Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 June 2019
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Addiction and Attachment View all 23 articles

Frustration Tolerance and Personality Traits in Patients With Substance Use Disorders

  • 1Mind-Brain Group, Institute for Culture and Society, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 2Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 3Department of Health Sciences, Public University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 4Department of Psychobiology and Research Center for Mind, Brain, and Behavior (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain

Previous research has suggested the prevalence of certain personality traits, some of which are related to a disorganized attachment, in substance abuse disorders. Further, frustration tolerance (FT) has been proposed as an important factor in addiction, both at the inception—following the “self-medication” hypothesis—and regarding treatment compliance. In turn, an inadequate response to frustrating events has been also associated with a disrupted attachment. Our goal is to explore the mediational role of FT in the relationship between personality traits and two different treatments for substance addiction: therapeutic community (TC) and ambulatory treatment (AT). Eighty-four subjects with substance abuse disorder were recruited in total (22 female), including 46 volunteers (13 female) in TC and 38 (9 female) in AT. They were assessed with Rosenzweig’s test for FT and the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III) test to evaluate personality factors. By comparing with a control sample (335 volunteers, 268 female), we found that FT was lower in patients. Between therapeutic groups, FT was significantly lower in TC. Depressive, antisocial, sadistic, negativistic, schizotypal, borderline, paranoid, anxiety, dysthymia, alcohol use, drug use, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), thought disorder, and delusional disorder traits were suggestive of pathology in the clinical samples and were significantly different between control, AT, and TC groups. Further, anxiety and PTSD traits were higher in TC than in AT. A mediational analysis revealed that the effect of anxiety and PTSD scales on therapeutic group was partially mediated by FT. In conclusion, FT and its interplay with personality traits commonly related to disorganized attachment (anxiety and PTSD) might be important factors to consider within therapeutic programs for persons with substance addiction.

Introduction

Drug addiction withdrawal implies a set of physiological and psychological challenges that should be faced by the patient and taken into account by the therapist (13). Physiological alterations appear a few hours after the cessation of drug administration, do not extend longer than 3 weeks (1), and can be alleviated by pharmacological treatment (4). However, psychological challenges are more persistent (5), and pharmacological intervention is not always effective to treat them (6, 7). Frustration is one of the common negative emotions involved in withdrawal (818), and its experience during detoxification significantly contributes to relapse and treatment discontinuation (7). Moreover, following the self-medication hypothesis, it has been proposed that illicit-drug consumption—and subsequent addiction—could be used as a means of alleviating negative emotions such as frustration (19).

Taking this into account, the ability of the patient to tolerate frustrating events might be an important factor in substance addiction, both in the development of the disorder and during treatment. Frustration is defined as a negative emotional response triggered after the omission and/or devaluation of an expected reward (20). Animal research has extensively demonstrated the influence of this negative emotion on substance abuse [see, for example, Ref. (21)]. Concerning humans, frustration tolerance (FT) is negatively associated with the number of relapses (22) and positively predicts recovery from alcoholism (23, 24). It is also an essential component of the complex construct of distress tolerance (25), which is, in turn, an important factor of withdrawal (2628).

Previous studies have suggested the attachment hypothesis of addiction as an alternative to self-medication. Attachment is defined in developmental psychology as the emotional tie that connects the child with his or her main caregiver (29, 30). In her first works, Mary Ainsworth described three main types of attachment, namely, secure, anxious insecure, and avoidant insecure. Nowadays, the most common classification includes organized (secure, insecure–avoidant, or insecure–resistant) and insecure–disorganized attachment types (31). Within organized attachment, insecure children are hesitant to rely on their main caregiver in distress situations, due to a prior unreliable response from the adult. They are considered organized because children can develop strategies to handle stressful situations. In contrast, children with a disorganized attachment show a fearful, conflicted, or apprehensive behavior when reunited with their caregivers after distress (32). It is important to note that a disorganized attachment may co-exist with any of the organized subtypes, mostly insecure (33). In adults, insecure attachment is mostly manifested in enhanced avoidant and anxious behaviors. Like children, adults expect unresponsive peers in stressful situations, leading them to avoid close relationships or to desire proximity but lacking trust in their partners (32). Current research has proposed that an insecure (disorganized) attachment may be a risk factor for developing substance addiction (3436). Moreover, a disorganized mother–child attachment can be associated with poorer FT in children (37, 38), and adults with insecure attachment are prone to show problems coping with emotion regulation (39, 40). Therefore, since FT is a relevant factor for both the self-medication hypothesis of addiction and disorganized attachment (which in turn may be a risk factor for developing substance addiction), the assessment of FT in patients with substance use disorders may help bring together both fields of research.

Interestingly, other authors have highlighted the influence of personality traits on addiction and attachment. Magor-Blatch and collaborators (41) explored pathological symptoms and clinical syndromes, assessed by the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III) (42), in a large sample of amphetamine-type substance users. They found scores in the pathological range in anxiety, bipolar, borderline, dependent, narcissistic, negativistic, and sadistic scales, although these values did not predict completion of the therapeutic program. Fernandez-Montalvo et al. (43) found that over 76% of patients in therapeutic community (TC) had a pathological personality, antisocial being the most prevalent trait (43%). Other studies report a significant presence of antisocial, anxiety, depressive, borderline, bipolar, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) traits in patients with substance dependency (4447). Psychopathology is understood under the scope of attachment theory as an adaptive resource to compensate for an insecure attachment (48). Previous research has demonstrated a relationship between certain elements of a dysfunctional attachment (unresolved loss and unresolved trauma) and personality (49). Using the Adult Attachment Interview, these authors found that borderline and anxiety traits were higher in trauma inpatients with unresolved attachment. Furthermore, within the scope of the self-medication hypothesis of addiction, antisocial personality was described as a mediator of the relationship between alcohol consumption and parental bonds in male college students (50). Besides, other authors found a relationship between anxiety and insecure attachment styles in alcohol-addicted inpatients: anxiety traits were significantly higher in participants with insecure, compared with secure, attachment (51). Beyond the scope of addictive disorders, a disorganized attachment has been associated with higher values of paranoid, borderline, and histrionic traits in adults (52).

The present study intends to contribute to this field of research by evaluating personality traits and FT in subjects included in two different therapeutic approaches: TC and ambulatory treatment (AT). To the best of our knowledge, our research is the first to explore FT and personality traits in both types of treatment, since previous works have focused on TCs (11, 14, 22, 41, 5355). Although we do not measure attachment directly, we aim to evaluate the association between the type of withdrawal program (TC and AT) (which is an indirect indicator of addiction severity and risk of social exclusion) (56), the presence of pathological personality traits that have been previously related to a disorganized attachment (i.e., antisocial, anxiety, PTSD, borderline) [see, for example, Ref. (57)], and the role of FT as a mediating factor between them.

