 reinterviewed, yielding a conditional response rate of 87.6%. The unconditional response rate, i.e., the percentage of baseline respondents interviewed in both surveys, was 72.2% (0.876 × 0.824). Relative to the baseline, NCS-2 respondents were significantly more likely to be female, well-educated, and residents of rural areas. A nonresponse adjustment (a sampling weight) was used to correct for these discrepancies.
Diagnostic Assessment
The baseline NCS assessed lifetime DSM-III-R disorders using a modified version of the World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI), a structured interview administered by trained nonclinician interviewers (17). NCS-2 assessed psychiatric disorders present during the follow-up using a more recent version of the CIDI (18), which was based on the DSM-IV criteria. The interviews yielded psychiatric diagnoses and information about the course of disorders, such as age at first occurrence of symptoms.
The diagnoses considered were mania (MAN), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), major depressive episode (MDE), dysthymia (DYS), posttraumatic stress disorder (PSTD), agoraphobia (AGO), panic disorder (PD), social phobia (SOP), specific phobia (SP), alcohol abuse (ALC), drug abuse (DRG), and antisocial personality disorder (APD). The diagnoses were operationalized without hierarchy rules and assessed with a 9-year recency criterion with the exception of APD, for which only lifetime diagnosis was available. The recency criterion was chosen to optimize coverage while avoiding temporal overlap between the two follow-ups. Bipolar disorder and conduct disorder were excluded from analyses because their recency could not be assessed at baseline and because their more severe variants (MAN and APD) were already included.
Statistical Analyses
Cross-Sectional Factor Structure
As a preliminary analysis, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted to examine the cross-sectional factor structure of psychopathology at baseline (T1) and follow-up (T2) and to determine if the factor loadings were as expected based on previous literature. To establish the optimal number of factors to be extracted in EFA, parallel analysis was performed (19). After conducting EFA, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to investigate the fit of the possible factor structures. For completeness, we fitted and compared five different CFA models: 1) a one-factor model, 2) a two-factor correlated factors model with internalizing and externalizing factors, 3) a three-factor correlated factors model with fear, distress, and externalizing factors, 4) a bifactor model with a general factor and internalizing- and externalizing-specific factors, and 5) a bifactor model with a general factor and fear-, distress-, and externalizing-specific factors. Model fit for competing CFA models was assessed using the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Current conventions suggest that good fit is indicated by a CFI and TLI >0.95 and an RMSEA <0.06 (20), whereas adequate fit is indicated by a CFI and TLI >0.90 and an RMSEA between 0.06 and 0.08. We additionally report chi-square test values and degrees of freedom but omit p values because the test is oversensitive in large samples (21). Construct reliability was assessed with Hancock’s H (22), an index of latent construct reliability. All CFA models were estimated using Mplus version 7 (23) with mean- and variance-adjusted weighted least squares (WLSMV) estimation. For the best-fitting models, we also provide Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) values obtained using robust maximum likelihood estimation (MLR) in the supplementary material.
Longitudinal Invariance
Longitudinal invariance was tested for the bifactor model and the correlated factors model. Instead of estimating separate models for each measurement occasion as in cross-sectional CFA, data from both occasions were combined for the invariance analysis and longitudinal CFA models with lagged correlations were fitted. For both the models tested, we first evaluated whether the same number of factors and the same pattern of factor loadings characterize the structure of psychopathology at T1 and T2 (configural invariance, i.e., the unconstrained model). The stability of factor loadings (weak invariance) was then examined by constraining all corresponding factor loadings to be equal across the two time points. Finally, threshold invariance (strong invariance) was tested by additionally constraining indicator thresholds across T1 and T2. With binary indicators, such as disorder statuses, the thresholds indicate the cut-point on an assumed unobserved continuum that provides the observed disorder prevalence. Delta parameterization was used in the model specification, and for purposes of model identification, factor means were constrained to zero and factor variances were fixed at unity. Lagged factor correlations, lagged indicator correlations, and measurement error variances (uniquenesses) were freely estimated.
