Mind wandering refers to a state where attention is withdrawn from the task or situation currently at hand and drifts to internal thoughts, feelings or perceptions. This ubiquitous state has been reported to occupy up to half of the waking time of individuals (1). Until quite recently research has largely neglected this phenomenon, underlining why mind wandering has been referred to as the dark matter of cognitive neuroscience [e.g., (2)]. Mind wandering can be experimentally accessed, for instance, by thought probes and behavioral markers (e.g., errors, reaction times) during sustained attention tasks. It can have positive and negative effects (3). Amongst the positive effects are enhanced creativity and better future planning. The negative effects range from errors in ongoing perceptual and cognitive tasks to increased stress levels and a decline in mood. Since reduced mood conversely causes more frequent episodes of mind wandering (4) this mutual interdependence constitutes a vicious circle.
Rumination is a subtype of mind wandering and is a characteristic of patients suffering from major depressive disorder (MDD). Rumination is defined as repetitive, often obsessive thoughts with similar content. In depressive subjects these thoughts are often concerned with negative issues originating from past experiences (5). Rumination has been linked to the onset (6) and maintenance of depression (7), as well as to its severity. From 11 studies reviewed by Morrison and O'Conner (8) 10 reported that increased rumination is associated with increased suicidality. It has been proposed that mind wandering, which is negatively valenced, thematically homogeneous, and associatively narrowed is maladaptive and gives way to rumination leading to depressive symptoms (9). The relationship between mind wandering and depression has recently been experimentally quantified. Hoffmann and colleagues (10) used a non-demanding task requiring continuous attention, which from time to time was interrupted by so-called “experience sampling” probes. These probes consist of questions inquiring as to whether the subject's attention was focused on the task or not, and the content of their thoughts. Patients suffering from MDD reported more mind wandering episodes than healthy controls and that their thoughts were less positive, more past-oriented and self-related.
Hence, methods reducing the propensity or modulating the content characteristics of mind wandering represent potential therapeutic approaches in MDD. One approach is trying to enhance mindfulness or, in other words, meta-awareness of inner experiences, for instance, in the context of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (11). However, mindfulness training is demanding, requires disciplined practice, which is beyond the capabilities of some patients, and can produce adverse effects [e.g., (12, 13)]. Brain stimulation techniques may provide an alternative option. But is there any evidence that mind wandering can be modulated using brain stimulation? Mind wandering has been shown to be related to activity in default mode network (DMN) and executive control regions (14, 15). Recently, two studies applying transcranial electrical stimulation provided evidence for a modulation of mind wandering (16, 17). Both studies targeted the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), an executive control region, one via anodal [(16), DLPFC], the other via cathodal stimulation [(17), LPFC], which are thought to increase/decrease cortical activation respectively [e.g., (18)]. Indeed, under anodal stimulation of the DLPFC, the propensity to mind wander was reported to increase (16), whereas under cathodal stimulation, a decline in mind wandering was observed (17). However, it remained unclear in these studies, whether the propensity to mind wander or just meta-awareness of the mind wandering episodes was modulated, which is indeed a prerequisite for reporting mind wandering and may also be related to DLPFC activity (19). Moreover, the DLPFC may be involved in the later, rather than in the early stages of mind wandering, e.g., in the elaboration and evaluation of the thoughts (20, 21). The most effective approach to modulate mind wandering, therefore, would be to target the region responsible for initiating mind wandering.
The big question is: which brain region initiates mind wandering? For the first time, a recent fMRI study has shed light on this question (22). In this study participants with long-term experience in mindfulness meditation (> 3,000 h) were instructed to immediately signal to the investigator the arising of spontaneous thoughts. The methodological strength of this study lies in the fact that subjects highly experienced in the observation of thought processes were recruited, which are capable of instantly recognizing the onset of thoughts—a rather difficult or impossible task for untrained subjects. Ellamil and colleagues found that activity in default mode and executive control regions, as well as in regions associated with the monitoring of inner experiences, were related to the occurrence of the spontaneous thoughts. Most interestingly, the region which was activated first, even before subjects signaled the onset of a thought, was the hippocampus (i.e., a default mode region), which is mainly known for its role in memory processes. The authors speculated that the hippocampus is well-suited to initiate spontaneous thoughts due to its capacity for novel connections at the synaptic level and its dense links to other brain areas (21, 22). Moreover, hippocampal activity is crucial for remembering past events and envisioning the future, which are typical contents of mind wandering [e.g., (23)]. Indeed, mental time travel during mind wandering has been shown to be accompanied by increased interaction between the hippocampus and other DMN regions (24).