Considering this, the objectives of our research are: 1) to evaluate FT in TC and AT groups; 2) to explore the personality traits of the whole clinical sample under study; 3) to compare personality traits between therapeutic groups, focusing on those previously related to disorganized attachment; and 4) to explore whether the influence of personality on treatment group is mediated by FT. A deeper knowledge of negative psychological emotions involved in withdrawal, such as frustration, and its mediation in the relationship between personality and treatment, may help improve these therapeutic programs and their probability of success.

Methods

Participants

Participants under treatment for substance abuse were recruited through an advertisement made by the therapists of Asociación Proyecto Hombre in the centers of Navarra and Granada. All volunteers signed an informed consent, and the protocol was approved by the Committee for Ethics in Research of the University of Navarra. Data were collected between 2015 and 2016. Proyecto Hombre verified, by means of urine tests, that participants were not consuming any drug—other than tobacco—during the 2 weeks prior to their psychological assessment. The total number of participants was 84 patients (22 female) from both centers. Age ranged from 20 to 63 years (40.06 ± 1.10, mean ± standard error of the mean), and it was not significantly different between male (39.31 ± 1.17) and female (42.18 ± 2.58) participants: Student’s t(82) = −1.156, p = 0.251. Concerning the therapeutic program, 46 participants (13 female) were in TC, whereas 38 received AT (9 female). The mean age of these groups was also similar: age(TC) = 38.70 ± 1.54, age(AT) = 41.71 ± 1.53; t(82) = −1.377, p = 0.172. A chi-square test of independence showed that gender composition was not different between therapeutic groups (χ2(1) = 0.225, p = 0.635). At the time of assessment, the duration of treatment was similar in both groups: TC, 4.12 ± 0.38 months; AT, 4.80 ± 0.34 months, t(82) = 1.317, p = 0.192. We did not collect information about psychopharmacological medication prescribed to volunteers (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Characteristics of the clinical and control samples included in the study.

Inclusion criteria for the participants were: i) fulfilling the diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) for substance dependence, substance abuse, and substance withdrawal; ii) participation in TC or AT for at least 2 weeks before data collection; and iii) presenting physical or psychological harm and/or dependence due to substance consumption in the past.

In order to assess FT in substance abuse with respect to the general population, we recruited a control sample (N = 335, 268 female), which included community controls (next of kin of the patients) and university students. Exclusion criteria were: 1) alcohol or substance abuse or dependence, as assessed by the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) and Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST) tests; 2) self-reported previous treatment for substance abuse dependence; and 3) self-reported history of neurological or mental disorders. All control participants signed the informed consent to take part in the study. This sample was not matched in terms of sex and age with the clinical group. Thus, in the Results section, we present three different strategies to compare FT between both samples. In addition, a subsample of 76 participants from the initial control sample was assessed with the MCMI-III questionnaire (see below and Table 1 for details).

Procedure and Therapeutic Strategies

Asociación Proyecto Hombre is a Spanish institution for treating and preventing addiction, involving 27 centers and 16,600 persons under treatment annually. They work in three fundamental areas: prevention, rehabilitation, and reintegration of former users (www.proyectohombre.es). The tests were conducted in the centers of Proyecto Hombre in the Spanish provinces of Granada and Navarra. Among others, they offer TC and AT therapeutic strategies. The former is inpatient, and the latter is outpatient, involving 1.5–2 h of treatment 5 days a week (Monday to Friday). Patients are assigned to either program depending on their preferences, as well as on the therapists’ recommendation based on their social situation. It is important to note that the assignment to TC points to a more compromised social and/or pathological situation, since inclusion in TC is related to the fact that patients do not or cannot have employment, or they do not have enough social support to tackle withdrawal treatment by themselves (56). The most common profile of AT patients, however, is a jobholder supported by family and acquaintances. In spite of this, therapeutic strategy is the same in both programs: group therapy for psychological and emotional aspects such as problem solving, anxiety control, emotional regulation, or relapse prevention, for instance. The only difference, apart from the number of hours spent daily in the program, is the emphasis on achieving personal and social autonomy in TC. Although the total time spent in the program depends on each person, TC treatment usually lasts 6–10 months, and AT is maintained for at least 1 year. Participants were allowed to smoke tobacco before assessment, so nicotine withdrawal is not expected to influence the study.

All data were anonymized and coded for each participant. First, the MCMI-III test was administered for approximately 45 min. Then, subjects were evaluated with the Rosenzweig test for about 25 min. After a short break in the same room, participants were screened for substance use, abuse, dependency, and damage. This procedure was also followed by the control sample, although the MCMI-III test was administered only to a subsample of 76 participants, including university students and community controls.

Questionnaires

Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III). This test (42) is used for testing clinical personality patterns (11 scales), severe personality pathology (3 scales), clinical syndromes (7 scales), and severe clinical syndromes (3 scales). It consists of 175 true/false items, and it also contains 3 control scales. This inventory identifies personality features underlying symptoms, and it is commonly used to assist clinicians in diagnosis and for therapy selection. According to the standards (58), scores between 60 and 74 are suggestive of symptoms at a subclinical level, values of 75–85 indicate presence and prevalence of the pathology or syndrome, and scores over 85 point to prominence of the pathology or syndrome.

Rosenzweig Picture Frustration Test, PFT. This is a semi-structured projective test to assess tolerance or intolerance to a frustrating situation (59). It consists of 24 vignettes where pairs of characters are interacting. The subject assumes the role of one of the characters and provides his or her expected behavior in that situation. The test provides an FT index that is calculated through a simple procedure: the response for each vignette is scored by two different raters (from 0 to 2) considering the degree of aggression, avoidance, blocking, or coping (2 = aggressive response, 0 = non-emotional response). In our study, inter-rater reliability of this assessment was very high (average intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.943). In spite of the high reliability, the final score of each vignette was agreed upon in case of a mismatch between the raters. Higher scores indicate lower FT (higher level of aggressive response).

Substance use, abuse, and dependence. All participants were screened with a 13-item test to identify the substances they used (in the case of patients, before joining the therapeutic program) (alcohol, ecstasy, heroin, speed, cocaine, caffeine, tobacco, cannabis, hallucinogens, tranquilizers, and marijuana). The two last items indicated substance dependency and physical or psychological harm due to substance consumption. This test is based on the ASSIST 3.0, published by the World Health Organization (WHO) (60).

Subjects were also screened with the AUDIT (WHO) in its Spanish version (61).

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed in Stata 12.1. We present three different strategies to compare FT between both samples: 1) unpaired t test including all subjects (clinical vs. control sample); 2) multiple regression to predict FT (dependent variable) from group (case = 1, control = 0), controlling by age and sex (male = 1, female = 0); and 3) randomly choosing age- and sex-matched subsamples of both groups with MedCalc (Ostend, Belgium) and then comparing FT with an unpaired t test. In order to achieve matching, the software randomly selected the maximum number of cases within group that produced non-significant differences between groups for age and sex. Therefore, matches were not necessarily exact. A Shapiro–Wilk test was conducted to assess normality. For normally distributed samples, means and standard errors of the mean are reported; for non-normal distributions, we report medians and interquartile ranges (IQRs). Comparisons between therapeutic groups were carried out with parametric t tests, due to sample sizes larger than 30. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to test the omnibus differences in personality traits between therapeutic groups. The effect of covariates (i.e., duration of treatment) was assessed with analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) and a MANOVA, respectively.