When the sample size is large, the chi-square difference test is likely to detect trivial decrements in model fit as statistically significant (21, 24, 25). Consequently, we did not perform chi-square difference test but rather used the change in CFI (ΔCFI) as an empirically supported (24) criterion to compare models with different levels of invariance. Although there is no consensus on the standards by which significant decreases in model fit should be evaluated when testing for invariance with categorical indicators, a ΔCFI >0.01 has previously been suggested to indicate that invariance should be rejected (21).
Factor Structure of Change in Psychopathology
The structure of change in psychopathology was examined using change scores of disorder status. The scores were calculated for each individual disorder by subtracting the T1 disorder status (1 = yes, 0 = no) from the T2 disorder status. A score of −1 reflected disorder recovery, a score of 0 indicated no change in disorder status, and a score of 1 reflected disorder onset between T1 and T2. Again, correlations among underlying liabilities (risks) of these ordinal-valued changes were modeled rather than raw correlations.
Results
Cross-Sectional Factor Structure
The detailed results from the preliminary cross-sectional analyses, including factor loadings from EFA and CFA, model fit indices for all CFA models tested, and construct reliability coefficients are presented in Supplementary Tables 1–4. Parallel analysis suggested three underlying factors for both T1 and T2 data. In confirmatory analyses, the bifactor model with a general factor and two specific factors showed superior fit over all other factor structures tested (CFI = 0.989, TLI = 0.983, RMSEA = 0.017 for T1). The second-best fitting model, a correlated three-factor model, also demonstrated good model fit (CFI = 0.978, TLI = 0.972, RMSEA = 0.022 for T1). In both the models, the latent factors showed good construct reliability, with the exception of the specific internalizing factor, which was not well defined, as it included both positive and negative loadings and some of the loadings were low in magnitude. Increasing the number of specific factors in the bifactor model (i.e., specifying separate factors for fear and distress) resulted in an inadmissible solution, potentially due to the extraction of too many factors. The bifactor model with two, rather than three, specific factors was thus selected for further analysis. A simplified representation of the two best fitting models is presented in Figure 1.
Figure 1 Simplified representations of the correlated factors model (A) and the bifactor model (B). MAN, mania; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MDE, major depressive episode; DYS, dysthymia; PSTD, posttraumatic stress disorder; AGO, agoraphobia; PD, panic disorder; SOP, social phobia; SP, specific phobia; ALC, alcohol abuse; DRG, drug abuse; APD, antisocial personality disorder; DIST, distress; EXT, externalizing; INT, internalizing; GEN, general.
Longitudinal Invariance
Given that the correlated three-factor model and the bifactor model had the best fit to cross-sectional data, we examined longitudinal invariance for these two factor models (Table 1). In the unconstrained longitudinal models with all parameters free to vary, both models had the same pattern of factor loadings over time (as indicated by good model fit at steps 1a and 1b of Table 1). In contrast to the longitudinal correlated factors model with positive loadings on each factor, the specific internalizing factor in the longitudinal bifactor model included both positive and negative loadings that represented mainly high distress and low fear (see Table 2 for the unconstrained factor loadings). In addition to the invariant pattern of loadings over time, the magnitude of loadings was also invariant in both the correlated factors and the bifactor model (as indicated by the ΔCFI value of <0.01 at steps 2a and 2b).
Table 2 Unconstrained factor loadings (standard errors in parentheses) for the longitudinal correlated factors model and the longitudinal bifactor model.
Because the data were comprised of binary measures indicating the presence or absence of a psychiatric disorder, each disorder had a single measurement occasion-specific threshold that was a direct function of its prevalence rate. The occasion-specific thresholds were, therefore, equal in the bifactor and the correlated factors model. Adding threshold constraints did not significantly worsen the models’ fits (steps 3a and 3b), meaning that threshold invariance was supported for both models. The bifactor model and correlated factors model thus exhibited an equal degree of strong longitudinal invariance. The fully constrained model parameters for both the models are presented in Table 3.
Table 3 Constrained factor loadings (standard errors in parentheses) and disorder prevalence for the longitudinal correlated factors model and the longitudinal bifactor model.