Evidence for an involvement of the hippocampus in mind wandering also comes from a recent study investigating subjects with bilateral hippocampal damage (25). Based on a small sample [6 patients vs. 12 controls) the authors have reported that these patients engaged in as much mind wandering (decoupled spontaneous thoughts) as control participants. However, average mind wandering was numerically smaller in the patients and the hypothesis of decreased mind wandering was rejected based on a rather low p-value (two-tailed p = 0.12). Importantly, mind wandering of the patients was less past, but more present oriented and less visual-episodic, but more verbally-semantic. Other studies have reported a prominent contribution of hippocampal activity patterns to rumination in MDD (26) and altered DMN-hippocampus connectivity after sad memory-recall in remitted-MDD (27). Furthermore, Golchert et al. (28) have described structural magnetic resonance imaging differences associated with individual variations in the intentionality of mind wandering (i.e., spontaneous vs. deliberate). They found that spontaneous mind wandering is related to increased cortical thickness in the retrosplenial cortex, which is a node region for information transfer between the hippocampus and prefrontal, parietal and occipital regions [e.g., (29)]. They interpreted these results as indicating “that the spontaneous onset of mind wandering occurs when activity in the hippocampus is integrated into an ongoing train of thought via the retrosplenial cortex.” In line with these arguments, a recent fMRI study has found that the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC), a region involved in the control and modulation of mood, becomes decoupled from the hippocampus and the retrosplenial cortex after administration of ketamine (30). Ketamine, a dissociative anestetic, is known to have rapid-acting antidepressant properties (31). Wong and colleagues (30) interpreted their results as being related to the reduction of mind wandering and depressive rumination. In accordance with this interpretation, a metanalysis of neuroimaging studies has shown that functional connectivity between the subgenual prefrontal cortex (including the sgACC) and the DMN is increased in major depression and often predicts levels of rumination (32). Taken together, the studies described above, in my opinion, point into the direction that the hippocampus is a core or even initiating region for mind wandering and rumination.
Could the hippocampus be targeted using brain stimulation in order to modulate the content characteristics or reduce the propensity of mind wandering in MDD? With regard to electric stimulation, the hippocampus cannot be easily accessed by the transcranial technique. Thus, the impact of conventional electric stimulation on the hippocampus can only be directly investigated in patients, in which intracranial electrodes are implanted, for instance, for the presurgical evaluation of epileptic seizure onset zones (33). However, several non-invasive brain stimulation approaches have recently been developed which may allow targeting of the hippocampus. One such method is pulsed transcranial ultrasound stimulation, which has been shown to be able to modulate hippocampal activity in mice (34). Recently, this technique has been successfully applied to humans for the modulation of activity in primary somatosensory (35) and visual cortex (36). Another technique is transcranial electric stimulation with temporally interfering electric fields (37). This technique uses the superposition of high-frequency electric fields comprised of slightly different frequencies, resulting in a low-frequency amplitude modulation. These interfering electric field patterns produce a spatially focused form of deep brain stimulation. It has been demonstrated in mice that the hippocampus can be selectively stimulated with this technique (37). However, the safety and transfer of this method to humans still has to be proven. In the auditory domain a similar technique has been established: auditory beat stimulation. This stimulation either consists of the superposition of two nearby frequencies identically applied to both ears (monaural beats), or the application of each frequency separately to each ear (binaural beats). Research in presurgical epilepsy patients has shown that hippocampal activity can be modulated by this kind of auditory stimulation (38).
Of course, there may be negative side effects of hippocampal stimulation, which should be considered. One potential side effect could be, for instance, that brain stimulation reducing hippocampal activity may not only decrease mind wandering, but also affect memory functioning. Since memory deficits are common in MDD—as are deficits across most cognitive domains [e.g., (39)], an additive stimulation-related memory impairment may be hard to tolerate. Another aspect to consider is that hippocampal functioning may contribute to the recovery from depression by supporting cognitive flexibility (40). Hence, the time period of hippocampal stimulation with regard to a possible transition from maladaptive to adaptive mind wandering (9) may be an important issue.
Is there evidence for a correlated modulation of rumination and hippocampal activity by antidepressant medication? Although it has been reported that treatment with selective serotonin-reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and serotonin-noradrenalin-reuptake inhibitors (SSNRIs) reduces rumination in major depression [e.g., (41)] and dysthymia [e.g., (42)], it is yet unknown whether certain antidepressants specifically target rumination. With regard to emotional processing, a meta-analysis of 9 fMRI and PET studies revealed decreased activation of the hippocampus and other limbic regions following antidepressant drug treatment mostly using SSRIs and SSNRIs (43). However, emotional processing is not always accompanied by rumination [e.g., (44)]. Regarding resting state network dynamics a single-dose of the SSRI sertraline in healthy subjects reduced functional connectivity (fMRI) between the limbic system, including the hippocampus, and the executive control network (45). Furthermore, a graph theory analysis of resting state functional connectivity (fMRI) in MDD patients vs. controls showed decreased nodal efficiency of the left hippocampus and an increase of nodal efficiency after treatment with antidepressants associated with an improvement of depression scores (46). The relation of these network changes to rumination, however, is not obvious. Thus, direct evidence for a correlated modulation of rumination and hippocampal activity by antidepressant medication is yet lacking.
