Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 12 March 2019
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychopathology Among Youth in the 21st Century: Examining Influences from Culture, Society and Technology View all 21 articles

Influence of Family Processes on Internet Addiction Among Late Adolescents in Hong Kong

  • Department of Applied Social Sciences, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong, China

The present study investigated how the quality of the parent–child subsystem (indexed by behavioral control, psychological control, and parent–child relationship) predicted Internet addiction (IA) levels and change rates among senior high school students. It also examined the concurrent and longitudinal influence of the father- and mother-related factors on adolescent IA. At the beginning of the 2009/2010 school year, we randomly selected 28 high schools in Hong Kong and invited Grade 7 students to complete a questionnaire annually across the high school years. The present study used data collected in the senior high school years (Wave 4–6), which included a matched sample of 3,074 students (aged 15.57 ± 0.74 years at Wave 4). Growth curve modeling analyses revealed a slight decreasing trend in adolescent IA in senior high school years. While higher paternal behavioral control predicted children's lower initial level of and a slower drop in IA, maternal behavioral control was not a significant predictor of these measures. In contrast, higher maternal but not paternal psychological control showed a significant relationship with a higher initial level of and a faster drop in adolescent IA. Finally, better father–child and mother–child relationships predicted a lower initial level of IA among adolescents. However, while a poorer mother–child relationship predicted a faster decline in adolescent IA, father–child relationship quality did not. With the inclusion of all parent–child subsystem factors in the regression analyses, paternal behavioral control and maternal psychological control were identified as the two unique concurrent and longitudinal predictors of adolescent IA. The present findings delineate the essential role of parental control and the parent–child relationship in shaping children's IA across senior high school years, which is inadequately covered in the scientific literature. The study also clarifies the relative contribution of different processes related to the father–child and mother–child subsystems. These findings highlight the need to differentiate the following: (a) levels of and rates of change in adolescent IA, (b) different family processes in the parent–child subsystem, and (c) father- and mother-related factors' contribution to adolescent IA.

Introduction

The use of the Internet has grown rapidly in the past two decades, especially amongst the adolescent population. As the Internet becomes pervasive and increasingly indispensable in young individuals' lives, Internet addiction (IA) has also emerged as a serious public health issue due to its close relationships with adolescent health problems, risk behaviors, and social functioning problems (13).

Preventing IA requires efforts taken in various sectors such as family, school, and other social institutions. Family factors, such as positive family functioning, parental monitoring, and healthy parental–child relationship, play a vital role in this process (46). Among different parenting characteristics, parental control was intimately related to problematic adolescent behaviors [e.g., (7, 8)].

Parental control includes behavioral and psychological control (9). Behavioral control pertains to the use of disciplinary strategies and supervisory functions to regulate children's behavior, while psychological control attempts to shape children's behaviors through strategies like guilt or anxiety induction, and love withdrawal (10). Research findings have consistently shown that the behavioral control of parents is positively related to favorable developmental outcomes in children, while psychological control harms adolescent health (7, 8, 11). As Barber et al. (11) concluded, higher parental behavioral control is related to higher levels of adolescent competence, self-discipline, and school performance as well as lower levels of problematic behaviors. When parents provide clear regulations regarding children's Internet use or monitor their Internet use in an appropriate way, adolescents have a lower chance to show symptoms of IA. In contrast, parental psychological control tends to harm children's self-esteem, and increase developmental problems and maladjustment, because psychological control hurts adolescents' emotional functioning and the sense of self (12). For example, in a sample of 5,806 seventh graders, Wang et al. (8) found that the psychological control of parents predicted students' dampened emotional functioning in China and the United States.

There are three issues that should be addressed when examining the influence of parents on adolescent developmental outcomes. First, while parental control has been identified as an important determinant of adolescent development, the impact of the parent–child relationship has not been adequately examined. According to Shek (13), adolescents' satisfaction with their parents' control and their willingness to communicate with their parents are important relational qualities that should be considered when examining adolescent adjustment. Primarily, children's satisfaction with their parents' control reflects the quality of the parent-child relationship. Whether adolescents themselves regard parental control as reasonable or not is an important factor to be considered when understanding the influence of parental behavioral and psychological control. Pomerantz and Eaton (14) argued that some adolescents might regard parental control as an expression of care. Hence, these adolescents may gain more positive influence from parental control. This argument was supported by Kakihara (15), who found that adolescents interpreted strong parental behavioral control in specific domains as indicative of competence or intrusiveness. Thus, Kakihara drew attention to the possibility of multiple yet “contradictory” interpretations of parenting reported by adolescents. Hence, it is important to consider the quality of parent–child relationship when examining the impact of parental control.

Besides, parent–adolescent communication also influences adolescent development. Research showed that healthy parent–adolescent communication provides a safe environment for adolescents to comfortably disclose themselves (16), while problematic parent–adolescent communication is often associated with increased adolescent risk behaviors (17). Forehand et al. (18) found that families of adolescents with behavior problems experienced disagreements more frequently and had less supportive parent–child relationships than their counterparts did. Cottrell et al. (19) revealed that open parent–child communication was positively associated with parental monitoring. However, as Burk and Laursen (20) pointed out, little is known about how specific relationship attributes contribute to specific developmental problems such as IA.

The second issue concerns the differential influence of fathers and mothers on adolescent development. Historically, there has been an absence of parenting research examining the relationships between adolescents and their fathers (21). In addition, instead of separating paternal and maternal impacts, many studies just considered overall parenting characteristics (i.e., adolescent perception of their parents), which hinders the understanding of the complex dynamics in this process (22). Some recent studies have recognized the distinction between maternal and paternal factors on adolescent developmental outcomes, but the majority adopted a single perspective focusing on either mother' or fathers' influence. For example, Leung et al. (23) explored maternal control in a sample of 432 poor Chinese single-mother families. Studies on paternal involvement were reviewed in Pleck and Masciadrelli's (24) research, which showed that paternal involvement was related to positive child development. However, to have a holistic picture of how family factors systematically influence adolescent IA, it is important to include both maternal and paternal factors. Hence, it is argued that when examining parental impacts, both maternal and paternal factors should be taken into account.

However, the existing research findings regarding the differential effects of fathers and mothers are equivocal. For example, Giles and Price (25) found that only maternal psychological control positively predicted problematic computer use in children. In contrast, Lansford et al. (26) revealed that only paternal psychological control accounted for specific variance in children's developmental problems. The authors reported a similar result on the unique predictive effect of paternal knowledge on boys' externalizing problems. In Xu's research (2) involving 5,122 Chinese adolescents, results showed that compared to the father–child relationship, the mother–child relationship had a stronger association with children's IA. While these studies illustrated the differences between maternal and paternal impacts, the findings were inconclusive. Furthermore, they generally focused on a single parenting factor (e.g., psychological control) rather than simultaneously considering different aspects of the parent–child subsystem.