In order to assess the mediational role of FT on the influence between personality traits and therapeutic group, we performed mediational analyses as described by MacKinnon and Dwyer (62), and as explained by Kenny (63). We used the command binary_mediation available in Stata’s repositories. In our case, we wanted to test whether the relationship between certain personality traits (for example, anxiety) and the assignment to a particular therapeutic group (TC or AT) was influenced by FT. In short, the analysis explores first the total effect of the independent variable (i.e., anxiety personality trait) on the dependent variable (i.e., inclusion in either AT or TC), in this case with a logistic regression (AT = 0, TC = 1). Then, the total effect is divided into the direct effect (the influence of the independent variable, i.e., anxiety) on the dependent variable (AT or TC) controlling for the mediator (FT) and the indirect effect (the influence of the independent variable, i.e., anxiety) on the mediator (FT), and the effect of the mediator (FT) on the dependent variable (AT or TC); if the direct effect is negligible and the other tests are significant, the mediational role of the variable may be assumed. Practically, to fully assess the mediational role of a variable, four steps must be fulfilled: 1) the independent variable (i.e., anxiety) must have an effect on the dependent variable (treatment: TC = 1 or AT = 0); 2) the independent variable must have a significant effect on the mediational variable (FT as assessed by the PFT); 3) the mediational variable must have a significant effect on the dependent variable when controlling for the independent variable; and 4) the direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable, when controlling for the mediational variable, must be null (total mediation), or at least lower than when the mediational variable is considered (partial mediation).

Step 1 is assessed by a logistic regression and gives the total effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable (coefficient c). Step 2 is evaluated by a linear regression, taking FT as the dependent variable and the personality trait as predictor, and it provides coefficient a. Steps 3 and 4 are assessed with a logistic regression, including treatment as the dependent variable and the personality trait and FT as predictors. The influence of FT on treatment (controlling by personality trait) is coefficient b in the mediational model, and the effect of the personality trait on treatment (controlling for FT) is coefficient c’.

Results

Frustration Tolerance

Overall, the whole clinical sample (AT and TC) showed a score of 16 ± 6.5 (median ± IQR; Shapiro–Wilk p = 0.0235) in the Rosenzweig test (Figure 1). This is considered a medium FT according to the standards, as defined by Rosenzweig and based on the Dollard et al. theory of frustration–aggression (64), and points to an avoidant or blocking behavior when facing frustrating situations, masking a desire for aggression that the subject intentionally conceals (65). Note that higher scores on this test indicate a lower FT.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Box plots showing differences in frustration tolerance (FT) between clinical and control samples, as well as between therapeutic programs. (A) Rosenzweig Picture Frustration Test (PFT) scores in the unmatched clinical and control samples. (B), median scores for the MedCalc-matched samples. (C) Rosenzweig PFT scores in therapeutic community and ambulatory treatment. Note that high values indicate poor FT.

The control sample showed lower values on average (8 ± 8, Shapiro–Wilk p < 0.0001) (Figure 1), which reflect a strategic resolution of the frustrating vignettes based on social skills, empathy, and assertiveness. The comparison between these samples, unmatched for sex and age, yielded statistically significant results: t(417) = −9.99, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.22, pointing to a better FT in controls than in the clinical sample. In order to test whether this result was due to the difference in age (control, 21.09 ± 0.32 yr; clinical sample, 40.06 ± 1.1 yr) or sex composition (control, 80% female; clinical sample, 26.2%), we performed a linear regression to predict FT values from group, sex, and age. A significant regression equation was found [F(3,415) = 33.178, p < 0.001, ηp² = 0.19], with a corrected R2 = 0.188. The only significant regressor was group (p(group) < 0.001, p(sex) = 0.695, p(age) = 0.964), controlling by sex and age. Participants’ predicted FT was 9.18 + 7.10(group), where group was coded as 0 = control, 1 = clinical sample. These results were confirmed by randomly selecting subsamples for each group with MedCalc (Ncontrol = 26, 20 female; Nclinical = 29, 21 female; age(control) = 36.31 ± 2.37; age(clinical) = 35.55 ± 2.16). In this case, we used non-parametric statistics since N < 30: Mann–Whitney U = 63.5, z = -5.295, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.815. In these subsamples, PFT was 10 ± 5 for controls and 18 ± 6 for the clinical sample (Figure 1).

In conclusion, as hypothesized, FT was lower in the clinical group than in the control sample, as measured by Rosenzweig’s PFT.

With respect to therapeutic groups, PFT values were higher in TC (18.22 ± 0.95, mean ± SEM; Shapiro–Wilk p = 0.0912) than in AT (13.34 ± 0.98; Shapiro–Wilk p = 0.0645) (Figure 1). As explained in the Methods section, these samples were matched for sex and age. A parametric t test confirmed statistical differences: t(82) = −3.54, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.78. Therefore, in accordance with our hypothesis, FT was lower in TC than in AT. Even though treatment duration was similar between groups, we performed an ANCOVA to confirm the difference between TC and AT in FT controlling for treatment duration (in months). As expected, between-group differences held when controlling by treatment duration at the time of assessment [F(1,25) = 1.83, p = 0.0302, ηp² = 0.07].

Personality Factors and Treatment Groups

We explored personality traits in TC and AT. Scores from the MCMI-III were processed as described by Millon et al. (58). All patients were assessed with this tool, whereas it was restricted to a subsample of 76 controls. Considering all the scales, Cronbach’s α was 0.90, showing a high internal consistency. With respect to individual subscales, values ranged from 0.9509 (borderline) to 0.9556 (narcissist).

In order to analyze the clinical relevance of each personality trait, we focused on those that were significantly different between groups (control, AT, and TC) and with a median equal to or above 60 for at least one of the clinical groups. Thus, we performed an ANOVA for each scale, using Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (p = 0.05/24 = 0.0021). Depressive, antisocial, sadistic, negativistic, schizotypal, borderline, paranoid, anxiety, dysthymia, alcohol use, drug use, PTSD, thought disorder, and delusional disorder traits fulfilled these criteria (Table 2). Due to the possible clinical relevance of these 14 scales, we focused on them for subsequent analysis.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 MCMI-III personality traits in the control, therapeutic community, and ambulatory treatment samples.

In order to assess whether these personality traits were different between therapeutic groups, we performed a MANOVA with the MCMI scales as dependent variables and treatment group as an independent variable. There was a statistically significant difference between treatment groups in MCMI scales, F(14, 69) = 2.06, p = 0.025, Wilks’ lambda = 0.705. As post hoc analyses, we performed two-tailed independent t tests for each variable, assuming a critical p value of 0.0036 (i.e., 0.05/14). Anxiety [t(82) = −3.42, p = 0.001; Hedges g = 0.743) and PTSD (t(82) = −3.23, p = 0.0018; Hedges g = 0.703) survived this threshold (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Between-group (clinical samples) differences in the MCMI-III personality traits suggestive of symptoms at a subclinical or clinical level.