In summary, the reduction or modification of contents of mind wandering is a potential therapeutic approach for the treatment of major depression. Recent findings suggest that the hippocampus is a core structure involved in, or even initiating mind wandering. Several non-invasive brain stimulation techniques have been developed, which may allow for the modulation of hippocampal activity. Therefore, I propose to explore whether a reduction of the propensity or modification of the content characteristics of mind wandering via modulation of hippocampal activity can be achieved with these techniques. If so, this line of research may open a new avenue for the treatment of major depression.
Author Contributions
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The author would like to thank Leila Chaieb, Christian Hoppe and the two reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions.
References
1. Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT. A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science (2010) 330:932. doi: 10.1126/science.1192439
2. Callard F, Margulies DS. The industrious subject: cognitive neuroscience's revaluation of ‘rest’. In: Cognitive Architecture: From Bio-politics to Noo-politics – Architecture & Mind in the Age of Communication and Information. ed. Hauptmann D, Neidich W Rotterdam: 010 Publishers (2011). p. 324–346.
3. Mooneyham BW, Schooler JW. The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: a review. Can J Exp Psychol. (2013) 67:11–8. doi: 10.1037/a0031569
4. Smallwood J, Fitzgerald A, Miles LK, Phillips LH. Shifting moods, wandering minds: negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion (2009) 9:271–6. doi: 10.1037/a0014855
5. Nolen-Hoeksema S. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. (2000) 109:504–11. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504
6. Robinson SM, Alloy LB. Negative inferential style and stress-reactive rumination: interactive risk factors in the aetiology of depression. Cogn Ther Res. (2003) 27:275–91. doi: 10.1023/A:1023914416469
7. Nolen-Hoeksama S. Response to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J Abnorm Psychol. (1991) 100:569–82. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569
8. Morrison R, O'Connor RC. A systematic review of the relationship between rumination and suicidality. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2008) 38:523–38. doi: 10.1521/suli.2008.38.5.523
9. Marchetti I, Koster EHW, Klinger E, Alloy LB. Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: the dark side of the wandering mind. Clin Psychol Sci. (2016) 4:835–57. doi: 10.1177/2167702615622383
10. Hoffmann F, Banzhaf C, Kanske P, Bermpohl F, Singer T. Where the depressed mind wanders: self-generated thought patterns as assessed through experience sampling as a state marker of depression. J Affect Disord. (2016) 198:127–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.005
11. Segal Z, Teasdale J, Williams M. (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
12. Farias M, Wikholm C, Delmonte R. What is mindfulness-based therapy good for? Eviden Limitat Controvers Lancet Psychiatry (2016) 3:1012–3. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30211-5
13. Groves P. Mindfulness in psychiatry – where are we now? B J Psych Bull. (2016) 40:289–92. doi: 10.1192/pb.bp.115.052993
14. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science (2007) 315:393–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295
15. Christoff K, Gordon AM, Smallwood J, Smith R, Schooler JW. Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contribution to mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2009) 106:8719–24. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900234106
16. Axelrod V, Rees G, Lavidor M, Bar M. Increasing propensity to mind-wandr with transcranial direct current stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2015) 112:3314–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421435112
17. Kajimura S, Nomura M. Decreasing propensity to mind-wander with transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuropsychologia (2015) 75:533–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.013
18. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology (2001) 57:1899–901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899
19. Fox KC, Christoff K. Transcranial direct current stimulation to lateral prefrontal cortex could increase meta-awareness of mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2015) 112:E2414. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504686112
20. Fox KC, Spreng RN, Ellamil M, Andrews-Hanna JR, Christoff K. The wandering brain: meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage (2015) 111:611–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039
21. Christoff K, Irving ZC, Fox KC, Spreng RN, Andrew-Hanna JR. Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2016) 17:718–31. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.113
22. Ellamil M, Fox KC, Dixon ML, Pritchard S, Todd RM, Thompson E, et al. Dynamics of neural recruitment surrounding the spontaneous arising of thoughts in experienced mindfulness practitioners. Neuroimage (2016) 136:186–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.034
23. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Huang C, Buckner RL. Evidence for the default network's role in spontaneous cognition. J Neurophysiol. (2010) 104:322–35. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2009
24. Karapanagiotidis T, Bernhardt BC, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Tracking thoughts: exploring the neural architecture of mental time travel during mind-wandering. Neuroimage (2017) 147:272–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.031