The third issue pertains to research design, in that most of the existing findings in this field are based on cross-sectional studies. Few studies have used longitudinal data to examine the growth rate of IA related to different family factors (4, 6). For example, Wang et al. (17) research using three-year longitudinal data on 913 Bahamian students revealed that parental control during early adolescence predicted a decrease in risk behavior in middle adolescence. Yu and Shek's (27) longitudinal research on students in Hong Kong revealed that good family functioning predicted a lower probability of having IA. Regarding the impacts of parental factors on the growth rate of IA, Shek et al. (28) study involving 3,328 Chinese students indicated that stronger paternal behavioral control was associated with a slower drop in children's IA, and stronger maternal psychological control was linked to a faster decrease in children's IA. Although their findings filled the knowledge gaps in the field, the study only covered early adolescence. More efforts should be made to examine the differences between maternal and paternal parenting styles among late adolescents (29).

In short, longitudinal research focusing on senior high school students' IA is urgently needed to understand how the parent–child relationship qualities shape adolescent IA. First, the dynamics of parenting and the family relationship functioning of senior high school students of IA are different from that of junior students. Students in late adolescence are physically and psychologically mature, demanding more freedom and flexibility. Parental control may not lead to the desired effect to adolescents at this stage. For example, Rogers et al. (30) suggested a stronger association between parental psychological control and internalizing problems for senior high school students than for junior students, because older adolescents might have stronger need for autonomy and more diverse internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Second, as the patterns in which mother- and father-related factors influence adolescents may evolve differently as both parents and adolescents age, longitudinal research would enhance our understanding of the differences between the influence of maternal and paternal factors on adolescent IA.

The Present Study

To fill these three research gaps, the current study investigated how parent–child subsystem factors influence the levels and rates of change in adolescent IA over senior high school years. Specifically, the study addressed the following three research questions. The first question is as follows: “do parental control (behavioral and psychological) and father– and mother–child relationship qualities predict children's initial level of IA at the beginning of senior high school life?” Drawing from extant literature (7, 8, 11) and related findings in early adolescence (28), it was expected that higher paternal and maternal behavioral control would predict a lower initial level of adolescent IA in senior high school years (Hypotheses 1a and 1b), while higher paternal and maternal psychological control would be associated with a higher initial level of adolescent IA (Hypotheses 1c and 1d). Regarding the parent–child relationship, we hypothesized that better relationships between the child and both parents would predict a lower initial level of IA (Hypotheses 1e and 1f).

The second research question is as follows: “how are parent–child subsystem qualities related to the developmental trajectory of adolescent IA across senior high school years?” In this field, only one previous study found that paternal behavioral control, maternal psychological control, as well as relationships between the child and both parents exerted significant predictive effects on the rate of change in IA during junior high school years (28). However, the related findings were at odds with the general expectations that favorable parental factors would be associated with a faster drop in IA as an indicator of positive adjustment in the long run. Given that only limited evidence is available, we still formed the present hypotheses based on the general theoretical expectations that positive parenting and a better parent–child relationship would be related to children's positive development (31). Specifically, it was expected that higher behavioral control of fathers (Hypothesis 2a) and mothers (Hypothesis 2b), as well as better father–child (Hypothesis 2c) and mother–child relationships (Hypothesis 2d), would predict a faster decline in children's IA because these factors were considered as positive aspects of parental impacts. Additionally, as a form of negative parenting, higher paternal and maternal psychological control were expected to predict a slower decrease in adolescent IA (Hypotheses 2e and 2f, respectively).

The third question is as follows: “what is the concurrent and longitudinal influence of paternal and maternal factors on adolescent IA during senior high school years?” While there is support for the relatively stronger influence of paternal parenting (26), other findings suggest a stronger influence for maternal parenting (2, 25). Besides the inconclusive picture, very few studies have distinguished different aspects of parenting practices (e.g., behavioral and psychological control and the relationship between parents and children) and examined the long-term impact using longitudinal data (28). Due to these factors, the following two general competing hypotheses were advanced: fathers are more influential than mothers in influencing adolescent IA (Hypothesis 3a), and mothers are more influential than fathers in influencing adolescent IA (Hypothesis 3b). All the above hypotheses are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of hypotheses and findings of the present study.

The present study also considered important demographic characteristics including student gender, family economic status, and family intactness. Specifically, boys often report a higher level of Internet use and a higher interest in online games than girls do (4). Some studies found that parenting effects vary among boys and girls. For example, Shek (32) examined parenting functions among Chinese adolescents with disadvantageous background and found that paternal parenting exerted more influence on boys' mental health and problem behavior, while maternal parenting played a major role in affecting girls' mental health and problem behavior. In contrast, Rogers et al. (30) reported that fathers' psychological control was more influential on daughters than on sons regarding externalizing behavior. Shi et al. (4) examined the relation between family functioning and children's IA and found that the path from family function to IA (via loneliness) was significant only for girls. By including student gender, this study attempted to contribute to the ongoing discussion about differential parenting influence on IA amongst boys and girls.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedures

This study was derived from a 6-year longitudinal project that investigated the adjustment of Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. A cluster sampling method was used. First, based on a school list provided by the Educational Bureau in Hong Kong, we formed a list of candidate schools including 399 government-funded or aided Chinese-speaking secondary schools in different districts of Hong Kong. Second, 30 schools were randomly selected and invited to join the project. If the selected school rejected our invitation, we invited an alternate school randomly selected from the candidate schools in the same district. Eventually, 28 schools agreed to join the project (Hong Kong Island: 5 schools; Kowloon: 7 schools; New Territories: 16 schools). Third, at the beginning of the 2009/2010 academic year, all the students in the first year of high school study (i.e., Grade 7) in the 28 participating schools were invited to complete a questionnaire and were followed up annually through the high school years, resulting in a 6-wave data set.

In 2009/2010, the number of Grade 7 students in the participating schools (N = 4,531) accounted for 7.12% of the total number of Grade 7 students in all the candidate schools (N = 63,620). At Wave 1 data collection, 3,328 students completed the questionnaire, suggesting a response rate of 73.45%. As demographic information of non-respondents who did not participate in any wave of data collection was not available, we were not able to compare participating students with non-respondents regarding their background characteristics. However, according to Shek et al. (33), the sample attributes of the project were similar to the demographic profile of the general adolescent population in secondary schools in Hong Kong.

Ethical approval for this project was obtained from the Human Subjects Ethics Sub-committee (HSESC) (or its Delegate) at The Hong Kong Polytechnic University. All involved parties, including participating schools, student participants, and their parents, provided informed written consent. In all occasions of data collection, trained research staff administrated the questionnaires, using a paper-and-pencil mode, in quiet classrooms at the participating schools. Administrators clearly instructed the student participants to provide honest responses based on their own interpretation of the questions.

The present study utilized Wave 4–6 data collected in the senior high school years (Grades 10–12). While data collection at Wave 5 took place 1 year (i.e., 12 months) after Wave 4, Wave 6 data collection was conducted approximately 10 months after Wave 5. This is because students at Grade 12 in Hong Kong have to concentrate on preparing for the public examination during the last few months of senior high school. The number of participants completing the questionnaire at each wave varied (Wave 4: N = 3,973, Wave 5: N = 3,683, and Wave 6: N = 3,498) due to students' absence at the time of data collection, transferring schools, or dropping out of schools. Among the 3,973 students who completed the survey at Wave 4, 3,397 and 3,237 completed the survey at Wave 5 and 6, respectively. From Wave 4 to Wave 5, were more participants withdrew from the study. One possible explanation is that some students might take vocational education, or they might go to work after reaching Hong Kong's legal working age of 15 years old. Across the three waves, 3,074 participants (aged 15.57 ± 0.74 years at Wave 4) were successfully matched, which included 1,577 (51.30%) boys and 1,497 (48.70%) girls. The matched sample was used in the present study.