Influence of Personality Traits and Frustration Tolerance on Treatment Group

We hypothesized that FT may act as a mediational variable in the relationship between pathological personality traits and treatment group, which is an indicator of addiction severity. In order to test mediation, we followed the recommendations by Kenny (63), which assume the fulfillment of the four steps described in the Methods section of the present manuscript. We independently evaluated the effect of each scale suggestive of pathology (the 14 traits mentioned in the previous section) on treatment (TC = 1, AT = 0). Hence, we followed an approach suitable for dichotomous outcomes (66). Since we performed several different analyses, the critical p value was Bonferroni-corrected (p = 0.05/14 = 0.0036).

As expected from the results of the previous section, only anxiety and PTSD survived Bonferroni correction when evaluating the influence of the independent variables (personality traits) on the dependent variable (treatment group) (Step 1): anxiety, χ2(1) = 10.91, p = 0.001; PTSD, χ2(1) = 9.80, p = 0.0017. These traits also influenced FT (Step 2), as assessed by a linear regression: anxiety, F(1,82) = 9.689, p = 0.003, ηp² = 0.106; PTSD, F(1,82) = 11.84, p = 0.0009, ηp² = 0.126. Both fulfilled Step 3, that is, PFT values significantly influenced treatment group when controlling for the corresponding personality trait (see standardized coefficients in Table 4): anxiety, BPTF = 0.107 ± 0.044, p = 0.015; PTSD, BPTF = 0.110 ± 0.044, p = 0.013. Finally, the influence of each personality trait on treatment group was reduced when controlling for FT, although they remained significant (Step 4; see Table 4 for details): Banxiety = 0.020 ± 0.009, p = 0.024; BPTSD = 0.023 ± 0.011, p = 0.037. The significance of direct and indirect effects in each case was tested using bootstrapping procedures on standardized coefficients (5,000 samples), and the bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals are reported in Table 3 (see also Figure 2).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Mediational analyses between personality traits, frustration tolerance, and treatment group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Mediation analyses between personality traits, FT, and treatment program (therapeutic community or ambulatory treatment). Personality traits are anxiety (A) and PTSD (B). Numbers are standardized regression coefficients. Paths from each personality trait to FT correspond to coefficient a; paths from FT to treatment correspond to coefficient b; and paths between personality traits and treatment are total effects (c) (in parentheses, direct effect (c’) when controlling for FT).

In other words, an increase of one standard deviation in anxiety and PTSD personality traits increases the odds of receiving TC treatment to 1.49 and 1.47, respectively. Part of this effect (27.7% in the case of anxiety and 32.1% for PTSD) was mediated by FT, suggesting the presence of hidden mediators.

Discussion

The present results show an association between personality traits, FT, and two different therapeutic strategies to overcome substance abuse: TC and AT. Concerning MCMI-III, there were scores suggestive of pathology and significant differences between the control and clinical samples in depressive, antisocial, sadistic, negativistic, schizotypal, borderline, paranoid, anxiety, dysthymia, alcohol use, drug use, PTSD, thought disorder, and delusional disorder scales. Regarding personality and addiction, a previous report (41) analyzed personality factors in TCs of substance users. They also found scores in the pathological range in drug and alcohol use, antisocial, sadistic, and anxiety traits, among others. Overall, MCMI-III scores were higher in their study, but the general organization of clinical personality and syndrome scales resembles our results. The globally lower scores of our participants may be due to the different nature of the samples included in both studies: whereas the present report included subjects addicted to alcohol, marijuana, cocaine, or benzodiazepines, that of Magor-Blatch et al. (41) was restricted to amphetamine-type prior users. Besides, we administered the MCMI-III test two weeks after the initiation of the program, whereas they assessed participants at the commencement of the therapy. Interestingly, the control sample assessed with the MCMI-III in our research showed relatively high values (≥60) in several subscales (histrionic, narcissist, antisocial, compulsive, bipolar, alcohol and drug use, and delusional disorder). Whereas antisocial, alcohol and drug use, and delusional disorder scores were significantly lower than in the clinical samples, the median value for histrionic and compulsive traits was significantly higher in control participants than in patients. The latter result is not surprising, since elevated scores in histrionic, narcissistic, and compulsive scales have been extensively reported for nonclinical groups, pointing to normal traits; moreover, they are associated with less severity of the disorder when present in clinical samples (67). With respect to the relatively high values in alcohol and substance use, the absence of dependence and harm indicators based on the AUDIT and ASSIST justifies the consideration of this sample as “control,” compared with the clinical samples.

One of the main goals of our research was to seek differences in personality factors between TC and AT groups. To our knowledge, our study is the first to compare personality patterns of substance users between both programs. Previous reports have focused on TCs (11, 14, 22, 41, 5355, 68), while studies on outpatients addressed low-intensity drug abuse such as tobacco (69, 70), alcohol, or cannabis (17). In our study, focusing on those scales with a putative pathological meaning (with scores ≥60) that showed significant differences between the three groups, patients in TC showed higher scores than those in AT in all of the traits that were statistically different between both clinical samples (see Table 3). Patients in TC are more vulnerable due to the higher severity of their condition and/or a risk of social exclusion; in the case of Proyecto Hombre, the institution that hosted the volunteers included in our study, the main reason to be included in TC is insufficient family or social support. This, together with a severe substance addiction disorder, may pose difficulties for the patient to find or retain a job, stable housing, and consequently solid ground to recover from his or her condition. Due to this, patients in TC are at a higher risk for social exclusion. Our results confirm significant differences in a number of scales, pointing to an overall pathological condition that should be considered during treatment. The subscales that showed significant differences were anxiety and PTSD, with medium-large effect sizes (Hedges g values above 0.80 are considered large effects) (71). Previous research suggests usual co-morbidity of cocaine addiction with psychiatric disorders such as major depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, PTSD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), anxiety, or borderline disorder (72). In fact, the European Monitoring Center for Drugs and Drug Addiction in 2015 stated that dual pathology and co-morbidity between drug addiction and mental disorders had risen up to 60%. According to our results, although we have not assessed co-morbidity per se, the higher scores in TC than in AT for most of the personality traits point to an enhanced vulnerability of patients in the former to develop mental disorders. This highlights the potential importance of a thorough personality profile before assigning a patient to either therapeutic program, as other authors suggest (6, 7).

