- 1Mental Health Center Glostrup, Broendby, Denmark
- 2Center for Subjectivity Research, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
- 3Early Psychosis Intervention Center, Roskilde, Denmark
- 4Research Unit for General Practice, Department of Public Health, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
- 5Mental Health Center Amager, Copenhagen, Denmark
A potential link between creativity and mental illness has been a longstanding topic for human studies and empirical research. The major problem is defining creativity and establishing its measurable indicators. A few high-quality epidemiological studies have been undertaken and point to a link between creativity and vulnerability to mental illness. Demonstrating such a shared vulnerability could expand our understanding of mental illnesses and open up new avenues of empirical research. In this epidemiological study, we defined scientists (academics) at the universities as individuals assumed to exhibit “more creativity” than the background population. In a register coupling with a population of 588,532 people, we examined successful university academics' first- and second-degree relatives for diagnosed mental disorders and compared those figures with controls from the background population controlling for educational level. The relatives of the academics had significantly increased risk of suffering from schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. For bipolar disorder, it is perhaps temperamental features and high energy levels that contribute to this association. In the case of schizophrenia, the mediating bridge may involve an amplification of human tendency to question the obvious and “taken-for-granted.” Creativity and an increased risk for mental disorders seem to be linked by a shared vulnerability that is not manifested by clinical mental disorders in the academics.
Introduction
The relationship between creativity and mental illness has been a topic of human studies for centuries, and more recently for empirical research as well (1). For a succinct presentation of the evolution of methodologies see (2). It is being debated whether creative abilities are general in nature or domain specific (3) and there are multiple theories on the cognitive and biological processes involved in creativity (4). In a common-sense view, creativity is understood as an ability or process involved in the creation of something novel, original, and valued. In a review article creativity is defined in a similar way as involving “…the production of novel, useful products” (5). Thus, the ultimate validation whether a given psychological process was creative or not is testified by the achievement. In other words, declaring someone for being creative is a post hoc judgement.
In the context of empirical research there are many unresolved conceptual problems (4). These include the definition of creativity and the conceptualizations of creative processes which are all a matter of ongoing debates: “Reviews of the existing literature […] have shown that the field is heavily fragmented and its neuroscientific findings are invalidated by false category formations and compound constructs.” (4).
The research literature is far from an agreement on the relationship between creativity and vulnerability to mental disorder (1). Studies that report a positive association depict such associations as an inverted U, i.e., vulnerability to or low levels of psychopathology are associated with creative abilities which sharply decline with increasing levels of psychopathology (6, 7).
From our particular interest in schizophrenia (8) we will emphasize four important epidemiological studies. Karlsson (9) examined relatives of schizophrenia patients in Iceland and compared them with a control population and found around twice as many in artistic professions among the biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Kyaga et al. performed a register study in Sweden searching for individuals with psychiatric diagnoses and coupled these data with census information of self-reported occupational status (10). They found that individuals with bipolar disorder and healthy siblings of people with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder were overrepresented among the scientific and artistic professions. In a subsequent register study of another patient sample Kyaga et al. replicated their previous results (11). In addition, they found that individuals with self-reported creative professions were not more likely to suffer from mental disorders except for bipolar disorder.
Recently, Powers et al. studied around 25% of the population of Iceland (86,000). On the basis of the polygenetic schizophrenia risk score they identified 6% treated schizophrenia patients and 2% of members of artistic organizations. The findings were statistically highly significant. They conclude: “Thus, the main finding presented here is that creativity, conferred, at least in part, by common genetic variants, comes with an increased risk of psychiatric disorders conferred by the same genetic variants” (12).
The observed link between creativity and bipolar illness is perhaps due to temperamental features such as high energy and activity levels and the propensity to rapid and combinatorial thinking (13). In the case of schizophrenia there may be envisaged several mechanisms such as a propensity to non-ordinary cognitive styles [e.g., the so called “divergent thinking” (14)] and/or subtle experiential changes expressed in the schizotypal phenotype (15). Creative abilities seem to involve activity in specific neural networks (16).
Our own (17–20) and others [for a review, see (15)] research suggests that it is a spectrum of disorders with varying manifestations but with a certain fundamental phenotypic core gestalt shared by manifest schizophrenia and attenuated schizotypy (21–24). In a phenomenological perspective (8, 24) this core gestalt, i.e., the most elementary phenotypic vulnerability manifestation to schizophrenia consists in a disorder of pre-reflective attunement between the self and the world. This involves instability in the basic self with a tendency to solipsistic experiences of another ontological dimension and profound insight. It also involves the loss of “common sense,” i.e., an experience of the lack of naturalness and obviousness of the world. Moreover, self-disorders seem to be associated with certain alterations of imaginative processes (25–27). Jointly, these experiential changes may imply a propensity to hyperreflectivity, experiences of enlightenment and a tendency to question what ordinary people take for granted. This is perhaps an aspect of human experience (28) that may provide here a bridge between schizophrenia and creativity. Thus, we hypothesize that vulnerability to schizophrenia will be associated with an increased ability to novel and original thoughts, feelings, and expressions.