Comparisons between the matched sample included in this study (N = 3,074) and who withdrew from the study after Wave 4 (i.e., N of dropouts = 899) showed no significant differences in family economic status and family intactness. However, the matched sample included a higher percentage of female adolescents (χ2(1) = 10.26, p < 0.01, ϕ = 0.05). Besides, adolescents in the matched sample (aged 15.57 ± 0.74 at Wave 4) were slightly younger than the dropouts [aged 15.88 ± 0.94, t(3861) = −9.95, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.37]. Regarding the key variables considered in the current study, no significant differences were observed in IA [t(3971) = −1.61, p = 0.11], paternal psychological control [F(1, 3543) = −3.12, p = 0.08], and maternal behavioral control [F(1, 3543) = −2.69, p = 0.10] measured at Wave 4. However, participants in the matched sample reported a slightly higher level of paternal behavioral control, better relationships with fathers and mothers, and a slightly lower level of maternal psychological control (F values ranged from 5.18 to 9.25, ps < 0.05, partial η2 ranged from 0.001 to 0.003) at Wave 4. However, the effect size was low.

Instruments

Among the multiple measures used in the questionnaire, IA and quality of the parent–child subsystem were the key measures, and gender, participants' family economic condition, and family intactness were the control variables in the present study.

Internet Addiction (IA)

Kimberly Young has developed several questionnaires to assess addicted behaviors related to Internet use, including the brief 8-item questionnaire, 10-item questionnaire, and the 20-item questionnaire. For Young's 8-item IA questionnaire, items were modified from criteria for pathological gambling in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) (34). The 10-item questionnaire included 10 Internet addiction symptoms (35). Shek and his collaborators translated the 10-item scale into a Chinese IA measurement, which exhibited good psychometric properties in previous research (3638). This study utilized the 10-item Chinese IA questionnaire to assess adolescent IA. Participants responded to 10 items using a dichotomous scale (“Yes” or “No”) to indicate whether they demonstrated the listed 10 addiction behaviors related to Internet use in the past year (see Table 2). Participants' IA was indexed by the number of “yes” answers they provided in the questionnaire. In this study, the Cronbach's αs of the questionnaire ranged from 0.79 to 0.82 across the three waves (see Table 3), indicating good internal consistency.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participants' answers on Internet Addiction questionnaire across the three waves (N = 3,074).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Reliability of scales and description of variables across the three waves.

Quality of the Father– and Mother–Child Subsystems

In the questionnaire, different paternal and maternal factors were measured by a reliable and validated scale entitled “Parent–Child Subsystem Quality Scale (PCSQS)” (16, 39). The PCSQS includes two 17-item subscales on paternal and maternal factors, respectively. The 17 items in each subscale can be further grouped into three dimensions. The first dimension includes seven items that assess paternal/maternal behavioral control, as indexed by each parent's expectation (e.g., “my father/mother expects me to exhibit good behavior in school”), knowledge (e.g., “my father/mother asked me about what I did after school”), and monitoring (e.g., “my father/mother actively understands my afterschool activities”). The second dimension comprises four items measuring the father's/mother's psychological control (e.g., “my father/mother often wants to change my mind or feelings about things”). The third dimension comprises six items that assess the quality of the father–/mother–child relationship, indexed by children's satisfaction with their parents' control (e.g., “my father's/mother's discipline of me is reasonable”) and children's active communication with their parents (e.g., “I share my feelings with my father/mother”). A 4-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree,” 4 = “strongly agree”) is used to rate each item. Participants' score on each dimension is indicated by the average score across all items included in it. In the present study, all subscales of the PCSQS demonstrated good reliability, as reflected by the high Cronbach's αs (0.88–0.91) across waves (see Table 3).

Family Economic Condition

Participants' family economic conditions were indexed by whether their family was living on welfare received from the “Comprehensive Social Security Assistance (CSSA) Scheme” of the Hong Kong Government. In the current study, the 182 (5.9%) adolescents who reported that their family was living on welfare from the CSSA at Wave 4 were grouped as “having family economic disadvantage,” and the other 2,684 (87.3%) participants whose family was not living on welfare from the CSSA at Wave 4 were categorized as “not having family economic disadvantage.”

Family Intactness

Parental marital status reported by the participants at Wave 4 was used to index family intactness. Specifically, if parents were in the first marriage, participants (n = 2,528, 82.2%) were categorized as “having an intact family.” Separation, divorce, or second marriage of parents was treated as an indicator of living in a “non-intact family” (n = 534, 17.4%).

Plan of Analysis

The statistical analysis plan was the same as that used in Shek et al.'s (28) study involving early adolescents. Specifically, we first analyzed the reliability of measures, descriptions, and correlations among variables. Subsequently, individual growth curve (IGC) modeling was utilized to investigate the predictive effects of different parenting factors on the initial level of adolescent IA as well as its developmental trajectory across senior high school, to address the first two research questions. In the present IGC analysis, time (i.e., Wave 4 = 0, Wave 5 = 1, and Wave 6 = 1.83), as the Level 1 predictor, was nested into Level 2 predictors, which included both control variables and measures of the parent-child subsystem quality, leading to 2-level hierarchical models.

Testing of the hierarchical models in the present study followed procedures that have been widely adopted in previous research (28, 4042). Basically, four models were compared. Model 1 was an unconditional mean model. Model 2 was a linear growth model that only involved Level 1 predictors (i.e., time). Model 3 was also a linear growth model, which further involved the control variables as Level 2 predictors in addition to Level 1 predictors. Model 4 further included the different aspects of the parent-child subsystem quality as Level 2 predictors in addition to Model 3 predictors. Using these procedures, any individual variability in the initial level of and the change rate of IA caused by Level 2 predictors could be identified. In this study, the three parental factors (i.e., behavioral control, psychological control, and quality of the parent-child relationship) were investigated as Level 2 predictors in Model 4a, 4b, and 4c, respectively. To explore any potential gender effect regarding parental influence on the initial level of adolescent IA and its change over time, we also tested gender-based IGC models.

Following previous studies (41, 42), we used three indices to index model fit, “−2log likelihood,” “Akaike Information Criterion” (AIC), and “Bayesian Information Criterion” (BIC). For these indices, a smaller value indicates a better model fit. Before performing IGC analyses, we dummy coded the three control variables as follows: “female” = “−1,” “male” = “1”; “having family economic disadvantage” = “−1,” “without family economic disadvantage” = “1”; “non-intact family” = “−1,” “intact family” = “1.” Meanwhile, parental factors were standardized.

The third research question was addressed by multiple regression analyses examining cross-sectional as well as longitudinal predictive effects of father–related factors, mother–related factors, and all parenting factors on adolescent IA. In short, the present study examined cross-sectional effects of parental factors on children's IA at all three waves. For longitudinal predictive effects, we examined the predictive effects of parental factors at Wave 4 on children's IA at Waves 5 and 6. In addition, to test whether children's gender would moderate parental influence on adolescent IA, we further included the interactions between gender and each factor related to parent–child subsystem quality in regression analyses.