With respect to negative emotional symptoms, our results show a lower FT in the clinical sample than in controls, and in TC compared with AT. The effect sizes of these results are quite large, indicating that about 96% of the matched control sample have better FT (lower scores) than the clinical sample, and about 79% of the TC sample have lower FT than the AT sample. Hence, this construct may be an important distinctive factor in substance abuse. Different stages of addiction, and relapse in particular, have been understood as a behavioral response to overcome the negative emotions (dysphoria, anxiety, irritability, etc.) that occur after the cessation of drug administration (73). Our result on the difference in FT between patients and controls may point to a crucial role in the development of addiction itself: in the presence of frustrating situations, when negative emotions are linked to a response of the pituitary–hypothalamus–adrenal axis (74), a poorer management of frustration may lead to its alleviation through the euphoric effects of recreational drugs. As has been recently suggested in children, negative emotions such as frustration may be regulated by the lateral prefrontal cortex (75). Moreover, a poor FT may trigger treatment discontinuation or relapse during withdrawal (22). We show here that patients in TC, where more frustrating situations are expected to occur due to the radical change in daily conditions, have a lower FT even when excluding the effect of treatment duration. Therefore, we suggest that a specific treatment for frustration intolerance may be useful in TCs. These results fit well with the “strength model” of self-regulation, which uses the metaphor of muscular exercise (and fatigue) to explain a decrease in self-control under stressful situations, leading to an “ego depletion” (extreme fatigue of self-control) (76, 77). In our study, FT would be a manifestation of self-control, which would be “depleted” in a greater extent in TC patients, due to the more severe conditions of the treatment and their lack of social support. As we propose below, future longitudinal studies could help clarify this potential inclusion of FT within the strength model of self-regulation.

Furthermore, we propose that clinical personality traits, mediated by a poor FT, may influence addiction severity, which is manifested in the type of treatment in which the patient is enrolled. Thus, a particular set of clinical personality features could be associated with a worse prognosis; however, this total effect might be better understood by the mediating role of FT. According to our results, this is the case for anxiety and PTSD personality traits. For example, a one-standard-deviation (23.8) increase in the MCMI-III anxiety scale would increase the odds to receive TC treatment by 49%. If the mediating role of FT were excluded, the association between both variables would decrease, and the odds would only increase 33.5%. In other words, a poor FT worsens the addiction prognosis of patients with higher levels of anxiety or PTSD personality traits. To our knowledge, this is the first time that the interaction between personality and FT has been explored in the context of addiction, and our results may suggest new strategies to improve withdrawal treatments. On the other hand, our conclusions are in line with classical psychotherapeutic approaches, such as rational emotive behavior therapy (78). According to this, even though a negative emotion is important to understand a behavioral outcome, implicit beliefs have a more important role in such emotion and its causes. The final goal of therapists, according to this account, is to help patients minimize anxiety, guilt, and depression by accepting themselves (79); alleviate their aggression by accepting others (80); and reduce their low FT by accepting negative events (81). Hence, frustration is considered a healthy negative emotion, but a low FT is an “irrational belief” (78). Similarly, our results suggest that an improvement in FT, as a mediational variable, may alleviate the influence of more stable personality traits on addiction severity.

Our study has a number of limitations. One aspect that should be considered is the gender composition of the samples. Females were underrepresented, and therefore, the conclusions of the study could be driven by male participants. This is a usual caveat in studies about addictions. We tackled this limitation by randomly selecting age- and sex-matched subsamples with MedCalc software. Furthermore, the size of the patient sample was relatively small. Access to a clinical sample cognitively intact and willing to participate in a research project was limited; besides that, it was challenging to complete evaluation in all cases because of treatment discontinuation. With respect to the control sample and its personality assessment, median scores are relatively high (=60) for alcohol and substance use subscales; however, in our opinion, it qualifies as a control sample for two main reasons: 1) large differences in these traits with respect to the clinical samples and 2) absence of alcohol and substance dependence and/or harm after evaluation with the AUDIT and ASSIST tools, which are more specific to detecting alcohol and substance problematic use. It should be taken into account that some of the participants in the control sample were the next of kin of those in the clinical sample. Therefore, a certain degree of similarity in the psychological variables that we assessed, such as personality traits, might occur due to an akin genetic or educational background. Considering this, the differences that were found become even more relevant. Concerning the assessment of FT, we used a semi-projective test where participants were asked to explain how they would react in a frustrating situation. It could be criticized that we did not use a task that actually induced frustration in participants. However, construct validity of Rosenzweig’s PFT has been adequately confirmed (82), it is considered a useful tool in clinical practice (83), and it has been shown to predict actual problem solving and stress coping in experimental settings (84, 85). In any case, future research should confirm our results with tasks eliciting actual frustration. Also, we did not collect information about the psychopharmacological medication that volunteers in the clinical samples were taking. Finally, it should be considered that the interplay between personality, FT, and treatment should be adequately assessed in longitudinal studies, including rates of relapse and dropouts. The conclusions of our study justify further research to elucidate this relationship.

Our study also suggests future directions to investigate the relationship between addiction, personality, and attachment. One crucial limitation is that we did not assess attachment styles in any of our samples, although our results can be interpreted to draw some preliminary conclusions and inspire future research. First of all, previous results show the relationship between a disorganized attachment and a higher probability of suffering anxiety (86), depression (87), addiction (88), or PTSD (89), and expressing personality disorders such as borderline, avoidant, or antisocial, among others (90). In turn, recent research has shown an association between insecure attachment, alcohol/substance addiction, and an increased amount of borderline personality organization (57). Even though the borderline trait did not survive Bonferroni correction in our study, it was suggestive of symptoms at a subclinical level in the clinical samples included in our study. Moreover, the relationship between attachment styles and substance addiction has been proven to have a biological correlate, in particular, white matter integrity: in a sample of poly-drug users, Unterrainer et al. (91) showed a decreased fractional anisotropy compared with recreational users or non-users. Furthermore, impairment of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and corona radiata was associated with an insecure attachment and negative affectivity. Interestingly, this research group previously found a relationship between white matter integrity, attachment styles, and personality factors in the aforementioned tracts (92): in this case, structural connectivity impairment positively correlated with anxious attachment and personality dysfunctioning, whereas white matter integrity positively correlated with openness and agreeableness.

Early attachment relationships, which are based on the mental models that children build about themselves, their interrelationships with their caregivers, and their environment, are essential for them to acquire the abilities of emotional management, attentional control, mentalizing, and autonomy (93). In turn, attachment theory is becoming strongly influential in research and intervention on personality disorders (9496). According to Adshead and Sarkar (96), these disorders include an intrapersonal component (related to a dysregulation of arousal, impulse, and affect systems in response to stress), an interpersonal component (dysfunctional attachment patterns), and a social component (dysfunction in social behaviors). To some extent, our research covers an interpersonal (FT) and social component (substance addiction inpatient or outpatient treatment), which can be related with the interpersonal component. According to our results, anxiety and PTSD are those personality traits more affected in the TC group, and under the influence of FT, they predict the inclusion in TC or AT treatment. The link between attachment styles and anxiety has been extensively demonstrated [see, for example, the review by Ref. (97)]. According to these authors, anxiety is more frequent in adolescents who experienced resistant attachment during childhood compared to those with secure or avoidant styles. This association remained when considering attachment-related negative experiences during childhood (such as parental divorce or loss) or attachment states of mind (i.e., preoccupied), instead of self-reported assessment of attachment. Similarly, PTSD has been related to disorganized attachment. For instance, unresolved attachment-related state of mind is associated with a higher risk (7.5) of expressing PTSD (98). Furthermore, posttraumatic symptoms through midlife and old age are associated with adult attachment insecurity (99). In conclusion, our results point to a plausible interplay between disorganized attachment, FT, and certain personality traits (mainly anxiety and PTSD) in substance abuse disorders. Future research on these topics from a unitary perspective may increase our understanding of substance addiction, improving prevention policies, and hopefully designing improved individualized treatments for patients suffering this devastating disorder.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Committee of Ethics in Research of the University of Navarra. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