25. McCormick C, Rosenthal CR, Miller TD, Maguire EA. Mind-wandering in people with hippocampal damage. J Neurosci. (2018) 12:1812–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1812-17.2018
26. Mandell D, Siegle GJ, Shutt L, Feldmiller J, Thase ME. Neural substrates of trait ruminations in depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2014) 123:35–48. doi: 10.1037/a0035834
27. Figueroa CA, Mocking RJT, van Wingen G, Martens S, Ruhé HG, Schene AH. Aberrant default-mode network-hippocampus connectivity after sad memory-recall in remitted-depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:1803–13. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx108
28. Golchert J, Smallwood J, Jefferies E, Seli P, Huntenburg JM, Liem F, et al. Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks. Neuroimage (2017) 146:226–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025
29. Vann SD, Aggleton JP, Maguire EA. What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:792–802. doi: 10.1038/nrn2733
30. Wong JJ, O'Daly I, Mehta MA, Young AH, Stone JM. Ketamine modulates subgenual cingulate connectivity with the memory-related neural ciruit - a mechanism of relevance to resistant depression? PeerJ. (2016) 4:e1710. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1710
31. Abdallah CG, Sanacora G, Duman RS, Krystal JH. Ketamine and rapid-acting antidepressants: a window into a new neurobiology for mood disorder therapeutics. Annu Rev Med. (2015) 66:509–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev-med-053013-062946
32. Hamilton JP, Farmer M, Fogelman P, Gotlib IH. Depressive rumination, the default-mode network, and the dark matter of neuroscience. Biol Psychiat. (2015) 78:224–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.020
33. Lee H, Fell J, Axmacher N. Electric engram: how deep brain stimulation affects memory. Trends Cogn Sci. (2013) 17:574–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.002
34. Tufail Y, Matyushov A, Baldwin N, Tauchmann ML, Georges J, Yoshihiro A, et al. Transcranial pulsed ultrasound stimulates intact brain circuits. Neuron (2010) 66:681–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.008
35. Legon W, Sato TF, Opitz A, Mueller J, Barbour A, Williams A, et al. Transcranial focused ultrasound modulates the activity of primary somatosensory cortex in humans. Nat Neurosci. (2014) 17:322–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.3620
36. Lee W, Kim HC, Jung Y, Chung YA, Song IU, Lee JH, et al. Transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation of human primary visual cortex. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:34026. doi: 10.1038/srep34026
37. Grossman N, Bono D, Dedic N, Kodandaramaiah SB, Rudenko A, Suk HJ, et al. Noninvasive deep brain stimulation via temporally interfering electric fields. Cell (2017) 169:1029–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.024
38. Becher AK, Höhne M, Axmacher N, Chaieb L, Elger CE, Fell J. Intracranial electroencephalography power and phase synchronization changes during monaural and binaural beat stimulation. Eur J Neurosci. (2015) 41:254–63. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12760
39. Ahern E, Semkovska M. Cognitive functioning in the first-episode of major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology (2017) 31:52–72. doi: 10.1037/neu0000319
40. Anacker C, Hen R. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility – linking memory and mood. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2017) 18:335–46. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.45
41. Bieling PJ, Hawley LL, Bloch RT, Corcoran KM, Levitan RD, Young LT, et al. Treatment-specific changes in decentering following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy versus antidepressant medication or placebo for prevention of depressive relapse. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2012) 80:365–72. doi: 10.1037/a0027483
42. Kelly O, Matheson K, Ravindran A, Merali Z, Anisman H. Ruminative coping among patients with dysthymia before and after pharmacotherapy. Depress Anxiety (2007) 24:233–43. doi: 10.1002/da.20236
43. Delaveau P, Jabourian M, Lemogne C, Guionnet S, Bergouignan L, Fossati P. Brain effects of antidepressants in major depression: a meta-analysis of emotional processing studies. J Affect Disord. (2011) 130:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.09.032
44. Lanciano T, Curci A, Zatton E. Why do some people ruminate more or less than others? The role of emotional intelligence ability. Eur J Psychol. (2010) 2:65–84. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v6i2.185
45. Klaassens BL, van Gorsel HC, Khalili-Mahani N, van der Grond J, Wyman BT, Whitcher B, et al. Single-dose serotonergic stimulation shows widespread effects on functional brain connectivity. Neuroimage (2015) 122:440–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.012
Keywords: mind wandering, brain stimulation, hippocampus, major depressive disorder, mood disorders
Citation: Fell J (2018) Is the Hippocampus a Potential Target for the Modulation of Mind Wandering in Major Depression? Front. Psychiatry 9:363. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00363
Received: 26 February 2018; Accepted: 20 July 2018;
Published: 10 August 2018.
Edited by:
Gianluca Serafini, Ospedale San Martino (IRCCS), ItalyReviewed by:
Sara Costi, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United StatesJames Stone, King's College London, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2018 Fell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Juergen Fell, anVlcmdlbi5mZWxsQHVrYi51bmktYm9ubi5kZQ==