Results

Correlations Among Variables

Table 4 shows the correlation coefficients among the variables examined in the present study. While parental behavioral control and relationships between parents and children were negatively associated with children's IA, there were positive correlations between parental psychological control and children's IA. These results support the general expectations. In addition, compared to female adolescents, male adolescents demonstrated a higher level of IA across all waves.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations among variables.

Developmental Trajectory of Adolescent IA and Predictive Effects of Control Variables

Model 1 (i.e., the unconditional mean model) showed a relatively high intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC; 0.575) (see Table 5), indicating that individual differences accounted for 57.5% of the variance in IA levels. Thus, both Level 1 and Level 2 predictors should be considered (41). Comparison of model fit indices between Models 1 and 2 (i.e., the unconditional linear model) suggested that Model 2 fit the data better [Δχ(3)2 = 81.85, p < 0.001, ΔAIC = 75.85, ΔBIC = 54.46]. According to Model 2, the level of adolescent IA declined slightly during the senior high school years (β = −0.120, p < 0.001) (see Figure 1).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of IGC models (Model 1–3) for adolescent Internet addiction (Wave 4–6).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Growth trajectories of adolescent Internet addiction as a function of gender. The figure for the full sample was plotted based on Model 2 shown in Table 5. The figures for male and female samples were plotted based on results of gender-based analyses for Model 2 shown in Table 5.

In Model 3, the control variables and time were treated as Level 2 predictors. Results showed that gender and family economic status were significant predictors of adolescents' IA level at Wave 4 (see Table 5). More specifically, male participants (β = 0.210, p < 0.001) or those with family economic disadvantage (β = −0.221, p < 0.05) had higher initial levels of IA. Additionally, gender had a significant effect on the rate of change in IA. Specifically, compared with female peers, male adolescents exhibited a faster drop in IA from Wave 4 to Wave 6 (β = −0.070, p < 0.01).

Further gender-based analyses for Model 2 revealed that the level of IA dropped significantly among male adolescents (β = −0.190, p < 0.001) and female adolescents, (β = −0.047, p = 0.04, one-tailed), although the magnitude was lower in female adolescents (see Table 5 and Figure 1). The results suggest that most of the variation in IA over time among the full sample is attributable to male adolescents.

Predictive Effects of Parental Factors on the Initial Level of Adolescent IA

Model 4a, 4b, and 4c considered parental behavioral control, psychological control, and quality of the relationship between parents and children as predictors, respectively. Results of gender-based analyses for Model 4a, 4b, and 4c suggested that parental factors showed similar predictive effects on the initial level of IA among male and female adolescents. Thus, we only elaborated related findings below based on the full sample shown in Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of IGC models with level-2 predictors for adolescent Internet addiction (Wave 4–6, full sample).

For the full sample, compared to Model 3 shown in Table 5, Model 4a (Δχ(4)2 = 70.64, p < 0.001, ΔAIC = 62.64, ΔBIC = 34.42), Model 4b (Δχ(4)2 = 55.35, p < 0.001, ΔAIC = 47.35, ΔBIC = 19.13) and Model 4c (Δχ(4)2 = 64.26, p < 0.001, ΔAIC = 56.26, ΔBIC = 28.04) had better model fits than Model 3 did (see Table 6).

According to Model 4a, while paternal behavioral control significantly predicted the initial level of adolescent IA at Wave 4 (β = −0.327, p < 0.001), maternal behavioral control did not (β = −0.037, p > 0.05). Thus, Hypothesis 1a was supported, while Hypothesis 1b was not.

According to Model 4b, while maternal psychological control was a significant predictor of the initial level of adolescent IA (β = 0.247, p < 0.001), paternal psychological control was not (β = 0.073, p > 0.05). These results did not support Hypothesis 1c, but they supported Hypothesis 1d.

According to Model 4c, the quality of the relationship between fathers and children (β = −0.258, p < 0.001) and between mothers and children (β = −0.122, p < 0.01) were both significant predictors of children's level of IA at Wave 4. Thus, Hypotheses 1e and 1f were supported.

Predictive Effects of Parental Factors on the Growth Rate of Adolescent IA

It was noteworthy that parental factors generally did not have significant impacts on the change rate of female adolescents' IA. Predictive effects of parental factors on the growth rate of IA based on the full sample were largely contributed by male adolescents (i.e., parental factors showed a similar pattern of predictive effects between the full sample and the male sample). In the following sections, we mainly outline the results based on the full sample (see Table 6).

It was found that paternal behavioral control significantly predicted the growth rate of children's IA across three waves (β = 0.082, p < 0.01), but maternal behavioral control did not (β = 0.028, p > 0.05) (see Table 6). Results indicated that participants with higher paternal behavioral control showed a lower level of IA at Wave 4 but a slower decrease in IA over time (see Figure 2). Although parental behavioral control tended to significantly predict the linear change rate of adolescent IA, the direction was contrary to our hypothesis. Thus, both Hypotheses 2a and 2b were not supported.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Growth trajectories of adolescent Internet addiction as a function of paternal behavioral control. The figures were plotted based on Model 4a shown in Table 6. High level indicates 1SD higher than the mean value; low level indicates 1SD lower than the mean value.

Based on the results presented in Table 6, while paternal psychological control was not significantly associated with the growth rate of adolescent IA (β = 0.018, p > 0.05), higher maternal psychological control was linked to a faster decrease in children's IA from Wave 4 to Wave 6 (β = −0.052, p < 0.05, see Figure 3). The direction of maternal psychological control's predictive effect on the change rate in adolescent IA was opposite to our hypothesis. Hence, Hypotheses 2e and 2f were not supported.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Growth trajectories of adolescent Internet addiction as a function of maternal psychological control. The figures were plotted based on Model 4b shown in Table 6. High level indicates 1SD higher than the mean value; low level indicates 1SD lower than the mean value.

With reference to Model 4c, the quality of relationship between mothers and children was a significant predictor of the growth rate of adolescent IA (β = 0.061, p < 0.05), while father–child relationship quality was not (β = 0.038, p > 0.05) (see Table 6). These findings suggested that a better mother–child relationship predicted a slower decline in IA during senior high school years (see Figure 4). Thus, Hypothesis 2c and 2d were not supported.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Growth trajectories of adolescent Internet addiction as a function of mother–child relational quality. The figures were plotted based on Model 4c shown in Table 6. Good quality indicates 1SD higher than the mean value; poor quality indicates 1SD lower than the mean value.

Relative Concurrent and Longitudinal Influence of Paternal and Maternal Factors

Results of the relative concurrent and longitudinal predictive effects of paternal and maternal factors have been present in Tables 7, 8. Further analyses, including interactions between gender and each parental factor, revealed that children's gender did not substantially moderate the concurrent and longitudinal predictive effects of parental factors. In this case, we outlined regression results based on the full sample in sections below.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Concurrent predicting effects of parent–child subsystem qualities on Internet addiction.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Longitudinal predicting effects of parent–child subsystem qualities on Internet addiction.