On the basis of these considerations we decided to study the frequency of schizophrenia and other mental illnesses among the relatives of successful academics i.e., scientists employed in tenured positions at Danish universities. We assumed that such population would reflect a quasi-objective creative achievement compared to the background population. Thus, we hypothesized an increased risk for schizophrenia spectrum disorders among biological relatives of successful university academics.
Materials and Methods
Population
We designed a study with elements from both matched cohort studies as well as case-control studies. We received information on all academic-scientific employees in tenured positions at three main Danish universities (Copenhagen University, Aarhus University, and the University of Southern Denmark in Odense), in total 11,803 individuals [hereafter referred to as “Academics” (A)]. At birth, all Danish citizens are assigned a unique personal identification number in the Danish Civil Registration System (29). Via the register the Academics were matched 1:6 on age, gender and municipality of residence with randomly selected controls (C) from the background population. The Danish Civil Registration System allowed us to identify first- and second- degree relatives of the Academics (RA) and controls (RC).
In order to track familiar transmission patterns, we divided this population into five subgroups: children, parents, grandparents, siblings, and nephews/nieces. Grandchildren were also identified, but ultimately excluded from analysis due to the very low age.
Outcome
The Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register holds information on all admissions to psychiatric hospitals from 1969 and outpatient contacts from 1995 and onwards (30). From this register we received information on psychiatric diagnoses on Academics, controls and their relatives (A, RA, C, RC).
For the whole sample, we collected all psychiatric diagnoses and classified each person into one of six diagnostic outcome groups: Schizophrenia, non-affective psychosis, bipolar disorder, melancholia, any other mental disorder, or no psychiatric diagnosis. We imposed a hierarchy following ICD-10 using the methods of other register studies (10, 31).
Covariates
RA and RC were not matched (only the Academics and Controls were), thus necessitating adjustment for age and gender. Furthermore, we wished to adjust for intelligence level as it has been shown to be the most important epidemiological risk factor for schizophrenia (32, 33) and therefore constitutes a confounder. We used educational level as a proxy for intelligence. Information on educational level was provided by Statistics Denmark. We were not able to obtain information on Faculty membership of the Academics.
Analyses
The five subgroups of relatives were analyzed in identical fashion; in a logistic model with “relation to Academics or control” as the dependent variable and the six outcome types as the independent variable, adjusted for education level, gender, and age. Adjustment for the latter was done by way of a cubic spline, in order to obtain a good adjustment while spending only a few degrees of freedom. The A and C were analyzed separately from the relative subgroups.
Results
The population of the study is presented in Table 1. The Odds Ratio (OR) for the Academics to be diagnosed with any mental disorder was 0.440 (p < 0.0000; 95% CI 0.398–0.485), for bipolar disorder 0.434 (p < 0.0007; 95% CI 0.269–0.702), and for schizophrenia 0.167 (p < 0.0000; 95% CI 0.108–0.257).
The Forest plots in Figure 1 indicate that there was a significant (p < 0.05) increased risk for schizophrenia among siblings, children, and nephews/nieces of the Academics. For bipolar disorder the OR was significantly (p < 0.05) increased for the academics' parents, grandparents, and nephews/nieces. Furthermore, the OR for the Academics' siblings was borderline significant (p = 0.05) for bipolar disorder. Additionally, we analyzed the siblings divided into maternal half-siblings and paternal half-siblings. We found that the OR for maternal half-siblings were significantly increased for schizophrenia however, that was not the case for paternal half-siblings.
Figure 1. Forest plots for the five subgroups of relatives for the four diagnostic groups and any mental disorder.
Discussion
We focus our discussion on the relationship between our finding and the epidemiological and genetic studies mentioned in the introduction. It is beyond our scope to address other scientific approaches linking creativity and psychopathology, notably the flourishing neurobiological research.