FG, JB, and IM conceived the study; DR-C, CG-R, FG, and IM collected data and organized the database; DR-C, IM, and JB performed the statistical analyses; JF-M wrote sections of the manuscript and supervised data analyses. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Drs. Gonzalo Arrondo, Maite Aznarez, and Jose Ignacio Murillo for their technical and theoretical advice on this research. We also acknowledge the collaboration of the users and personnel of Fundación Proyecto Hombre Navarra and Asociación Proyecto Hombre Granada for making possible this research. This project was funded by Fundación Caja de Ahorros de Navarra (grant number 70721) and the Spanish Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad (2016/057).

References

1. Hodding GC, Jann M, Ackerman IP. Drug withdrawal syndromes—a literature review. West J Med (1980) 133:383–91. doi: 10.1089/dia.2011.0127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Fernández-Serrano MJ, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2011) 35:377–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Koob GF. The dark side of emotion: the addiction perspective. Eur J Pharmacol (2015) 753:73–87. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.11.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. McLellan AT, Lewis DC, O’Brien CP, Kleber HD. Drug dependence, a chronic medical illness. J Am Med Assoc (2000) 284:1689–95. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.13.1689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Waltman D. Key ingredients to effective addictions treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat (1995) 12:429–39. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(95)02018-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Karoly HC, Yorkwilliams SL, Hutchison KE. Clinical neuroscience of addiction: similarities and differences between alcohol and other drugs. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2015) 39:2073–84. doi: 10.1111/acer.12884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Dakwar E, Nunes EV. New directions in medication-facilitated behavioral treatment for substance use disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2016) 18:64. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0703-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Washton AM. Preventing relapse to cocaine. J Clin Psychiatry (1988) 49:34–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Delmonico RL, Hanley-Peterson P, Englander J. Group psychotherapy for persons with traumatic brain injury: management of frustration and substance abuse. J Head Trauma Rehabil (1998) 13:10–22. doi: 10.1097/00001199-199812000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barragan Torres L, Gonzalez Vazquez J, Medina-Mara M. Adaptation of a model of cognitive–behavioral intervention for dependent users of alcohol and other drugs in Mexico: a preliminary study. Salud Ment (2005) 28:61–71.

Google Scholar

11. Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Bornovalova MA, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Brown RA. Distress tolerance as a predictor of early treatment dropout in a residential substance abuse treatment facility. J Abnorm Psychol (2005a) 114:729–34. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Chen G. Social support, spiritual program, and addiction recovery. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol (2006) 50:306–23. doi: 10.1177/0306624X05279038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Eiden RD, Leonard KE, Colder CR, Homish GG, Schuetze P, Gray TR, et al. Anger, hostility, and aggression as predictors of persistent smoking during pregnancy. J Stud Alcohol Drugs (2011) 72:926–32. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kant CK, Plummer D. Unpacking drug detoxification in Nepal: in-depth interviews with participants to identify reasons for success and failure. Int J Psychosoc Rehabil (2012) 16:50–61.

Google Scholar

15. Psaros C, Pajolek H, Park ER. The role of negative affect management in postpartum relapse to smoking. Arch Womens Ment Health (2012) 15:15–20. doi: 10.1007/s00737-011-0250-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Solar A. Improving abstinence in comorbid substance disorder in a mental health unit. Australas Psychiatry (2012) 20:356–7. doi: 10.1177/1039856212449674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Winward JL, Bekman NM, Hanson KL, Lejuez CW, Brown SA. Changes in emotional reactivity and distress tolerance among heavy drinking adolescents during sustained abstinence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2014) 38:1761–9. doi: 10.1111/acer.12415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Timko C, Schultz NR, Britt J, Cucciare MA. Transitioning from detoxification to substance use disorder treatment: facilitators and barriers. J Subst Abuse Treat (2016) 70:64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2016.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Torres C, Papini MR. Emotional self-medication and addiction. In: Preedy V, editor. Neuropathology of drug addictions and substance misuse. London: Elsevier (2016). p. 71–81. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800213-1.00007-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Amsel A. Frustration theory—many years later. Psychol Bull (1992) 112:396–9. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.3.396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Manzo L, Donaire R, Sabariego M, Papini MR, Torres C. Anti-anxiety self-medication in rats: oral consumption of chlordiazepoxide and ethanol after reward devaluation. Behav Brain Res (2015) 278:90–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Baars MY, Müller MJ, Gallhofer B, Netter P. Relapse (number of detoxifications) in abstinent male alcohol-dependent patients as related to personality traits and types of tolerance to frustration. Neuropsychobiology (2013) 67:241–8. doi: 10.1159/000350483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Miller L. Neuropsychodynamics of alcoholism and addiction: personality, psychopathology, and cognitive style. J Subst Abuse Treat (1990) 7:31–49. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(90)90034-N

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Miller L. Predicting relapse and recovery in alcoholism and addiction: neuropsychology, personality, and cognitive style. J Subst Abuse Treat (1991) 8:277–91. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(91)90051-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Glassman LH, Martin LM, Bradley LE, Ibrahim A, Goldstein SP, Forman EM, et al. A brief report on the assessment of distress tolerance: are we measuring the same construct? J Ration Emotive Cogn Behav Ther (2016) 34:87–99. doi: 10.1007/s10942-015-0224-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Cosci F, Ibrahim HMH, Nannini A, Schruers K. Experimental study on the effects of anxiety sensitivity and somatosensory amplification on the response to the 35% CO₂ challenge in abstinent smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol (2015) 23:464–76. doi: 10.1037/pha0000048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hassan A, Al-jubari I. Basic psychological needs satisfaction, need frustration and entrepreneurial intention. Int J Bus Manage (2015) 1:1–11.