First, after the three social-demographic factors were controlled, the three concurrent father-related factors explained 3.4, 2.9, and 2.3% of variance in children's IA at Wave 4, 5, and 6 respectively. Table 7 shows that father–child relationship quality did not show cross-sectional effect in any wave. While paternal behavioral control significantly predicted children's IA negatively (Wave 4: β = −0.16, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.013; Wave 5: β = −0.15, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.012; Wave 6: β = −0.11, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.007). Paternal psychological control positively predicted children's IA at the three time points (Wave 4: β = 0.10, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.009; Wave 5: β = 0.14, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.018; Wave 6: β = 0.13, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.014). For longitudinal effects, paternal factors assessed at Wave 4 explained 1.7 and 1.5% of the variance in children's IA at the latter two time points, respectively. Table 8 shows that the father–child relationship did not predict adolescent IA over time. While paternal behavioral control was a negative longitudinal predictor of children's IA (Wave 5: β = −0.09, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.004; Wave 6: β = −0.08, p < 0.01, Cohen's f2 = 0.003), paternal psychological control was a positive longitudinal predictor of children's IA (Wave 5: β = 0.07, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.004; Wave 6: β = 0.09 p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.006).

Second, the three concurrent maternal factors explained 2.4, 1.6, and 1.8% of the variance in children's IA across Wave 4 to 6 (see Table 7), respectively. Specifically, the quality of the relationship between mothers and children was not a significant concurrent predictor of children's IA. Maternal behavioral control had a significant negative predictive effect at Wave 4 (β = −0.07 p < 0.01, Cohen's f2 = 0.003), but not at Waves 5 and 6. In contrast, maternal psychological control positively predicted children's IA (β ranged between 0.12 and 0.13, ps < 0.001, Cohen's f2 ranged between 0.012 and 0.015). The longitudinal effects of maternal factors measured at Wave 4 explained 1.3 and 1.0% of the variance in children's IA at the latter two waves, respectively. Table 8 shows that mother–child relational quality did not predict children's IA over time. In contrast, the longitudinal effect of maternal behavioral control on adolescent IA was significant at Wave 5 (β = −0.06, p < 0.05, Cohen's f2 = 0.002), but not significant at Wave 6 (β = −0.04, p > 0.05, Cohen's f2 = 0.001). However, maternal psychological control predicted children's IA at the two waves (Wave 5: β = 0.10, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.009; Wave 6: β = 0.10, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.008).

The above results indicated that paternal factors explained a larger amount of variance in adolescent IA than did maternal factors. Thus, the findings tended to support Hypothesis 3a rather than Hypothesis 3b.

Third, when the concurrent paternal factors and maternal factors were investigated simultaneously, they uniquely explained 4.4, 3.5, and 2.9% of the variance in children's IA at the three waves, respectively (see Table 7). Specifically, while father– and mother–child relationship quality and maternal behavioral control did not show cross-sectional effects, paternal behavioral control (β ranged between −0.15 and −0.12, ps < 0.001, Cohen's f2 ranged between 0.006 and 0.011) and psychological control (β ranged between 0.05 and 0.11, ps < 0.05, Cohen's f2 ranged between 0.002 and 0.009), as well as maternal psychological control (β ranged between 0.07 and 0.10, ps < 0.01, Cohen's f2 ranged between 0.004 and 0.007) were significant concurrent predictors in the three waves. Regarding longitudinal effects, parental factors measured at Wave 4 explained 2.3% and 1.8% of the variance in children's IA measured at the latter two waves, respectively (see Table 8). Among the six parental factors, only paternal behavioral control and maternal psychological control showed robust longitudinal effects on children's IA. While paternal behavioral control exerted negative effects on children's IA over time (Wave 5: β = −0.08, p < 0.01, Cohen's f2 = 0.003; Wave 6: β = −0.07, p < 0.01, Cohen's f2 = 0.003), maternal psychological control showed positive longitudinal effects (Wave 5: β = 0.09, p < 0.001, Cohen's f2 = 0.005; Wave 6: β = 0.07, p < 0.01, Cohen's f2 = 0.004).

The present findings are summarized in Table 1 with reference to the hypotheses.

Discussion

The present study examined parental influence on the development of Internet Addiction (IA) among Chinese adolescents. Three research questions were focused upon in this study. First, by separating paternal and maternal factors, our study examined the impacts of multiple parenting factors and quality of the relationship between parents and children on the initial level of children's IA. Second, the predictive effects of parental factors on the growth rate of children's IA were also investigated. Third, we also examined and compared the influence of different parental factors on adolescent IA at a single time point and over time. While a recent study has addressed these questions in early adolescents (28), the present study involved late adolescents in senior high school. In addition, gender-based analyses were conducted to further explore the effects of children's gender.

While most of the present findings are similar to the observations reported on early adolescents, some are different from these previous findings (28). For the first research question, male adolescents showed a higher initial level of IA than did females, which is in line with previous findings (28, 43). In addition, we observed a decline trend in adolescent IA. It is plausible that students in the higher grade will be less devoted to activities related to the Internet as they must prepare for the university entrance examination. Additionally, the magnitude of decline in IA was greater among male adolescents than that among female students. This gender difference may be attributed to female adolescents' relatively low level of initial IA, which was unlikely to decline substantially (i.e., floor effect). Furthermore, we did not observe a significant effect regarding children's gender and parental impacts on IA levels, which is inconsistent with the previous findings that parental psychological control was more influential on girls' than on boys' externalizing behaviors (26, 30). Nevertheless, as no previous research has tested gender effect regarding change in IA over time, more research is needed to portray a conclusive picture.

Regarding the parental influence on the initial level of IA that was addressed by the first research question, four hypotheses (1a, 1d, 1e, and 1f) were supported, whereas Hypotheses 1b and 1c were not. Specifically, when each aspect of the quality of the parent–child subsystem was examined separately in individual growth curve (IGC) models based on the full sample, higher paternal but not maternal behavioral control, and better relationships with both parents, were significantly associated with lower levels of initial adolescent IA. Additionally, higher maternal but not paternal psychological control predicted a higher level of initial adolescent IA. Gender-based IGC models showed similar findings for male and female adolescents. On the one hand, these findings suggested that parents' behavioral control and better parent-child relationships are positive parenting factors, whereas psychological control is a negative factor, regardless of children's gender. This conclusion echoes the previous findings observed in both Chinese and Western contexts (7, 28, 44).

On the other hand, the observations shed light on the differential influence of fathers' and mothers' factors. Specifically, paternal parenting had a closer association with adolescent IA via behavioral control, while mothers exerted stronger influence through psychological control. These findings seem to be different from the results reported in Shek et al.'s (28) research, which found that paternal and maternal factors tend to function in the same way in predicting the initial level of adolescent IA in early adolescence. However, as indicated by the estimated coefficients of predictors in Shek et al.'s (28) research, paternal behavioral control in comparison to maternal behavioral control, and maternal psychological control in comparison to paternal psychological control, tended to have a stronger association with the initial level of adolescent IA. Taken together, these findings suggest that fathers and mothers might contribute to the development of adolescent IA through different parental behaviors at different stages of adolescence. Such an assumption can help explain the existing mixed findings on paternal vs. maternal impacts on adolescent development. This will be discussed with reference to regression analyses below.