This study shows that whereas successful academics as a group are less prone to mental disorder than the background population there are increased rates of schizophrenia and bipolar illness among their biological relatives. The study was based on the assumption that successful university academics as a group are more creative than the background population. Obtaining a tenured position at a Danish university is highly competitive and the academic promotion is based on standard academic evaluation criteria. However, this starting assumption may be criticized. Contemporary university career is very much framed by bureaucratic influences and being a researcher is no longer reserved for the few, truly fascinated by their scientific field, but has become much more akin to a standard type of job (34). Western universities have become increasingly invaded by corporate-managerial culture and academic career has become very much dependent on the quantity rather than the quality of publications. French sociologist Pierre Bourdieu (35) distinguished between “eternal” and “temporal” scientists. The eternal scientist is a sort of ideal type researcher fascinated by and digging ever deeper in his topic and emitting power through his reputation and esteem. The temporal scientist makes his career through networking, political engagements, committee and funding agencies, membership etc. Given the corporate managerial shift of the academic culture one could argue that the proportion of the temporal type of scientist has increased dramatically and thereby undermining our starting assumption of higher levels of creativity in our sample of academics. Moreover, we have to acknowledge that the findings of the association between the academic-scientific status and increased rates of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in the relatives may be caused by multiple other unknown factors unrelated to creativity. While we acknowledge these reservations we still believe that our sample of Danish scientists reflect more creativity compared with the background population. It should be emphasized that this study was solely undertaken on the specific basis of our hypothesis.
Importantly, our index group is characterized by possessing a quasi-objective scientific achievement. Our study, being epidemiological in nature lacks the information about the individual indicators of creativity among the study population.
The risk of any mental illness among the Academics were significantly lower than in the controls probably because an academic career is too demanding for people with mental disorders that are associated with a hospital contact. Thus, in this particular sample individuals with hospitalization-demanding disorder have been selected away. We failed to demonstrate an increase of manifest schizophrenia among the parents of Academics most likely because having a parent with early onset chronic mental illness reduces ones chances of a smooth educational career (36).
Our findings are concordant with the studies referred to in the introduction (9–12). Our study design resembles those of Kyaga et al. (10, 11) in being case control studies using creative professions as a creativity proxy. However, our study does not start with a psychiatric patient population, but rather with a non-clinical sample assumed to be more creative than the general population (successful academics). This design makes it difficult to compare minutiae of ours and Kyaga's results. However, all these studies support the notion of a link between creativity and the vulnerability to mental illness. The results are compatible with a shared polygenetic susceptibility to schizophrenia, bipolar illness, and creativity. Beyond a certain point the increase of liability will begin to impede with creative abilities and lead to manifest psychopathology and a status as a psychiatric patient (7). What seems apparent is that the hypothesized relationship between creativity in academics and the increased risk for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in their relatives must be mediated by a vulnerability that is not manifested by overt mental disorder in the academics, consistent with the inverted U-curve model (6, 7).
Data Availability
The datasets for this study will not be made publicly available because it is prohibited by the Danish Act of Privacy.
Ethics Statement
The study protocol was reviewed and accepted first by the dean of the Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Copenhagen and subsequently by the administrations of all three universities. The study was approved by the Danish National Committee on Health Research Ethics. All data were anonymized and the authors had no access to any personal or other data that could identify individuals.
Author Contributions
JP and JN designed and planned the study and wrote the first draft of the paper. KS and CV did the statistical analyses. All authors have contributed to and approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
CV was supported by an unrestricted grant from the Lundbeck Foundation (grant number: R155-2012-11280).
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Dean of the Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences of University of Copenhagen Dr. Ulla Waever and the former Rector of the University of Copenhagen Dr. Ralf Arnfred Hemmingsen for their support in conducting this study. Finally, we thank Dr. Mogens Vestergaard and the MEPRICA researchnetwork.
References
2. Becker G. A socio-historical overview of the creativity-pathology connection: from antiquity to contemparary times. In: Kaufmann JC. editor. Creativity and Mental Illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2014). p. 3–24.
3. Sternberg RJ. Domain-generality versus domain-specificity of creativity. In: Meusberuger P, Funke J, Wunder E. editors. Milieus of Creativity. Dordrecht: Springer. (2009). p. 25–38. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4020-9877-2_3
4. Dietrich A, Haider H. A neurocognitive framework for human creative thought. Front Psychol. (2017) 7:2078. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02078
5. Mumford MD. Where have we been, where are we going? Taking stock in creativity research. Creat Res J. (2003) 15:107–20. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2003.9651403
6. Abraham A. Is there an inverted-U relationship between creativity and psychopathology? Front Psychol. (2014) 5:570. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00750
7. Kinney D, Richards R. Creativity as “compensatory advantage”: Bipolar and Schizophrenic Liability, the Inverted-U Hypothesis, and Practical Implications. In: Kaufman JC, editor. Creativity and Mental Illness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. (2014). p. 295–317.