The bifactor model parameters presented in Table 3 were obtained from a model that included only homotypic factor correlations over time. A bifactor model with heterotypic factor correlations was also fitted (for correlations, see Supplementary Material). The inclusion of heterotypic factor correlations had no effect on model fit, and the more parsimonious bifactor model with only homotypic correlations was preferred. Furthermore, examining the correlations of the specific internalizing factor was considered not meaningful because the factor was not well defined. The results of invariance tests were the same regardless of whether the longitudinal bifactor model included heterotypic correlations or not.
Factor Structure of Change in Psychopathology
Parallel analysis indicated three underlying factors also in the change score data. A correlated three-factor model and a bifactor model with two specific factors were first estimated using EFA and then using CFA (for factor loadings, see Supplementary Tables 7 and 8). In CFA, the bifactor model demonstrated acceptable model fit (CFI = 0.959, TLI = 0.935, RMSEA = 0.025), whereas the fit of the correlated factors was poor (CFI = 0.903, TLI = 0.874, RMSEA = 0.035). The general factor of change exhibited acceptable construct reliability (H = 0.72), but other latent factors in the bifactor model or in the correlated factors model were not reliably defined in change score data (H range 0.46–0.67). A further inspection of correlations among the disorders revealed considerable positive correlations between disorders belonging to different factors of the correlated factors model, showing that the diagnostic statuses changed in the same direction across the fear, distress, and externalizing factors (see Supplementary Figure 1 for the correlations).
The model fit difference between the bifactor and the correlated factors model was more notable in the change score relative to the cross-sectional data (ΔCFI = 0.056, ΔTLI = 0.061, and ΔRMSEA = 0.010 in the change-score analysis compared to a ΔCFI = 0.011, ΔTLI = 0.011, and ΔRMSEA = 0.005 in the cross-sectional analysis; see Supplementary Table 9 for the comparison of model fit indices). Further examination of factor congruence coefficients (26) indicated that the specific internalizing factor in the bifactor model had a very low degree of congruence (ϕ = 0.18) between change score and cross-sectional data, suggesting that the pattern of specific internalizing factor loadings was different between the datasets. In comparison, the general factor and the specific externalizing factors were congruent across change score and cross-sectional data (ϕ = 0.98 and ϕ = 0.99, respectively).
To facilitate interpretation of the factors via post hoc analysis, we further investigated the bifactor model of disorder change scores by analyzing the factors’ demographic correlates and their associations with disorder onset, recovery, and recurrence. First, we regressed the factors on respondent age and gender within a single structural equation model. Higher age was associated with decreases in the general factor (β = −0.075, p = 0.007) and externalization factor (β = −0.171, p < 0.001). Gender was not associated with any of the latent factors for changes. Next, counts of disorder onsets and recoveries between T1 and T2 were calculated for each respondent and categorized into six levels from 0 = no change in disorder status to 5 = onset or recovery from five or more disorders. A count of disorder recurrence was also calculated and categorized into four levels from 0 = no recurring disorders to 3 = recurrence of three or more disorders. Analysis of variance indicated significant differences (p < 0.001) with large effect sizes in the predicted general change factor scores between different frequencies of disorder onset and recovery (effect size estimate η² = 0.39 for onset and η² = 0.42 for recovery). In a dose–response manner, the general change factor score increased as the number of disorder onsets grew larger and decreased as the number of disorder recoveries grew larger (Table 4, Figure 2). There were no significant differences in the general change factor scores by increasing levels of disorder recurrence, nor did the specific internalizing and externalizing change factor scores differ considerably across levels of disorder onset and recovery (data available upon request).
Table 4 General psychopathology factor extracted from change score data: Mean (M) factor scores and standard deviations (SD) by the number of disorders with recovery, disorders with onset, and disorders with recurrence.