For the second research question, all the hypotheses based on general theoretical models were not supported. However, the findings are consistent with the previous observations among early adolescents (28). It was found that higher paternal behavioral control and better mother–child relationship predicted a slower decline in adolescent IA during senior high school years. Meanwhile, higher psychological control was linked with a faster decrease in adolescent IA. In short, these findings suggest that—similar to the situation in early adolescence—parental impacts also gradually diminish over time during late adolescence. These observations coincide with the decreasing amount of variance in adolescent IA explained by parental factors, as revealed by regression analyses. For example, the percentage of adolescent IA accounted by all concurrent parental factors decreased from 4.4% at Wave 4 to 2.9% at Wave 6. As suggested by Shek et al. (28), this finding may be attributable to adolescents' decreasing dependence on parents and increasing devotion to other types of social relationships (e.g., peer relationships). Additionally, at later stages of senior high school, all students in Hong Kong must invest most of their time and energy into preparing for the public examination. Thus, adolescents would generally be less different from each other in terms of their IA behaviors. Furthermore, those with IA problems may have dropped out from the study. Therefore, parental influence on adolescent IA may become less significant. Nevertheless, as the present study is a pioneering attempt, these findings need to be confirmed in future replication studies.

Regarding the contribution of fathers and mothers to children's IA levels (i.e., the third research question), the present study revealed that paternal factors accounted for a higher percentage of adolescent IA than did maternal factors, both concurrently and longitudinally. The general greater paternal impact vs. maternal impact on children's IA is in line with previous studies involving different developmental outcome measures. For example, using global treatment style, responsiveness, and demandingness to index parenting qualities, Shek (45, 46) reported that, relative to maternal parenting qualities, paternal parenting qualities had a greater impact on Chinese adolescent morbidities such as hopelessness and psychological well-being indexed by self-esteem, purpose of life, and life satisfaction. One recent study also identified that paternal, but not maternal, expectations regarding children's future significantly affected children's well-being (47). Generally speaking, fathers are less involved in parenting tasks than mothers are, especially in Chinese society (48). In the present study, fathers were less controlling and had a poorer relationship with the children as compared to mothers. However, as fathers occupy a superior and controlling role in Chinese families, fathers may be more influential in their children's developmental outcomes although they are less involved and more detached. This interpretation is consistent with other findings in early adolescents (49).

Different from the present study, Shek et al. (28) found that paternal and maternal factors had equal impacts on children's IA levels. One possible explanation is that the age of the adolescents may have served as a moderator. Specifically, Shek et al.'s (28) study involved early adolescents (mean age = 12.59 years old) who were younger as compared to the present participants. A recent study in Korea supported the potential moderating effect of children's age on parental impacts (50). The study showed that, while parental affection negatively predicted children's problematic mobile game use in the elementary school group, parental monitoring lead to less problematic mobile game use in the high school group. Future research could directly compare paternal impacts on adolescent IA with reference to different age groups to test the possibility that paternal factors might have generally greater impacts as compared to maternal factors among older adolescents.

Differential parental impacts should also be discussed with reference to specific aspects of parenting. As mentioned earlier, when each dimension of the parent–child subsystem was considered in the IGC models, paternal behavioral control but not maternal behavioral control, and maternal psychological control but not paternal psychological control, were significantly linked with the initial level of children's IA. Basically, these findings are consistent with the concurrent and longitudinal predictive effects derived from the regression analyses. When paternal and maternal factors were examined simultaneously in regressions, paternal behavioral control and maternal psychological control were the two most robust predictors of adolescent IA, both concurrently and longitudinally. The consistent findings obtained from different analytical approaches informed the reliability of these findings.

Based on the findings, it can be conjectured that the impact of paternal factors is greater in terms of behavioral control, while maternal parenting is more influential in terms of psychological control over time. In previous studies, while some findings suggested stronger paternal impacts on adolescent development, some others supported stronger maternal impacts. One possible explanation for the mixed findings is that most of these studies focused on overall parenting without considering different dimensions of parenting or only referred to one dimension of parenting. For example, in studies that identified greater impacts of maternal psychological control on adolescent development outcomes, behavioral control or other parental factors were not examined simultaneously (25, 51). Chen et al. (52) identified stronger influence of fathers' indulgence, but they did not take psychological control into account. In fact, the results in a few studies that considered different domain-specific parental factors showed that both parents' influence differed from each other on specific aspects. For example, while maternal control showed a greater impact on children's achievement motivation and positive youth development than did paternal control, paternal sacrifice exerted a stronger influence than did maternal sacrifice on adolescents' positive youth development (53, 54). Thus, our findings suggest that it is important to distinguish between different processes of parenting in examining and comparing paternal and maternal influences.

There are two additional observations which deserve attention. First, the concurrent negative impacts of both parents' psychological control on adolescent IA are consistent with findings on early adolescence (28). Researchers advocated that parental psychological control would ruin children's age-appropriate sense of autonomy, which in turn results in adolescent misbehaviors and damaged psychological well-being. However, most of the findings were derived from samples of adolescents in Western cultures, which value autonomy and independence. Some scholars argued that in Asian countries, including China, parental psychological control may not be so detrimental as cultural norms in these areas emphasize children's obligations to the family and interdependence rather than independence (8, 55). Nevertheless, recent studies showed that in Chinese adolescents, parents' psychological control was concurrently associated with children's maladjustment, such as depression and anxiety as well as hopelessness (56). Thus, the present study adds to the extant literature by showing that the negative impacts of parents' psychological controlling behavior may be universal.

Second, the present longitudinal findings suggest that, during senior high school years, maternal psychological control might be more influential than paternal psychological control is. This observation is not consistent with the general view that negative paternal parenting is especially detrimental due to the greater authority and power of fathers in the family and the related attributes of the father–child dyad (30). For example, paternal psychological control was more closely associated with early adolescents' externalizing behaviors over time than maternal psychological control was (26, 30). Likewise, in early adolescence, paternal psychological control served as a more robust longitudinal predictor than maternal psychological control did for adolescent IA (28). Among university students, paternal, but not maternal, negative parenting in terms of denying and overprotectiveness resulted in a higher level of children's problematic Internet use (57). Nevertheless, it is possible that paternal and maternal negative parenting impact different adolescent developmental outcomes. For instance, Shek (58) found that paternal—but not maternal—psychological control showed a significant predictive effect on adolescents' life satisfaction in 1 year. Meanwhile, maternal—but not paternal—psychological control was a significant predictor of changes in adolescents' self-esteem. The present findings suggest the importance of examining this issue with reference to different measures of development outcomes among different age groups of adolescents.

The present study sheds light on the differential parental impacts on the concurrent and future levels and the growth rate of adolescents' IA during senior high school years. However, several limitations should be noted. First, the present study employed a quantitative design, which was unable to reveal the subjective feelings of participants and delineate potential mechanisms behind the quantitative findings. To portray a comprehensive picture and to triangulate the present findings, it is necessary to further investigate differential parental impacts using qualitative research strategies, such as focus group interview and case study.