8. Parnas J. The core Gestalt of schizophrenia. World Psychiatr. (2012) 11:67–9. doi: 10.1016/j.wpsyc.2012.05.002
9. Karlsson JL. Genetic association of giftedness and creativity with schizophrenia Hereditas. (1970) 66:177–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5223.1970.tb02343.x
10. Kyaga S, Lichtenstein P, Boman M, Hultman C, Langstrom N, Landen M. Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300,000 people with severe mental disorder. Br J Psychiatr. (2011) 199:373–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085316
11. Kyaga S, Landen M, Boman M, Hultman CM, Langstrom N, Lichtenstein P, et al. Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.09.010
12. Power RA, Steinberg S, Bjornsdottir G, Rietveld CA, Abdellaoui A, Nivard M, et al. Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder predict creativity. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:953–5. doi: 10.1038/nn.4040
13. Andreasen NC, Glick ID. Bipolar affective disorder and creativity: implications and clinical management. Compr Psychiatr. (1988) 29:207–17. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(88)90044-2
14. Acar S, Chen X, Cayirdag N. Schizophrenia and creativity: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr Res. (2017) 195: 23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.036
15. Parnas J, Licht D, Bovet P. The cluster a personality disorders: a review. In: Maj AHM, Mezzich JE, Okasha Chichester A, editors. Personality Disorders WPA Series: Evidence and Experience in Psychiatry. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. (2005). p. 1–74.
16. Beaty RE, Kenett YN, Christensen AP, Rosenberg M, Benedek Q, Chen A, et al. Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2018) 115:1087–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713532115
17. Matthysse S, Parnas J. Extending the phenotype of schizophrenia: implications for linkage analysis. J Psychiatr Res. (1992) 26:329–44. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(92)90039-Q
18. Parnas J, Cannon TD, Jacobsen B, Schulsinger H, Schulsinger F, Mednick SA, et al. Lifetime DSM-III-R diagnostic outcomes in the offspring of schizophrenic mothers. Results from the copenhagen high-risk study. Arch Gen Psychiatr. (1993) 50:707–14. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820210041005
19. Raballo A, Parnas J. The silent side of the spectrum: schizotypy and the schizotaxic self. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:1017–26. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq008
20. Raballo A, Saebye D, Parnas J. Looking at the schizophrenia spectrum through the prism of self-disorders: an empirical study. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:344–51. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp056
22. Meehl P. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am Psychol. (1962) 17:827–38. doi: 10.1037/h0041029
23. Meehl PE. Schizotaxia revisited. Arch Gen Psychiatr. (1989) 46:935–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810100077015
24. Parnas J. A disappearing heritage: the clinical core of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:1121–30. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr081
25. Rasmussen AR, Parnas J. Anomalies of imagination and disordered self in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychopathology. (2015) 48:317–23. doi: 10.1159/000431291
26. Rasmussen AR, Parnas J. Pathologies of imagination in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2015) 131:157–61. doi: 10.1111/acps.12323
27. Rasmussen AR, Saebye D, Parnas J. Anomalies of imagination in the schizophrenia-spectrum: empirical findings. Schizophr Res. (2018). doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.11.019
28. Parnas J, Henriksen MG. Mysticism and schizophrenia: a phenomenological exploration of the structure of consciousness in the schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Conscious Cogn. (2016) 43:75–88. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.05.010
29. Pedersen C. The Danish civil registration system. Scand J Public health. (2011) 39:22–5. doi: 10.1177/1403494810387965
30. Mors O, Perto GP, Mortensen PB. The Danish psychiatric central research register. Scand J Public Health. (2011) 39:54–47. doi: 10.1177/1403494810395825
31. Urfer-Parnas A, Lykke Mortensen E, Saebye D, Parnas J. Pre-morbid IQ in mental disorders: a Danish draft-board study of 7486 psychiatric patients. Psychol Med. (2010) 40:547–56. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990754
32. Joyce EM. Cognitive function in schizophrenia: insights from intelligence research. Br J Psychiatr. (2013) 203:161–2. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.109553
33. Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist J, Sundquist K. IQ and schizophrenia in a Swedish national sample: their causal relationship and the interaction of IQ with genetic risk. Am J Psychiatr. (2015) 172:259–65. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040516
35. Bourdieu P. Les usages sociaux de la science, pour une sociologie clinique du champ scientifique. In: Une Conference-Debat Organisee par le Groupe Sciences en Questions. Paris: INFRA. (1997).
Keywords: academics, schizophrenia, bipolar, creativity, vulnerability
Citation: Parnas J, Sandsten KE, Vestergaard CH and Nordgaard J (2019) Schizophrenia and Bipolar Illness in the Relatives of University Scientists: An Epidemiological Report on the Creativity-Psychopathology Relationship. Front. Psychiatry 10:175. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00175
Received: 26 October 2018; Accepted: 11 March 2019;
Published: 02 April 2019.
Edited by:
Anna Abraham, Leeds Beckett University, United KingdomReviewed by:
Sophie Schweizer, Heidelberg University Hospital, GermanyErik Thys, KU Leuven, Belgium
Copyright © 2019 Parnas, Sandsten, Vestergaard and Nordgaard. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Julie Nordgaard, anVsaWVfbm9yZGdhYXJkQGRhZGxuZXQuZGs=