Google Scholar

28. Williams CL, Vik PW, Wong MM. Distress tolerance in social versus solitary college student drinkers. Addict Behav (2015) 50:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bowlby J. The nature of the child’s ties to his mother. Int J Psychoanal (1958) 39:350–73.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

30. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall SN. Patterns of attachment: a psychological study of the strange situation. Oxford: Lawrence Erlbaum (1978). doi: 10.4324/9780203758045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Benoit D. Infant–parent attachment: definition, types, antecedents, measurement and outcome. Paediatr Child Health (Oxford) (2004) 9:541–5. doi: 10.1093/pch/9.8.541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Paetzold RL, Steven Rholes W, Kohn JL. Disorganized attachment in adulthood: theory, measurement, and implications for romantic relationships. Rev Gen Psychol (2015) 19:146–56. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. van Ijzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Dev Psychopathol (1999) 11:225–50. doi: 10.1017/S0954579499002035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cicchetti D, Toth C, Lynch M. Bowlby’s dream comes full circle: the application of attachment theory to risk and psychopathology. Adv Clin Child Psychol (1995) 17:1–75. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-9044-3_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Sroufe LA. Appraisal: Bowlby’s contribution to psychoanalytic theory and developmental psychology—attachment, separation, loss. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1986) 27:841–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00203.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gidhagen Y. Psychological treatment of outpatients with substance use disorders in routine care—attachment style, alliance, and treatment outcome. Linkoping University (2018). doi: 10.1111/papt.12172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jacobsen T, Huss M, Fendrich M, Kruesi MJP, Ziegenhain U. Children’s ability to delay gratification: longitudinal relations to mother–child attachment. J Genet Psychol (1997) 158:411–26. doi: 10.1080/00221329709596679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kerns KA, Abraham MM, Schlegelmilch A, Morgan TA. Mother–child attachment in later middle childhood: assessment approaches and associations with mood and emotion regulation. Attach Hum Dev (2007) 9:33–53. doi: 10.1080/14616730601151441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wei M, Vogel DL, Ku TY, Zakalik RA. Adult attachment, affect regulation, negative mood, and interpersonal problems: the mediating roles of emotional reactivity and emotional cutoff. J Couns Psychol (2005) 52:14–24. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.52.1.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Shaver PR, Mikulincer M. Adult attachment strategies and the regulation of emotion. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. Guilford Press (2007).

Google Scholar

41. Magor-Blatch LE, Keen JL, Bhullar N. Personality factors as predictors of programme completion of drug therapeutic communities. Ment Heal Subst Use Dual Diagnosis (2014) 7:110–24. doi: 10.1080/17523281.2013.806345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Craig RJ. Sensitivity of MCMI-III Scales T (drugs) and B (alcohol) in detecting substance abuse. Subst Use Misuse (1997) 32:1385–93. doi: 10.3109/10826089709039384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fernández-Montalvo J, López-Goñi JJ, Illescas C, Landa N, Lorea I. Evaluation of a therapeutic community treatment program: a long-term follow-up study in Spain. Subst Use Misuse (2008) 43:1362–77. doi: 10.1080/10826080801922231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jacobsen LK, Southwick SM, Kosten TR. Substance use disorders in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: a review of the literature. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:1184–90. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Skinstad AH, Swain A. Comorbidity in a clinical sample of substance abusers. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse (2001) 27:45–64. doi: 10.1081/ADA-100103118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ross S, Dermatis H, Levounis P, Galanter M. A comparison between dually diagnosed inpatients with and without Axis II comorbidity and the relationship to treatment outcome. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse (2003) 29:263–79. doi: 10.1081/ADA-120020511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Grant BF, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou SP. Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61:807–16. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Carlson E, Sroufe LA. Contribution of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology: vol. 1. Theory and methods. New York: Wiley (1995). p. 581–617.

Google Scholar

49. Riggs SA, Paulson A, Tunnell E, Sahl G, Atkison H, Ross CA. Attachment, personality, and psychopathology among adult inpatients: self-reported romantic attachment style versus adult attachment interview states of mind. Dev Psychopathol (2007) 19:263–91. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Patock-Peckham JA, Morgan-Lopez AA. Direct and mediational links between parental bonds and neglect, antisocial personality, reasons for drinking, alcohol use, and alcohol problems. J Stud Alcohol Drugs (2010) 71:95–104. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wedekind D, Bandelow B, Heitmann S, Havemann-Reinecke U, Engel KR, Huether G. Attachment style, anxiety coping, and personality-styles in withdrawn alcohol addicted inpatients. Subst Abus Treat Prev Policy (2013) 8:1. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-8-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Westen D, Nakash O, Thomas C, Bradley R. Clinical assessment of attachment patterns and personality disorder in adolescents and adults. J Consult Clin Psychol (2006) 74:1065–85. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Charles K, Eric B. An introduction to research on the social impact of the therapeutic community for addiction. Int J Soc Welf (2003) 12:204–210. doi: 10.1111/1468-2397.00487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Daughters SB, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Brown R. Psychological distress tolerance and duration of most recent abstinence attempt among residential treatment-seeking substance abusers. Psychol Addict Behav (2005b) 19:208–11. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.2.208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. De Wilde J, Broekaert E, Segraeus V, Rosseel Y. Is the “community as method” approach gender sensitive? Client and treatment characteristics in European therapeutic communities. Int J Soc Welf (2006) 15:150–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2397.2006.00374.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. López-Goñi JJ, Fernández-Montalvo J, Arteaga A, Esarte S. Searching objective criteria for patient assignment in addiction treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat (2017) 76:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2017.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hiebler-Ragger M, Unterrainer HF, Rinner A, Kapfhammer HP. Insecure attachment styles and increased borderline personality organization in substance use disorders. Psychopathology (2016) 49:341–4. doi: 10.1159/000448177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Millon T, Meagher S. The Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III). In: Hilsenroth M, Segal D, editors. Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, vol. 2: personality assessment. John Wiley & Sons Inc (2004).

Google Scholar

59. Rosenzweig S. The picture-association method and its application in a study of reactions to frustration. J Pers (1945) 14:3–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1945.tb01036.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Humeniuk R, Ali R, Babor TF, Farrell M, Formigoni ML, Jittiwutikarn J, et al. Validation of the Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST). Addiction (2008) 103:1039–47. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2013.172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Gómez A, Conde A, Santana JM, Jorrín A. Diagnostic usefulness of brief versions of Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) for detecting hazardous drinkers in primary care settings. J Stud Alcohol (2005) 66:305–8. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Mackinnon DP, Dwyer JH. Estimating mediated effects in prevention studies. Eval Rev (1993) 17:144–58. doi: 10.1177/0193841X9301700202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kenny D. Mediation. (Accessed October 1, 2018). Available at: http://davidakenny.net/cm/mediate.htm.

Google Scholar

64. Dollard J, Miller NE, Doob LW, Mowrer OH, Sears RR. Frustration and aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press (1939). doi: 10.1037/10022-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rosenzweig S, Ludwig DJ, Adelman S. Retest reliability of the Rosenzweig picture-frustration study and similar semiprojective techniques. J Pers Assess (1975) 39:3–12. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa3901_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Herr N. Mediation with dichotomous outcomes. (2016). [online] Nrhpsych.com. Available at: http://www.nrhpsych.com/mediation/logmed.html [Accessed 6 Sep. 2018].

Google Scholar

67. Craig R. Alternative interpretations for the histrionic, narcissistic, and compulsive personality disorder scales of the MCMI-III. In: Craig R, editor. New directions in interpreting the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc (2005). p. 71–93.