Second, the utilized data were obtained only from adolescent participants' self-report. Although social desirability is a major shortcoming of self-report methodology, self-report data have commonly been collected in adolescent research, especially in longitudinal studies [e.g., (59)], possibly due to ethical and practical concerns. Besides, it is efficient and cost-effective, and it can be argued that adolescents themselves know their own experiences and lives much better than others. In the present study, to reduce social desirability bias as much as possible, anonymity was emphasized, and the participants were clearly instructed to provide answers based on their true perceptions without communicating with others. Nevertheless, future research could employ individual interviews in addition to self-report or involve different informants such as parents and teachers.

Third, the present study surveyed Chinese adolescents in a single region (i.e., Hong Kong). To verify and expand the generalizability of the present findings, future studies will benefit from investigating related questions among adolescents in other Chinese communities, such as those from mainland China, and adolescents from other ethnicities. Fourth, from Wave 4 to Wave 6, 899 participants withdrew from the study. In particular, more than 500 students withdrew from the study after Wave 4. This fact might affect the present findings. However, comparisons between the matched sample and dropouts suggested that there were no substantial differences in key variables between the two groups. Thus, the present findings were unlikely to be significantly affected by systematic attrition. Finally, as a control variable, family intactness was indicated by parental marital status. Since there were other factors related to family intactness, such as family members' residential status (e.g., whether children are living with their parents) (60), future studies could consider more comprehensive measures of family intactness.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

DS designed the project and contributed to all steps of the work. XZ contributed to the development of the idea and data interpretation. XZ and DD drafted the work and revised it based on the critical comments and editing provided by DS. All authors approve of the final version of the manuscript and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

The longitudinal study in the Project P.A.T.H.S. and preparation for this paper are financially supported by the Hong Kong Jockey Club Charities Trust.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Cheng C, Li AY. Internet addiction prevalence and quality of (real) life: a meta-analysis of 31 Nations Across Seven World Regions. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2014) 17:755–60. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Xu J, Shen L, Yan C, Hu H, Yang F, Wang L, et al. Parent-adolescent interaction and risk of adolescent internet addiction: a population-based study in Shanghai. BMC Psychiatry. (2014) 14:112. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-14-112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Yen J, Ko C, Yen C, Wu H, Yang M. The Comorbid psychiatric pymptoms of Internet addiction: attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression, social phobia, and hostility. J Adoles Health. (2007) 41:93–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Shi X, Wang J, Zou H. Family functioning and Internet addiction among Chinese adolescents: the mediating roles of self-esteem and loneliness. Comput Hum Behav. (2017) 76:201–10. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.07.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lam LT, Peng Z, Mai J, Jing J. Factors associated with Internet addiction among adolescents. CyberPsychol Behav. (2009) 12:551–5. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Siomos K, Floros G, Fisoun V, Evaggelia D, Farkonas N, Sergentani E, et al. Evolution of Internet addiction in Greek adolescent students over a two-year period: the impact of parental bonding. EurChild Adoles Psychiatry. (2012) 21:211–9. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0254-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bleakley A, Ellithorpe M, Romer D. The role of parents in problematic internet use among US adolescents. Media Commun. (2016) 3:24–34. doi: 10.17645/mac.v4i3.523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Wang Q, Pomerantz EM, Chen H. The role of parents' control in early adolescents' psychological functioning: a longitudinal investigation in the United States and China. Child Development. (2007) 78:1592–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01085.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Barber BK, Stolz H, Olsen J, Collins WA, Burchinal M. Parental support, psychological control, and behavioral control: assessing relevance across time, culture, and method. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (2005) 70:i−147. Available online at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/3701442

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

10. Steinberg L, Elmen JD, Mounts NS. Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Dev. (1989) 60:1424–36. doi: 10.2307/1130932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Barber BK, Maughan SL, Olsen JA. Patterns of parenting across adolescence. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2005) 2005:5–16. doi: 10.1002/cd.124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Barber BK, Harmon EL. Violating the self: Parental psychological control of children and adolescents. In: Barber BK, editor. Intrusive Parenting: How Psychological Control Affects Children and Adolescents. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (2002). p. 15–52. doi: 10.1037/10422-002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Shek DTL. Perceived parental control and parent-child relational qualities in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. Sex Roles. (2005) 53:635–46. doi: 10.1007/s11199-005-7730-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pomerantz EM, Eaton MM. Developmental differences in children's conceptions of parental control:“ They love me, but they make me feel incompetent”. Merrill Palmer Q. (2000) 46:140–67. Available online at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23093346

Google Scholar

15. Kakihara F, Tilton-weaver L. Adolescents ' interpretations of parental control: differentiated by domain and types of control. Child Dev. (2009) 80:1722–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01364.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Shek DTL, Law MYM. Parental behavioral control, parental psychological control and parent-child relational qualities: relationships to Chinese adolescent risk behavior. In: Shek DTL, Sun RCF, editors. Chinese Adolescents in Hong Kong: Family Life, Psychological Well-Being and Risk Behavior. Singapore: Springer (2014). p. 51–69. doi: 10.1007/978-981-287-143-5_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wang B, Stanton B, Li X, Cottrell L, Deveaux L, Kaljee L. The influence of parental monitoring and parent-adolescent communication on Bahamian adolescent risk involvement: a three-year longitudinal examination. Soc Sci Med. (2013) 97:161–9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Forehand R, Long N, Hedrick M. Family characteristics of adolescents who display overt and covert behavior problems. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (1987) 18:325–8. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(87)90046-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Cottrell L, Yu S, Liu H, Deveaux L, Lunn S, Bain RM, et al. Gender-based model comparisons of maternal values, monitoring, communication, and early adolescent risk behavior. J Adolesc Health. (2007) 41:371–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Burk WJ, Laursen B. Mother and adolescent reports of associations between child behavior problems and mother-child relationship qualities: separating shared variance from individual variance. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2010) 38:657–67. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9396-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Day RD, Acock A. Youth ratings of family processes and father role performance of resident and nonresident fathers. In: Day RD, Lamb ME, editors. Conceptualizing and Measuring Father Involvement. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum (2004). p. 239–56.

Google Scholar

22. Gryczkowski MR, Jordan SS, Mercer SH. Differential relations between mothers' and fathers' parenting practices and child externalizing behavior. J Child Fam Stud. (2010) 19:539–46. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9326-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Leung JTY, Shek DTL, Lin L. Mother-child discrepancy in perceived parental control and adolescent filial piety in poor single-mother families. J Adolesc. (2017) 60:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Pleck JH, Masciadrelli BP. Paternal involvement by U.S. residential fathers: levels, sources, and consequences. In: Lamb ME. editor. The Role of the Father in Child Development, 4th ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley (2004). p. 222–71.