Discussion
This study tested the structural stability (i.e., longitudinal invariance) of a bifactor model of psychiatric comorbidity that includes a general psychopathology factor, as well as examined the models’ generalizability to change processes in psychopathology. The findings were compared to a correlated factors model that explains psychiatric comorbidity with correlated underlying factors but has no general psychopathology factor. Both the bifactor model and the correlated factors model displayed strong structural stability over time, meaning that the relationships between psychiatric disorders and the modeled latent factors remained stable over the 10-year period in adulthood. However, the bifactor model explained the observed structure of temporal changes in disorder status better than the correlated factors model. To our knowledge, these findings show for the first time that the general psychopathology factor’s meaning stays rather immutable over time and that it can be used to model development of and recovery from psychopathology in adulthood.
We found that individual differences in intraindividual patterns of change in psychopathology were characterized by a rather similar bifactor structure as the cross-sectional individual differences. Furthermore, the observed structure of temporal changes in disorder status supported the bifactor model over the traditional correlated factors model, suggesting that change in psychopathology results from one general liability factor to which specific, or “pure,” internalizing and externalizing tendencies give color, rather than from correlated primary internalizing and externalizing tendencies. The strength of the bifactor model thus appears to lie in its ability to separate general psychopathology from the specific residual factors, making it better-suited than the correlated factors model to describe the structure of changes in psychiatric disorders.
The present findings contribute to the recent discussion on the utility of the general psychopathology factor. The findings that the bifactor structure is stable and fits change data relatively better than the correlated factors model speak against the notion of general psychopathology factor being only a statistical artifact (9, 10). The results are also in line with previous evidence supporting the validity and utility of the general factor (27). The general factor robustly correlates with external variables such as demographic factors, negative emotionality, and cognitive ability (1, 28), and higher scores on the factor predict future psychopathology and life impairment (2, 3). Prior work has also demonstrated that the best-fitting structural model for genetic risk factors for psychopathology includes a general genetic factor (6). The general psychopathology factor, therefore, reflects the influence of genetic risk factors that are shared by multiple disorders, although this does not automatically implicate a pure genetic etiology (7, 29, 30). Our finding that the bifactor structure describes changes in disorder status suggest that there is important within-individual variation in the general psychopathology risk as well.
Once the general psychopathology factor was included in the factor structure, the specific internalizing factor was not reliably specified, and some of its the loadings became nonsignificant and/or negative. For example, in our cross-sectional examination of the bifactor model, the specific internalizing factor represented either high distress or high fear, depending on the measurement occasion. Correspondingly, we also observed diminishing general factor loadings among disorders reflecting distress and increasing loadings among disorders reflecting fear over time—a pattern similar to that reported previously (13). Prior work has documented similar anomalous findings also with regard to the subfactors of specific externalizing (28). The results are not that surprising given that the specific factors are residualized factors, and such anomalous results, in which the loadings on a specific factor differ strongly, are generally not uncommon when applying bifactor models (31). As was evident, longitudinal models hold the potential to resolve some of the technical weaknesses of bifactor models, as the loading indeterminacy of cross-sectional analyses got resolved in longitudinal modeling that implies drastically higher degrees of freedom. When examined longitudinally, the specific internalizing factor loadings were rather similar across both measurements and represented high distress and low fears.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the longitudinal invariance of the bifactor model of comorbidity and to evaluate the generalizability of structural comorbidity models to change processes in psychopathology. Several limitations should be considered when interpreting the results. First, although in terms of model fit indices, our results favored the bifactor model over the traditional correlated factors model, the question remains open as to when such differences become practically significant. Therefore, even though the findings support the bifactor conceptualization, they do not disprove the potential relevance of the correlated factors model. Second, the measurement of change in psychopathology was crude in that it did not reflect exact times of onset or recovery. Nevertheless, our change scores provided a simple-to-understand approach for examining change processes in psychopathology and were modeled as continuous-valued underlying changes using the liability-threshold model, which is the standard approach in structural equation modeling of ordinal-valued data. Third, we used DSM-III-R and DSM-IV rather than DSM-5 diagnoses, which means that the generalizability of the results to the most recent diagnostic criteria might be limited. Fourth, while the sample was representative of the general US population, the results may not generalize to populations outside the USA. Fifth, due to limited data on psychotic disorders, we could not extend the structural models to include a thought disorder factor that has been found in some other datasets (32, 33). Future studies covering a wider range of psychiatric disorders, and perhaps also pathological personality traits (7), are warranted to confirm and extend our findings.