Google Scholar

68. Bjork JM, Smith AR, Hommer DW. Striatal sensitivity to reward deliveries and omissions in substance dependent patients. Neuroimage (2008) 42:1609–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Brown R, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Distress tolerance and duration of past smoking cessation attempts. J Abnorm Psychol (2002) 111:180–5. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.111.1.180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Knott V, Cosgrove M, Villeneuve C, Fisher D, Millar A, McIntosh J. EEG correlates of imagery-induced cigarette craving in male and female smokers. Addict Behav (2008) 33:616–21. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ellis. The essential guide to effect sizes. (2012). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511761676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Volkow ND. Drug abuse and mental illness: progress in understanding comorbidity. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:1181–3. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Koob GF, Buck CL, Cohen A, Edwards S, Park PE, Schlosburg JE, et al. Addiction as a stress surfeit disorder. Neuropharmacology (2014) 76:370–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Papini MR. Comparative psychology of surprising nonreward. Brain Behav Evol (2003)62(2), 83–95. doi: 10.1159/000072439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Perlman SB, Luna B, Hein TC, Huppert TJ. FNIRS evidence of prefrontal regulation of frustration in early childhood. Neuroimage (2014) 85:326–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF. Self-regulation failure: an overview. Psychol Inq (1996) 7:1–15. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0701_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Baumeister RF, Tice DM, Vohs KD. The strength model of self-regulation: conclusions from the second decade of willpower research. Perspect Psychol Sci (2018) 13(2):141–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691617716946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Ellis A. Rational emotive behavior therapy. In: Corsini R, Wedding D, editors. Current psychotherapies. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Co Inc (2010). p. 196–234.

Google Scholar

79. Oltean H-R, Hyland P, Vallières F, David DO. Rational beliefs, happiness and optimism: an empirical assessment of REBT’s model of psychological health. Int J Psychol (2018). doi: 10.1002/ijop.12492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Fuller JR, DiGiuseppe R, O’Leary S, Fountain T, Lang C. An open trial of a comprehensive anger treatment program on an outpatient sample. Behav Cogn Psychother (2010) 38:485–90. doi: 10.1017/S1352465810000019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Ziegler DJ, Smith PN. Anger and the ABC model underlying rational-emotive behavior therapy. Psychol Rep (2004) 94:1009–14. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.3.1009-1014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Rosenzweig S, Adelman S. Construct validity of the Rosenzweig picture-frustration study. J Pers Assess (1977) 41:578–88. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4106_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Lilienfeld SO, Wood JM, Garb HN. The scientific status of projective techniques. Psychol Sci Public Interes (2000) 1:27–66. doi: 10.1111/1529-1006.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Dor-Shav NK, Mikulincer M. Learned helplessness, causal attribution, and response to frustration. J Gen Psychol (1990) 117:47–58. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1990.9917772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Kahn-Greene ET, Lipizzi EL, Conrad AK, Kamimori GH, Killgore WDS. Sleep deprivation adversely affects interpersonal responses to frustration. Pers Individ Dif (2006) 41:1433–43. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Kerns KA, Brumariu LE. Is insecure parent–child attachment a risk factor for the development of anxiety in childhood or adolescence? Child Dev Perspect (2014) 8:12–7. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Bifulco A, Kagan L, Spence R, Nunn S, Bailey-Rodriguez D, Hosang G, et al. Characteristics of severe life events, attachment style, and depression—using a new online approach. Br J Clin Psychol (2019). doi: 10.1111/bjc.12221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Schindler A, Bröning S. A review on attachment and adolescent substance abuse: empirical evidence and implications for prevention and treatment. Subst Abus (2015) 36:304–13. doi: 10.1080/08897077.2014.983586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Enlow MB, Egeland B, Carlson E, Blood E, Wright RJ. Mother–infant attachment and the intergenerational transmission of posttraumatic stress disorder. Dev Psychopathol (2014) 26:41–65. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Cirasola A, Hillman S, Fonagy P, Chiesa M. Mapping the road from childhood adversity to personality disorder: the role of unresolved states of mind. Personal Ment Health (2017) 11:77–90. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Unterrainer H-F, Hiebler-Ragger M, Koschutnig K, Fuchshuber J, Tscheschner S, Url M, et al. Addiction as an attachment disorder: white matter impairment is linked to increased negative affective states in poly-drug use. Front Hum Neurosci (2017) 11:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Unterrainer HF, Hiebler M, Ragger K, Froehlich L, Koschutnig K, Schoeggl H, et al. White matter integrity in polydrug users in relation to attachment and personality: a controlled diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain Imaging Behav (2016) 10:1096–107. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9475-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Bateman A, Gergely G, Strathearn L, Target M, et al. Attachment and personality pathology. In: Clarkin J, Fonagy P, Gabbard G, editors. Psychodynamic psychotherapy for personality disorders: a clinical handbook. Washington DC: APA Publishing (2010). p. 37–88.

Google Scholar

94. Aaronson CJ, Bender DS, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG. Comparison of attachment styles in borderline personality disorder and obsessive–compulsive personality disorder. Psychiatr Q (2006) 77:69–80. doi: 10.1007/s11126-006-7962-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Bakermans-Kranenburg M, van IJzendoorn MH. The first 10,000 Adult Attachment Interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups. Attach Hum Dev (2009) 11:223–63. doi: 10.1080/14616730902814762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Adshead G, Sarkar J. The nature of personality disorder. Adv Psychiatr Treat (2012) 18:162–72. doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.109.006981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Dozier M, Stovall-McClough KC, Albus KE. Attachment and psychopathology in adulthood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver P, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: The Guilford Press (2008). p. 718–44.

Google Scholar

98. Stovall-McClough KC, Cloitre M. Unresolved attachment, PTSD, and dissociation in women with childhood abuse histories. J Consult Clin Psychol (2006) 74:219–28. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.2.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Franz CE, Lyons MJ, Spoon KM, Hauger RL, Jacobson KC, Lohr JB, et al. Post-traumatic stress symptoms and adult attachment: a 24-year longitudinal study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2014) 22:1603–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: substance addiction, frustration tolerance, ambulatory treatment, MCMI-III, therapeutic community

Citation: Ramirez-Castillo D, Garcia-Roda C, Guell F, Fernandez-Montalvo J, Bernacer J and Morón I (2019) Frustration Tolerance and Personality Traits in Patients With Substance Use Disorders. Front. Psychiatry 10:421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00421

Received: 11 February 2019; Accepted: 28 May 2019;
Published: 14 June 2019.

Edited by:

Human Friedrich Unterrainer, University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

Martin Zack, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), Canada
Michaela Hiebler-Ragger, Medical University of Graz, Austria

Copyright © 2019 Ramirez-Castillo, Garcia-Roda, Guell, Fernandez-Montalvo, Bernacer and Morón. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Javier Bernacer, amJlcm5hY2VyQHVuYXYuZXM=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.