Google Scholar

25. Giles G, Price IR. Adolescent computer use: approach, avoidance, and parental control. Aust J Psychol. (2008) 60:63–71. doi: 10.1080/00049530701829896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lansford JE, Laird RD, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA. Mothers' and fathers' autonomy-relevant parenting: longitudinal links with adolescents' externalizing and internalizing behavior. J Youth Adolesc. (2014) 43:1877–89. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-0079-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yu L, Shek DTL. Internet addiction in Hong Kong adolescents: a three-year longitudinal study. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol. (2013) 26:S10–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2013.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Shek DTL, Zhu X, Ma CMS. The influence of parental control and parent-child relational qualities on adolescent Internet addiction: a 3-year longitudinal study in Hong Kong. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:642. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. McKinney C, Renk K. Differential parenting between mothers and fathers: implications for late adolescents. J Fam Issues. (2008) 29:806–27. doi: 10.1177/0192513X07311222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Rogers KN, Buchanan CM, Winchell ME. Psychological control during early adolescence: links to adjustment in differing parent/adolescent dyads. J Early Adolesc. (2003) 23:349–83. doi: 10.1177/0272431603258344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Shek DTL, Chi X, Yu L. Internet addiction in Hong Kong adolescents based on four waves of longitudinal data. In: Lee TY, Shek DTL, Sun RCF, editors. Student Well-Being in Chinese Adolescents in Hong Kong: Theory, Intervention and Research. Singapore: Springer (2015). p. 293–308. doi: 10.1007/978-981-287-582-2_22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Shek DTL. Paternal and maternal influences on the psychological well-being, substance abuse, and delinquency of Chinese adolescents experiencing economic disadvantage. J Clin Psychol. (2005) 61:219–34. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Shek DTL, Sun RCF, Ma CMS editors. Chinese Adolescents in Hong Kong: Family Life, Psychological Well-Being and Risk Behavior. Singapore: Springer (2014). p. 1–237. doi: 10.1007/978-981-287-143-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Young KS. Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychol Behav. (1998) 1:237–44. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Suler J. Computer and cyberspace “ddiction”. Int J Appl Psychoanal Stud. (2004) 1:359–62. doi: 10.1002/aps.90

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chi X, Lin L, Zhang P. Internet addiction among college students in China: prevalence and psychosocial correlates. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2016) 19:567–73. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Shek DTL, Tang VMY, Lo CY. Internet addiction in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong: Assessment, profiles, and psychosocial correlates. ScientificWorldJournal. (2008) 8:776–87. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2008.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Shek DTL, Yu L. Adolescent internet addiction in Hong Kong: prevalence, change, and correlates. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol. (2016) 29:S22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2015.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Shek DTL, Law MYM. Assessment of parent-child subsystem qualities in Chinese adolescents: behavioral control, psychological control and parent-child relational qualities. Int J Child Health Hum Dev. (2015) 8:207–17.

Google Scholar

40. Shek DTL, Liang LY. A 6-year longitudinal study of self-efficacy in Chinese secondary school students in Hong Kong. Int J Disabil Hum Dev. (2016) 15:377–86. doi: 10.1515/ijdhd-2017-5005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Shek DTL, Ma CMS. Longitudinal data analyses using linear mixed models in SPSS: concepts, procedures and illustrations. ScientificWorldJournal. (2011). 11:42–76. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2011.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Shek DTL, Zhu X. Self-reported risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention in Hong Kong adolescents: the role of moral competence and spirituality. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:430. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wu AM, Lau JT, Cheng K-M, Law RW, Tse VW, Lau MM. Direct and interaction effects of co-existing familial risk factors and protective factors associated with Internet addiction among Chinese students in Hong Kong. J Early Adolesc. (2018) 38:429–50. doi: 10.1177/0272431616671826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Li X, Li D, Newman J. Parental behavioral and psychological control and problematic Internet use among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of self-control. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2013) 16:442–7. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Shek DTL. Parenting characteristics and adolescent psychological well-being: a longitudinal study in a Chinese context. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. (1999) 125:27–44.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

46. Shek DTL. Paternal and maternal influences on the psychological well-being of Chinese adolescents. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. (1999) 125:269–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

47. Leung JTY, Shek DTL. The influence of parental beliefs on the development of Chinese adolescents experiencing economic disadvantage: maternal control as a mediator. J Fam Issues. (2016) 37:543–73. doi: 10.1177/0192513X13518776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Shek DTL. Perceived parental control and parent-child relational qualities in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong: parent gender, child gender and grade differences. Sex Roles. (2008) 58:666–81. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9371-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Shek DTL, Law MYM. Jia ting yin su yu xiang gang chu zhong sheng wang yin de guan xi [Relationships between family factors and Internet addiction among junior secondary school students in Hong Kong]. In: Shek DTL, Han X, Li X, editors. Review on Child, Youth and Family Social Work, Vol. 2. Shanghai: East China University of Science and Technology Press (2015). p. 140–62.

Google Scholar

50. Jang Y, Ryu S. The role of parenting behavior in adolescents' problematic mobile game use. Soc Behav Pers. (2016) 44:269–82. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2016.44.2.269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tam VCW. A comparison of fathers' and mothers' contributions in the prediction of academic performance of school-age children in Hong Kong. Int J Psychol. (2009) 44:147–56. doi: 10.1080/00207590801910242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Chen X, Liu M, Li D. Parental warmth, control, and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: a longitudinal study. J Fam Psychol. (2000) 14:401–19. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.14.3.401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Leung JTY, Shek DTL. Are family processes related to achievement motivation of Chinese adolescents experiencing economic disadvantage in Hong Kong? Int J Disabil Hum Dev. (2013) 12:115–25. doi: 10.1515/ijdhd-2012-0139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Leung JTY, Shek DTL. Parenting for resilience: family processes and psychosocial competence of Chinese adolescents experiencing economic disadvantage in Hong Kong. Int J Disabil Hum Dev. (2013) 12:127–37. doi: 10.1515/ijdhd-2012-0137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Greenfield PM, Keller H, Fuligni A, Maynard A. Cultural pathways through universal development. Ann Rev Psychol. (2003) 54:461–90. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Luebbe AM, Tu C, Fredrick JW. Socialization goals, parental psychological control, and youth anxiety in Chinese students: moderated indirect effects based on school type. J Youth Adolesc. (2018) 47:413–29. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0784-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Ni X, Qian Y, Wang Y. Factors affecting pathological Internet use among Chinese university students. Social Behavior and Personality. (2017) 45:1057–68. doi: 10.2224/sbp.6039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Shek DTL. A longitudinal study of perceived parental psychological control and psychological well-being in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. J Clin Psychol. (2007) 63:1–22. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Thornberry TP, Krohn MD. Comparison of self-report and official data for measuring crime. In: Pepper JV, Petrie CV, editors. Measurement Problems in Criminal Justice Research: Workshop Summary. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press (2003). p. 43–94.

Google Scholar

60. Rog DJ, Henderson KA, Lunn LM, Greer AL, Ellis ML. The interplay between housing stability and child separation: Implications for practice and policy. Am J Commun Psychol. (2017) 60:114–24. doi: 10.1002/ajcp.12148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adolescent, Chinese students, internet addiction, father, mother, growth curve modeling

Citation: Shek DTL, Zhu X and Dou D (2019) Influence of Family Processes on Internet Addiction Among Late Adolescents in Hong Kong. Front. Psychiatry 10:113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00113

Received: 18 November 2018; Accepted: 15 February 2019;
Published: 12 March 2019.

Edited by:

Takahiro A. Kato, Kyushu University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Masaru Tateno, Sapporo Medical University, Japan
Tae Young Choi, Catholic University of Daegu, South Korea

Copyright © 2019 Shek, Zhu and Dou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniel T. L. Shek, ZGFuaWVsLnNoZWtAcG9seXUuZWR1Lmhr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.