In conclusion, these findings highlight the utility of the bifactor model in understanding the structure of psychiatric comorbidity. The bifactor structure of psychopathology is stable over time, and both the cross-sectional pattern and the pattern of change in psychiatric comorbidity follow a bifactor structure. The finding that the structure with a general psychopathology factor describes both cross-sectional differences between individuals and differences in how individuals change suggests that the general psychopathology factor may represent a transdiagnostic entity that can both trigger and dissolve a range of psychiatric disorders. The general psychopathology factor implies that shared psychopathological mechanisms operate across multiple mental disorders. By identifying these shared mechanisms, it may be possible to develop interventions that focus on the common processes rather than diagnosis-specific symptoms. Emerging evidence suggests that such transdiagnostic treatment protocols may be as efficient as diagnosis-specific treatments (34). Future research on therapeutic change could benefit from using the bifactor model to disentangle the effects of both general and specific therapeutic processes (35).
Data Availability
The dataset analyzed for this study is publicly available at the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research website (www.icpsr.umich.edu).
Author Contributions
TR, MJ, and KG developed the study concept. KG performed the data analysis and drafted the paper, and TR and MJ provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the paper for submission.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This study was financially supported by the Emil Aaltonen Foundation. MJ was also supported by the Academy of Finland (project 311578).
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00594/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, et al. The p factor. Clin Psychol Sci (2014) 2(2):119–37. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473
2. Lahey BB, Applegate B, Hakes JK, Zald DH, Hariri AR, Rathouz PJ. Is there a general factor of prevalent psychopathology during adulthood? J Abnorm Psychol (2012) 121(4):971–7. doi: 10.1037/a0028355
3. Patalay P, Fonagy P, Deighton J, Belsky J, Vostanis P, Wolpert M. A general psychopathology factor in early adolescence. Br J Psychiatry (2015) 207(01):15–22. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.149591
4. Lahey BB, Zald DH, Perkins SF, Villalta-Gil V, Werts KB, Van Hulle CA, et al. Measuring the hierarchical general factor model of psychopathology in young adults. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res (2018) 27(1):e1593. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1593
5. Lahey BB, Krueger RF, Rathouz PJ, Waldman ID, Zald DH. A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychol Bull (2017) 143(2):142–86. doi: 10.1037/bul0000069
6. Pettersson E, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P. Common psychiatric disorders share the same genetic origin: a multivariate sibling study of the Swedish population. Mol Psychiatry (2016) 21(5):717–21. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.116
7. Rosenström T, Gjerde LC, Krueger RF, Aggen SH, Czajkowski NO, Gillespie NA, et al. Joint factorial structure of psychopathology and personality. Psychol Med (2018) In Press: 1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718002982
8. van Bork R, Epskamp S, Rhemtulla M, Borsboom D, van der Maas HLJ. What is the p -factor of psychopathology? Some risks of general factor modeling. Theory Psychol (2017) 27(6):759–73. doi: 10.1177/0959354317737185
9. Bonifay W, Cai L. On the complexity of item response theory models. Multivariate Behav Res (2017) 52(4):465–84. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2017.1309262
10. Bonifay W, Lane SP, Reise SP. Three concerns with applying a bifactor model as a structure of psychopathology. Clin Psychol Sci (2017) 5(1):184–6. doi: 10.1177/2167702616657069
11. McElroy E, Belsky J, Carragher N, Fearon P, Patalay P. Developmental stability of general and specific factors of psychopathology from early childhood to adolescence: dynamic mutualism or p-differentiation? J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2018) 59(6):667–675. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12849
12. Snyder HR, Young JF, Hankin BL. Strong homotypic continuity in common psychopathology-, internalizing-, and externalizing-specific factors over time in adolescents. Clin Psychol Sci (2017) 5(1):98–110. doi: 10.1177/2167702616651076
13. Greene AL, Eaton NR. The temporal stability of the bifactor model of comorbidity: an examination of moderated continuity pathways. Compr Psychiatry (2017) 72:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.09.010
14. Molenaar PCM, Campbell CG. The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Curr Dir Psychol Sci (2009) 18(2):112–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x
15. Kessler RC, Cox BJ, Green JG, Ormel J, McLaughlin KA, Merikangas KR, et al. The effects of latent variables in the development of comorbidity among common mental disorders. Depress Anxiety (2011) 28(1):29–39. doi: 10.1002/da.20760
16. Kessler RC. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1994) 51(1):8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002
17. Robins LN, Wing J, Wittchen HU, Helzer JE, Babor TF, Burke J, et al. The composite international diagnostic interview. an epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1988) 45(12):1069–77. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800360017003
18. Kessler RC, Ustün TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). Int J Methods Psychiatr Res (2004) 13(2):93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168
19. Garrido LE, Abad FJ, Ponsoda V. A new look at Horn’s parallel analysis with ordinal variables. Psychol Methods (2013) 18(4):454–74. doi: 10.1037/a0030005
20. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model A Multidiscip J (1999) 6(1):1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118
21. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model A Multidiscip J (2002) 9(2):233–55. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5
22. Hancock GR, Mueller RO. Rethinking construct reliability within latent variable systems. In: Cudeck R, du Toit S, Sorbom D, editors. Structural equation modeling: present and future. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International (2001). p. 195–216.
24. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model A Multidiscip J (2007) 14(3):464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834
25. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 4th ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2016).
26. Lorenzo-Seva U, Berge JMF. Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology (2006) 2(2):57–64. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.2.2.57
27. Lahey BB, Krueger RF, Rathouz PJ, Waldman ID, Zald DH. Validity and utility of the general factor of psychopathology. World Psychiatry (2017) 16(2):142–4. doi: 10.1002/wps.20410
28. Castellanos-Ryan N, Brière FN, O’Leary-Barrett M, Banaschewski T, Bokde A, Bromberg U, et al. The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates. J Abnorm Psychol (2016) 125(8):1039–52. doi: 10.1037/abn0000193
29. Turkheimer E, Pettersson E, Horn EE. A phenotypic null hypothesis for the genetics of personality. Annu Rev Psychol (2014) 65(1):515–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143752
30. Briley DA, Livengood J, Derringer J, Tucker-Drob EM, Fraley RC, Roberts BW. Interpreting behavior genetic models: seven developmental processes to understand. Behav Genet (2018) 140(5):1303–31. doi: 10.1007/s10519-018-9939-6
31. Eid M, Geiser C, Koch T, Heene M. Anomalous results in G-factor models: explanations and alternatives. Psychol Methods (2017) 22(3):541–62. doi: 10.1037/met0000083
32. Keyes KM, Eaton NR, Krueger RF, Skodol AE, Wall MM, Grant B, et al. Thought disorder in the meta-structure of psychopathology. Psychol Med (2013) 43(08):1673–83. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002292
33. Markon KE. Modeling psychopathology structure: a symptom-level analysis of Axis I and II disorders. Psychol Med (2010) 40(02):273. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990183
34. Barlow DH, Farchione TJ, Bullis JR, Gallagher MW, Murray-Latin H, Sauer-Zavala S, et al. The unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders compared with diagnosis-specific protocols for anxiety disorders. JAMA Psychiatry (2017) 74(9):875. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2164
Keywords: general psychopathology factor, p factor, comorbidity, bifactor model, invariance
Citation: Gluschkoff K, Jokela M and Rosenström T (2019) The General Psychopathology Factor: Structural Stability and Generalizability to Within-Individual Changes. Front. Psychiatry 10:594. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00594
Received: 05 February 2019; Accepted: 26 July 2019;
Published: 30 August 2019.
Edited by:
Danny Horesh, Bar-Ilan University, IsraelReviewed by:
Hannah R. Snyder, Brandeis University, United StatesElizabeth Long, Virginia Commonwealth University, United States
Copyright © 2019 Gluschkoff, Jokela and Rosenström. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Kia Gluschkoff, kia.gluschkoff@helsinki.fi